Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 17 June 2025

Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2025.1580045

Relationship between early musical training and detection of binaural gap based on interaural correlation change

  • 1Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3Kunming Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service Centre, Yunnan, China
  • 4Cognitive Science and Allied Health School, Institute of Life and Health Sciences, Key Laboratory of Language and Cognitive Science, Ministry of Education, Beijing, China

Introduction: The auditory fusion of binaural sounds and the perceived auditory image are determined by the similarities of the sounds at the two ears. Sensitivity to the change in interaural correlation, a measure of interaural similarity, is crucial to extract target sound from noisy background. Although musicians have been found to perform better than non-musicians in various types of auditory processing tasks such as frequency discrimination or temporal resolution, the relationship between musical training and the interaural correlation processing remains poorly understood.

Methods: Here we embedded a fragment of interaurally uncorrelated noises (interaural correlation = 0) into the interaurally identical marker noises (interaural correlation = 1) and constructed a binaural gap based on the change in interaural correlation (from 1 to 0 then back to 1). The minimum duration of interaurally uncorrelated fragment for detecting the binaural gap (duration threshold) was determined for groups of young adults without musical training and those who started musical training early (before 7 years of age) or late (after 8 years of age).

Results: When the binaural noises arrived simultaneously (Experiment 1), we found that the duration threshold was significantly correlated with the onset age of musical training for the early-trained musicians but no such significant correlation was observed for the late-trained musicians. Moreover, the duration thresholds for the early-trained musicians were significantly shorter than those for both the late-trained musicians and non-musicians. When interaural delay was introduced (Experiment 2), this early-musical-training-related enhancement in interaural correlation processing was maintained for binaural noises when the interaural delay was 2 ms, while no enhancement was found when the interaural delay was 4 ms.

Discussion: Our findings suggest that sensitivity to dynamic changes in interaural correlation might be influenced by musical training in early childhood, implying a sensitive period when musical training has a significant impact on interaural correlation processing.

Introduction

Musical training has a profound influence on auditory processing. Musicians outperformed non-musicians across a range of auditory tasks, including frequency discrimination (Boebinger et al., 2015; Deguchi et al., 2012; Kyrtsoudi et al., 2023; Lee et al., 2020; Madsen et al., 2019; Meha-Bettison et al., 2018; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009; Spiegel and Watson, 1984), temporal resolution (Kumar et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2016; Pirouzmand et al., 2022; Rammsayer and Altenmüller, 2006; Ryn Junior et al., 2022; Zendel and Alain, 2012) and speech recognition against noisy background (Başkent and Gaudrain, 2016; Bidelman and Yoo, 2020; Clayton et al., 2016; Coffey et al., 2017; Du and Zatorre, 2017; Hennessy et al., 2022; Kaplan et al., 2021; Kyrtsoudi et al., 2023; Li et al., 2021; Meha-Bettison et al., 2018; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009; Puschmann et al., 2019; Swaminathan et al., 2015; Zendel and Alain, 2012; Zhang et al., 2021). Musicianship-related enhancement for the processing spectral and temporal information have also been observed at both the cortical (Hao et al., 2023; Jünemann et al., 2023; Li et al., 2018; Pantev et al., 1998; Puschmann et al., 2021) and subcortical levels (Musacchia et al., 2007; Parbery-Clark et al., 2012; Tierney et al., 2015; Wong et al., 2007).

Critically, the timing of training onset plays a pivotal role in shaping these enhancements. For example, Pantev et al. (1998) found that the auditory cortical representation of piano tones in early-adulthood musicians was significantly correlated with the age at which they began training. Similarly, Bailey and Penhune (2012) reported a correlation between the age of training onset and behavioral performance in an auditory-motor rhythm synchronization task (RST). Further, Bailey and Penhune (2013) identified a non-linear relationship between training onset age and RST performance, with a critical shift around ages 7–9. When musicians are divided into early-trained (onset ≤ 7 years) and late-trained (onset > 7 years) groups, better performance was consistently found for early-trained group compared with the late-trained group and non-musicians (Bailey and Penhune, 2010, 2012, 2013; Bailey et al., 2014; Penhune et al., 2005; Vaquero et al., 2016; Watanabe et al., 2007), suggesting a sensitive period for the effect of musical training.

Beyond monaural processing (e.g., frequency discrimination and temporal resolution), binaural processing, i.e., the fusion and localization of sounds from both ears into a unified auditory object, relies on comparing inputs between the left and right ears (Cherry and Sayers, 1956; Cherry and Taylor, 1954). In recent years, the relationship between musical training and binaural processing has received more attention. Musicians were found to have superior binaural processing abilities in discrimination of interaural time and level differences (Madsen et al., 2019; Nisha et al., 2022; Nisha et al., 2023; Wright and Dai, 2024) and other studies showed that musicians were more adept at discriminating interaural phase differences than non-musicians (Bianchi et al., 2019; Ding et al., 2022; Pirouzmand et al., 2022). Notably, Wright and Dai (2024) found that early-trained musicians outperformed non-musicians in discrimination of interaural level difference but not interaural time difference. However, no studies to date have explored the relationship between musical training and interaural coherence processing, a fundamental mechanism for sound localization and speech-in-noise perception (Stern and Trahiotis, 1995; Licklider, 1948; Durlach et al., 1986).

Interaural correlation, one measure of interaural coherence, is defined as the maximum cross-correlation coefficient of the two sound waves at the left and right ears after one sound has been time shifted (Grantham, 1995). Dynamic processing of the interaural correlation can be assessed using the binaural gap detection paradigm (Akeroyd and Summerfield, 1999). In this paradigm, listeners detect a brief segment of binaurally independent noise (interaural correlation: 0) embedded in coherent noise (correlation: 1). The minimum detectable duration of this gap—the duration threshold—reflects sensitivity to dynamic changes in interaural coherence (Boehnke et al., 2002; Kong et al., 2012; Kong et al., 2015). The interaural correlation processing is strongly affected by interaural delay. For binaurally identical noises, human listener perceives a fused sound image when the interaural delay was 0 ms while the image becomes completed diffused for interaural delay of a few milliseconds (Blodgett et al., 1956; Mossop and Culling, 1998). Our previous studies showed that duration thresholds increase with interaural delay (Kong et al., 2012, 2015) and the maximal interaural delay for binaural gap detection was associated with speech in noise recognition under simulated reverberant conditions (Li et al., 2013). Intriguingly, Ding et al. (2022) determined the maximum interaural delay for detecting a binaural gap with a fix duration, however, the performance of musicians and non-musicians was similar (Ding et al., 2022).

The current study is going to address two questions: First, does musical training enhance binaural gap detection when sounds arrive simultaneously or with short interaural delays (2–4 ms)? Second, is this enhancement specific to individuals who began training during early sensitive periods? To answer these questions, we examined the duration thresholds for binaural gap detection in three participant groups: early-trained musicians (early-trained group), late-trained musicians (late-trained group) and non-musicians (control group) when the binaural noises arrived at the two ears simultaneously (Experiment 1) and with interaural delays of 2 ms or 4 ms (Experiment 2).

Materials and methods

Experiment 1

Participants

Forty-one individuals between the ages of 17 and 25 (Mean = 20.0, SD = 2.2) took part in this study. All participants were undergraduate or graduate students from universities in the Beijing vicinity. Participants claimed that they were neurologically healthy and the experience of musical training, including the onset age of training, the duration of training, and whether musicians still kept training was also determined through a questionnaire that was developed within our laboratory. Among the 41 participants, 27 were professional musicians (19 females and 8 males) and 14 were non-musicians (10 females and 4 males). The non-musicians had no more than 1 year of formal music training, excluding compulsory music courses during school education, and belonged to the control group. All musicians had at least 8 years of training experience (Mean = 12.7, SD = 2.3) and were currently undergoing continuous training. Those who began music training before age 7 were classified into the early-trained group (n = 15), and those who began at or after age 8 were classified into the late-trained group (n = 12). The selection criteria for the onset ages of the early-trained and late-trained groups were similar to previous studies (Bailey and Penhune, 2013; Steele et al., 2013).

Participants’ pure-tone thresholds were no larger than 20 dB HL for frequencies from 0.125 to 8 kHz and their binaural hearing was balanced in that the differences between the pure-tone thresholds for the two ears were less than 15 dB at each frequency. All participants have provided written informed consent, with parental proxy signatures for underage participants. Additionally, all participants have received compensation. All the experiments in this study were approved by the Ethics and Bioethics Committee of Beijing Normal University (BNU202401250008).

Apparatus and stimuli

The participant sat on a chair at in a sound attenuated chamber. The experimental stimuli were Gaussian white noise synthesized using the “randn()” function in MATLAB (the MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The Gaussian wideband noises (0–10 kHz) had a duration of 1000 ms (rise/fall time: 30 ms). The sampling rate was 48 kHz and the amplitude quantization was 16-bit.

For binaurally identical noises, the noise in the right ear were copied from the left ear (interaural correlation = 1). In order to construct a binaural gap, a segment in the temporal middle of noise for the right ear was substituted by another segment of noise that was binaural independent to the corresponding part of noise for the left ear (binaural gap: interaural correlation = 0). The noises at the two ears always started simultaneously (interaural time delay = 0 ms). Notice that the energy and spectrum of the binaural noises are not changed by the insertion of the binaural gap while the auditory image is modified, i.e., the perceptual compactness/diffuseness, number, placement (Bilsen and Goldstein, 1974; Edmonds and Culling, 2009). If only the sound at one ear was delivered, any changes during the noise could not be detected.

These binaural noises were delivered using a Creative Sound Blaster G6 (Creative SB1770, Creative Technology Ltd., Singapore) and headphones (ATH-MSR7, Audio-Technica Co., USA). Sound intensity was calibrated to 60 dB SPL using an Audiometer (Model 831, Larson Davis, Depew, NY, USA).

Procedure

We used a two-interval, two-alternative, forced-choice adaptive procedure. The noises with binaural gap were randomly assigned to one interval and binaurally identical noises was delivered to another interval. The two intervals were separated by 1000 ms.

When the participant arrived at the lab, we asked the participant to wear the headphones and listen to the stimulus with and without the binaural gap. When the sound with the binaural gap was delivered, we asked the participant to report if a change of sound image occurred during the sound. If the participant was able to report the change of sound image, the participant was asked to tell which of the two intervals contained the binaural gap. All participants completed a practice stage to discriminate the binaural noises with and without binaural gap, in order to ensuring that they understood the tasks and requirements before the formal test.

During the threshold measurement, we manipulated the gap duration using a three-down-one-up procedure (Levitt, 1971). If the participant consecutively identified the correct interval with the binaural gap for three times, the gap duration was decreased while the gap duration was increased if the participant made just one incorrect response. The manipulation of the gap duration started at 33 ms and the step size of the change started at 16 ms. Every time the gap duration changed from increase to decrease or from decrease to increase, the step size was reduced by a factor of 0.5 until reaching 1 ms. If the participant made the wrong selection, visible feedback (a red square) would be displayed on the computer screen.

We terminated the test procedure when 12 reversals were reached and used the last 6 reversals to calculate the duration threshold (arithmetic mean). Each participant completed 4 runs of test and we used the best three duration threshold to calculate the duration threshold for this participant.

Data analysis

The experimental data were recorded using Presentation (Version 20.1, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA). Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS (Version 27.0, IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). The relationship between the duration or onset age of musical training and duration threshold was tested by the Pearson correlation analysis. The effect of musical training and interaural delay on the duration threshold was tested by the repeated-measures ANOVA. There were two independent variables: one is the interaural delay (0 ms, 2 ms, 4 ms), another is the participant groups (musical-training group and the control group, or early-trained group, late-trained group and control group). For each interaural delay, the effect of musical training on the duration threshold was tested by the one-way ANOVA. The effect of musicians’ onset age while controlling training duration was examined by a hierarchical regression analysis with the musicians only. Step 1 included training duration as a control variable, and step 2 included onset age as an independent variable.

Experiment 2

Participants

Fifteen early-trained musicians, 12 late-trained musicians, and 14 non-musicians participated in Experiment 2 who also took part in Experiment 1.

Apparatus and stimuli

We used similar binaural-gap stimuli in Experiment 1 except interaural delays were introduced. We added a quiet segment (2 ms or 4 ms) to the beginning of the noise at the right ear and the end of the noise at the left ear. There were two conditions for interaural delay: 2 ms and 4 ms.

Procedure

The procedure in Experiment 2 was the same as that in Experiment 1. The duration thresholds for the two interaural-delay conditions (2 ms, 4 ms) were determined. A practice session was also completed before the formal test.

Data analysis

Same as Experiment 1.

Results

Experiment 1

When there was no interaural delay (0 ms), all participants perceived fused noise at a position in the middle of the head for binaurally identical noises and were able to detect the binaural gap. Therefore, data from all participants were included in the statistical analysis.

First, we performed a Mann-Whitney U test between all the participants with musical training (musical-training group) and those without (control group). The result showed that the duration threshold of musical-training group was not significantly different from that of control group (p = 0.151, Figure 1). Then, we tested the relationship between the duration or onset age of musical training and the duration thresholds for the binaural gap detection. We found the onset age of musical training was significant correlated with the duration threshold (r = 0.542, p < 0.01, 95% CI [0.194, 0.760], n = 27, Figure 2A). In contrast, no significant correlation was found between the duration of musical training and duration threshold (r = −0.005, p = 0.979, 95% CI [−0.384, 0.376], n = 27, Figure 2B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Duration thresholds for musical-training and control group (interaural delay = 0 ms). The dark filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the musical training group and the gray filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the non-musicians. Error bars show standard deviation for each group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Correlation between onset age of training and the duration thresholds. (B) Correlation between the duration of musical training and duration thresholds. Significant differences are marked with asterisks (**p < 0.01).

We further analyzed the relationship between the onset age of musical training and the duration thresholds for the early-trained and late-trained groups, respectively. A one-way ANOVA was then performed to test the effect of participant group (early-trained group, late-trained group and control group) on the duration threshold. The result showed that the main effect of group was significant [F(2,38) = 9.804, p < 0.001, eta squared: 0.340, Figure 3]. The Bonferroni post hoc test showed that the duration thresholds for early-trained musicians were significantly shorter than those for non-musicians (p < 0.01) and the late-trained musicians (p < 0.001), while the difference in the duration threshold between late-trained musicians and non-musicians was not significant (p = 1.000). In addition, there was a significant correlation between the onset age and duration threshold for the early-trained group (r = −0.758, p < 0.01, 95% CI [−0.911, −0.380], n = 15) while no significant correlation was found between the onset age and duration threshold for the late-trained group (r = −0.040, p = 0.902, 95% CI [−0.599, 0.548], n = 12, Figure 4A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Duration thresholds for early-trained group, late-trained group and control group (interaural delay = 0 ms). The red filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the early-trained group, the blue filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the late-trained group and the gray filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the non-musicians. Error bars show standard deviation for each group (**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Correlation between the onset age of musical training and duration thresholds for early-trained group (red circles) and late-trained group (blue circles) (interaural delay = 0 ms). (B) Duration of training for early-trained group (red bar) and late-trained group (blue bar). Significant differences are marked with asterisks (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

As the duration of training for the early-trained group was significantly longer than that of the late-trained group [t(25) = 2.845, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d: 2.04, Figure 4B], further analysis was necessary to clarify the effect of the onset age of musical training on the binaural gap detection. We analyzed the prediction of the onset age on musical-training-related improvement in performance using hierarchical multiple regression while removing the role of training duration. The regression shows that the final model was statistically significant [R2 = 0.542, F(2,24) = 14.195, p < 0.001]. The addition of the onset age improved the explanation of the dependent variable by 54.2%. Thus, after controlling for the contribution of training duration, the onset age still had a significant effect on predicting the musician’s performance on the task (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Hierarchical models of onset age and predictive value of the age of onset beyond duration of musical training to duration threshold of interaural correlation when the interaural delay is 0 ms (B is the regression coefficient and beta is the standardized regression coefficient; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).

Experiment 2

First, the effect of interaural delay (2 ms, 4 ms) on detecting the binaural gap was tested for the early-trained, late-trained and control groups. Because the participants in Experiment 2 were identical to those in Experiment 1, the data from Experiment 1 (interaural delay = 0 ms) were also included. A repeated-measures ANOVA, with one within-subject factor (interaural delay: 0, 2, 4 ms) and one between-subject factor (early-onset group, late-onset group and control group) showed that the main effect of interaural delay was significant [F(2,76) = 169.626, p < 0.001, eta squared: 0.817]. The Bonferroni post hoc test showed that the duration thresholds when the interaural delay was 0 ms were significantly shorter than those when the interaural delay was 2 ms (p < 0.001) and when the interaural delay was 4 ms (p < 0.001). The duration thresholds when the interaural delay was 2 ms were also significantly shorter than those when the interaural delay was 4 ms (p < 0.001, Figure 5). The effect of the group was not significant [F(2,38) = 1.023, p = 0.369, eta squared: 0.051] and the interaction between these two factors was also not significant [F(4,76) = 0.143, p = 0.874, eta squared: 0.007].

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effect of interaural delay on the duration thresholds for early-trained group (A), late-trained group (B) and control group (C). Significant differences are marked with asterisks (***p < 0.001).

Similar to Experiment 1, a one-way ANOVA was performed to test the effect of participant group (early-trained group, late-trained group and control group) on the duration threshold when the interaural delay was 2 ms and 4 ms, respectively. When the interaural delay was 2 ms, there was a significant main effect of group [F(2,38) = 5.597, p < 0.01, eta squared: 0.228, Figure 6A]. The Bonferroni post hoc test showed that the early-trained musicians outperformed both the non-musicians (p < 0.05) and the late-trained musicians (p < 0.05). There was no significant difference in duration threshold between late-trained musicians and non-musicians (p = 1.000). In contrast, the main effect of group was not significant [F(2,38) = 0.250, p = 0.780, eta squared: 0.013] when the interaural delay was 4 ms (Figure 6B).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. (A) Duration thresholds for early-trained group, late-trained group and control group (interaural delay = 2 ms). (B) Duration thresholds for early-trained group, late-trained group and control group (interaural delay = 4 ms). The red filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the early-trained group, the blue filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the late-trained group and the gray filled circle represents the individual data and mean for the non-musicians. Error bars show standard deviation for each group (*p < 0.05).

The relationship between the onset age of training and duration threshold was tested for the early-trained group and late-trained group. The result showed that there was no significant correlation between the onset age of musical training and the duration threshold no matter for the early-trained group (r = −0.155, p = 0.581, 95% CI [−0.615, 0.393], n = 15) or for the late-trained group (r = −0.123, p = 0.703, 95% CI [−0.648, 0.489], n = 12) when the interaural delay was 2 ms (Figure 7A). Similarly, when the interaural delay was 4 ms, there was also no significant correlation between the duration threshold and the onset age of musical training either, no matter for the early-trained groups (r = 0.159, p = 0.571, 95% CI [−0.389, 0.617], n = 15) or for the late-trained group (r = 0.017, p = 0.959, 95% CI [−0.563, 0.584], n = 12, Figure 7B).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. (A) Correlation between the onset age of musical training and duration thresholds for early-trained group (red circles) and late-trained group (blue circles) (interaural delay = 2 ms); (B) Correlation between the onset age of musical training and duration thresholds for early-trained group (red circles) and late-trained group (blue circles) (interaural delay = 4 ms).

We also used hierarchical regression to analyze the effect of onset age on musicians’ task performance when the interaural delay was 2 ms after removing the factor of training duration. The procedures were exactly the same as those in Experiment 1. The results demonstrated that the final model (Model 2) included both onset age and training duration with marginal statistical significance [R2 = 0.187, F(2,24) = 2.755, p = 0.084]. When the onset age was included, the explanation of the dependent variable increased by 18.4% (Table 2). This result implies that even after controlling for training duration, differences in task performance across musicians who began training at various ages remained when the interaural delay was 2 ms.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hierarchical models of onset age and predictive value of the age of onset beyond duration of musical training to duration threshold of interaural correlation when the interaural delay is 2 ms (B is the regression coefficient and beta is the standardized regression coefficient; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01).

Discussion

Our study was to investigate the relationship between musical training and auditory processing of dynamic changes in interaural correlation when there was no interaural delay (Experiment 1) or there were interaural delays (Experiment 2).

Relationship between musical training and binaural gap detection with no interaural delay

First, the relationship between musical training and binaural gap detection was discussed when all the musicians were included in one group. There was no significant difference in the task performance between the musicians and non-musicians in group level (Figure 1), which implies that musical training might not be a critical factor for binaural gap detection when the interaural delay is 0 ms. This finding partly agrees with that of Ding et al. (2022), who determined the maximum interaural delay at which a binaural gap of fixed duration could be detected and found no significant difference between participants with and without musical training. However, we observed a significant correlation between the onset age of training and detection of binaural gap (Figure 2A) while the detection of binaural gap was not significantly correlated with the duration of musical training (Figure 2B). Considering the previous evidences that early-trained musicians outperformed late-trained musicians of several auditory-motor tasks (Bailey and Penhune, 2010, 2012, 2013), we divided the musicians to an early-trained group and late-trained group and did further analysis for the duration thresholds among three participant groups.

Second, the effect of musical training was compared among three groups: early-trained group, late-trained group and control (non-musicians). We found that early-trained musicians had significantly shorter duration thresholds for binaural gap detection that those for non-musicians and late-trained musicians when the interaural time delay was 0 ms (Figure 3). In contrast to Ding et al. (2022), the results of our study provided the first evidence for the relationship between musical training and interaural correlation processing. Considering the musical-training-related improvement in sensitivity to interaural correlation change was found only in early-trained musicians but not in late-trained musicians, our results support the notion of a sensitive period for the impact of music training on interaural correlation processing. A sensitive period of musical training before age of 7 has been shown on the synchronization performance of visuomotor rhythm (Penhune et al., 2005; Watanabe et al., 2007) and the auditory-motor rhythm (Bailey and Penhune, 2010; Bailey et al., 2014), and also the temporal precision in their piano performance (Vaquero et al., 2016).

In the physiological level, previous studies revealed increased white matter integrity in the corpus callosum (Steele et al., 2013; Schlaug et al., 1995) and enhanced gray matter volume in the ventral premotor cortex (Bailey et al., 2014), which facilitate auditory-motor integration and interhemispheric communication. Early training also strengthens striatal-cortical connectivity (van Vugt et al., 2021) and refines cerebellar-putaminal circuits (Vaquero et al., 2016), supporting precise temporal resolution. These neuroplastic changes align with our behavioural findings, as such networks mature rapidly before age 7 (Skoe and Kraus, 2013; Penhune, 2011), creating a window for experience-dependent optimization. Moreover, electrophysiological evidence from deaf children who received cochlear implants (CIs) showed abnormal cortical response latencies if they received CI stimulation after 7 years of age (Sharma and Campbell, 2011) which suggests the sensitive period of auditory cortex comes to an end at 7 years old. As auditory cortex was shown to play an important role in interaural correlation processing (Chait et al., 2005), it looks reasonable for us to find that the musical training before the age of 7 has more significant impacts on interaural correlation processing compared with musicians who started the training later.

Third, the correlation between the onset age of musical training and binaural gap detection was tested for the early-trained and late-trained groups. For the early-trained group, a significant correlation was observed between the age of onset of musical training and duration thresholds under the condition of 0 ms interaural delay. In contrast, no significant correlation was observed in the late-trained group (Figure 4A). This finding was supported by finding from Bailey and Penhune (2013) that age of onset was correlated with sensorimotor-synchronization performance for early-trained musicians but not for the late-trained musicians. In contrast to the positive correlation found by Bailey and Penhune (2013), the duration thresholds were negatively correlated with the onset age of musical training for the early-trained group in our study (Figure 4A), which indicates that duration thresholds decrease with increasing age of onset training in the early-trained period. Note that the negative correlation between the onset age of musical training and duration thresholds for the early-trained group was also opposite to the positive correlation between these two factors for the entire musician group. A non-linear quadratic fit might be appropriate to explain the relationship between onset age of musical training and duration thresholds for the musical training group, which potentially supporting a “sensitive period” hypothesis. We used a quadratic model for the effect of the onset age and found that the quadratic model’s adjusted R2 value (0.45) is higher than the linear model’s (0.30). The quadratic coefficient (onset-age2) is statistically significant (p < 0.05). The critical age of ∼8.2 (vertex of the quadratic curve) years suggests a non-linear relationship, supporting a “sensitive period”.

This negative correlation could be explained in the neural physiological development of the human auditory system. It has been found that neurons in the superior olivary complex, inferior colliculus and auditory cortex encoded the interaural correlation (Coffey et al., 2006; Joris and Yin, 2007). The auditory cortex was found to be important for the processing of the interaural correlation change in human beings (Chait et al., 2005). In contrast to the early maturation for the auditory brainstem before 1 year old (Moore et al., 1995), the auditory cortex develops much slower and the mature thalamocortical axons fill the deeper cortical layers by age 5–6 years (Moore and Guan, 2001). Thus, it is reasonable to propose that the effect of musical training on binaural gap detection may be constrained by the immaturity of auditory cortex development in musicians who began training at a very young age, such as 4 and 5 years old and those started at age 6 might benefit from the musical training because their auditory cortex is more mature for the processing of the interaural correlation and their age was still in the sensitive period.

Relationship between musical training and binaural gap detection with interaural delays

First, the binaural gap detection was strongly affected by interaural delay. We found that the duration thresholds for binaural gap detection became longer as the interaural delay increased from 0 to 4 ms, no matter for musicians or non-musicians (Figure 5). This finding was in consistent with the findings from our previous studies (Kong et al., 2012; Kong et al., 2015). The reason is that the sound image of noises with an interaural correlation of 1 became more and more diffuse when the interaural delays increased from 0 to several milliseconds (Blodgett et al., 1956; Mossop and Culling, 1998). Our previous study also proved that the increase of interaural delay and decrease of interaural correlation of the marker similarly affected the detection of binaural gap, which implied a trade-off between the decrease of interaural correlation and increase of interaural delay on interaural correlation processing (Kong et al., 2015). Thus, the increase of interaural delay makes the auditory system harder to perceive the difference between the binaural gap and the marker in which binaural gap was embedded.

Second, the relationship between musical training and binaural gap detection was discussed when there were interaural delays. Similar to the findings when there was no interaural delay, we found that the early-trained group still retained some advantage when the interaural delay was 2 ms. Early-trained musicians significantly outperformed late-trained musicians and non-musicians in the detection of binaural gap (Figure 6A). However, the musical-training-related improvement in interaural correlation processing was not observed when the interaural delay was 4 ms (Figure 6B). Oure results partially agree with the finding of Ding et al. (2022) that there was no significant difference between binaural gap detection for musicians and non-musicians when the interaural time delay was extremely large, i.e., more than 10 ms.

Limitations of the current study

We should note that there are some limitations of the current study. First, our sample size was relatively small which may compromise the reliability of the correlation analysis. For example, the sample size for the late-trained group is 12 and future studies with larger sample size are needed to confirm the absence of a correlation between the onset age and duration thresholds. Moreover, whether early-trained musicians retain a long-term advantage in auditory skills after stopping music training remains to be investigated. Recently, it has been found that former musicians who had given up music training exhibited an advantage in prosodic pitch perception (Toh et al., 2023). In the future, it will be interesting to determine whether former musicians who began music training during a sensitive period and have ceased training still show better ability in interaural correlation processing.

In summary, our findings demonstrate while the interaural correlation processing was strongly affected by interaural delay, early-trained musicians had better performance than the non-musicians. However, late-trained musicians did not show such benefit. Our findings imply that there is a sensitive period for the musical training on the interaural correlation processing.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics and Bioethics Committee of Beijing Normal University (BNU202401250008). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

MW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing. SH: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JL: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MA: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. LK: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing, Funding acquisition.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31800962), the Beijing Social Science Foundation (19YYC020), the Teaching Research Foundation of Beijing Language and Cultural University (JPZY202206).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author declares that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Akeroyd, M. A., and Summerfield, A. Q. (1999). A binaural analog of gap detection. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 2807–2820. doi: 10.1121/1.426897

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. A., and Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization performance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 91–101. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2299-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. A., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). The relationship between the age of onset of musical training and rhythm synchronization performance: Validation of sensitive period effects. Front. Neurosci. 7:227. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2014). Early musical training is linked to gray matter structure in the ventral premotor cortex and auditory-motor rhythm synchronization performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 755–767. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J., and Penhune, V. B. (2012). A sensitive period for musical training: Contributions of age of onset and cognitive abilities. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 163–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06434.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Başkent, D., and Gaudrain, E. (2016). Musician advantage for speech-on-speech perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 139, EL51–EL56. doi: 10.1121/1.4942628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, F., Carney, L. H., Dau, T., and Santurette, S. (2019). Effects of musical training and hearing loss on fundamental frequency discrimination and temporal fine structure processing: Psychophysics and modeling. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 20, 263–277. doi: 10.1007/s10162-018-00710-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidelman, G. M., and Yoo, J. (2020). Musicians show improved speech segregation in competitive, multi-talker cocktail party scenarios. Front. Psychol. 11:1927. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01927

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilsen, F. A., and Goldstein, J. L. (1974). Pitch of dichotically delayed noise and its possible spectral basis. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 55, 292–296. doi: 10.1121/1.1914500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blodgett, H. C., Wilbanks, W. A., and Jeffress, L. A. (1956). Effect of large interaural time differences upon the judgment of sidedness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 28, 639–643. doi: 10.1121/1.1908430

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boebinger, D., Evans, S., Rosen, S., Lima, C. F., Manly, T., and Scott, S. K. (2015). Musicians and non-musicians are equally adept at perceiving masked speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 137, 378–387. doi: 10.1121/1.4904537

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehnke, S. E., Hall, S. E., and Marquardt, T. (2002). Detection of static and dynamic changes in interaural correlation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 1617–1626. doi: 10.1121/1.1504857

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chait, M., Poeppel, D., de Cheveigné, A., and Simon, J. Z. (2005). Human auditory cortical processing of changes in interaural correlation. J. Neurosci. 25, 8518–8527. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1266-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherry, E. C., and Sayers, B. A. (1956). “Human ‘cross-correlator”’—a technique for measuring certain parameters of speech perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 28, 889–895. doi: 10.1121/1.1908506

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherry, E. C., and Taylor, W. K. (1954). Some further experiments upon the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 26, 554–559. doi: 10.1121/1.1907373

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, K. K., Swaminathan, J., Yazdanbakhsh, A., Zuk, J., Patel, A. D., and Kidd, G. Jr. (2016). Executive function, visual attention and the cocktail party problem in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS One 11:e0157638. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157638

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, C. S., Ebert, C. S. Jr., Marshall, A. F., Skaggs, J. D., Falk, S. E., Crocker, W. D., et al. (2006). Detection of interaural correlation by neurons in the superior olivary complex, inferior colliculus and auditory cortex of the unanesthetized rabbit. Hear. Res. 221, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, E. B. J., Mogilever, N. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2017). Speech-in-noise perception in musicians: A review. Hear. Res. 352, 49–69. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deguchi, C., Boureux, M., Sarlo, M., Besson, M., Grassi, M., Schön, D., et al. (2012). Sentence pitch change detection in the native and unfamiliar language in musicians and non-musicians: Behavioral, electrophysiological and psychoacoustic study. Brain Res. 1455, 75–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.03.034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, Y., Lei, M., and Cao, C. (2022). The relationship between interaural delay in binaural gap detection and sensitivity to temporal fine structure in young adults with or without musical training experience. Front. Neurosci. 16:957012. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.957012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, Y., and Zatorre, R. J. (2017). Musical training sharpens and bonds ears and tongue to hear speech better. Proc.Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 13579–13584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712223114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlach, N. I., Gabriel, K. J., Colburn, H. S., and Trahiotis, C. (1986). Interaural correlation discrimination: II. Relation to binaural unmasking. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79, 1548–1557. doi: 10.1121/1.393681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmonds, B. A., and Culling, J. F. (2009). Interaural correlation and the binaural summation of loudness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125, 3865–3870. doi: 10.1121/1.3120412

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grantham, D. W. (1995). “Spatial hearing and related phenomena,” in Hearing (handbook of perception and cognition, ed. B. C. J. Moore (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 297–345.

Google Scholar

Hao, J., Zhong, Y., Pang, Y., Jing, Y., Liu, Y., Li, H., et al. (2023). The relationship between music training and cognitive flexibility: An ERP study. Front. Psychol. 14:1276752. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1276752

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennessy, S., Mack, W. J., and Habibi, A. (2022). Speech-in-noise perception in musicians and non-musicians: A multi-level meta-analysis. Hear. Res. 416:108442. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2022.108442

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joris, P., and Yin, T. C. (2007). A matter of time: Internal delays in binaural processing. Trends Neurosci. 30, 70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jünemann, K., Engels, A., Marie, D., Worschech, F., Scholz, D. S., Grouiller, F., et al. (2023). Increased functional connectivity in the right dorsal auditory stream after a full year of piano training in healthy older adults. Sci. Rep. 13:19993. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-46513-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E. C., Wagner, A. E., Toffanin, P., and Başkent, D. (2021). Do musicians and non-musicians differ in speech-on-speech processing? Front. Psychol. 12:623787. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.623787

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, L., Xie, Z., Lu, L., Qu, T., Wu, X., Yan, J., et al. (2015). Similar impacts of the interaural delay and interaural correlation on binaural gap detection. PLoS One 10:e0126342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, L., Xie, Z., Lu, L., Wu, X., and Li, L. (2012). Sensitivity to a break in interaural correlation is co-modulated by intensity level and interaural delay. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132, EL114–EL118. doi: 10.1121/1.4734241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P. V., Rana, B., and Krishna, R. (2014). Temporal processing in musicians and non-musicians. J. Hear. Sci. 4, 35–42. doi: 10.17430/892228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P., Sanju, H. K., and Nikhil, J. (2016). Temporal resolution and active auditory discrimination skill in vocal musicians. Int. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 20, 310–314. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1570312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyrtsoudi, M., Sidiras, C., Papadelis, G., and Iliadou, V. M. (2023). Auditory processing in musicians, a cross-sectional study, as a basis for auditory training optimization. Healthcare 11:2027. doi: 10.3390/healthcare11142027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Han, J. H., and Lee, H. J. (2020). Long-term musical training alters auditory cortical activity to the frequency change. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:329. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49, 467–477. doi: 10.1121/1.1912375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Kong, L., Wu, X., and Li, L. (2013). Primitive auditory memory is correlated with spatial unmasking that is based on direct-reflection integration. PLoS One 8:e63106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Q., Wang, X., Wang, S., Xie, Y., Li, X., Xie, Y., et al. (2018). Musical training induces functional and structural auditory-motor network plasticity in young adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 2098–2110. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23989

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Zatorre, R. J., and Du, Y. (2021). The microstructural plasticity of the arcuate fasciculus undergirds improved speech in noise perception in musicians. Cereb. Cortex 31, 3975–3985. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Licklider, J. C. R. (1948). The influence of interaural phase relations upon the masking of speech by white noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 20, 150–159. doi: 10.1121/1.1906358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madsen, S. M. K., Marschall, M., Dau, T., and Oxenham, A. J. (2019). Speech perception is similar for musicians and non-musicians across a wide range of conditions. Sci. Rep. 9:10404. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46728-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meha-Bettison, K., Sharma, M., Ibrahim, R. K., and Mandikal Vasuki, P. R. (2018). Enhanced speech perception in noise and cortical auditory evoked potentials in professional musicians. Int. J. Audiol. 57, 40–52. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2017.1380850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. K., and Guan, Y. L. (2001). Cytoarchitectural and axonal maturation in human auditory cortex. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2, 297–311. doi: 10.1007/s101620010052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. K., Perazzo, L. M., and Braun, A. (1995). Time course of axonal myelination in the human brainstem auditory pathway. Hear. Res. 87, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(95)00073-d

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mossop, J. E., and Culling, J. F. (1998). Lateralization of large interaural delays. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104, 1574–1579. doi: 10.1121/1.424369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Musacchia, G., Sams, M., Skoe, E., and Kraus, N. (2007). Musicians have enhanced subcortical auditory and audiovisual processing of speech and music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 15894–15898. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701498104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisha, K. V., Durai, R., and Konadath, S. (2022). Musical training and its association with age-related changes in binaural, temporal, and spatial Processing. Am. J. Audiol. 31, 669–683. doi: 10.1044/2022_AJA-21-00227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisha, K. V., Parmar, A., Shivaiah, C., and Prabhu, P. (2023). Differential advantages of musical backgrounds on binaural integration and interaction skills in instrumentalists, vocalists, and non-musicians. J. Otol. 18, 185–192. doi: 10.1016/j.joto.2023.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantev, C., Oostenveld, R., Engelien, A., Ross, B., Roberts, L. E., and Hoke, M. (1998). Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392, 811–814. doi: 10.1038/33918

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parbery-Clark, A., Anderson, S., Hittner, E., and Kraus, N. (2012). Musical experience offsets age-related delays in neural timing. Neurobiol. Aging 33:1483.e1–1483.e14834. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., and Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear Hear. 30, 653–661. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Penhune, V., Watanabe, D., and Savion-Lemieux, T. (2005). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: Evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 265–268. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.049

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Penhune, V. B. (2011). Sensitive periods in human development: evidence from musical training. Cortex 47, 1126–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirouzmand, Y., Nazeri, A., Jalilvand, K. L., Akbarzadeh, B. A., and Majidpour, A. (2022). Effect of musical training on temporal resolution and temporal fine structure processing. Audit. Vestib. Res. 31, 69–73. doi: 10.18502/avr.v31i1.8137

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puschmann, S., Baillet, S., and Zatorre, R. J. (2019). Musicians at the cocktail party: Neural substrates of musical training during selective listening in multispeaker situations. Cereb. Cortex 29, 3253–3265. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puschmann, S., Regev, M., Baillet, S., and Zatorre, R. J. (2021). MEG intersubject phase locking of stimulus-driven activity during naturalistic speech listening correlates with musical training. J. Neurosci. 41, 2713–2722. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0932-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rammsayer, T., and Altenmüller, E. (2006). Temporal Information Processing in Musicians and Nonmusicians. Music Percept. 24, 37–48. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.37

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryn Junior, F. V., Lüders, D., Casali, R. L., and Amaral, M. I. R. D. (2022). Temporal auditory processing in people exposed to musical instrument practice. Codas 34:e20210256. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20212021256pt

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., and Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science 267, 699–701. doi: 10.1126/science.7839149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, A., and Campbell, J. (2011). A sensitive period for cochlear implantation in deaf children. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 24, 151–153. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2011.607614

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skoe, E., and Kraus, N. (2013). Musical training heightens auditory brainstem function during sensitive periods in development. Front. Psychol. 4:622. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00622

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, M. F., and Watson, C. S. (1984). Performance on frequency-discrimination tasks by musicians and nonmusicians. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 76, 1690–1695.

Google Scholar

Steele, C. J., Bailey, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). Early musical training and white-matter plasticity in the corpus callosum: Evidence for a sensitive period. J. Neurosci. 33, 1282–1290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3578-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, R. M., and Trahiotis, C. (1995). “Models of binaural interaction,” in Hearing (handbook of perception and cognition, ed. B. C. J. Moore (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 347–386.

Google Scholar

Swaminathan, J., Mason, C. R., Streeter, T. M., Best, V., Kidd, G. Jr., and Patel, A. D. (2015). Musical training, individual differences and the cocktail party problem. Sci. Rep. 5:11628. doi: 10.1038/srep11628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, A. T., Krizman, J., and Kraus, N. (2015). Music training alters the course of adolescent auditory development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 10062–10067. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1505114112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Toh, X. R., Tan, S. H., Wong, G., Lau, F., and Wong, F. C. K. (2023). Enduring musician advantage among former musicians in prosodic pitch perception. Sci. Rep. 13:2657. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-29733-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Vugt, F. T., Hartmann, K., Altenmüller, E., Mohammadi, B., and Margulies, D. S. (2021). The impact of early musical training on striatal functional connectivity. Neuroimage 238:118251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripollés, P., Rojo, N., Sierpowska, J., François, C., et al. (2016). Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of musical training onset. Neuroimage 126, 106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: Evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Exp. Brain Res. 176, 332–340. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0619-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. C., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., and Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 420–422. doi: 10.1038/nn1872

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, B. A., and Dai, H. (2024). Musicians have an advantage on a spatial-hearing task only when they are highly trained, start training early, and continue to play. Hear. Res. 451:109078. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2024.109078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zendel, B. R., and Alain, C. (2012). Musicians experience less age-related decline in central auditory processing. Psychol. Aging 27, 410–417. doi: 10.1037/a0024816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Fu, X., Luo, D., Xing, L., and Du, Y. (2021). Musical experience offsets age-related decline in understanding speech-in-noise: Type of training does not matter, working memory is the key. Ear Hear. 42, 258–270. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000921

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: musical training, auditory, binaural, interaural correlation, sensitive period

Citation: Wang M, Hu S, Liu J, Ai M and Kong L (2025) Relationship between early musical training and detection of binaural gap based on interaural correlation change. Front. Neurosci. 19:1580045. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1580045

Received: 13 March 2025; Accepted: 16 May 2025;
Published: 17 June 2025.

Edited by:

Victoria M. Bajo Lorenzana, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Seung-Goo Kim, Max Planck Society, Germany
Jiping Zhang, East China Normal University, China

Copyright © 2025 Wang, Hu, Liu, Ai and Kong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lingzhi Kong, a29uZ2xpbmd6aGlAYmxjdS5lZHUuY24=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.