- 1The Second Affiliated Hospital of Xinjiang Medical University, Urumqi, China
- 2Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders & National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
- 3Advanced Innovation Center for Human Brain Protection, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
- 4School of Biomedical Sciences and Gerald Choa Neuroscience Institute, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, China
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a childhood-onset neurobiological disorder that often persists into adulthood. Adult ADHD is an important public health concern due to its great social damage and challenges in clinical recognition, resulting in a significant disease burden. Nonetheless, the diagnosis of adult ADHD remains challenging due to the absence of specific symptoms and biological markers. The aims of this systematic review were as follows: (1) To discern whether there were any differences in resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) and event related potential (ERP) between adult ADHD and healthy controls (HCs). (2) To ascertain whether ERP specific manifestations associated with executive function (EF) deficiencies. (3) To conduct an exploration into the mechanisms of specific electrophysiologic alterations. This review was conducted in PubMed-Medline and Web-of-Science from 1971 to August 15th, 2024 to summarize the EEG changes of adult ADHD. We focused on resting-state EEG to report spectral power across different frequency bands and ERPs under different experimental tasks, 68 studies were finally included. When studying the characteristics of resting-state EEG in adult ADHD patients, we observed that theta power exhibits a consistent upward trend. Congruous reduction Pe, P3, and N2 amplitudes during response inhibition tasks, with a further decrease in P3 and N2 amplitudes in sustained attention tasks. These EEG changes may stem from impairments in error detection, cognitive control, and attention allocation, meaning that core EFs are affected in adults with ADHD. Overall, consistent changes in resting-state EEG and ERPs could provide insight for the identification of ADHD in adults.
1 Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a prevalent childhood-onset neurobiological disorder, nearly 60% of patients diagnosed in childhood continue to exhibit symptoms in adulthood (Anbarasan et al., 2020). The global prevalence of persistent adult ADHD was found to be 2.58%, while the prevalence of symptomatic adult ADHD was 6.76%. This equates to 139.84 million and 366.33 million affected adults respectively (Song et al., 2021). ADHD is associated with an increased risk of other psychiatric disorders, educational and occupational failure, accidents, criminal behavior, and additions (Amiri et al., 2020). Moreover, ADHD shows significant correlations with a wide range of comorbid psychiatric disorders, including depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, substance misuse, placing a considerable burden on patients, family and society (Austerman, 2015; Thapar and Cooper, 2016; Pagán et al., 2023).
The clinical presentation of ADHD tends to evolve and diminish across the developmental course (Adamou et al., 2020). Adult ADHD is relatively neglected in epidemiological studies compared with childhood ADHD (Sibley et al., 2016). Diagnosis can be challenging as symptoms are non-specific, researchers are attempting to find objective biological markers to help identify adult ADHD. Symptoms of adult ADHD include insufficient inhibitory control, defects in working memory, impaired socioemotional processing, and challenges in completing tasks that require sustained self-regulation of attention (Nijmeijer et al., 2008). Based on these observations, scholars proposed that executive function (EF) deficits which closely related to the frontal lobes may be the core features of ADHD (Nigg and Casey, 2005; Willcutt et al., 2005; Roth and Saykin, 2004; Barkley, 1997). EF is an umbrella term encompassing a range of higher cognitive processes that are both distinct and interrelated (Friedman and Miyake, 2017; Karr et al., 2018). There is currently no consensus on the classification of EFs. Some scholars believed four core EFs are conflict monitoring, response inhibition, set- shifting, and working memory updating (Miyake et al., 2000; Smit et al., 2023). There were still other researchers who divided EFs into cool and hot. According to them, cool-EFs impairment mainly manifests in response inhibition, working memory and cognitive flexibility, while hot-EFs impairment usually involves delayed gratification, reward/punishment-related decision-making, self-regulation, and emotion regulation (Willoughby et al., 2011).
Assessment of ADHD has been largely relayed on subjective reports from patients and clinical observations. Whether EEG could be utilized in clinical practice as a diagnostic aid to assist diagnosis or not, it would provide a potentially non-invasive and economical method with which to objectify the assessment process. There are a considerable number of studies for the use of electroencephalogram (EEG) in adult ADHD. In 2014 one published review (Lenartowicz and Loo, 2014) explored the use of EEG in diagnosing adult ADHD, concluding that EEG was not an appropriate diagnostic tool but has a potentially promising future. In 2020, based on the accession of 5 years of literature, it was concluded that EEG activities are potentially unique to adult ADHD, strongest support was derived for elevated theta and alpha activity (Adamou et al., 2020). In 2022, a systematic review suggested that resting-state and event-related modulation of alpha, beta and theta power, as well as the N2 and P3 components have potential for EEG measures, which can provide meaningful insights into the heterogeneity of ADHD (Slater et al., 2022).
Based on the background above, this systematic review focused on adult ADHD, innovatively categorized ERP studies according to the EFs domains, which adding conceptual clarity and enhanced clinical relevance. We included both resting-state EEG and event related potential (ERP) studies, providing a broad and integrated perspective on electrophysiological correlates of EF deficits. Objectives of this systematic review are as followed: (1) To discern whether there were any differences in resting state EEG and ERP between adult ADHD and healthy population. (2) To ascertain whether ERP specific manifestations associated with EF deficiencies. (3) To conduct an initial exploration into the mechanisms of specific electrophysiologic alterations.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Search strategy and information sources
We conducted a comprehensive search of English-language literature from 1971 to August 15th, 2024, in publicly available datasets PubMed and Web of Science, there was no limitation on publication date in search strategy. The current review was performed in compliance with the Preferred Reported Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta Analyses (PRISMA) 2021 guidelines (Page et al., 2021) (PRISMA checklists see Supplementary Tables 6, 7). The following combinations of keywords were used in title and abstract: (”ADHD” OR “Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity”) AND (“EEG” OR “Electroencephalography” OR “Electroencephalogram” OR “Evoked Potential” OR “Event-Related Potential” OR “Wave”) AND (“Adult” OR “adult*” OR “Young adult”) AND (“Prospective studies” OR “Retrospective Studies” OR “Follow-up studies” OR “Cohort studies” OR prospective* OR retrospective* OR longitudinal* OR followup OR cohort*). In addition, hand searched of published systematic reviews, and references of the selected articles were undertaken.
2.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria
The objectives and the inclusion criteria of this study were structured based on the elements of the PICOS model (Population of interest, Interventions, Comparators, Outcomes, and Study design), The specific items of inclusion and exclusion criteria have been listed in Table 1.
2.3 Data extraction
Data extractions were conducted in duplicate by two independent reviewers, and discrepancies were resolved through discussion and consultation with a third reviewer. We roughly classify ERPs into five categories according to EFs, which are: Response Inhibition, Working Memory, Self-regulation of affects, Sustained Attention and others. From each included article was extracted and entered into tables, the extracted data included the following information: (1) authors and year of publication, (2) demographic characteristics (sample size, sex, age), (3) recording condition (eyes closed or eyes open), measures of frequency bands and range, spectral power type utilized (for resting state EEG), (4) Experimental task (for ERPs), (5) main findings. The selected articles and their data have been shown in the data extraction (Tables 2–9). In addition, other information such as the country, IQ, Co-morbidities condition and medication and rules of reduction/interruption are recorded in Supplementary Tables 1, 2, whereas the study outcomes were discussed in the results section. The analysis of the results has been generally explained in the discussion part.
2.4 Study quality assessment
Two raters independently assessed study quality using the modified Newcastle Ottawa Scale (NOS). The detailed criteria of modified NOS in Supplementary Table 9. The case–control studies subscale was used for assessing the risk of bias. NOS provides three domains: (1) selection, (2) comparability and (3) exposure. The highest score is 9. A score from 9 to 7 indicates high quality, from 6 to 4 moderate quality, and from 3 to 0 low quality.
3 Results
3.1 Literature search and assessment of risk of bias
A total of 443 articles were initially identified from Pub-Med and Web-of-Science databases using our search terms (Figure 1), 85 duplicate articles were removed. After reading titles and abstracts, 281 articles were excluded. Upon further reading the full text, 9 articles were excluded, including 2 studies that ADHD participants under 18 years old. Adult ADHD in two articles have comorbidities. Five studies did not utilize the measurements we were intended to include. Ultimately, 68 articles were included, and the mean score of quality assessment of the 68 studies was 5.9, indicating a moderate quality (quality assessment of included studies see Supplementary Table 8).

Figure 1. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram of study selection.
3.2 General character of the study
3.2.1 Resting state spectral power
In the resting state (Table 2; Figure 2), the most investigated frequency band was theta (17 studies), 10 of them had consistently elevated results (the specific changes of frequencies during resting state see Figure 3). Three articles manifested that theta power increased across the whole brain (Dupuy et al., 2021; Clarke et al., 2019; Li et al., 2019). One included study indicated that theta power increased in Centro-parietal area (Kiiski et al., 2020b), another concluded that relative theta power increased in frontal region (Skirrow et al., 2015).
The least studied frequency band was gamma, with only 4 studies involved, among which 3 showed no significant difference between adult ADHD and HC, while one study suggested a decrease in gamma band power in right centroparietal region (Tombor et al., 2019).
The measurement results of delta, alpha, and beta frequency bands are not consistent. For delta frequency band of adult ADHD, 4 included studies indicated increases delta power compared to HCs, whereas 2 articles indicated decreased delta activities (Clarke et al., 2008; Clarke et al., 2019). Significantly, Bresnahan and Barry in 2002 indicated that absolute delta power of adult ADHD patients was higher compared to HC, but there was no significant difference in relative delta power (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002).
For alpha band of adult ADHD, 4 studies showed an increase in band power, 3 studies suggested that adult ADHD has lower alpha power than HCs. 8 studies showed no significant difference in alpha power between ADHD and HC in adults. Kiiski and Bennett in 2020 drew conclusions that theta power of adult ADHD was increased in centro-parietal region (Kiiski et al., 2020a). Li in 2019 indicated that the relative alpha power was decreased across the whole brain (Li et al., 2019).
The results of researches in beta waves are most heterogeneous among all frequencies, 7 articles showed an increase in band power, while 5 studies indicated decreased beta activities. Interestingly, one study found that beta power (13–16 Hz) increased in frontal region, and beta activities (16–20 Hz) became higher in parietal region in adult ADHD (Kiiski et al., 2020a). Another study indicated that absolute beta power decreased in midline but increased in right posterior region (Clarke et al., 2008). Yoon in 2024 reached the same outcome: adult ADHD have lower absolute beta power in midline to HC (Yoon et al., 2024).
It is worth noticing that the classification of five main frequency bands have subtle differences, and some articles further divided them into sub-bands (Fig. 3 indicated the frequency bands ranges and changes of each study). Among them, 3 studies indicated sub-component of specific frequency band in patients with ADHD were consistent (Kiiski et al., 2020a; Han et al., 2022; Tombor et al., 2019). One study founded alpha 1 (8–10 Hz) and alpha 2 (10–13 Hz) have inconsistent changes of adult ADHD participants compared to HCs (Poil et al., 2014).
3.2.2 Main experimental tasks and ERPs of EFs
Figures 4, 5 and Supplementary Tables 3, 5 separately show the main experimental paradigms and EFs of included studies (The exact numbers are marked in Figures 4, 5).

Figure 4. Number of included studies by experimental tasks. The chart summarizes the experimental tasks used during EEG recording in the included studies. (Tasks that are performed less than 3 times are not included).
As demonstrated in Figure 4, a variety of experimental paradigms were employed in the assessment of EFs. Of these, the Go/No-Go paradigm was conducted in 15 studies, thus rendering it the most frequently used. In this paradigm, participants are required to respond to frequent “GO” stimuli but withhold responses to rare “NO-GO” stimuli, it has been used to assess response inhibition, sustained attention, and self-regulation of affect. The Oddball task was the second most used, with 9 studies adopting it. In this task, participants need to detect rare target stimuli (“oddballs”) embedded in a sequence of frequent standard stimuli. It particularly involves selective attentional processes, which are defined as the ability to focus on goal-relevant events while ignoring irrelevant information. The Continuous Performance Test (CPT) was utilized in 8 studies. This well-established behavioral task is designed to investigate response inhibition and sustained attention. Participants are required to respond to specific target stimuli while inhibiting responses to non-targets over a prolonged period. The Flanker task was implemented in 5 studies. In this test, participants are required to identify a central target stimulus while ignoring flanking distractors that may be congruent or incongruent with the target, it is another task that involves inhibition function.
In addition, working memory tasks were adopted in 4 studies. In these tasks, participants temporarily store and manipulate visual (e.g., shapes, colors) and spatial (e.g., locations, movements) information (The describe and related EFs of paradigms of ERPs see Supplementary Table 3).
As illustrated in Figure 5, 18 of these researches evaluated response inhibition in adult ADHD patients, among them, 12 articles employed the Go/No-Go experimental paradigm, 5 articles utilized the Flanker task, and 1 study adopted the Stroop and Stop-Signal task. Of the 18 articles that evaluated the sustained attention in participants with ADHD, the oddball task and the CPT task were the most frequently used (8 each). Seven studies reported on the assessment of working memory, two of which utilized the N-back experimental paradigm. Seven studies discussed the self-regulation of affect, two employed the emotional go/no go experimental paradigm. Since some experiments cannot be categorized among the four executive functions that we have divided, we classify them as “other execution function (Table 7).
Figure 6 and Supplementary Table 4 illustrates the ERPs involved in our included studies (the exact numbers are marked in Figure 6), of which the most used is P3 (P300, P3d, P3b, GoP3 etc.) in 20 studies. Among them, six are for response inhibition, 12 are for sustained attention, one is for working memory and one is for self- regulation of affect. The second most used ERP metric is N2 (N2pc, N2d) in 12 studies, of these, five each on response inhibition and sustained attention, one each on working memory and self- regulation of affect. After that, Error Positivity (Pe) was recorded in seven studies, of which six were employed for the purpose of detecting response inhibition and one for the assessment of self-regulation of affect. It is important to note that all Contralateral Delay Activity (CDA) presents in the assessment of WM. Measurements that are recorded less than 3 times were not included.

Figure 6. Number of included studies by EEG measurements. (measurements that are recorded less than 3 times are not included).
4 Discussion
This systematic review sought to explore EEG alterations in ADHD adults across both resting and event-related states. We are the first to categorize the ERP results in ADHD patients based on their EF performance across diverse tasks. Our goal is to identify neuro-electrophysiological markers associated with specific dimensions of EF deficits and to explore potential neuropathological mechanisms behind these changes. The findings indicate that ADHD adults tend to demonstrate a consistent increase in theta band power compared to HCs when in a resting state. Additionally, during response inhibition tasks, the amplitudes of ERP components such as Pe, P3, and N2 were consistently lower in ADHD patients. During sustained attention tasks, there were more pronounced reductions of P3 and N2 amplitudes in ADHD patients than in controls.
4.1 Increased theta oscillation in the resting state
The outcomes of literatures included in this review indicate that ADHD adults exhibit heightened theta band power relative to HCs. Although the specific mechanism remains unclear, two hypotheses—the maturational lag hypothesis and the cortical hypo-arousal hypothesis—may provide insight into this manifestation (Ji et al., 2022; Byeon et al., 2020). According to the maturational lag hypothesis, slow-wave activity typically diminishes with age, whereas fast-wave activity increases and is eventually predominant. In children with ADHD, however, a delay in brain development has been observed, characterized by increased slow-wave activity (delta and theta waves) and decreased fast-wave activity (alpha and beta waves) (Clarke et al., 2019; Mann et al., 1992). Previous studies have found that approximately 65% of ADHD children continue to exhibit symptoms in adulthood (Lara et al., 2009). It is crucial to determine whether children with ADHD maintain delayed brain development into adulthood or if these delays represent a persistent dysfunction that extends from childhood to adulthood. An 11-year longitudinal study conducted by Clarke found that children ADHD patients exhibited increased relative theta wave power and decreased alpha wave power at the whole brain level compared to normal children. In adulthood, elevated theta waves persist in ADHD patients compared to controls, although the degree of EEG abnormalities is less pronounced than in childhood. These findings suggest that adult ADHD patients still experience a lag in brain development, supporting the maturational lag hypothesis (Clarke et al., 2019).
Some scholars have interpreted the increased slow-wave activities like delta and theta bands as indicative of a cortical low arousal state (Satterfield and Cantwell, 1974; Rowe et al., 2005; Satterfield and Dawson, 1971). The neurochemical mechanisms underlying ADHD are thought to involve a complex set of imbalances in different neurotransmitters and neural networks, in particular the “overactivity” in inhibitory interneurons within the neocortex and the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN), which may contribute to low levels arousal. The increase in interneuron activity associated with slow-wave activity can be further explained by the activation of cholinergic and/or norepinephrine (NA) metabolic receptors with inhibitory effects (Rowe et al., 2005).
Clinical studies have shown that impulsivity and inattention symptoms are still prevalent in adults with ADHD, although hyperactivity symptoms tend to diminish with age (Biederman et al., 2000). According to a study investigated EEG changes of ADHD in patients of different age groups (children, adolescents, and adults), researchers found that beta activity decreased while theta activity increased with age, aligning with clinical observations. Based on these findings, Bresnahan et al. proposed that beta activity may be associated with hyperactivity, whereas theta activity may be linked to impulsivity (Bresnahan et al., 1999). The low arousal model suggests that the increased theta power in adults with ADHD reflects a low arousal state of the central nervous system. ADHD compensatory symptoms, such as susceptibility to external distractions, difficulty in concentrating, and manifestation of hyperactivity, can be a way of trying to stimulate the nervous system.
In the included articles, 10 out of 17 studies reported an increase in theta band power, while 7 researches indicated no significant change in this frequency band of adult ADHD patients when compared to HCs. We noticed that the participants’ age in unchanged studies are higher than elevated theta studies. For the ages of participants in the included literature, ADHD patients was greater than 18 years, and there was no upper limit. Herrmann and his colleges divided the adult ADHD into two subgroup, youngster subgroup and elderly subgroup, the mean age of youngster group is 25 and for elderly is 40. EEG outcomes changes in youngster group but not in elderly samples (Herrmann et al., 2010). This result may indicate some ADHD related deficits vanish with age. Furthermore, according to the maturational lag hypothesis and the cortical hypo-arousal hypothesis, ADHD patients’ brain undergoes compensatory changes that may result in symptom relief and theta waves normalized.
In addition, the classification of ADHD exerts certain influences on EEG results. This disease typically categorized into inattentive, predominantly hyperactive/impulsive, and combined subtypes based on clinical manifestations. In a systematic review published recently, researcher indicated that resting state and task-related modulation of EEG and ERPs are different among ADHD subtypes (Slater et al., 2022). Buyck and Wiersema indicated fast wave activity decrease in inattentive subtype, but not in Combined subtype (Buyck and Wiersema, 2015).
Furthermore, gender, intelligence quotient (IQ), and recording context may further influence the results of studies. Several studies have found that male adults with ADHD are more likely to exhibit elevated theta band power (Clarke et al., 2008; Skirrow et al., 2015). Comparisons between high-IQ (IQ ≥ 120) adult ADHD patients and HCs have also shown that theta relative power is elevated globally (Li et al., 2019). In 2015, Kitsune et al. tried to investigate the impact of recording context difference on the results, recording the resting state before (Time-1) and after (Time-2) a 1.5-h cognitive task, they found that theta power only increased at Time-1 (Kitsune et al., 2015).
4.2 Decreased Pe, P3, and N2 in response inhibition
In the included studies of this systematic review, adult ADHD patients consistently exhibited a decrease in the amplitude of Pe, P3, and N2 components compared to HCs during tasks related to inhibition function. This suggests that adult ADHD patients may have deficiencies in inhibitory control. Pe typically reaches peak at centro-parietal sites around 200–450 ms after the occurrence of the erroneous response, which is thought to be an ERP that associated with erroneous responses, signifying conscious recognition of error (Falkenstein et al., 1991). The abnormal decrease in Pe may be due to reduced activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (O’Connell et al., 2009). ACC is a critical area for effective error handling (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004), which plays a key role in complex cognitive processes (object detection, response selection, error supervision, and reward-based decision-making) (Bush et al., 2002). Numerous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have found that ACC dysfunction exist in ADHD patients (Rubia et al., 1999; Schulz et al., 2004). Additionally, studies have shown that functional connectivity between ACC and other brain regions reduced in ADHD patients (Castellanos et al., 2008).
The P3 typically peaks at 300–600 ms after stimulation (Cycowicz and Friedman, 2004; Debener et al., 2005), and the component is thought to be associated with attention resources allocation for task implementation (Marquardt et al., 2018). Previous studies have shown that adult ADHD patients not only exhibit a decrease in P3 amplitude but also demonstrate a higher rate of response errors and increased reaction time variability during inhibition-related tasks. The amplitude of the P3 component is negatively correlated with clinical symptom levels (Marquardt et al., 2018). Furthermore, higher IQ levels are associated with fewer missed errors, shorter reaction times, and larger No-Go P3 amplitudes (Münger et al., 2021), suggesting that high IQ ADHD patients may have more attention resources. The decrease in P3 amplitude observed in ADHD patients during inhibition-related tasks may indicate that fewer attentional resources are allocated to inhibitory control and related assessment processes (Woltering et al., 2013).
N2 is a negative potential located in the frontal-central region, typically measured at 200–400 ms, and is thought to be associated with conflict monitoring, response inhibition and selection (Wiersema and Roeyers, 2009). The neural sources of anterior N2 are primarily believed to originate from ACC, an area closely associated with conflict monitoring and attention control (Bekker et al., 2005). Previous studies have demonstrated that N2 amplitude in adult ADHD patients correlates with ADHD symptoms (reduced amplitudes associated with more serious symptoms) (Woltering et al., 2013). This suggests that lower N2 amplitude may be linked to poorer inhibitory control and self-regulation in ADHD patients. These findings indicate that reduced N2 amplitude in adult ADHD may reflect underlying deficits in inhibitory control and self-regulation, which are critical for managing attention and behaviors (Wiersema and Roeyers, 2009).
4.3 Decreased P3, N2 in sustained attention
Adult ADHD patients consistently show reductions in the amplitude of P3 and N2 components compared to HCs when completing sustained attention related tasks. The decreased amplitude of P3 component may reflect deficits in attention, stimulus processing, and evaluation abilities, or an inappropriate attentional resources allocation (Barry et al., 2009; Itagaki et al., 2011; Jonkman et al., 2004). These alterations may be due to impaired connectivity between cognitive control network and default mode network in ADHD (Castellanos et al., 2008). However, Barry et al. have not observed changes in P3 amplitude among adult ADHD patients during the oddball task, they proposed that this might result from patients’ high concentration during tasks, which may partially compensate for ADHD information processing deficits (Barry et al., 2009). N2 components, particularly posterior scalp N2, were thought to be involved in conflict monitoring (Münger et al., 2022). In situations requiring multitasking or suppression of distracting information, a decrease in N2 amplitude may indicate that patients are less effective at resolving conflicts or inhibiting distractions. According to literatures included in this systematic review, adult ADHD patients consistently show reductions in N2 amplitude when completing sustained attention tasks (Barry et al., 2009; Münger et al., 2021; Kaur et al., 2019), suggesting a potential disorder in conflict monitoring. This impairment may make it difficult for adults with ADHD to sustain attention over prolonged periods.
4.4 Inconsistent results
However, current studies on the resting-state alpha and beta oscillations in adult ADHD have not reached a consistent conclusion. Some investigators have found that alpha power is higher in adult ADHD patients compared to HCs (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Poil et al., 2014; Kiiski et al., 2020b; Koehler et al., 2009). According to these researchers, increases or decreases in alpha power reflect states of cortical inhibition or excitability, respectively (Haegens et al., 2011). Elevated alpha power may be associated with a decreased ability to process stimuli, leading to inhibition of attention or increased distractibility (Mathewson et al., 2009). Conversely, other researchers have found that alpha-band power is reduced in adult ADHD patients compared to controls (Woltering et al., 2012; Loo et al., 2009; Li et al., 2019), these researchers suggest that decrease in alpha power may indicate a reduction in cortical inhibition, which can lead to increased subcortical signaling. This may conversely bring about behavioral manifestations such as excitability, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Sauseng et al., 2013).
In addition, some studies have found that beta band power in adults with ADHD is higher than that in HCs. Researchers hypothesize that this increase in beta band activity may be associated with heightened cerebral cortex activity and impaired emotion control (Clarke et al., 2001, 2011). Specifically, in children with ADHD, beta hyperactivity is more likely to associated with mood fluctuation and aggressive behaviors. Furthermore, Li and colleagues observed an increase in the relative power of beta frequency band in adult ADHD patients at rest and noted that this change may be strongly associated with emotion regulation disorders, particularly emotional instability and rapidly changing emotional states (Li et al., 2019). Jaworska et al. recorded resting state EEG data from 14 adult patients with ADHD, 14 anger control disorder and 14 HCs, the results indicated that the ADHD patients exhibited higher beta wave activities. Researchers believe this may reflect a chronic state of hypervigilance associated with the development of anger emotions (Jaworska et al., 2013). However, some investigators have reported decreased beta power in adult ADHD patients compared to HCs (Dupuy et al., 2021; Yoon et al., 2024). This finding is consistent with the trend of decreasing ADHD symptoms with age, suggesting that beta activity may be associated with the severity of ADHD symptoms (Bresnahan et al., 1999). In summary, the results regarding beta power changes are not entirely consistent, which may be related to various clinical subtypes of ADHD and evolution of clinical symptoms with age growing.
4.5 Limitation
Based on a comprehensive analysis of the included literatures, we observed that adult patients with ADHD demonstrated consistent EEG characteristics and changes in ERP components in the context of specific EFs compared to HCs. However, there were significant inconsistencies results across studies. These differences may be attributed to various confounding factors, Tables 8, 9 and Supplementary Tables 1, 2 summarize the general characteristics, experimental paradigms of studies included in this review.
Firstly, in term of general demographic characters, several studies have reported divergent EEG changes due to differences in gender, age, IQ. and subtype. Other than that, ADHD patients have high frequency of co-morbidities, this condition is unavoidable. Some studies have recorded combination with other psychiatric disorders (see Supplementary Tables 1, 2), but the nomenclature and reference criteria are divergent thus cannot be unified. Moreover, methylphenidate (MPH) as a central nervous system stimulant is commonly prescribed as a first-line medication for ADHD. Tombor and KKuaszi reported MPH treatment was associated with increased gamma activity compared to MPH naïve (Tombor et al., 2019). Bresnahan found there was reduction in slow wave activity in ADHD patients receiving stimulant medication compared to untreated group (Bresnahan et al., 2006). A meta-analysis demonstrated MPH tend to increase P300 amplitude in individuals with ADHD, contributing to normalize brain activity (Barroso et al., 2025). Medication use for ADHD and durations of medication refrainment were recorded in Supplementary Tables 1, 2. The findings demonstrated inconsistency results to whether medication influenced EEG outcomes. Therefore, future researchers should focus on standardizing diagnosis and screened criteria and controlling for confounding factors, and conducting long-term longitudinal studies to track disease progression.
Secondly, the subgroup of particular component of ERPs and its correlation with cognitive domains has been inconsistent across studies (see Supplementary Table 4). Thirdly, in terms of experimental design, many experimental paradigms involve multiple EFs assessment (see Supplementary Table 3 and Supplementary Figures 1, 2), it is difficult to define exactly which tasks are specifically related to working memory, response inhibition, sustained attention, or self-regulation of affect. It is recommended that future endeavors focus on refining the meanings and roles of ERPs components, with the object of correspondence with explicit cognitive processes.
5 Conclusion and future direction
In summary, resting-state EEG of adult ADHD patients is characterized by an increase in theta band power, which may be consistent with the cortical low arousal model. During functional tasks, especially the response inhibition tasks, ADHD patients tend to exhibit a significant reduction in the amplitude of ERP components such as Pe, N2, and P3. These reductions may reflect deficiencies in error detection and control functions. Additionally, reduction in P3 and N2 components during sustained attention tasks suggests that ADHD patients may have difficulties in effectively allocating attentional resources. However, these findings should be interpreted with caution due to the presence of numerous confounding factors in the studies. Of note, this systematic review did not perform a meta- analysis, primarily because of heterogeneous protocols, inconsistent reporting and clinical diversity. To advance the field, future research should standardize methodologies, comprehensive clinical documentation and control confounders. Through these efforts, the field can establish robust electrophysiological biomarkers for ADHD, ultimately improving diagnostic precision and effective interventions.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.
Author contributions
ZS: Formal Analysis, Conceptualization, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YW: Validation, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. BW: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. CH: Validation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. HZ: Methodology, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Validation. YG: Visualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. YC: Writing – original draft, Visualization. XZ: Project administration, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft. YS: Validation, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82201701), the Beijing Municipal Hospital Research and Development Project (PX2021068), Capital’s Funds for Health Improvement and Research (CFH2024-2-1174), and the Sci-Tech Innovation 2030 -Major Project of Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Intelligence Technology (2021ZD0200600).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2025.1617307/full#supplementary-material
References
Adamou, M., Fullen, T., and Jones, S. L. (2020). ‘EEG for diagnosis of adult ADHD: A systematic review with narrative analysis’. Front. Psychiatry 11:871. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00871
Amiri, S., Esmaeili, E., Salehpour, F., Mirzaei, F., Barzegar, H., Mohammad Namdar, A., et al. (2020). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in patients with and without head trauma. Open Access Emerg. Med. 12, 405–410. doi: 10.2147/OAEM.S265883
Anbarasan, D., Kitchin, M., and Adler, L. A. (2020). Screening for adult ADHD. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 22:72. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-01194-9
Austerman, J. (2015). ADHD and behavioral disorders: Assessment, management, and an update from DSM-5. Cleve Clin. J. Med. 82(11 Suppl. 1), S2–S7. doi: 10.3949/ccjm.82.s1.01
Balogh, L., Kakuszi, B., Papp, S., Tombor, L., Bitter, I., and Czobor, P. (2017). Neural correlates of error monitoring in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder after failed inhibition in an emotional Go/No-Go task. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 29, 326–333. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16100183
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65
Barroso, B. C. R., Mendonça, F., Mazer, P., Prata, C., and Pinto, J. O. (2025). Methylphenidate and P300 in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 211:112553. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2025.112553
Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., Brown, C. R., and Heaven, P. C. (2009). Event-related potentials in adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: An investigation using an inter-modal auditory/visual oddball task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 71, 124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.09.009
Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., and Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 221–231. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011
Biederman, J., Mick, E., and Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 816–818. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816
Bozhilova, N., Kuntsi, J., Rubia, K., Asherson, P., and Michelini, G. (2022). Event-related brain dynamics during mind wandering in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An experience-sampling approach. Neuroimage Clin. 35:103068. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103068
Bresnahan, S. M., Anderson, J. W., and Barry, R. J. (1999). Age-related changes in quantitative EEG in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 1690–1697. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00042-6
Bresnahan, S. M., and Barry, R. J. (2002). Specificity of quantitative EEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 112, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00190-7
Bresnahan, S. M., Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., and Johnstone, S. J. (2006). Quantitative EEG analysis in dexamphetamine-responsive adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 141, 151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.002
Bush, G., Vogt, B. A., Holmes, J., Dale, A. M., Greve, D., Jenike, M. A., et al. (2002). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 99, 523–528. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012470999
Buyck, I., and Wiersema, J. R. (2014). Resting electroencephalogram in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Developmental course and diagnostic value. Psychiatry Res. 216, 391–397. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.055
Buyck, I., and Wiersema, J. R. (2015). Task-related electroencephalographic deviances in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 29, 433–444. doi: 10.1037/neu0000148
Byeon, J., Choi, T. Y., Won, G. H., Lee, J., and Kim, J. W. (2020). A novel quantitative electroencephalography subtype with high alpha power in ADHD: ADHD or misdiagnosed ADHD? PLoS One 15:e0242566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242566
Castellanos, F. X., Margulies, D. S., Kelly, C., Uddin, L. Q., Ghaffari, M., Kirsch, A., et al. (2008). Cingulate-precuneus interactions: A new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 332–337. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.025
Cheung, C. H. M., McLoughlin, G., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., and Kuntsi, J. (2017). Neurophysiological correlates of attentional fluctuation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Topogr. 30, 320–332. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0554-2
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Dupuy, F. E., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., and Heaven, P. C. (2011). Childhood EEG as a predictor of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 73–80.
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Heaven, P. C., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., and Byrne, M. K. (2008). EEG in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 70, 176–183. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.001
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Johnstone, S. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikowitz, M. (2019). EEG development in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: From child to adult. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 1256–1262. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.05.001
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikowitz, M. (2001). EEG-defined subtypes of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2098–105.
Cowley, B. U., Juurmaa, K., and Palomäki, J. (2022). Reduced power in fronto-parietal theta EEG linked to impaired attention-sampling in adult ADHD. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0028-21.2021. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0028-21.2021
Cycowicz, Y. M., and Friedman, D. (2004). The old switcheroo: When target environmental sounds elicit a novelty P3. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1359–1367. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.008
Czobor, P., Kakuszi, B., Németh, K., Balogh, L., Papp, S., Tombor, L., et al. (2017). Electrophysiological indices of aberrant error-processing in adults with ADHD: A new region of interest. Brain Imag. Behav. 11, 1616–1628. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9610-x
Debener, S., Makeig, S., Delorme, A., and Engel, A. K. (2005). What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 309–321. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.006
Dhar, M., Been, P. H., Minderaa, R. B., and Althaus, M. (2010). Information processing differences and similarities in adults with dyslexia and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during a continuous performance test: A study of cortical potentials. Neuropsychologia 48, 3045–3056. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.014
Doehnert, M., Brandeis, D., Schneider, G., Drechsler, R., and Steinhausen, H. C. (2013). A neurophysiological marker of impaired preparation in an 11-year follow-up study of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 260–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02572.x
Dubreuil-Vall, L., Ruffini, G., and Camprodon, J. A. (2020). Deep learning convolutional neural networks discriminate adult ADHD from healthy individuals on the basis of event-related spectral EEG. Front. Neurosci. 14:251. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00251
Dupuy, F. E., Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikowitz, M. (2021). DSM-5 adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Sex differences in EEG activity. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 46, 377–388. doi: 10.1007/s10484-021-09522-2
Ehlis, A. C. E., Deppermann, S. D., and Fallgatter, A. J. F. (2018). Performance monitoring and post-error adjustments in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An EEG analysis. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 43, 396–406. doi: 10.1503/jpn.170118
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., and Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78, 447–455. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9
Freichel, R., Zink, N., Chang, F. Y., Vera, J. D., Truong, H., Michelini, G., et al. (2024). Alpha event-related decreases during encoding in adults with ADHD - An investigation of sustained attention and working memory processes. Behav. Brain Res. 469:115003. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2024.115003
Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 86, 186–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
Gu, C., Liu, Z. X., Tannock, R., and Woltering, S. (2018). Neural processing of working memory in adults with ADHD in a visuospatial change detection task with distractors. PeerJ 6:e5601. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5601
Gumenyuk, V., Korzyukov, O., Tapaskar, N., Wagner, M., Larson, C. R., and Hammer, M. J. (2023). Deficiency in re-orienting of attention in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 54, 141–150. doi: 10.1177/15500594221115737
Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R., and Jensen, O. (2011). α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 19377–19382. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117190108
Han, C., Wang, T., Wu, Y., Li, H., Wang, E., Zhao, X., et al. (2022). Compensatory mechanism of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder recovery in resting state alpha rhythms. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 16:883065. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2022.883065
Hasler, R., Perroud, N., Meziane, H. B., Herrmann, F., Prada, P., Giannakopoulos, P., et al. (2016). Attention-related EEG markers in adult ADHD. Neuropsychologia 87, 120–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.008
Herrmann, M. J., Mader, K., Schreppel, T., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., et al. (2010). Neural correlates of performance monitoring in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). World J. Biol. Psychiatry 11(2 Pt 2), 457–464. doi: 10.1080/15622970902977552
Herrmann, M. J., Schreppel, T., Biehl, S. C., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., et al. (2009). Emotional deficits in adult ADHD patients: An ERP study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 340–345. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp033
Ibáñez, A., Petroni, A., Urquina, H., Torrente, F., Torralva, T., Hurtado, E., et al. (2011). Cortical deficits of emotional face processing in adults with ADHD: Its relation to social cognition and executive function. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 464–481. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.620769
Itagaki, S., Yabe, H., Mori, Y., Ishikawa, H., Takanashi, Y., and Niwa, S. (2011). Event-related potentials in patients with adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 189, 288–291. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.03.005
Jang, K. M., Kim, M. S., and Kim, D. W. (2020). The dynamic properties of a brain network during spatial working memory tasks in college students with ADHD traits. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:580813. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.580813
Jaworska, N., Berrigan, L., Ahmed, A. G., Gray, J., Korovessis, A., Fisher, D. J., et al. (2013). The resting electrophysiological profile in adults with ADHD and comorbid dysfunctional anger: A pilot study. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 44, 95–104. doi: 10.1177/1550059412465607
Ji, Y., Choi, T. Y., Lee, J., Yoon, S., Won, G. H., Jeong, H., et al. (2022). Characteristics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes in children classified using quantitative electroencephalography. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 18, 2725–2736. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S386774
Jonkman, L. M., Kenemans, J. L., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., and van Engeland, H. (2004). Dipole source localization of event-related brain activity indicative of an early visual selective attention deficit in ADHD children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1537–1549. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.022
Karch, S., Segmiller, F., Hantschk, I., Cerovecki, A., Opgen-Rhein, M., Hock, B., et al. (2012). Increased γ oscillations during voluntary selection processes in adult patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 1515–1523. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.07.017
Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G. L., and Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). The unity and diversity of executive functions: A systematic review and re-analysis of latent variable studies. Psychol. Bull. 144, 1147–1185. doi: 10.1037/bul0000160
Kaur, S., Singh, S., Arun, P., Kaur, D., and Bajaj, M. (2019). Event-Related potential analysis of ADHD and control adults during a sustained attention task. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 50, 389–403. doi: 10.1177/1550059419842707
Kiiski, H., Bennett, M., Rueda-Delgado, L. M., Farina, F. R., Knight, R., Boyle, R., et al. (2020a). EEG spectral power, but not theta/beta ratio, is a neuromarker for adult ADHD. Eur. J. Neurosci. 51, 2095–2109. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14645
Kiiski, H., Rueda-Delgado, L. M., Bennett, M., Knight, R., Rai, L., Roddy, D., et al. (2020b). Functional EEG connectivity is a neuromarker for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 330–342. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.08.010
Kitsune, G. L., Cheung, C. H., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., McLoughlin, G., et al. (2015). A matter of time: The influence of recording context on EEG spectral power in adolescents and young adults with ADHD. Brain Topogr. 28, 580–590. doi: 10.1007/s10548-014-0395-1
Köchel, A., Leutgeb, V., and Schienle, A. (2012). Affective inhibitory control in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Abnormalities in electrocortical late positivity. Neurosci. Lett. 530, 47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.053
Koehler, S., Lauer, P., Schreppel, T., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., et al. (2009). Increased EEG power density in alpha and theta bands in adult ADHD patients. J. Neural Transm. 116, 97–104. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0157-x
Kropotov, J. D., Pronina, M. V., Ponomarev, V. A., Poliakov, Y. I., Plotnikova, I. V., and Mueller, A. (2019). Latent ERP components of cognitive dysfunctions in ADHD and schizophrenia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.01.015
Lara, C., Fayyad, J. de Graaf, R., Kessler, R. C. Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Angermeyer, M., et al. (2009). Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 46–54.
Lenartowicz, A., and Loo, S. K. (2014). Use of EEG to diagnose ADHD. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 16:498. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0498-0
Leroy, A., Petit, G., Zarka, D., Cebolla, A. M., Palmero-Soler, E., Strul, J., et al. (2018). EEG dynamics and neural generators in implicit navigational image processing in adults with ADHD. Neuroscience 373, 92–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.01.022
Li, H., Zhao, Q., Huang, F., Cao, Q., Qian, Q., Johnstone, S. J., et al. (2019). Increased beta activity links to impaired emotional control in adhd adults with high IQ. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 754–764. doi: 10.1177/1087054717739120
Liechti, M. D., Valko, L., Müller, U. C., Döhnert, M., Drechsler, R., Steinhausen, H. C., et al. (2013). Diagnostic value of resting electroencephalogram in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Brain Topogr. 26, 135–151. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0258-6
Loo, S. K., Hale, T. S., Macion, J., Hanada, G., McGough, J. J., McCracken, J. T., et al. (2009). Cortical activity patterns in ADHD during arousal, activation and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 47, 2114–2119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.013
Luo, X., Guo, J., Liu, L., Zhao, X., Li, D., Li, H., et al. (2019). The neural correlations of spatial attention and working memory deficits in adults with ADHD. Neuroimage Clin. 22:101728. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101728
Mann, C. A., Lubar, J. F., Zimmerman, A. W., Miller, C. A., and Muenchen, R. A. (1992). Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatr. Neurol. 8, 30–36. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(92)90049-5
Markovska-Simoska, S., and Pop-Jordanova, N. (2017). Quantitative EEG in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Comparison of absolute and relative power spectra and theta/beta ratio. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 48, 20–32. doi: 10.1177/1550059416643824
Marquardt, L., Eichele, H., Lundervold, A. J., Haavik, J., and Eichele, T. (2018). Event-Related-Potential (ERP) correlates of performance monitoring in adults With Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Front. Psychol. 9:485. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00485
Marzinzik, F., Wahl, M., Krüger, D., Gentschow, L., Colla, M., and Klostermann, F. (2012). Abnormal distracter processing in adults with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One 7:e33691. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033691
Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., and Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: Prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness. J. Neurosci. 29, 2725–2732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3963-08.2009
Mauriello, C., Pham, E., Kumar, S., Piguet, C., Deiber, M. P., Aubry, J. M., et al. (2022). Dysfunctional temporal stages of eye-gaze perception in adults with ADHD: A high-density EEG study. Biol. Psychol. 171:108351. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108351
Mayer, K., Wyckoff, S. N., and Strehl, U. (2016). Underarousal in adult ADHD: How are peripheral and cortical arousal related? Clin. EEG Neurosci. 47, 171–179. doi: 10.1177/1550059415577544
McLoughlin, G., Albrecht, B., Banaschewski, T., Rothenberger, A., Brandeis, D., Asherson, P., et al. (2009). Performance monitoring is altered in adult ADHD: A familial event-related potential investigation. Neuropsychologia 47, 3134–3142. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.013
Micoulaud-Franchi, J. A., Lopez, R., Cermolacce, M., Vaillant, F., Péri, P., Boyer, L., et al. (2019). Sensory gating capacity and attentional function in adults with ADHD: A preliminary neurophysiological and neuropsychological study. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 1199–1209. doi: 10.1177/1087054716629716
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Münger, M., Candrian, G., Kasper, J., Abdel-Rehim, H., Eich, D., Müller, A., et al. (2021). Behavioral and neurophysiological markers of ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults: A large-scale clinical study. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 52, 311–320. doi: 10.1177/1550059421993340
Münger, M., Sele, S., Candrian, G., Kasper, J., Abdel-Rehim, H., Eich-Höchli, D., et al. (2022). Longitudinal analysis of self-reported symptoms, behavioral measures, and event-related potential components of a cued go/nogo task in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and controls. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:767789. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.767789
Nigg, J. T., and Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Dev. Psychopathol. 17, 785–806. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050376
Nijmeijer, J. S., Minderaa, R. B., Buitelaar, J. K., Mulligan, A., Hartman, C. A., and Hoekstra, P. J. (2008). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social dysfunctioning. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 692–708. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.003
O’Connell, R. G., Bellgrove, M. A., Dockree, P. M., Lau, A., Hester, R., Garavan, H., et al. (2009). The neural correlates of deficient error awareness in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropsychologia 47, 1149–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.011
Pagán, A. F., Huizar, Y. P., Short, T. R., Gotcher, Z., and Schmidt, A. T. (2023). Adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A narrative review of biological mechanisms, treatments, and outcomes. Curr. Neurol Neurosci. Rep. 23, 451–460. doi: 10.1007/s11910-023-01280-4
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71
Papp, S., Tombor, L., Kakuszi, B., Balogh, L., Réthelyi, J. M., Bitter, I., et al. (2020). Impaired early information processing in adult ADHD: A high-density ERP study. BMC Psychiatry 20:292. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02706-w
Poil, S. S., Bollmann, S., Ghisleni, C., O’Gorman, R. L., Klaver, P., Ball, J., et al. (2014). Age dependent electroencephalographic changes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1626–1638. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.118
Raz, S., and Dan, O. (2015a). Altered event-related potentials in adults with ADHD during emotional faces processing. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 514–523. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.06.023
Raz, S., and Dan, O. (2015b). Behavioral and neural correlates of facial versus nonfacial stimuli processing in adults with ADHD: An ERP study. Neuropsychology 29, 726–738. doi: 10.1037/neu0000176
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., and Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306, 443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301
Rodriguez, P. D., and Baylis, G. C. (2007). Activation of brain attention systems in individuals with symptoms of ADHD. Behav Neurol. 18, 115–130. doi: 10.1155/2007/865717
Roth, R. M., and Saykin, A. J. (2004). Executive dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Cognitive and neuroimaging findings. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 27, 83–96, ix. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00112-6
Rowe, D. L., Robinson, P. A., Lazzaro, I. L., Powles, R. C., Gordon, E., and Williams, L. M. (2005). Biophysical modeling of tonic cortical electrical activity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Neurosci. 115, 1273–1305. doi: 10.1080/00207450590934499
Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study with functional MRI. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 891–896. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.6.891
Salomone, S., Fleming, G. R., Bramham, J., O’Connell, R. G., and Robertson, I. H. (2020). Neuropsychological deficits in Adult ADHD: Evidence for differential attentional impairments, deficient executive functions, and high self-reported functional impairments. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 1413–1424. doi: 10.1177/1087054715623045
Satterfield, J. H., and Cantwell, D. P. (1974). Proceedings: CNS function and response to methylphenidate in hyperactive children. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 10, 36–37.
Satterfield, J. H., and Dawson, M. E. (1971). Electrodermal correlates of hyperactivity in children. Psychophysiology 8, 191–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1971.tb00450.x
Sauseng, P., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F. C. (2013). Two brakes are better than one: The neural bases of inhibitory control of motor memory traces. Neuroimage 65, 52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.048
Schneidt, A., Höhnle, N. M., and Schönenberg, M. (2020). Cognitive and electrophysiological markers of adult full syndrome and subthreshold attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 127, 80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.05.004
Schneidt, A., Jusyte, A., Rauss, K., and Schönenberg, M. (2018). Distraction by salient stimuli in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for the role of task difficulty in bottom-up and top-down processing. Cortex 101, 206–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.021
Schulz, K. P., Fan, J., Tang, C. Y., Newcorn, J. H., Buchsbaum, M. S., Cheung, A. M., et al. (2004). Response inhibition in adolescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during childhood: An event-related FMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 1650–1657. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1650
Shushakova, A., Ohrmann, P., and Pedersen, A. (2018a). Exploring deficient emotion regulation in adult ADHD: Electrophysiological evidence. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 268, 359–371. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017-0826-6
Shushakova, A., Wiesner, C. D., Ohrmann, P., and Pedersen, A. (2018b). Electrophysiological evidence of an attentional bias towards appetitive and aversive words in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 1937–1946. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.06.019
Sibley, M. H., Mitchell, J. T., and Becker, S. P. (2016). Method of adult diagnosis influences estimated persistence of childhood ADHD: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 1157–1165. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30190-0
Skirrow, C., McLoughlin, G., Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Kuntsi, J., and Asherson, P. (2015). Normalisation of frontal theta activity following methylphenidate treatment in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.09.015
Slater, J., Joober, R., Koborsy, B. L., Mitchell, S., Sahlas, E., and Palmer, C. (2022). Can electroencephalography (EEG) identify ADHD subtypes? a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 139:104752. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104752
Smit, D., Trevino, L., Mohamed, S. M. H., and Enriquez-Geppert, S. (2023). Theta power and functional connectivity as neurophysiological markers of executive functions in individuals with cognitive complaints in daily life. Biol. Psychol. 178:108503. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108503
Song, P., Zha, M., Yang, Q., Zhang, Y., Li, X., Rudan, I., et al. (2021). The prevalence of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Glob. Health 11:04009. doi: 10.7189/jogh.11.04009
Spronk, M., Vogel, E. K., and Jonkman, L. M. (2013). No behavioral or ERP evidence for a developmental lag in visual working memory capacity or filtering in adolescents and adults with ADHD. PLoS One 8:e62673. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062673
Thapar, A., and Cooper, M. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 387, 1240–1250. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X
Thoma, P., Soria Bauser, D., Edel, M. A., Juckel, G., and Suchan, B. (2020). Configural processing of emotional bodies and faces in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 42, 1028–1048. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2020.1840521
Tombor, L., Kakuszi, B., Papp, S., Réthelyi, J., Bitter, I., and Czobor, P. (2019). Decreased resting gamma activity in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 20, 691–702. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2018.1441547
Wiegand, I., Hennig-Fast, K., Kilian, B., Müller, H. J., Töllner, T., Möller, H. J., et al. (2016). EEG correlates of visual short-term memory as neuro-cognitive endophenotypes of ADHD. Neuropsychologia 85, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.011
Wiersema, J. R., and Roeyers, H. (2009). ERP correlates of effortful control in children with varying levels of ADHD symptoms. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 327–336. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9288-7
Wiersema, J. R., van der Meere, J. J., and Roeyers, H. (2009). ERP correlates of error monitoring in adult ADHD. J. Neural. Transm. 116, 371–379. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0165-x
Wiersema, R., van der Meere, J., Antrop, I., and Roeyers, H. (2006). State regulation in adult ADHD: An event-related potential study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 1113–1126. doi: 10.1080/13803390500212896
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., and Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006
Willoughby, M., Kupersmidt, J., Voegler-Lee, M., and Bryant, D. (2011). Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 162–180. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549980
Woltering, S., Jung, J., Liu, Z., and Tannock, R. (2012). Resting state EEG oscillatory power differences in ADHD college students and their peers. Behav. Brain Funct. 8:60. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-60
Woltering, S., Liu, Z., Rokeach, A., and Tannock, R. (2013). Neurophysiological differences in inhibitory control between adults with ADHD and their peers. Neuropsychologia 51, 1888–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.023
Yoon, S. H., Oh, J., Um, Y. H., Seo, H. J., Hong, S. C., Kim, T. W., et al. (2024). differences in electroencephalography power and asymmetry at frontal region in young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A quantitative electroencephalography study. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 22, 431–441. doi: 10.9758/cpn.23.1104
Keywords: adult ADHD, electroencephalograph, resting-state, event related potential, neural oscillation
Citation: Su Z, Wang Y, Wang B, Han C, Zhang H, Gu Y, Chen Y, Zhao X and Shi Y (2025) Executive function and neural oscillations in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 19:1617307. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1617307
Received: 24 April 2025; Accepted: 12 June 2025;
Published: 15 July 2025.
Edited by:
Hongjian Pu, University of Pittsburgh, United StatesReviewed by:
Serap Aydin, Hacettepe University, TürkiyeAli Rahimpour Jounghani, Stanford University, United States
Luis Nuñez-Jaramillo, University of Quintana Roo, Mexico
Tanju Surmeli, Living Health Center for Research and Education
Valeria Isaac, Adolfo Ibáñez University, Chile
Dimitri Marques Abramov, Oswaldo Cruz Foundation (Fiocruz), Brazil
Copyright © 2025 Su, Wang, Wang, Han, Zhang, Gu, Chen, Zhao and Shi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yuwei Shi, d3cxMzE5OTg3MTMxNkAxNjMuY29t; Xixi Zhao, emhhb3hpeGlAY2NtdS5lZHUuY24=