Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 24 May 2024
Sec. Perception Science

Binocularly suppressed stimuli induce brain activities related to aesthetic emotions

  • 1Department of Sports Medicine, Osaka Metropolitan University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan
  • 2Precision Medicine Centre, Hokuto Hospital, Obihiro, Japan

Introduction: Aesthetic emotions are a class of emotions aroused by evaluating aesthetically appealing objects or events. While evolutionary aesthetics suggests the adaptive roles of these emotions, empirical assessments are lacking. Previous neuroscientific studies have demonstrated that visual stimuli carrying evolutionarily important information induce neural responses even when presented non-consciously. To examine the evolutionary importance of aesthetic emotions, we conducted a neuroscientific study using magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure induced neural responses to non-consciously presented portrait paintings categorised as biological and non-biological and examined associations between the induced responses and aesthetic ratings.

Methods: MEG and pre-rating data were collected from 23 participants. The pre-rating included visual analogue scales for object saliency, facial saliency, liking, and beauty scores, in addition to ‘biologi-ness,’ which was used for subcategorising stimuli into biological and non-biological. The stimuli were presented non-consciously using a continuous flash suppression paradigm or consciously using binocular presentation without flashing masks, while dichotomic behavioural responses were obtained (beauty or non-beauty). Time-frequency decomposed MEG data were used for correlation analysis with pre-rating scores for each category.

Results: Behavioural data revealed that saliency scores of non-consciously presented stimuli influenced dichotomic responses (beauty or non-beauty). MEG data showed that non-consciously presented portrait paintings induced spatiotemporally distributed low-frequency brain activities associated with aesthetic ratings, which were distinct between the biological and non-biological categories and conscious and non-conscious conditions.

Conclusion: Aesthetic emotion holds evolutionary significance for humans. Neural pathways are sensitive to visual images that arouse aesthetic emotion in distinct ways for biological and non-biological categories, which are further influenced by consciousness. These differences likely reflect the diversity in mechanisms of aesthetic processing, such as processing fluency, active elaboration, and predictive processing. The aesthetic processing of non-conscious stimuli appears to be characterised by fluency-driven affective processing, while top-down regulatory processes are suppressed. This study provides the first empirical evidence supporting the evolutionary significance of aesthetic processing.

1 Introduction

Aesthetic processing is a complex human function encompassing psychological processes, including perceptual, cognitive, and emotional evaluations of aesthetically appealing information, leading to emotional and hedonic experiences. Emotional reactions within these processes are specifically referred to as ‘aesthetic emotions,’ aroused in response to the aesthetic qualities of the stimulus, and are predictive of aesthetic pleasure or displeasure and approach–avoidance behaviour. Examples include ‘feeling of beauty,’ ‘being moved,’ and ‘experiencing suspense’ (Wassiliwizky and Menninghaus, 2021). Humans are assumed to have evolved to aesthetically evaluate the sensory and perceptual qualities of objects, a concept extensively discussed in the field of Darwinian aesthetics (Grammer et al., 2003) or evolutionary aesthetics (Voland and Grammer, 2003). Therefore, the neural processes underlying aesthetic emotions are considered to be evolutionarily grounded and strongly registered functions of the human brain.

Evolutionary aesthetics investigates aesthetic preferences for sensory inputs that facilitate selective attention and emotional responses to perceived objects, leading to adaptive decision-making and problem-solving. Evolutionary aesthetics considers any object as a sensory input, ranging from biological (such as faces, bodies, movements, and vocalisations) to artefactual/cultural objects and events (such as artworks, ornaments, and artful decorations) (Voland and Grammer, 2003). Aesthetic emotions toward biological objects have been associated with mate choice decisions, with the arousal of such emotions contributing to more optimal selections and, thus, fitness benefits. Darwin first introduced the concept of ‘sexual selection’ in ‘The Descent of Man’ (Darwin, 1871), explaining how females in the species prefer certain traits in males, typically features that signify health, superiority, and strength over others, which indicate increased probability of successful reproduction. Size, colour, shape, voice, and smell serve as representative indices of sexual selection. For instance, a ‘good’ body proportion and facial symmetry are common aesthetically favoured traits used by humans during mate selection, as they are indicative of good genotypes (Thornhill et al., 1999; Grammer et al., 2003; Rhodes, 2005; Little et al., 2011; Chatterjee, 2014). Although cultural and individual differences exist, balanced faces and well-proportioned bodies are generally recognised as ‘beautiful’ and are considered attractive when encountered. This finding indicates that the aesthetic emotion aroused by biological objects originated as a physiological reaction in response to important information related to mate selection; therefore, it can be considered congenital. Several studies have suggested that new-borns and infants exhibit strong preferences for symmetrical and attractive faces (Bornstein et al., 1981; Langlois et al., 1987; Grammer et al., 2003; Rhodes, 2005). This implies that the neural system governing the aesthetic emotion aroused by biological objects is registered at a genetic level and has been shaped by natural selection.

When considering non-biological artefactual objects or events as sources of aesthetic emotion, the evolutionary explanation becomes more complicated. Anthropological studies have suggested that the creation of artworks or decorations is a defining feature of the human species, alongside the use of symbolic communication systems, the formation of societies, and the manufacture of complex tools (Mcbrearty and Brooks, 2000; d’Errico and Stringer, 2011). While this explains the evolutionary background of creation, it does not capture the appreciation (including evaluation and subsequent emotional reactions and judgements) of artistic activities. Some researchers have proposed that the aesthetic appreciation of artefactual objects is merely a by-product of adaptation and lacks evolutionary importance (Pinker, 1997; Carroll, 1998; Pinker and Fodor, 2005). In contrast, evolutionary aesthetics provides adaptive explanations regarding the aesthetic emotion aroused by artefactual objects, distinct from that of biological objects. For example, the creation and appreciation of artful objects can be likened to a specific animal behaviour known as ‘display,’ which is often performed by males to attract females, who, in turn, evolve to evaluate male displays to attract the best mate (Etcoff, 1999; Rothenberg, 2011). Similarly, artistic communication can be linked to the behaviour of ‘gift’ in some species, such as providing food or nests, or displaying features such as agility, strength, or hunting skills, all of which are useful for reproduction and evaluated by females (Voland and Grammer, 2003). Moreover, cultural neuroscience research suggests that human cultural differences shape our neural systems, from basic perceptual systems to higher-order systems such as social cognition, as highlighted by the quote ‘culture is, after all, stored in people’s brains’ (Ames and Fiske, 2010), which implies a congenital factor in the affective responses to cultural objects. Taken together, supportive views regarding the evolutionary significance of the aesthetic emotion aroused by non-biological objects appear to be more dominant, despite lacking scientific support. An empirical study examining the qualitative differences in the links to evolutionary significance between biological and artefactual objects demonstrated that the magnitude of aesthetic emotion aroused by biological objects was less likely to be influenced by external opinions compared to that of artefactual objects (Bignardi et al., 2021). The aesthetic ratings for natural objects (faces and landscapes) have also shown stronger inter- and intra-individual agreement than those of artefactual objects (artworks and architectures) (Vessel et al., 2014, 2018), suggesting that shared characteristics of biological stimuli arouse aesthetic emotion. These findings correspond with the evolutionary implication that aesthetic emotions aroused by biological objects are strongly ingrained at the genetic level. Furthermore, these studies indicate that objects recognised as ‘more biological’ and ‘less biological’ have distinct evolutionary relevance. Therefore, investigating both categories separately is warranted when determining the evolutionary importance of aesthetic emotion.

To establish a comprehensive evolutionary explanation of an adaptive trait, its proximate mechanisms (including neural circuitry and emotions controlling mental representations and behaviour) must be clarified (Tinbergen, 1963; Rusch and Voland, 2013). From a neuroscientific perspective, the evolutionary significance of the emotional processing of biological objects has been examined using the subliminal presentation of emotionally arousing faces (Brooks et al., 2012; Dahlén et al., 2022). Studies have demonstrated that arousing faces activate brain regions responsive to emotional processing, even when presented non-consciously or without observer awareness, such as when presented very briefly and immediately masked (masking paradigm), suppressed binocularly (such as continuous flash suppression: CFS paradigm), or under other experimental conditions (Axelrod et al., 2015). Similar findings have been observed for non-biological stimulus categories comparable to emotional faces, such as spiders, snakes, guns, and valenced words (Carlsson et al., 2004; Wendt et al., 2008; Alpers et al., 2009; van Gaal et al., 2014; Fang et al., 2016; Mudrik and Deouell, 2022). High sensitivity and responsiveness to emotional stimuli are directly linked to evolutionary importance because these characteristics allow individuals to quickly respond to approaching harm, thereby enhancing the chance of survival. Non-conscious studies have indicated that the neural processing system is tuned to rapidly detect signals of approaching harm with great sensitivity to avoid threats. Further evidence supports the idea that neural processing systems are tuned for both avoidance and approach. For example, neural responses were recorded for non-consciously presented positive emotions (such as happiness and surprise) and words (Killgore and Yurgelun-Todd, 2004; Somerville et al., 2004; Sergerie et al., 2008; Brooks et al., 2012; van Gaal et al., 2014; Dahlén et al., 2022). A series of neuroscientific findings suggest that the neural system is designed to be responsive to evolutionarily significant information presented non-consciously through any stimulus category (biological or non-biological). Notably, the specialised neural system for processing this evolutionarily important information is not entirely universal or ‘hard-coded’; it may be congenital but can also be shaped by environmental factors. Although the neural system is strongly attuned to certain basic visual features of biological stimuli, such as ‘good’ body proportions and facial symmetry, which are universally recognised across individuals, individual-level fine-tuning remains possible. For example, the ‘other-race effect’ (Stelter and Schweinberger, 2023) explains the phenomenon where facial stimuli belonging to the same ethnic group as the viewer induce distinct autonomic neural responses compared to those from other ethnic groups (Pesciarelli et al., 2021). Facial familiarity also produces similar effects (Axelrod et al., 2015). These studies indicate individual differences in the processing of ‘biological’ visual features that are connected to their evolutionary significance. Therefore, we operationally define the biological and non-biological categories as discrete conversions of a continuous level of ‘biologi-ness’, reflecting the individual’s level of experience in considering an object as biological, based on their intuitive feeling, prior knowledge, and/or experiences.

In this study, we aimed to investigate whether aesthetic emotion has an adaptive function in humans by examining neural responses to aesthetically appealing biological and non-biological objects presented non-consciously. To achieve non-conscious presentation, we used CFS, which allowed for a longer duration of presentation (unlike the masking paradigm) without manipulating the colours of the stimuli (unlike the binocular fusion/rivalry paradigms) (Tsuchiya and Koch, 2004, 2005). Although the precise neural mechanisms underlying the processing of non-consciously presented emotional stimuli remain unclear, a subcortical pathway, which bypasses cortical input and rapidly conveys emotional information to the emotional brain, has been reported to potentially characterise non-conscious processing (LeDoux, 1996, but see Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010). To capture the temporal dynamics with good spatial resolution (Baillet, 2017; Hari and Puce, 2017), we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure neural responses during the CFS task. We hypothesised that if aesthetic emotion has adaptive significance, non-consciously presented aesthetically appealing stimuli should induce MEG responses related to emotional processing. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first neuroscientific study to reveal the evolutionary significance of aesthetic emotions.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and ethics

This study involved 26 healthy adult Japanese participants with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders or professional education in artistic disciplines. Three participants were excluded from the analysis: one due to unremovable artefacts (e.g., saturated signals) in the recorded MEG data from previous dental work, and two for failure to maintain dichoptic vision, leading to ineffective stimulus suppression with CFS. Data obtained from the remaining 23 participants (16 females, all right-handed, mean age 35.74 ± 10.35 years) were analysed. This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hokuto Hospital (approval number: #1108) and Osaka Metropolitan University Graduate School of Medicine (2022-034). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their involvement in the study.

2.2 Stimuli

A set of 5 portrait paintings by 10 artists was sourced from WikiArt1 (Supplementary Table 1). The paintings were selected to achieve gender balance (women, men, and unknown) among the painted models portrayed by each artist, ensuring a difference in the number of female and male models of < 2 (e.g., three women and two men). All paintings were of Western origin, spanning various styles from the 16th to the 20th centuries, and explicitly depicted human faces (portraits) or contained identifiable keywords depicting the presence of human faces, such as ‘head’ and ‘man with a moustache.’ All paintings were cropped to focus solely on the facial areas (from the top of the head to the chin) in a square of 500 × 500 pixels, converted into greyscale, and equated for spatial frequencies and histograms using the SHINE toolbox (Willenbockel et al., 2010). Finally, to enhance the suppression effect, the image edges were blurred, blending them into a background colour, defined as the mean luminance (intensity) of all images. For this process, an additional 25 pixels with the background colour were added to each side of the image, which were blurred by filtering with a two-dimensional Gaussian smoothing kernel with a σ (standard deviation of the Gaussian distribution, indicating the filtering effect strength) determined as a normal probability density function relative to the location of the pixel to the edge. Essentially, pixels closer to the edge were more strongly blurred, and vice versa, ensuring a smooth transition between the image and background colour. The facial stimuli were sorted based on the painters and their era (Supplementary Table 1), with their representative images depicted in Figure 1A.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Examples of facial stimuli after editing; (left top) the stimulus that scored the highest average ‘biologi-ness’ score (ID#19), (right top) the stimulus that scored the highest average beauty score (ID#06), (left bottom) the stimulus that scored the lowest average ‘biologi-ness’, object and facial saliency, and beauty scores (ID#48), and (right bottom) the stimulus that scored the lowest average liking score (ID#35). (B) A non-magnetic stereoscope, placed on the MEG dewer before starting the MEG experiment. (C) Setup for the MEG experiment showing one of our researchers laying down on the bed (instead of the participant) for the purposes of photographing. (D) Schematic illustration of the CFS task. Participants were presented different stimuli to the dominant and non-dominant eyes through the stereoscope. All trials started from the presentation of the fixations, then diverted according to the condition (FF, MF, and MB). FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; MB, Mondrian-background condition; VA, visual angle.

2.3 Pre-ratings

Prior to the MEG experiment, participants completed pre-rating questionnaires to assess the intensity of the aesthetic emotion aroused by each facial stimulus. The paper-based questionnaire contained five questions/ratings for each stimulus, covering ‘biologi-ness’ (‘How much do you find the painted objects biological?’), object saliency (‘How well can you recognise the painted object?’), facial saliency (‘How well can you recognise a face in the painting?’), liking (‘How much do you like the painting?’), and beauty (‘How much beauty do you feel from the painting?’). Object and facial saliency scores were obtained to assess the intensity of perceptual and cognitive processing, an influential factor in aesthetic emotion (Reber et al., 1998, 2004). The beauty score was the main target of the analysis, representing a direct measure of positive aesthetic emotion, while the liking score was used to measure the intensity of aesthetic pleasure predicted by aesthetic emotion (Wassiliwizky and Menninghaus, 2021). Participants used a visual analogue scale (VAS) to indicate their subjective experience by drawing a vertical line on a horizontal line ranging from each extreme (‘biologi-ness’: from ‘not biological at all’ to ‘very biological’; object saliency: from ‘I cannot recognise at all’ to ‘I can recognise clearly’; facial saliency: from ‘I cannot recognise at all’ to ‘I can recognise clearly’; liking: from ‘I do not like it at all’ to ‘I like it very much’; and beauty: from ‘not beautiful at all’ to ‘very beautiful’). The VAS scores were normalised, that is, converted from 0 (negative extreme) to 1 (positive extreme) by measuring the position of the vertical line (response) from the edge of the horizontal line and used for the analysis. Notably, paper-based pre-rating questionnaires were used to minimise the duration of the MEG experiment and reduce potential eye fatigue among participants while maintaining dichoptic vision with stereoscope, which would lead to poor suppression effects of CFS. However, this repeated exposure of the stimuli through the use of pre-rating may have influenced the intensity of aesthetic pleasures, a limitation we have acknowledged in section 4.5. To validate the pre-rating scales, particularly for the unique measure employed in this study (‘biologi-ness’), three participants underwent a retest after 1 year of the initial pre-rating date (range, 409–598 days). The test-retest correlation (Spearman’s rho) of the ‘biologi-ness’ rating was comparable to that of object and facial saliency scores, indicating good reliability and replicability of the measurement (Supplementary Table 2).

2.4 MEG experiment: procedure, task, and apparatus

The MEG experiment was conducted within 5 days of completing the pre-ratings. First, participants received instructions about the tasks and familiarised themselves with dichoptic vision using a handmade, non-magnetic stereoscope (Figure 1B). Subsequently, the participants lay down in a supine position in a magnetically shielded room (Figure 1C), with the stereoscope installed on the MEG dewer (between the participant’s face and the screen). Through the stereoscope, participants viewed the left half of the screen with their left eye and vice versa. To achieve the best suppression effect in CFS, the positions of stimuli presented to the left and right eyes were individually adjusted whereby the participants were presented with two squares (8.0° × 8.0° in visual angle) on the screen, one for each visual field, and asked to move around and find positions where the two overlapped perfectly in subjective perception (dichoptic vision). All images and stimuli were projected onto a screen placed 28.7 cm in front of the participants from outside the magnetically shielded room using a projector (PROPIxx; VPixx Technologies, Saint-Bruno, Canada) through a mirror. MEG data were recorded during the CFS task, which comprised four sessions, each containing 30 trials. Of the 120 trials, 50 were categorised as face-face (FF), another 50 as Mondrian-face (MF), and the remaining 20 as Mondrian-background (MB). In the FF and MF conditions, 50 facial stimuli were randomly presented once for each condition. The order of the three conditions was pseudo-randomised to ensure that no more than three consecutive trials belonged to the same condition. Figure 1D depicts the differences between the conditions. In the FF condition, each trial started with the presentation of fixation (2.7° × 2.7° in visual angle; fixation phase, 500 ms). Then, the facial stimulus faded in (fading-in phase, 700 ms), remained fixed on the screen (face phase, 2,000 ms), faded out (fading-out phase, 700 ms), and its aftereffect was suppressed by presenting a noisy mask (masking phase, 500 ms). The noisy mask comprised an image filled with 10,000 greyscale dots (arranged in a 100 × 100 matrix within a square of 8.0° × 8.0° in visual angle) of random intensity. In this condition, identical content was presented to both eyes, allowing participants to clearly visualise and consciously process the content. In the MF condition, flashing Mondrian images were presented to the participant’s dominant eye instead of a facial stimulus. The Mondrian images were randomly generated images with 1,000 randomly sized ellipses coloured in one of eight primary colours: red ([255 0 0] in RGB), green ([0 255 0]), blue ([0 0 255]), cyan ([0 255 255]), magenta ([255 0 255]), yellow ([255 255 0]), black ([0 0 0]), and white ([255 255 255]). The presentation of Mondrian images commenced 100 ms before the fading-in phase (preparation phase), where the images were sequentially refreshed with a temporal frequency tuned at 6.25 Hz, as previous studies indicated peak effectiveness of the masking/suppression at approximately 6 Hz (Zhu et al., 2016; Drewes et al., 2018). Participants who successfully maintained dichoptic vision were aware of only the flashing Mondrian stimuli presented to the dominant eye and were unaware of the facial stimuli presented to the non-dominant eye. Thus, the facial stimuli were processed non-consciously in the MF condition. In the MB condition, Mondrian images were presented to the dominant eye, while a stable background-coloured square was presented to the non-dominant eye. Similar to the MF condition, participants were aware of only the flashing Mondrian stimuli and were unaware of the background-coloured image presented to the non-dominant eye.

After each trial, the participants were asked to respond to two-alternative forced choice questions: Q1: ‘Did you see a face?,’ which was asked by presenting ‘See?’ on the screen (response phase 1) and Q2: ‘Do you feel beauty from the painting?,’ which was asked by presenting ‘Beauty?’ on the screen (response phase 2). Participants indicated answers by pressing one of the two buttons corresponding to ‘yes’ and ‘no’ using their left and right thumbs, with positions counterbalanced between participants. Participants were instructed to respond ‘yes’ to the Q1 (‘see?’ question) not only when the whole facial stimulus was visible but also if any minute part of the stimulus was visible despite Mondrian suppression. Furthermore, participants were instructed to guess the answer to Q2 (‘beauty?’ question) when they did not (consciously) see any faces in the trial. Responses to Q1 were used solely for screening and rejecting trials where the facial stimuli were not perfectly masked in the MF condition. Each response phase lasted until the participants provided their responses, with an inter-trial interval set at 1 s. Each session lasted approximately 4–5 min, varying based on the response time. The entire experimental procedure, including preparation, lasted approximately 60–90 min.

2.5 MEG experiment: scanning details

Cortical activity during the CFS task was recorded using a 160-channel whole-head-type MEG system (MEG vision PQ1160C; Yokogawa Electric Co., Kanazawa, Japan). During the scan, participants were asked to remain calm in a supine position in a magnetically shielded room, with scanning conditions controlled for consistency and comfort. The sensor and reference coils were gradiometers of 15.5 mm in diameter and 50 mm in baseline. Each pair of sensor coils was separated by a distance of 23 mm. The sampling frequency was 2,000 Hz, with 500 Hz low-pass filtering during recording. To co-register the MEG data with the anatomical brain images, five fiducial magnetic marker coils were placed on each participant’s face (40 mm above the nasion, bilaterally 10 mm in front of the tragus, and bilateral pre-auricular points) before the MEG scan, and their spatial coordinates were measured immediately before each session. During the scan, participants were monitored by a technical staff member using a video camera installed in the magnetically shielded room. Among the 23 participants, the individual magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data of 5 participants were obtained retrospectively from another study. These were anatomical T1-weighted MR images acquired using a 3.0-T scanner (SIGMA Excite 3.0T, GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, USA) with a standard head coil and three fiducial markers (Medtronic Surgical Navigation Technologies Inc., Broomfield, CO, USA) positioned at the three magnetic marker coils placed on the forehead.

2.6 MEG data analysis

MEG data were analysed offline using RICOH MEG Analysis software (RICOH, Tokyo, Japan), MATLAB (MathWorks, MA, USA), Brainstorm, which is documented and freely available for download online under the GNU general public licence2 (Tadel et al., 2011), and the FreeSurfer image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online.3 First, continuous MEG signals were cleaned using a dual-signal subspace projection algorithm (Sekihara et al., 2016) available in the vendor-provided software (RICOH MEG Analysis), comparable to the temporally extended signal-space separation algorithm, with the only difference being the approximation of the signal subspace projector (Cai et al., 2019). Next, to remove the remaining artefacts, the signals were decomposed via independent component analysis using the infomax algorithm implemented in Brainstorm (Makeig et al., 1996). Each component from the independent component analysis was visually inspected, and those with cardiac, blinking, and other salient artefacts were rejected. The data were divided into 6,000 ms epochs, each starting 1,500 ms before the preparation phase (1,000 ms before the fixation phase) and lasting 1,000 ms after the fading-out phase. This epoch length was designed to avoid edge effects in the time window of interest (from the preparation to the fading-out phase) caused by the filtering process for computing the envelopes, as described later. The starting point of the time window of interest (the onset of the preparation phase) was considered to be 0 ms throughout the analysis. The offset was removed using baseline signals averaged across −1,000 to 0 ms for each sensor and epoch. Epochs (trials) were rejected and excluded from the following analyses based on three criteria: (i) epochs corresponding to trials in the MF condition where participants responded ‘yes’ to the Q1 (‘see?’ question), indicating imperfect masking of facial stimuli (ii) technical issues resulting in the last trial of the session, during which the MEG measurement was finished, not being recorded, and (iii) trials contaminated with remaining artefacts. Based on these criteria, the number of trials rejected was 1.17 ± 2.48 trials (with a maximum of 9 trials) per participant, 1 trial from two participants and 2 trials from one participant, and 3.57 ± 3.07 trials (with a maximum of 10 trials) per participant, respectively. Consequently, the number of trials considered for the analyses were: FF: 48.70 ± 1.40 trials (19.48 ± 0.59 trials for biological, and 19.35 ± 1.07 trials for non-biological stimuli), MF: 47.09 ± 2.97 trials (18.91 ± 1.28 trials for biological and 18.74 ± 1.48 trials for non-biological stimuli), and MB: 19.30 ± 0.88 trials.

The remaining artefact-free signals were projected onto the cortical source using the default parameters of the Brainstorm toolbox. For the structural MRI data, ICBM152, a template anatomical brain image prepared by Brainstorm, was used for analysis. The ICBM152 is a non-linear average of 152 MR images from different subjects (Fonov et al., 2009) and is provided along with the cortical segments. In cases where individual MRI data were obtained retrospectively from another study (5 out of 23 participants), the T1-weighted MR images were segmented using the FreeSurfer pipeline ‘recon-all’ (Fischl, 2012) and then imported into Brainstorm. The signal source was restricted to the cortex, which was segmented into 15,000 vertices. Each MEG session was co-registered with the anatomical image using the spatial coordinates of five fiducial points and the nasion, and the relationship between 160 MEG channels and 15,000 vertices (leadfield matrix) was modelled (forward modelling) using an overlapping sphere model, a recommended option for MEG data in the Brainstorm. Before computing the source signals, the characteristics of the MEG sensor noise were modelled as a covariance matrix for each pair of channels (noise covariance), computed from an empty room recording (≥ 5 min) measured before starting the MEG experiment (< 1 h) for each participant using an identical MEG machine with the same acquisition setting. Using the forward model and noise covariance matrix, the source signals of MEG data were computed using the weighted minimum norm estimation (wMNE) method (Lin et al., 2006), which restricts the sources of the inverse problem by minimising the energy (L2 norm), while weighting the deep sources to facilitate their detection. This algorithm was selected because (i) it was recommended as a default option in Brainstorm and (ii) it is suitable for cases where a template brain is used instead of individual MR images, which only returns rough approximations of the forward model and is unsuitable for other inversion algorithms (such as beamformer), which require a better model approximation than the wMNE method. The orientation of the neural sources was restricted to be normal to the cortex. The data obtained from the source reconstruction process consisted of epoch time-series signals for each of the 15,000 cortical vertices. High-dimensional data were limited to 68 anatomical regions of interest (ROIs) defined by the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., 2006) by averaging the signals of the vertices included in each anatomical region. During averaging, the signs of the signals were flipped in the vertices, where the normal orientation was opposite to the dominant orientation of the corresponding region. For visual inspection, the ROI time series were averaged across trials under the same conditions (FF, MF, and MB) and categories (biological and non-biological) and across all or occipital ROIs. Finally, to obtain the non-phase-locked induced oscillatory power of the MEG data, a time-frequency (TF) analysis was performed. TF data were computed as envelopes of the epoched ROI time series before averaging, which were extracted using the Hilbert transform. The signals were narrow-band filtered for delta (δ, 2–4 Hz), theta (θ, 5–7 Hz), alpha (α, 8–12 Hz), beta (β, 15–29 Hz), low-gamma (lγ, 30–48 Hz), and high-gamma (hγ, 52–90 Hz) frequency activities, and their envelopes were computed as power of the Hilbert transform for each frequency band. The frequency windows for gamma bands (lγ and hγ) were designed to avoid power line noise at 50 Hz. The envelopes were flattened by multiplying the amplitudes by frequency (1/f compensation), normalised against a baseline period from −1,000 to −500 ms (ITI), and used in the statistical analyses.

2.7 Statistical analysis

2.7.1 Behavioural data

Statistical analyses of behavioural data were performed using MATLAB, the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (MathWorks), and the Multiple Testing Toolbox (Martínez-Cagigal, 2021).

First, for each participant, the 20 stimuli with the highest ‘biologi-ness’ rating were assigned to the biological category, and the lowest 20 were assigned to the non-biological category. Therefore, different stimuli were included in the biological and non-biological categories for each participant. This categorisation was used for subsequent statistical analyses.

Pre-rating scores (‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, facial saliency, liking, and beauty) were compared using a non-parametric bootstrapping approach to examine the differences in aesthetic appeal levels between biological and non-biological categories. Participant-wise differences in each rating were computed between categories, which were then averaged using resampling with replacement data across all participants 10,000 times. The percentage of resampled averages larger or smaller than 0 (the smaller value) determined the significance level (P-value). Next, non-parametric correlations between the five pre-rating scores were examined within each stimulus category (biological and non-biological) using a bootstrapping approach. For each score pair, participant-wise Spearman’s rho was computed, which were averaged using resampling with replacement data across all participants 10,000 times. The percentage of resampled average coefficients larger or smaller than 0 (the smaller value) determined the significance level (P-value). In the pre-rating data analysis, bootstrapping analysis was performed repeatedly, which increased the risk of Type I error (Curran-Everett, 2000). Therefore, to control for the false discovery rate (FDR), P-values were adjusted using the Benjamini–Hochberg method (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). The P-values were also multiplied by two to account for the two-tailed test.

Prior to analysing the responses to the CFS task, trials in the FF and MF conditions were further subdivided into two subsets based on the categories of the presented stimuli (biological and non-biological), which resulted in the CFS trials being categorised into five conditions, each containing 20 trials: FF-biological, FF-non-biological, MF-biological, MF-non-biological, and MB. Responses to Q2 (‘beauty?’ question) were compared between the five conditions by calculating the proportions of trials that responded as ‘yes’ to Q2 (proportion of ‘beauty’ response). These comparisons were made using the bootstrapping approach with FDR correction. Finally, to examine whether presentations of aesthetically appealing stimuli modified behavioural responses to Q2 (‘beauty?’ question), pre-rating scores were compared between stimuli presented in ‘beauty’ and ‘non-beauty’ trials within each condition (FF-biological, FF-non-biological, MF-biological, and MF-non-biological). These comparisons were also made using the bootstrapping approach with FDR correction. The grand mean and 95% bootstrap confidence interval of the statistics (i.e., mean proportion) are reported.

2.7.2 MEG data

Statistical analyses of MEG data were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA), Fieldtrip toolbox (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007; Oostenveld et al., 2011), Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA), and Multiple Testing Toolbox (Martínez-Cagigal, 2021). Initially, the temporal resolution of TF data was reduced from 2,000 to 250 Hz to accommodate the large TF data in the permutation tests and achieve realistic computational costs in terms of time and memory. The downsampled TF data in the target time window (0–3,500 ms) were used for statistical analysis.

First, to examine differences in induced brain activity between FF (conscious) and MF (non-conscious) conditions, normalised regional TF data were compared between the conditions for each category (FF-biological vs. MF-biological, FF-non-biological vs. MF-non-biological) using a non-parametric cluster-based permutation approach implemented in the Fieldtrip toolbox (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). This approach is used broadly for statistically examining TF data under the control of the family-wise error rate (FWER) in clinical (Machetanz et al., 2021; Dell’Acqua et al., 2022, 2023; Hoshi et al., 2024) and basic (Piai et al., 2012; Popov et al., 2018; Stegemöller et al., 2021) research. It involves two major steps: cluster definition and significance calculation. During cluster definition, for each pair of conditions, the T-statistic of the dependent samples was calculated in the TF space (time: 876 × frequency: 6) for each region (68) (observed statistics). Notably, these T-statistics were only used for cluster definition and not for statistical inference or for calculating the significance probability of the cluster. The T-statistic was also computed using a shuffled dataset across participants and conditions 1,000 times, generating a probability distribution of random statistics. For each data point in the TF space, the observed statistics were examined to determine whether they were above or below the critical value (0.025) in the right or left tail of the random probability distribution, respectively. Data points where the observed statistics exceeded the tails were clustered based on their temporal and frequency adjacencies on each side of the tail and defined as positive and negative clusters. This procedure empirically defines ‘clusters,’ encompassing multiple adjacent data points in the TF data which exhibit similar behaviours concerning the effect of interest (T-statistics). For significance calculation, the observed T-statistic was summed in each cluster (observed cluster statistics), while the random distribution of cluster statistics (i.e., null hypothesis distribution) was generated by collecting the maximum of summed T-statistics among detected clusters in the shuffled data for each of the 1,000 iterations (‘maxsum’ method in the cluster-based permutation algorithm in the Fieldtrip toolbox). The proportion of the null hypothesis distribution that was larger (for positive clusters) or smaller (for negative clusters) than the observed cluster statistics was considered as the significance level (P-value) of each observed cluster. The P-values were multiplied by two to account for the two-tailed test. This approach controlled the FWER at 0.05 because the P-value of each cluster was derived from its ranking in the null hypothesis distribution, which was shaped dynamically by the number of comparisons and correlations between data points (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007; Groppe et al., 2011). For each cluster, the size of the cluster, average T-statistic across data points included in each cluster, and P-value multiplied by two to account for the two-tailed test were reported. Additionally, a peak was defined as the data point in the TF space with the maximum absolute T-statistics in each cluster, whose T-statistic, time, and frequency were reported.

Next, to investigate the relationship between the aesthetic appeal level of the stimuli and neural responses, correlations between the pre-rating scores (object saliency, facial saliency, liking, and beauty) and normalised regional TF data were examined for each condition (FF-biological, FF-non-biological, MF-biological, and MF-non-biological). For each score, non-parametric correlations with the TF data (Spearman’s rho) were computed in the TF space (time: 876 × frequency: 6) for each participant and region (68) (correlation map). The statistical significance of the correlation maps was assessed using the non-parametric cluster-based permutation approach. For each correlation map, a one-sample T-statistic against zero was obtained in the TF space (time: 876 × frequency: 6) (observed statistics). Cluster definition and significance calculation followed the same procedure for the FF vs. MF comparisons. For each cluster, we reported the cluster size, average T-statistic across data points included in each cluster, and P-value, as well as the T-statistic, time, and frequency of a peak in each cluster. Notably, while the cluster-based permutation approach addressed the multiple comparison issue between TF data points by controlling for the FWER (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007), we did not address the problem between multiple pairs of the cluster-based permutation tests (i.e., repeated use of the tests). This was because the analyses were exploratory, investigating the conditional differences in TF data or correlations between pre-rating scores and TF data. These analyses lacked a priori hypotheses and focus on specific conditions or pre-ratings.

Finally, to replicate the results without relying on the ‘biologi-ness’ rating, a series of statistical analyses were repeated using all data (FF-all and MF-all conditions) without categorising stimuli into biological and non-biological, as detailed in section C of the Supplementary materials.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioural data

3.1.1 Pre-rating

In this study, the stimuli were pre-rated using five VAS scores (‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, facial saliency, liking, and beauty), with further subcategorisation into biological and non-biological based on the ‘biologi-ness’ score. Supplementary Figure 1 presents the pre-rating results, indicating that stimuli with younger IDs (< #30) received higher ‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, and facial saliency scores. Supplementary Figure 2 summarises the subcategorisation outcomes, revealing that stimuli with IDs < 30 were more frequently allocated to the biological category, whereas those with ID ≥ 30 were allocated to the non-biological category. Figure 2A depicts the raw data for pre-rating scores for each category, demonstrating a ceiling effect in ‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, and facial saliency ratings within the biological category. The biological category scored higher than the non-biological category on all pre-rating scales (all P < 0.001, FDR-corrected) (Table 1). Supplementary Figures 3, 4 and Supplementary Tables 3, 4 outline the correlations between scores. In the biological category (Supplementary Figure 3 and Supplementary Table 3), all pairs within the pre-rating scores exhibited positive correlations, except for the facial saliency × beauty scores. In the non-biological category (Supplementary Figure 4 and Supplementary Table 4), all pairs exhibited positive correlations, except for those that included liking scores.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of behavioural data analysis for the biological (left panels) and non-biological (right panels) categories. (A) A violin plot visualising the distribution of each pre-rating score, with the mean value represented by a red line. (B) A spider plot contrasting the mean of each pre-rating score in the FF conditions, in which participants responded as ‘yes’ (red: ‘beauty’) and ‘no’ (blue: ‘non-beauty’) to the Q2 (‘beauty?’ question), respectively. (C) A spider plot contrasting the mean of each pre-rating score in the MF conditions, in which participants responded as ‘yes’ (red: ‘beauty’) and ‘no’ (blue: ‘non-beauty’) to the Q2 (‘beauty?’ question), respectively. An asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference in pre-ratings scores between ‘yes’ (‘beauty’ response) and ‘no’ (‘non-beauty’ response) trials. FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; BIOL, ‘biologi-ness’ rating; OBJ, object saliency rating; FACE, facial saliency rating; LIKE; liking rating; BEAU, beauty rating.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the pre-rating data.

3.1.2 MEG experiment

During CFS sessions, three presentation conditions were employed: FF (conscious), MF (non-conscious), and MB (Mondrian only). These conditions were further subdivided based on the category of the presented stimuli: FF-biological, FF-non-biological, MF-biological, MF-non-biological, and MB. Binary behavioural responses (‘yes’ or ‘no’) to the question regarding the aesthetic quality of stimuli (Q2; ‘Do you feel beauty from the painting?’) were compared between the conditions. Compared with the MB condition (0.320), the proportion of beauty (‘yes’) responses increased in the FF-biological condition (0.543) (P < 0.001), decreased in the FF-non-biological condition (0.115) (P < 0.001), and was the same in the MF-biological (0.302) (P = 0.573) and MF-non-biological (0.337) (P = 0.493) conditions. Comparing between categories, the proportion was higher for the FF-biological condition (0.527) than for the FF-non-biological condition (0.155) (P < 0.001), which corresponded to the pre-rating differences (Figure 2A and Table 1). The proportion of beauty responses was not different between the MF-biological and MF-non-biological conditions (P = 0.291).

The relationships between the pre-rating and behavioural responses to Q2 are shown in the spider plots in Figures 2B, C and Table 2. For the FF conditions (FF-biological and FF-non-biological) (Figure 2B and Table 2A), all scores were higher for the beauty trials than for the non-beauty trials (red and blue lines in Figure 2B, respectively), except for the facial saliency rating in the FF-biological condition. Pre-rating differences were also observed in the MF conditions (MF-biological and MF-non-biological) (Figure 2C and Table 2B) for the object saliency rating in the MF-biological condition and ‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, and facial saliency ratings in the MF-non-biological condition.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of the relationships between pre-rating scores and response data.

3.2 MEG data

3.2.1 Visual inspection of waveforms

Figure 3 illustrates the average ROI time series signals for each condition (FF-biological, FF-non-biological, MF-biological, MF-non-biological, and MB). Clear visual responses to the flashing Mondrian images (6.25 Hz) were observed under MF and MB conditions (Figures 3B, D, E). A strong event-related field (ERF) appeared at the onset of Mondrian flashes (approximately 100 ms after onset), with amplitudes reducing in the later time windows but repeating periodically until the end of the flashes (3,500 ms). The oscillatory ERF was evident in the occipital ROIs (red lines in Figures 3B, D, E). No explicit ERF was identified in the FF condition, except for weak signal fluctuations between 0 and 1,500 ms (Figures 3A, C). No clear difference was observed between the biological and non-biological categories.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. ROI time-series averaged within trials of the (A) FF-biological, (B) MF-biological, (C) FF-non-biological, (D) MF-non-biological, and (E) MB conditions. The top plot in each panel visualises the butterfly plot, where each line represents each of the 68 ROIs. The bottom plot in each panel visualises the root-mean-square (RMS) waveform, where the signals were averaged across all (black line) or occipital (red line) ROIs. All waveforms were corrected for baseline (–100 to 0 ms). FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; MB, Mondrian-background condition.

3.2.2 TF data: conscious vs. non-conscious conditions

Figure 4 and Tables 3, 4 summarise the differences observed in induced brain activity (TF data) between the FF (conscious) and MF (non-conscious) conditions for each category (biological and non-biological). Overall, the results revealed strong frequency-tagged responses to Mondrian images in the MF condition, in addition to category-specific responses to facial stimuli, which differed between the biological and non-biological categories. In both stimulus categories, the MF condition induced higher theta and adjacent band activities in the occipitotemporal regions, including the bilateral pericalcarine, lateral occipital, and inferior parietal cortices, lingual gyri, cuneus, and right parahippocampal cortex, than those of the FF condition, which were sustained throughout the trial (0–3,500 ms) (Figure 4 and Tables 3, 4). In the biological category (Figure 4A and Table 3), the FF condition also showed lower frequency activities in the bilateral superior frontal and inferior temporal cortices, and the left middle temporal, entorhinal, and insula cortices than the MF condition. In the non-biological category (Figure 4B and Table 4), the FF condition exhibited higher delta-band activity in the left precentral and postcentral gyri and temporal pole, higher theta activity in the right pars orbitalis, higher alpha-band activity in the right superior parietal lobule, and lower theta-band activity in the bilateral fusiform and right middle temporal gyri than those in the MF condition.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests between the FF vs. MF conditions for the (A) biological and (B) non-biological categories. All TF images are scaled equally, with significant clusters emphasised using black bold lines. The regions (ROIs) in the template brain images were coloured red when any positive clusters were found in the ROI or blue when any negative clusters were found in the ROI. The results for hγ band are not displayed, because no significant clusters were found in the band. FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; lγ, low-gamma; hγ, high-gamma.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for TF data between the FF-biological and MF-biological conditions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for TF data between the FF-non-biological and MF-non-biological conditions.

3.2.3 TF data: correlations to pre-ratings in the biological category

The correlations between induced brain activities (TF data) and four pre-rating scores (object saliency, facial saliency, liking, and beauty) were examined within each condition (FF-biological, MF-biological, FF-non-biological, and MF-non-biological). Figure 5 and Table 5 present a summary of the results for the conditions in the biological category (FF-biological and MF-biological).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for evaluating correlations between regional TF data and pre-rating scores [(A,E) object saliency, (B,F) facial saliency, (C,G) liking, and (D,H) beauty] in each condition [(A–D) FF-biological and (E–H) MF-biological] in the biological category. All TF images are scaled equally, with significant clusters emphasised using black bold lines. The regions (ROIs) in the template brain images were coloured red when any positive clusters were found in the ROI or blue when any negative clusters were found in the ROI. The results for hγ band are not displayed, because no significant clusters were found in the band. FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; lγ, low-gamma; hγ, high-gamma.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for evaluating correlations between the TF data and pre-rating scores for the biological category.

In the FF (conscious)-biological condition, the object saliency score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left inferior parietal cortex, left pars opercularis, left supramarginal gyrus, right entorhinal cortex, and right temporal pole and negatively correlated with delta-band activity in the right pars opercularis and rostral middle frontal gyrus (Figure 5A and Table 5A). The facial saliency score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the right paracentral and supramarginal gyri, pars opercularis, and pars triangularis (Figure 5B and Table 5B). The liking score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), right lateral occipital cortex and right superior frontal gyrus (Figure 5C and Table 5C). The beauty score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left cuneus, left lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC), and right transverse temporal gyrus (Figure 5D and Table 5D).

In the MF (non-conscious)-biological condition, the object saliency score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), right inferior parietal lobule, and right rACC (Figure 5E and Table 5E). The facial saliency score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left parahippocampal and superior temporal cortices (Figure 5F and Table 5F). The liking score was positively correlated with theta-band activity in the left insula, left middle temporal cortex, and right superior temporal cortex and negatively correlated with alpha-band activity in the right caudal ACC (cACC) (Figure 5G and Table 5G). Finally, the beauty score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left lingual gyrus and right frontal pole and negatively correlated with theta-band activity in the right lingual cortex and precentral gyrus (Figure 5H and Table 5H).

3.2.4 TF data: correlations to pre-ratings in the non-biological category

Figure 6 and Table 6 present a summary of the results for the conditions in the non-biological category (FF-non-biological and MF-non-biological).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for evaluating correlations between regional TF data and pre-rating scores [(A,E) object saliency, (B,F) facial saliency, (C,G) liking, and (D,H) beauty] in each condition [(A–D) FF-non-biological and (E–H) MF-non-biological] in the non-biological category. All TF images are scaled equally, with significant clusters emphasised using black bold lines. The regions (ROIs) in the template brain images were coloured red when any positive clusters were found in the ROI or blue when any negative clusters were found in the ROI. The results for hγ band are not displayed, because no significant clusters were found in the band. FF, face-face condition; MF, Mondrian-face condition; LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; lγ, low-gamma; hγ, high-gamma.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of the cluster-based permutation tests for evaluating correlations between the TF data and pre-rating scores for the non-biological category.

In the FF (conscious)-non-biological condition, the object saliency score was positively correlated with delta-band activity in the right pericalcarine cortex and negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left transverse temporal gyrus and right mOFC (Figure 6A and Table 6A). The facial saliency score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the right pericalcarine and superior parietal cortices and right postcentral gyrus and negatively correlated with delta-band activity in the left bankssts (cortical areas around the superior temporal sulcus [STS]) and left inferior temporal cortex (Figure 6B and Table 6B). The liking score was negatively correlated with theta-band activity in the left rACC (Figure 6C and Table 6C). The beauty score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the right lateral occipital cortex and superior frontal gyrus and negatively correlated with theta-band activity in the left cuneus (Figure 6D and Table 6D).

In the MF (non-conscious)-non-biological condition, the object saliency score was positively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left inferior temporal cortex and the right superior parietal lobule (Figure 6E and Table 6E). The facial saliency score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in the left cACC, left precentral gyrus, and left rostral middle frontal gyrus (Figure 6F and Table 6F). The liking score was negatively correlated with delta-band activity in the right pars triangularis (Figure 6G and Table 6G). Finally, the beauty score was positively correlated with theta-band activity in the right pars opercularis and negatively correlated with low-frequency activities in distributed regions, including the left insula, right temporal pole, right caudal middle frontal, postcentral, and precentral gyri (Figure 6H and Table 6H).

4 Discussion

This study revealed two main findings: (i) the non-conscious presentation of portrait paintings induced spatiotemporally distributed low-frequency brain activities for both the biological and non-biological categories, and (ii) these brain activities exhibited distinct patterns between the biological and non-biological categories and between the conscious and non-conscious conditions.

In this study, we examined the induced MEG signals recorded during a CFS task, where aesthetically appealing facial stimuli, subcategorised into biological and non-biological stimuli, were non-consciously presented to participants. We hypothesised that the non-conscious presentation of aesthetically appealing stimuli would induce neural responses related to emotional processing if aesthetic emotion holds evolutionary significance for humans. For the biological and non-biological categories, aesthetic appeal induced MEG responses, even when suppressed using CFS (MF conditions) (Figures 5E–H, 6E–H, and Tables 5E–H, 6E–H), thereby supporting our hypothesis. However, the neural responses to the aesthetic appeal of stimuli differed between (i) biological vs. non-biological categories and (ii) conscious vs. non-conscious presentations in the spatial, temporal, and oscillatory senses. In the following sections, we highlight the variations (i) in behavioural data (section 4.1) and discuss the interaction between (i) and (ii) in MEG data (sections 4.2, 4.3 and 4.4), where the results in the FF (conscious)-biological (section 4.2), MF (non-conscious)-biological (section 4.3), and non-biological (FF and MF) (section 4.4) conditions were introduced.

4.1 Behavioural-level differences in aesthetic processes of biological and non-biological stimuli

In this study, we subcategorised the stimuli into two subsets based on the ‘biologi-ness’ score of the pre-ratings by each participant. Despite controlling for low-level features, qualitative differences between the categories influenced the results. Overall, the biological category scored higher on all pre-ratings than the non-biological category (Table 1), supporting the processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure (Reber et al., 1998, 2004). This theory proposes that ‘the more fluently perceivers can process an object, the more positive their aesthetic response,’ where prototypicality is a variable influencing fluency. In the present study, the biological category scored higher in the ‘biologi-ness’ rating, where the faces were depicted more clearly (higher in object and facial saliency ratings). Hence, as these stimuli were more prototypical than the other facial stimuli, they were processed more fluently, leading to enhanced affective ratings, such as liking and beauty, as exemplified by positive correlations between the rating scores (Supplementary Table 3). However, in the non-biological category, the correlation revealed that the liking score was independent of ‘biologi-ness’, object saliency, and

facial saliency scores (Supplementary Table 4). This indicated that factors beyond processing fluency influenced the aesthetic liking scores because the non-biological stimuli were low in prototypicality and their fluent processes were interrupted (Reber et al., 1998, 2004). The pleasure-interest model of aesthetic liking (PIA model) (Graf and Landwehr, 2015) distinguishes aesthetic

liking based on its construction mechanisms. Pleasure-based liking is influenced by processing fluency and triggered by bottom-up stimulus-driven automatic processing, whereas interest-based liking is elicited by the reduction of disfluency during top-down perceiver-driven controlled processing (Graf and Landwehr, 2017). For non-biological stimuli, including paintings in abstract styles (such as cubism, abstract art, and surrealism), participants are more likely to engage in deeper active elaboration to resolve the disfluency of the abstract paintings, triggering interest-based aesthetic liking. Therefore, interest-based liking is assumed to be another contributor to liking scores for non-biological stimuli. Additionally, the integration of processing fluency and learning theories for making future predictions provides explanations of aesthetic processing (Brielmann and Dayan, 2022; Starr, 2023). Within this line of research, the role of predictive processing (PP) has recently been receiving attention as a mental function accounting for the aesthetic experience (Frascaroli et al., 2024). This framework suggests that encountering unpredictable stimuli (e.g., artworks), which deviates from the schema of the viewers, gives rise to prediction error and ‘uncertainty,’ whose resolution (i.e., change) drives a positive affective experience (Van De Cruys et al., 2024). When presenting the stream of portrait paintings in our study, participants may have naturally expected the presence of facial images, and the presentation of less typical facial stimuli, namely the ones in the non-biological category, may have increased prediction error and uncertainty. Contemplation or elaborations of such stimuli might have led to the resolution of uncertainty (e.g., identification of facial images or finding them to ‘make sense’), which consequently enhanced the aesthetic pleasure measured by the liking score. In addition to interest-based liking, this serves as a unique source of the liking score in non-biological stimuli. Notably, the PP framework complicates the processing fluency theory that hedonic experience driven by fluency is biassed by prior expectation to the stimuli or fluency itself (e.g., ‘more fluent than expected,’ ‘prefer to be kept in a state of puzzlement’) (Yoo et al., 2024). Furthermore, the prototypicality of the stimuli, which is a key driver of the aesthetic emotion in processing fluency theory, is formed by repeated PP throughout the life of the human being. As exemplified by the ‘other-race effect’ (Pesciarelli et al., 2021; Stelter and Schweinberger, 2023) and facial familiarity effects (Axelrod et al., 2015), the ‘biological’ visual features for carrying evolutionary important information vary individually, although they share some common characteristics, such as symmetry and good proportion. This indicates that the ‘personally’ formed prototypical representation of an object, which is updated daily through individual active inferences (i.e., PP), is considered to carry evolutionarily important information and has prioritised access to the emotional system. Therefore, the PP framework not only plays a role as a unique contributor to aesthetic emotion for non-biological stimuli but also serves as a generative function of prototypical images and influences fluency-driven aesthetic emotion. Upon examining the association between pre-ratings and behavioural responses during the CFS task (Figures 2B, C and Table 2), we found that all pre-rating scores, except the facial saliency score, differed between beauty and non-beauty responses in the FF (conscious)-biological condition (Figure 2B and Table 2A). Moreover, a weak link between the facial saliency and beauty ratings was demonstrated by the low correlation coefficients (Supplementary Table 3). This finding may be attributed to a ceiling effect on the facial saliency score in biological stimuli (Figure 2A). Owing to the strong correlation between facial saliency and ‘biologi-ness’ scores and selection criteria that only retained stimuli with very high ‘biologi-ness’ scores in the biological category, both scores hit the ceiling. Consequently, correlations between facial saliency and ‘biologi-ness’ scores with other pre-rating scores were lower for the biological category than those of the non-biological category, which did not experience this effect. Notably, the non-consciously presented stimuli modified the behavioural responses during the CFS task, such that the object saliency score was higher for beauty than for non-beauty responses in the MF-biological condition, while the ‘biologi-ness’ and object and facial saliency scores were higher for beauty than for non-beauty responses in the MF-non-biological condition (Table 2B). This implies that non-conscious neural inputs of salient stimuli may bias subsequent aesthetic labelling behaviour. Previous studies have revealed that non-consciously presented faces undergo not only emotional, but also perceptual and cognitive processes (Axelrod et al., 2015), such as discrimination of face vs. scrambled face (Jiang and He, 2006), upright vs. inverted faces (Jiang et al., 2007; Stein et al., 2012), and familiarity (Gobbini et al., 2013). As image saliency directly influences perceptual and cognitive processes, it may interfere with the perceptual and/or cognitive fluency of non-conscious processing of facial stimuli and change their gut-level feelings captured by guessing the aesthetic quality of covert stimuli. This aligns with a concept of ‘unfelt’ fluency, which occurs on a lower, perceptual, and sub-personal level, reflects the level of matches between perceptual information with basic visual expectations derived from our visual systems (i.e., prototypical patterns of neural activity, determined congenitally and formed by PP), and operates as a non-conscious process (Brouillet and Friston, 2023). As beauty and liking scores are the results of conscious aesthetic evaluations, they are dynamic reflections of various perceptual and cognitive processes, such as fluency, elaboration, and prediction. However, in the non-conscious presentation scenario, the top-down regulative process is inactive for stimuli with weak intensity (such as subliminal stimuli) (Baars, 1988; Dehaene et al., 1998), indicating that only limited perceptual and cognitive processes can contribute to aesthetic emotion. For example, the interest-based liking system and PP for resolving uncertainty require a top-down regulation of incoming information to drive aesthetic emotion; thus, these would not be involved in the processing of non-consciously presented stimuli. Therefore, in the present study, the pleasure-based liking system, driven by ‘unfelt’ processing fluency, was likely dominant for aesthetic processing in the MF condition. The behavioural responses to the non-consciously presented stimuli were modified by the saliency scores but not by the beauty or liking scores, indicating that non-consciously presented information accesses limited processes associated with the perceptual saliency of the stimuli, such as the ‘unfelt’ fluency; hence, the behavioural response observed was likely based upon such limited information. Therefore, the results of the aesthetic labelling based on limited information (behavioural responses to the non-consciously presented stimuli) would be distinctive from those based on comprehensive information (beauty and liking scores in the pre-rating).

4.2 Conscious aesthetic processing of biological stimuli

After highlighting the behavioural differences between the biological and non-biological categories in section 4.1 we discussed the interaction between biological vs. non-biological and conscious vs. non-conscious contrasts in the correlation analysis between the pre-ratings and TF data. Here, we have focused on the results in the most representative FF (conscious)-biological condition (Figures 5A–D and Tables 5A–D) and structured the following paragraphs to sequentially discuss spatial, oscillatory, and temporal dimensions for clarity.

Regarding the spatial (regional) dimension of the results, the saliency scores were positively correlated with low-frequency activities (from the delta to alpha bands) in parietal and temporal regions (Figures 5A, B). These results aligned with those of previous studies indicating that emotional faces induce delta and theta band activities, particularly in occipitotemporal regions (Güntekin and Başar, 2014). Moreover, the level of processing fluency in facial stimuli was associated with neural responses in occipitotemporal and parietal regions (Natu and O’Toole, 2011). Multiple face-selective areas in the occipital and temporal cortices include the occipital face area (Haxby et al., 1999; Gauthier et al., 2000), fusiform face area (Kanwisher et al., 1997), posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Hoffman and Haxby, 2000), anterior temporal lobe (Tsao et al., 2008; Rajimehr et al., 2009), and anterior superior temporal sulcus (Pitcher et al., 2011), which border the clusters identified relevant to the saliency scores. Stimuli with higher saliency scores were processed more fluently and exhibited enhanced slow band activities in these regions, signifying involvement of bottom-up perceptual and cognitive processes related to visual images. In contrast, affective processing of the stimuli, quantified by liking and beauty scores, negatively correlated with low-frequency activities mainly in frontal regions, such as the lOFC and superior frontal gyrus (Figures 5C, D). Previous studies have demonstrated that prefrontal regions respond to aesthetic ratings of various stimuli (Nakamura et al., 1998; Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Di Dio et al., 2007; Winston et al., 2007; Kirk, 2008; Chatterjee et al., 2009; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Kedia et al., 2014; Martín-Loeches et al., 2014; Ferrari et al., 2015, 2017; Chuan-Peng et al., 2020). This indicates that reductions in low-frequency activities in the ‘emotional brain’ coincide with the arousal of aesthetic emotions.

Upon exploring the oscillatory characteristics of the induced response in the FF-biological condition, perceptual/cognitive (object and facial saliency) and affective (liking and beauty) processes exhibited correlations in the same low-frequency activities, but in different regions and opposite directions. While the former exhibited positive relationships (Figures 5A, B), the latter exhibited negative relationships (Figures 5C, D). Similar contradictory results have been reported in studies on emotional faces, with some reporting augmented or induced event-related synchronisation (ERS) in delta and theta band activities (Aftanas et al., 2001, 2004; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2007, 2009; Başar et al., 2008; Balconi et al., 2009b,a; Bamidis et al., 2009; Güntekin and Başar, 2009, 2014; Knyazev et al., 2009), while others reporting their attenuation or event-related desynchronisation (ERD) (Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2009). This inconsistency between ERS and ERD during emotional picture processing has also been documented in alpha and lower-beta bands (Schubring and Schupp, 2021). These discrepancies may be explained by the physiological antagonism between cognitive and affective processes in fluency-driven aesthetic processing (Hoshi and Menninghaus, 2018). A previous study demonstrated that highly affective texts predict larger pupil dilations, whereas highly fluent texts predict smaller pupil dilations (Hoshi and Menninghaus, 2018), indicating that the cognitive and affective processes of aesthetically appealing stimuli cause antagonistic reactions to the autonomic nervous system. Briefly, the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic reactions drives pupillary dilation, with excitement in sympathetic activity and inhibition of parasympathetic activity evoking dilation, and vice versa (van der Wel and van Steenbergen, 2018). Although studies focusing on the relationship between task-related autonomic nervous system reactions and cortical oscillatory activity are limited, one study revealed that autonomic behaviour is associated with delta and theta band responses to emotional stimuli (Balconi et al., 2015). These findings imply that fluency-driven aesthetic processes induce cognitive and affective responses in antagonistic directions in delta and theta band activities in the central nervous system. Previous studies on visual emotional processing have revealed inconsistent results between ERS and ERD using standard picture groups, such as ‘Pictures of Facial Affect’ (Ekman and Friesen, 1976) and ‘International Affective Picture System’ (Lang et al., 1997). Researchers typically assumed that these standardised sets would be equally processed by all individuals in both cognitive and affective senses. However, the potential variabilities in cognitive and affective processes were often overlooked in these studies, which may have led some studies to emphasise cognitive processing and others to emphasise affective processing induced by the stimulus set, resulting in contradictory findings.

Upon examining the temporal dynamics in the FF-biological condition, we found that neural processes occur in multiple time windows. Remarkably, for the beauty score, a negative cluster was identified in a very early time frame in the left lOFC (preparation and fading-in phases, 0–1000 ms) (Figure 5D). Previous neuroaesthetics studies have used electrophysiological measurements, such as electroencephalography (EEG) (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2003; Muñoz and Martín-Loeches, 2015; Sarasso et al., 2020) and MEG (Cela-Conde et al., 2004, 2013), to study the temporal dimensions of aesthetic processing. Among these, two studies have suggested that aesthetic processing comprises two temporally distinct steps. One EEG-based study (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2003) examined event-related potential and found that graphical patterns judged as ‘not beautiful’ evoked early frontal negativity after 300 ms, whereas judgements of their symmetry evoked sustained posterior negativity approximately 600 ms after stimulus onset. Another MEG-based study (Cela-Conde et al., 2013) using paintings as stimuli reported distinct connectivity patterns between two time windows: an early window (250–750 ms), characterised by dense local connections within the occipital region and extending links to orbitofrontal regions for making quick judgements regarding beauty, and the later window (1,000–1,500 ms), with distributed global connection, which was active only for ‘beautiful’ stimuli and involved in appraisals of detailed aspects of beauty. Despite their different timeframes, these two studies reported temporally distinct processes: early processes related to aesthetic emotion for making quick impressions and later processes for detailed evaluations of stimuli. This two-step process corresponds to the temporal dynamics observed in our study, where early low-frequency activities in the OFC were associated with the affective dimension of aesthetic appeal. As the OFC is a core region for the subjective experience of aesthetic emotion, namely beauty (Ishizu and Zeki, 2011), the early processes might be related to aesthetic emotion for making quick impressions. Subsequently, low-frequency activities were induced by cognitive and affective processes in the other brain regions, where the stimuli could be aesthetically evaluated in detail.

4.3 Non-conscious aesthetic processing of biological stimuli

In contrast to the FF (conscious)-biological condition discussed in section 4.2 we focused on the results of the correlation analysis in the MF (non-conscious)-biological condition (Figures 5E–H and Tables 5E–H).

Before discussing the results in the non-conscious condition, it is worth noting that the differences between conscious and non-conscious conditions were strongly biassed by frequency-tagged responses (steady-state visually evoked field) (Parkkonen et al., 2008; Vialatte et al., 2010) to Mondrian images, which entail robust visual evoked responses at a frequency corresponding to the refreshing rate of the presented stimuli. To optimise suppression effects (Zhu et al., 2016; Drewes et al., 2018), we set the refresh rate of the Mondrian images to 6.25 Hz (section 2.4). Visual inspection of the ROI time series (Figure 3) revealed clear ERFs at approximately 6 Hz under MF and MB conditions. The results of the TF data should be interpreted with a consideration of frequency-tagged responses in the theta band (5–7 Hz). As Mondrian images were presented throughout the time window of interest (0–3,500 ms), the sustained activity increased in the theta band, which was commonly found for both categories (biological and non-biological), should be considered as frequency-tagged responses to the Mondrian images. Sustained augmentation of theta band activity was found in the bilateral pericalcarine, lateral occipital, and inferior parietal cortices, lingual gyri, cuneus, and right parahippocampal cortex in the MF conditions (MF-biological and MF-non-biological) (Figure 4). The stimulus-related activities found in these regions must be contaminated by frequency-tagged responses and should be interpreted with caution.

In this section, the results of the MF-biological condition are discussed for saliency scores (object and facial saliency scores) and affective scores (liking and beauty). Saliency scores were negatively associated with low-frequency activity in the fronto-temporal regions (Figures 5E, F), such as the mOFC, rACC, parahippocampal, superior temporal, and inferior parietal cortices. The temporal regions overlapped with the face-selective areas, such as the anterior and posterior parts of the superior temporal sulcus (Duchaine and Yovel, 2015), indicating the involvement of bottom-up visual processing related to the facial images. However, the correlation direction was negative, which is opposite from that of temporal and parietal clusters identified in the FF-biological condition (Figures 5A, B). Additionally, more negative clusters were exclusively found in the frontal and limbic regions in the MF-biological condition, such as the mOFC and rACC (Figure 5E). Despite being designed to capture perceptual and cognitive processing of the stimuli, the saliency score was sensitive to affective processing (Supplementary Table 3). Furthermore, as demonstrated by the behavioural data (section 4.1), the pleasure-based liking system, driven by ‘unfelt’ processing fluency, was considered dominant for aesthetic processing in the MF condition. As saliency scores were directly linked to perceptual fluency, the association between the saliency score and affective processing may have been more emphasised in the MF-biological condition than in the FF-biological condition, leading the distinct correlation patterns between the two conditions. The negative correlations in the frontal and limbic regions found for saliency scores in the MF-biological condition (Figure 5E) were similar to the results for affective ratings in the FF-biological condition (Figures 5C, D). For example, the ACC, a part of the reward circuit (Haber and Knutson, 2009), is responsive to aesthetic emotions (Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Vartanian and Goel, 2004; Brown et al., 2011; Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011; Boccia et al., 2016), and is often coactivated with the OFC. Taken together, we can infer that the saliency scores likely became more strongly associated with affective processing during aesthetic labelling of non-consciously presented images, which resulted in negative clusters in the frontal and limbic regions and modifications of behavioural responses.

Regarding the affective ratings (liking and beauty) in the MF-biological condition, the results showed two types of correlations: (i) negative correlations in low-frequency activities in the cACC and precentral and right lingual gyri, and (ii) positive correlations in low-frequency activities in the temporal region, left lingual gyrus, and frontal pole (Figures 5G, H). For the negative correlations, the cluster in ACC may capture the affective processing of the covert stimuli, akin to the overt stimuli (Figure 5C). Additionally, the negative cluster in the precentral gyrus may also be related to the affective response as the presentation of non-beautiful, ‘ugly,’ or aversive stimuli is associated with neural responses in the precentral and postcentral gyri (Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Hayes and Northoff, 2012). Traditionally, it is assumed that the non-conscious processing of emotional information follows a rapid subcortical pathway, bypassing the slower cortical route to directly relay information to the amygdala and ‘emotion system’ (LeDoux, 1996; Tamietto and De Gelder, 2010). Therefore, it would be reasonable that the non-consciously presented biological stimuli with lower beauty scores induced fast and low-frequency activities in the precentral gyrus during the very early time window, approximately 500 ms after stimulus onset. For the positive correlations, clusters were primarily found in the occipitotemporal regions at the theta band. Although the significant clusters were temporally limited, the TF maps (Figures 5G, H) revealed sustained theta band activities throughout the stimulus presentation period. This requires cautious interpretation, given that the pattern indicates frequency-tagged responses to the Mondrian images used in the CFS paradigm. While the exact reason for the correlations between affective ratings and frequency-tagged responses remains unknown, we can infer that the properties of the masked stimuli can influence the masking effect of the CFS, including the paradigm known as ‘breaking CFS’ (Jiang et al., 2007; Stein et al., 2011). Thus, the affective properties of the biological stimuli, which are associated with low-frequency brain activities (Figures 5C, D), may modulate the frequency-tagged responses induced by CFS in the similar frequency band (theta).

4.4 Conscious and non-conscious aesthetic processing of non-biological stimuli

The results for non-biological stimuli were different from those for biological stimuli. In the FF (conscious)-non-biological condition, the object saliency score was negatively correlated with the low-frequency activities in the mOFC (Figures 6A, B), suggesting associations between perceptual/cognitive saliency and affective process. As discussed in section 4.1 fluency, as well as the interest-based liking system and PP framework, contribute to driving the aesthetic emotion aroused by non-biological stimuli. Our results revealed traces of these processes in non-biological stimuli. For example, the bankssts (STS), where the negative correlation was found with the facial saliency score (Figure 6B), is involved in detecting social information in various stimuli, such as biological motion and eye gaze, and relaying the information to other regions for further processing (Iidaka, 2014; Deen et al., 2015). Similarly, another study revealed that the STS innervates the amygdala to deliver emotional information about facial stimuli (Pitcher et al., 2017). This indicates that the bankssts were involved in the interest-based liking system and PP, contributing to resolving disfluency or uncertainty in stimuli by detecting social and emotional cues in the abstract, non-biological images and relaying the information to the other regions to modify emotional responses. Additionally, there is substantial evidence that awareness of ambiguous perceptual images modulates the activities in V1 (Tong, 2003; Zeki, 2004). The positive correlations between saliency scores and low-frequency activity in the pericalcarine cortex (V1) (Figures 6A, B) may reflect the relationship between perceptual saliency and awareness (i.e., resolving disfluent and uncertain states). Similarly, the superior parietal cortex, where the facial saliency score was positively correlated to the theta/alpha-band activities, has been associated with the disambiguation process of bistable images (Kanai et al., 2011). Furthermore, the inferior temporal cortex is famously associated with object recognition (Logothetis and Sheinberg, 1996; Conway, 2018); thus, the negative cluster identified in this region for facial saliency score would indicate the contribution of the recognition process to disambiguate the visual information to drive affective processing. Although the neurological mechanisms underlying the interest-based liking system and PP framework in the context of aesthetic emotion remain unknown, these regions may play a pivotal role in the processes. Regarding the affective scores, the liking score correlated negatively with the low-frequency activity in the rACC (Figure 6C), consistent with biological stimuli (Figure 5C), supporting the link between ACC activity and aesthetic pleasure. The beauty score correlated with theta-band activity in the occipital regions, such as the lateral occipital cortex and cuneus (Figure 6D), reflecting that the level of aesthetic emotion was associated with the visual information processing, such as the fluency, elaboration, and prediction. Notably, the role of cortical areas in such visual information processing cannot be specified because we did not measure detailed behavioural information for speculating the dominant mental processes underlying aesthetic emotion, which may have varied stimulus-wise (e.g., processing fluency, interest-based liking, or PP). Therefore, our interpretations of the MEG data do not exclude other possibilities; for example, the involvement of the inferior temporal cortex may not reflect its contribution to the disambiguation process (related top-down interest-based liking system and PP framework), but rather the level of prototypicality of the stimuli (related to bottom-up fluency processes).

In the MF (non-conscious)-non-biological condition, facial saliency score was negatively correlated with low-frequency activity in the frontal and limbic regions, including the cACC, precentral, and rostral middle frontal gyri (Figure 6F), similar to negative clusters observed in the frontal and limbic regions for biological stimuli (MF-biological condition) (Figure 5E). As discussed in section 4.3 the negative correlations likely indicate the increased link between saliency scores and affective processing during the aesthetic labelling of non-consciously presented images. This suggests that, for non-consciously presented stimuli, regardless of their categories, the pleasure-based liking system driven by ‘unfelt’ processing fluency was likely dominant for aesthetic processing, and the other ‘top-down’ frameworks were largely suppressed. The liking and beauty scores in the MF-non-biological condition demonstrated negative low-frequency clusters, mainly in the fronto-temporal and central regions (Figures 6G, H). Negative clusters in the central region were also found for the biological category under the same condition (Figure 5H), which would be indicative of aversive responses to the ‘non-beautiful’ stimuli. The neural processing for the non-consciously presented aversive stimuli may share similar neural systems across categories. Negative clusters were also found in the caudal middle frontal gyrus and insular cortex (Figure 6H). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, including the middle frontal gyrus, has been shown to be responsive to aesthetic emotion (Cela-Conde et al., 2004; Cattaneo et al., 2014). Similarly, the insula is considered to be a region responsive to emotional (Singer et al., 2009; Gasquoine, 2014; Uddin et al., 2017) and aesthetically appealing stimuli (Di Dio et al., 2007; Di Dio and Vittorio, 2009; Brown et al., 2011; Chatterjee and Vartanian, 2014; Boccia et al., 2016). These findings indicate that the non-conscious presentation of non-biological stimuli induced neural responses related to aesthetic emotion, observed as negative correlations in the low-frequency bands.

4.5 Limitations

This study has four main limitations. First, the CFS method for non-conscious presentation imposed technical constraints; the stimuli faded in and out to enhance the suppression effects, allowing us to study only the induced components (oscillations) of the MEG signals, which were contaminated by the signals induced by the Mondrian mask at 6.25 Hz. Future studies investigating this topic should consider using time-locked evoked activities. Second, owing to environmental constraints, the number of stimuli (trials) and pre-rating scales were limited. The MEG system was installed in a hospital setting and regularly used for clinical examinations; thus, the experimental procedures were designed to be completed within a limited time slot, restricting the number of trials in the CFS task, variations in stimuli, and pre-ratings. Although the stimuli were carefully selected (section 2.2), they did not cover before the Renaissance periods or modern art styles. In addition, owing to the limited number of pre-rating scales, the results may be limited to specific positively valenced aesthetic emotions (liking and beauty), preventing generalisability to other negatively valenced aesthetic emotions (Istók et al., 2009; Augustin et al., 2012; Knoop et al., 2016). Third, the ratings and neural responses were obtained non-simultaneously and separated by a gap of up to 5 days between the pre-rating and MEG experiment, because of the aforementioned environmental factors and to reduce the physical burden among participants (section 2.3). Repeated exposure is known to alter the hedonic experience to the stimuli (Montoya et al., 2017). Moreover, prior exposure to the stimuli (i.e., learning) might reduce the experiences of unconformity and uncertainty to the unexpected (i.e., non-biological) stimuli in a PP sense, which may only trigger a ‘faint copy’ of uncertainty resolution (Van De Cruys et al., 2024). As such, the actual in-the-moment aesthetic experience may not be captured, but the stimuli would be labelled as aesthetically appealing owing to their past affordance of the aesthetic experience. Therefore, the aesthetic processes operated during the pre-rating and MEG measurements might not be quantitatively and qualitatively identical. Thus, our findings should be replicated in a future study using a simultaneous recording design. Fourth, the definitions of biological and non-biological stimuli were arbitrary. We operationally defined the biological and non-biological categories as extremes of the single continuous scale of ‘biologi-ness’ (section 1), but this idea needs further experimental support. Additionally, the stimuli sets were selected based on the relative scores within each participant (the top and bottom 20 images in the ‘biologi-ness’ rating). Another categorisation approach, such as the use of the absolute ‘biologi-ness’ score, may show different results; however, this is beyond the scope of the present study and should be investigated further in future psychological and neuroscientific studies.

5 Conclusion

This study examined the interaction between consciousness and aesthetic emotions using an electrophysiological method. The results revealed that the non-conscious presentation of biological and non-biological stimuli induced low-frequency brain activities associated with aesthetic ratings, suggesting that aesthetic emotions have adaptive significance. The underlying neural processes were distinctive for each stimulus category, possibly because of different aesthetic processing mechanisms, such as processing fluency, active elaboration, and PP. Furthermore, we discovered that the induced activities differed between the conscious and non-conscious conditions, with the latter thought to emphasise fluency-driven affective processing and suppress top-down regulative processes. Neural responses to aesthetic processing are determined by the interplay between perceptual, cognitive, and affective processes, which are further modified by the intervention of consciousness. This study provides the first empirical evidence supporting the evolutionary significance of aesthetic processing, motivating future studies to clarify the complex relationships among aesthetic processing, facial processing, and consciousness.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: Hoshi, H. (2023). MEGstudy on aesthetic emotion. Mendeley Data. 1. doi: 10.17632/8v8p82cy23.1.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of Hokuto Hospital and Osaka Metropolitan University Graduate School of Medicine. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

HH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AI: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. YS: Data curation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. TY: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The authors declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was partially sponsored by the ‘Grant for Groundbreaking Young Researchers’ from Suntory Foundation and JSPS KAKENHI (grant no. 22K11732). The funders were not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ms. Kaori Hoshi for developing the stereoscope employed in this study and Mr. Toyoji Okada for sharing technical advice regarding the MEG facility. We would also like to thank Dr. Hajime Kamada for providing the hospital facilities. Finally, we thank Editage (www.editage.com) for providing the English language editing services.

Conflict of interest

HH was employed by RICOH Co., Ltd. YS led a joint research project supported by RICOH Co., Ltd and another project supported by ITO EN, Ltd.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2024.1339479/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

CFS, continuous flash suppression; MEG, magnetoencephalography; FF, face-face, MF, Mondrian-face; MB, Mondrian-background; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging, ROI, region of interest; TF, time-frequency; FDR, false discovery rate; ERF, event-related field; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; IOFC, lateral orbitofrontal cortex; mOFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex; cACC, caudal ACC; STS, superior temporal sulcus; ERS, event-related synchronisation; ERD, event-related desynchronisation; EEG, electroencephalography.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.wikiart.org/
  2. ^ http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm
  3. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/

References

Aftanas, L. I., Reva, N. V., Varlamov, A. A., Pavlov, S. V., and Makhnev, V. P. (2004). Analysis of evoked EEG synchronization and desynchronization in conditions of emotional activation in humans: Temporal and topographic characteristics. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 34, 859–867. doi: 10.1023/B:NEAB.0000038139.39812.EB/METRICS

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aftanas, L. I., Varlamov, A. A., Pavlov, S. V., Makhnev, V. P., and Reva, N. V. (2001). Affective picture processing: Event-related synchronization within individually defined human theta band is modulated by valence dimension. Neurosci. Lett. 303, 115–118. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01703-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alpers, G. W., Gerdes, A. B. M., Lagarie, B., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., and Stark, R. (2009). Attention and amygdala activity: An fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia. J. Neural Transm. 116, 747–757. doi: 10.1007/S00702-008-0106-8/FIGURES/4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, D. L., and Fiske, S. T. (2010). Cultural neuroscience. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 13, 72–82. doi: 10.1111/J.1467-839X.2010.01301.X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Augustin, M. D., Wagemans, J., and Carbon, C. C. (2012). All is beautiful? Generality vs. specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychol. 139, 187–201. doi: 10.1016/J.ACTPSY.2011.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelrod, V., Bar, M., and Rees, G. (2015). Exploring the unconscious using faces. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2014.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, B. (1988). A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Baillet, S. (2017). Magnetoencephalography for brain electrophysiology and imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 327–339. doi: 10.1038/nn.4504

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Brambilla, E., and Falbo, L. (2009a). BIS/BAS, cortical oscillations and coherence in response to emotional cues. Brain Res. Bull. 80, 151–157. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRESBULL.2009.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Falbo, L., and Brambilla, E. (2009b). BIS/BAS responses to emotional cues: Self report, autonomic measure and alpha band modulation. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 858–863. doi: 10.1016/J.PAID.2009.07.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Grippa, E., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2015). What hemodynamic (fNIRS), electrophysiological (EEG) and autonomic integrated measures can tell us about emotional processing. Brain Cogn. 95, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/J.BANDC.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Lucchiari, C. (2006). EEG correlates (event-related desynchronization) of emotional face elaboration: A temporal analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 392, 118–123. doi: 10.1016/J.NEULET.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Pozzoli, U. (2007). Event-related oscillations (EROs) and event-related potentials (ERPs) comparison in facial expression recognition. J. Neuropsychol. 1, 283–294. doi: 10.1348/174866407X184789

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Pozzoli, U. (2009). Arousal effect on emotional face comprehension: Frequency band changes in different time intervals. Physiol. Behav. 97, 455–462. doi: 10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2009.03.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamidis, P. D., Klados, M. A., Frantzidis, C., Vivas, A. B., Papadelis, C., Lithari, C., et al. (2009). A framework combining delta Event-Related Oscillations (EROs) and synchronisation effects (ERD/ERS) to study emotional processing. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2009:549419. doi: 10.1155/2009/549419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Başar, E., Schmiedt-Fehr, C., Öniz, A., and Başar-Eroğlu, C. (2008). Brain oscillations evoked by the face of a loved person. Brain Res. 1214, 105–115. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2008.03.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bignardi, G., Ishizu, T., and Zeki, S. (2021). The differential power of extraneous influences to modify aesthetic judgments of biological and artifactual stimuli. PsyCh. J. 10, 190–199. doi: 10.1002/PCHJ.415

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boccia, M., Barbetti, S., Piccardi, L., Guariglia, C., Ferlazzo, F., Giannini, A. M., et al. (2016). Where does brain neural activation in aesthetic responses to visual art occur? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 60, 65–71. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2015.09.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. H., Ferdinandsen, K., and Gross, C. G. (1981). Perception of symmetry in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 17, 82–86. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.82

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brielmann, A. A., and Dayan, P. (2022). A computational model of aesthetic value. Psychol. Rev. 129, 1319–1337. doi: 10.1037/REV0000337

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. J., Savov, V., Allzén, E., Benedict, C., Fredriksson, R., and Schiöth, H. B. (2012). Exposure to subliminal arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular cortex and primary visual cortex: A systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroimage 59, 2962–2973. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.09.077

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouillet, D., and Friston, K. (2023). Relative fluency (unfelt vs felt) in active inference. Conscious. Cogn. 115:103579. doi: 10.1016/J.CONCOG.2023.103579

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., Gao, X., Tisdelle, L., Eickhoff, S. B., and Liotti, M. (2011). Naturalizing aesthetics: Brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. Neuroimage 58, 250–258. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, C., Kang, H., Kirsch, H. E., Mizuiri, D., Chen, J., Bhutada, A., et al. (2019). Comparison of DSSP and tSSS algorithms for removing artifacts from vagus nerve stimulators in magnetoencephalography data. J. Neural Eng. 16:066045. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab4065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlsson, K., Petersson, K. M., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., Ingvar, M., and Öhman, A. (2004). Fear and the amygdala: Manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but nonfeared) stimuli. Emotion 4, 340–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, J. (1998). Steven Pinker’s cheesecake for the mind. Philos. Lit. 22, 478–485. doi: 10.1353/PHL.1998.0036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattaneo, Z., Lega, C., Flexas, A., Nadal, M., Munar, E., and Cela-conde, C. J. (2014). The world can look better: Enhancing beauty experience with brain stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1713–1721. doi: 10.1093/SCAN/NST165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cela-Conde, C. J., Garcfa-Prieto, J., Ramasco, J. J., Mirasso, C. R., Bajo, R., Munara, E., et al. (2013). Dynamics of brain networks in the aesthetic appreciation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 10454–10461. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.1302855110/SUPPL_FILE/PNAS.201302855SI.PDF

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cela-Conde, C. J., Marty, G., Maestú, F., Ortiz, T., Munar, E., Fernández, A., et al. (2004). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 6321–6325. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.0401427101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A. (2014). The Aesthetic Brain: How we Evolved to Desire Beauty and Enjoy Art. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A., Thomas, A., Smith, S. E., and Aguirre, G. K. (2009). The neural response to facial attractiveness. Neuropsychology 23, 135–143. doi: 10.1037/A0014430

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A., and Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 370–375. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuan-Peng, H., Huang, Y., Eickhoff, S. B., Peng, K., and Sui, J. (2020). Seeking the ‘Beauty Center’ in the brain: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies of beautiful human faces and visual art. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 20, 1200–1215. doi: 10.3758/S13415-020-00827-Z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, B. R. (2018). The organization and operation of inferior temporal cortex. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 4, 381–402. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV-VISION-091517-034202

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran-Everett, D. (2000). Multiple comparisons: Philosophies and illustrations. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 279, R1–R8. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.1.r1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlén, A. D., Schofield, A., Schiöth, H. B., and Brooks, S. J. (2022). Subliminal emotional faces elicit predominantly right-lateralized amygdala activation: A systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Front. Neurosci. 16:868366. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2022.868366/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

Deen, B., Koldewyn, K., Kanwisher, N., and Saxe, R. (2015). Functional organization of social perception and cognition in the superior temporal sulcus. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4596–4609. doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHV111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., and Changeux, J. P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 14529–14534. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.95.24.14529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell’Acqua, C., Dal Bò, E., Moretta, T., Palomba, D., and Messerotti Benvenuti, S. (2022). EEG time–frequency analysis reveals blunted tendency to approach and increased processing of unpleasant stimuli in dysphoria. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-12263-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell’Acqua, C., Hajcak, G., Amir, N., Santopetro, N. J., Brush, C. J., and Meyer, A. (2023). Error-related brain activity: A time-domain and time-frequency investigation in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology 60:e14216. doi: 10.1111/PSYP.14216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

d’Errico, F., and Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366:1060. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2010.0340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Dio, C., and Vittorio, G. (2009). Neuroaesthetics: A review. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 682–687. doi: 10.1016/J.CONB.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Dio, D. C., Macaluso, E., and Rizzolatti, G. (2007). The golden beauty: Brain response to classical and renaissance sculptures. PLoS One 2:e1201. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0001201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drewes, J., Zhu, W., and Melcher, D. (2018). The edge of awareness: Mask spatial density, but not color, determines optimal temporal frequency for continuous flash suppression. J. Vis. 18:12. doi: 10.1167/18.1.12

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchaine, B., and Yovel, G. (2015). A revised neural framework for face processing. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1, 393–416. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV-VISION-082114-035518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.

Google Scholar

Etcoff, N. L. (1999). Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty. New York, NY: Anchor Books.

Google Scholar

Fang, Z., Li, H., Chen, G., and Yang, J. J. (2016). Unconscious processing of negative animals and objects: Role of the amygdala revealed by fMRI. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:146. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00146/ABSTRACT

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, C., Lega, C., Tamietto, M., Nadal, M., and Cattaneo, Z. (2015). I find you more attractive …after (prefrontal cortex) stimulation. Neuropsychologia 72, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2015.04.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, C., Nadal, M., Schiavi, S., Vecchi, T., Cela-Conde, C. J., and Cattaneo, Z. (2017). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex mediates the interaction between moral and aesthetic valuation: A TMS study on the beauty-is-good stereotype. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 707–717. doi: 10.1093/SCAN/NSX002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonov, V., Evans, A., McKinstry, R., Almli, C., and Collins, D. (2009). Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage 47:S102. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(09)70884-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frascaroli, J., Leder, H., Brattico, E., and Van De Cruys, S. (2024). Aesthetics and predictive processing: Grounds and prospects of a fruitful encounter. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 379:20220410. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2022.0410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasquoine, P. G. (2014). Contributions of the insula to cognition and emotion. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 77–87. doi: 10.1007/S11065-014-9246-9/TABLES/1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., and Anderson, A. W. (2000). The fusiform ‘face area’ is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 495–504. doi: 10.1162/089892900562165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobbini, M. I., Gors, J. D., Halchenko, Y. O., Rogers, C., Guntupalli, J. S., Hughes, H., et al. (2013). Prioritized detection of personally familiar faces. PLoS One 8:e66620. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0066620

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Graf, L. K. M., and Landwehr, J. R. (2015). A dual-process perspective on fluency-based aesthetics: The pleasure-interest model of aesthetic liking. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 19, 395–410. doi: 10.1177/1088868315574978

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Graf, L. K. M., and Landwehr, J. R. (2017). Aesthetic pleasure versus aesthetic interest: The two routes to aesthetic liking. Front. Psychol. 8:217446. doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2017.00015/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grammer, K., Fink, B., Møller, A. P., and Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics: Sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biol. Rev. 78, 385–407. doi: 10.1017/S1464793102006085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., and Kutas, M. (2011). Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: A critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology 48, 1711–1725. doi: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.2011.01273.X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntekin, B., and Başar, E. (2009). Facial affect manifested by multiple oscillations. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 71, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2008.07.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntekin, B., and Başar, E. (2014). A review of brain oscillations in perception of faces and emotional pictures. Neuropsychologia 58, 33–51. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2014.03.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haber, S. N., and Knutson, B. (2009). The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacol 35, 4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., and Puce, A. (2017). MEG–EEG Primer. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, doi: 10.1093/MED/9780190497774.003.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Clark, V. P., Schouten, J. L., Hoffman, E. A., and Martin, A. (1999). The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object peyrception. Neuron 22, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80690-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, D. J., and Northoff, G. (2012). Common brain activations for painful and non-painful aversive stimuli. BMC Neurosci. 13:60. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-60/FIGURES/3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, E. A., and Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 80–84. doi: 10.1038/71152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoshi, H., Hirata, Y., Fukasawa, K., Kobayashi, M., and Shigihara, Y. (2024). Oscillatory characteristics of resting-state magnetoencephalography reflect pathological and symptomatic conditions of cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1273738. doi: 10.3389/FNAGI.2024.1273738

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoshi, H., and Menninghaus, W. (2018). The eye tracks the aesthetic appeal of sentences. J. Vis. 18, 19–19. doi: 10.1167/18.3.19

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iidaka, T. (2014). Role of the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus in face perception and recognition: An empirical review. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 56, 33–45. doi: 10.1111/JPR.12018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishizu, T., and Zeki, S. (2011). Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS One 6:e21852. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0021852

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Istók, E., Brattico, E., Jacobsen, T., Krohn, K., Müller, M., and Tervaniemi, M. (2009). Aesthetic responses to music: A questionnaire study. Music Sci. 13, 183–206. doi: 10.1177/102986490901300201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, T., and Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 289–299. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.4.289/METRICS

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Costello, P., and He, S. (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: Advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychol. Sci. 18, 349–355. doi: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.2007.01902.X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., and He, S. (2006). Cortical responses to invisible faces: Dissociating subsystems for facial-information processing. Curr. Biol. 16, 2023–2029. doi: 10.1016/J.CUB.2006.08.084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Carmel, D., Bahrami, B., and Rees, G. (2011). Structural and functional fractionation of right superior parietal cortex in bistable perception. Curr. Biol. 21, R106–R107. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawabata, H., and Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1699–1705. doi: 10.1152/JN.00696.2003/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/Z9K0040437760007.JPEG

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kedia, G., Mussweiler, T., Mullins, P., and Linden, D. E. J. (2014). The neural correlates of beauty comparison. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 681–688. doi: 10.1093/SCAN/NST026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D. S., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2004). Activation of the amygdala and anterior cingulate during nonconscious processing of sad versus happy faces. Neuroimage 21, 1215–1223. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2003.12.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, U. (2008). The neural basis of object-context relationships on aesthetic judgment. PLoS One 3:e3754. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0003754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoop, C. A., Wagner, V., Jacobsen, T., and Menninghaus, W. (2016). Mapping the aesthetic space of literature ‘from below.’. Poetics 56, 35–49. doi: 10.1016/J.POETIC.2016.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knyazev, G. G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J. Y., and Bocharov, A. V. (2009). Event-related delta and theta synchronization during explicit and implicit emotion processing. Neuroscience 164, 1588–1600. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2009.09.057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, S., Greenwald, M., Dhman, A., Vaid, D., et al. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Univ. Florida Cent. Res. Psychophysiol. Gainesv. 1:3.

Google Scholar

Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., Rieser-Danner, L. A., and Jenkins, V. Y. (1987). Infant preferences for attractive faces: Rudiments of a stereotype? Dev. Psychol. 23, 363–369. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.3.363

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Lin, F. H., Witzel, T., Ahlfors, S. P., Stufflebeam, S. M., Belliveau, J. W., and Hämäläinen, M. S. (2006). Assessing and improving the spatial accuracy in MEG source localization by depth-weighted minimum-norm estimates. Neuroimage 31, 160–171. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2005.11.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and Debruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366:1638. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2010.0404

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Logothetis, N. K., and Sheinberg, D. L. (1996). Visual object recognition. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 577–621. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV.NE.19.030196.003045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Machetanz, K., Gallotti, A. L., Leao Tatagiba, M. T., Liebsch, M., Trakolis, L., Wang, S., et al. (2021). Time-frequency representation of motor evoked potentials in brain tumor patients. Front. Neurol. 11:633224. doi: 10.3389/FNEUR.2020.633224/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Makeig, S., Bell, A. J., Jung, T. P., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1996). ‘Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data,’ in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 8, eds D. Touretzky, M. Mozer, and M. Hasselmo (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 145–151.

Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Cagigal, V. (2021). Multiple Testing Toolbox. Available online at: https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/70604-multiple-testing-toolbox (accessed April 26, 2021).

Google Scholar

Martín-Loeches, M., Hernández-Tamames, J. A., Martín, A., and Urrutia, M. (2014). Beauty and ugliness in the bodies and faces of others: An fMRI study of person esthetic judgement. Neuroscience 277, 486–497. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2014.07.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcbrearty, S., and Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J. Hum. Evol. 39, 453–563. doi: 10.1006/JHEV.2000.0435

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., Vevea, J. L., Citkowicz, M., and Lauber, E. A. (2017). A re-examination of the mere exposure effect: The influence of repeated exposure on recognition, familiarity, and liking. Psychol. Bull. 143, 459–498. doi: 10.1037/BUL0000085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mudrik, L., and Deouell, L. Y. (2022). Neuroscientific evidence for processing without awareness. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 45, 403–423. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV-NEURO-110920-033151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muñoz, F., and Martín-Loeches, M. (2015). Electrophysiological brain dynamics during the esthetic judgment of human bodies and faces. Brain Res. 1594, 154–164. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2014.10.061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., Kawashima, R., Nagumo, S., Ito, K., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., et al. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of the assessment of facial attractiveness. Neuroreport 9, 753–757. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199803090-00035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Natu, V., and O’Toole, A. J. (2011). The neural processing of familiar and unfamiliar faces: A review and synopsis. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 726–747. doi: 10.1111/J.2044-8295.2011.02053.X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J.-M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkkonen, L., Andersson, J., Hämäläinen, M., and Hari, R. (2008). Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 20500–20504. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.0810966105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesciarelli, F., Leo, I., and Serafini, L. (2021). Electrophysiological correlates of unconscious processes of race. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-91133-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., and Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 773–782. doi: 10.1038/nrn2920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piai, V., Roelofs, A., and Van Der Meij, R. (2012). Event-related potentials and oscillatory brain responses associated with semantic and Stroop-like interference effects in overt naming. Brain Res. 1450, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2012.02.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Pinker, S., and Fodor, J. (2005). So how does the mind work? Mind Lang. 20, 1–24. doi: 10.1111/J.0268-1064.2005.00274.X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Dilks, D. D., Saxe, R. R., Triantafyllou, C., and Kanwisher, N. (2011). Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions. Neuroimage 56, 2356–2363. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.03.067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Japee, S., Rauth, L., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2017). The superior temporal sulcus is causally connected to the Amygdala: A combined TBS-fMRI Study. J. Neurosci. 37, 1156–1161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0114-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Popov, T., Oostenveld, R., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2018). FieldTrip made easy: An analysis protocol for group analysis of the auditory steady state brain response in time, frequency, and space. Front. Neurosci. 12:402658. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2018.00711/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajimehr, R., Young, J. C., and Tootell, R. B. H. (2009). An anterior temporal face patch in human cortex, predicted by macaque maps. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106, 1995–2000. doi: 10.1073/PNAS.0807304106/SUPPL_FILE/0807304106SI.PDF

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, R., Schwarz, N., and Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 364–382. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0804_3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, R., Winkielman, P., and Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychol. Sci. 9, 45–48. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G. (2005). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226. doi: 10.1146/ANNUREV.PSYCH.57.102904.190208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothenberg, D. (2011). Survival of the Beautiful: Art, Science, and Evolution. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.

Google Scholar

Rusch, H., and Voland, E. (2013). Evolutionary aesthetics: An introduction to key concepts and current issues. Aisthesis 6, 113–133. doi: 10.13128/AISTHESIS-13773

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarasso, P., Ronga, I., Kobau, P., Bosso, T., Artusio, I., Ricci, R., et al. (2020). Beauty in mind: Aesthetic appreciation correlates with perceptual facilitation and attentional amplification. Neuropsychologia 136:107282. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2019.107282

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubring, D., and Schupp, H. T. (2021). Emotion and brain oscillations: High arousal is associated with decreases in alpha- and lower beta-band power. Cereb. Cortex 31, 1597–1608. doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHAA312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekihara, K., Kawabata, Y., Ushio, S., Sumiya, S., Kawabata, S., Adachi, Y., et al. (2016). Dual signal subspace projection (DSSP): A novel algorithm for removing large interference in biomagnetic measurements. J. Neural Eng. 13:036007. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/13/3/036007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., and Armony, J. L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 811–830. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2007.12.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., and Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 334–340. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Somerville, L. H., Kim, H., Johnstone, T., Alexander, A. L., and Whalen, P. J. (2004). Human amygdala responses during presentation of happy and neutral faces: Correlations with state anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 897–903. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2004.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, G. G. (2023). Aesthetic experience models human learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1146083. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2023.1146083/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stegemöller, E. L., Ferguson, T. D., Zaman, A., Hibbing, P., Izbicki, P., and Krigolson, O. E. (2021). Finger tapping to different styles of music and changes in cortical oscillations. Brain Behav. 11:e2324. doi: 10.1002/BRB3.2324

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, T., Hebart, M. N., and Sterzer, P. (2011). Breaking continuous flash suppression: A new measure of unconscious processing during interocular suppression? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:13980. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2011.00167/BIBTEX

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, T., Sterzer, P., and Peelen, M. V. (2012). Privileged detection of conspecifics: Evidence from inversion effects during continuous flash suppression. Cognition 125, 64–79. doi: 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stelter, M., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2023). Understanding the mechanisms underlying the other-‘race’ effect: An attempt at integrating different perspectives. Br. J. Psychol. 114, 1–9. doi: 10.1111/BJOP.12615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tadel, F., Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., Pantazis, D., and Leahy, R. M. (2011). Brainstorm: A user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, 1–13. doi: 10.1155/2011/879716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamietto, M., and De Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn2889

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., et al. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01403-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of Ethology. Z. Tierpsychol. 20, 410–433. doi: 10.1111/J.1439-0310.1963.TB01161.X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, F. (2003). Primary visual cortex and visual awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 219–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn1055

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., Schweers, N., Moeller, S., and Freiwald, W. A. (2008). Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 877–879. doi: 10.1038/nn.2158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. (2004). Continuous flash suppression. J. Vis. 4:61. doi: 10.1167/4.8.61

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1096–1101. doi: 10.1038/nn1500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsukiura, T., and Cabeza, R. (2011). Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral judgments: implications for the Beauty-is-Good stereotype. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 138–148. doi: 10.1093/SCAN/NSQ025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q., Nomi, J. S., Hébert-Seropian, B., Ghaziri, J., and Boucher, O. (2017). Structure and function of the human insula. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 34:300. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Cruys, S., Frascaroli, J., and Friston, K. (2024). Order and change in art: Towards an active inference account of aesthetic experience. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 379:20220411. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2022.0411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Wel, P., and van Steenbergen, H. (2018). Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: A review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2005–2015. doi: 10.3758/S13423-018-1432-Y/METRICS

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gaal, S., Naccache, L., Meuwese, J. D. I., van Loon, A. M., Leighton, A. H., Cohen, L., et al. (2014). Can the meaning of multiple words be integrated unconsciously? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130212. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2013.0212

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, O., and Goel, V. (2004). Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport 15, 893–897. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404090-00032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vessel, E. A., Maurer, N., Denker, A. H., and Starr, G. G. (2018). Stronger shared taste for natural aesthetic domains than for artifacts of human culture. Cognition 179, 121–131. doi: 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2018.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vessel, E. A., Stahl, J., Maurer, N., Denker, A., and Starr, G. G. (2014). Personalized visual aesthetics. Hum. Vis. Electron. Imaging XIX 25:9014. doi: 10.1117/12.2043126

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vialatte, F. B., Maurice, M., Dauwels, J., and Cichocki, A. (2010). Steady-state visually evoked potentials: Focus on essential paradigms and future perspectives. Prog. Neurobiol. 90, 418–438. doi: 10.1016/J.PNEUROBIO.2009.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Voland, E., and Grammer, K. (2003). Evolutionary Aesthetics. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Wassiliwizky, E., and Menninghaus, W. (2021). Why and how should cognitive science care about aesthetics? Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 437–449. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2021.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, J., Lotze, M., Weike, A. I., Hosten, N., and Hamm, A. O. (2008). Brain activation and defensive response mobilization during sustained exposure to phobia-related and other affective pictures in spider phobia. Psychophysiology 45, 205–215. doi: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.2007.00620.X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiset, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., and Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: The SHINE toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 671–684. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.671/METRICS

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., and Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 45, 195–206. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2006.05.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, J., Jasko, K., and Winkielman, P. (2024). Fluency, prediction and motivation: How processing dynamics, expectations and epistemic goals shape aesthetic judgements. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 379:20230326. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2023.0326

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeki, S. (2004). The neurology of ambiguity. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 173–196. doi: 10.1016/J.CONCOG.2003.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, W., Drewes, J., and Melcher, D. (2016). Time for awareness: The influence of temporal properties of the mask on continuous flash suppression effectiveness. PLoS One 11:e0159206. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0159206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: magnetoencephalography, vision, continuous flash suppression, neuroaesthetics, aesthetic emotion

Citation: Hoshi H, Ishii A, Shigihara Y and Yoshikawa T (2024) Binocularly suppressed stimuli induce brain activities related to aesthetic emotions. Front. Neurosci. 18:1339479. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1339479

Received: 17 November 2023; Accepted: 16 April 2024;
Published: 24 May 2024.

Edited by:

Alexander Toet, Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Koichi Yokosawa, Hokkaido University, Japan
G. Gabrielle Starr, Pomona College, United States

Copyright © 2024 Hoshi, Ishii, Shigihara and Yoshikawa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hideyuki Hoshi, heurekaesthem.avir@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.