- 1School of Nursing, Chang Chun University of Chinese Medicine, Changchun, China
- 2Rehabilitation Department, The Second People's Hospital of Dalian, Dalian, China
Background: Sleep is vital for physical and mental health, yet sleep deprivation is a widespread issue that may impair cognitive flexibility, leading to rigid thinking and slower decision-making. This scoping review synthesizes evidence on the impact of sleep deprivation or sleep loss on cognitive flexibility.
Objective: To provide a comprehensive understanding of the complex and multifaceted effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility.
Methods: We searched PubMed, Web of Science, ClinicalKey, Cochrane, Scopus, SinoMed, and CNKI for studies evaluating the effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility. Two researchers independently screened and extracted data, assessing study quality using the Mixed Method Appraisal Tool (MMAT).
Results: Among the 410 retrieved articles, 6 randomized controlled trials and 11 non-randomized studies were included, focusing on the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility across children, adolescents, college students, clinicians, athletes, and other adults. Eight studies found that sleep deprivation reduces cognitive flexibility, six reported no significant impact, and two noted temporary improvements due to physical stress. One study highlighted that being overweight exacerbates the negative effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility.
Conclusions: Sleep deprivation may predominantly impair accuracy rather than reaction time. Total sleep deprivation consistently reduces task-switching accuracy and cognitive flexibility, whereas partial sleep deprivation's effects remain unclear. The primary biological mechanisms involve decreased cerebral oxygen supply, impaired cerebrovascular reactivity, and alterations in gene expression and hormone levels. Rigorous randomized trials with objective measures are needed to assess long-term impacts across populations and age groups.
Introduction
Sleep can be described as the brain's “valve,” an essential physiological process for the human body (Underwood, 2013). During sleep, the brain opens this “valve,” enabling cerebrospinal fluid to clear metabolic waste, maintain homeostasis, and support memory consolidation and the integration of fragmented daily information (Xie et al., 2013). Upon waking, the brain initiates new neural activities and information processing, thereby enabling normal brain function (Hauglund et al., 2025). However, 25% to 30% of the global population experiences sleep disorders, leading to poor sleep quality or insufficient sleep duration, resulting in sleep deprivation. Sleep deprivation can disrupt the biological clock and neurotransmitter balance, triggering various sleep disorders. These factors interact to form a vicious cycle, collectively impacting both physical and mental health (Kiley et al., 2019).
Sleep deprivation refers to a reduction in both the duration and phases of sleep due to environmental or personal factors, typically defined as obtaining <4 h of sleep within a 24-h period (Khan and Al-Jahdali, 2023). It encompasses both acute and chronic sleep deprivation (Choshen-Hillel et al., 2021). Research indicates that acute, short-term sleep deprivation may merely be a nuisance, but chronic sleep deprivation can have severe consequences for both young and elderly individuals (Madan Jha, 2022). On one hand, sleep deprivation can lead to low mood, irritability, fatigue, forgetfulness, weakened immunity, and varying degrees of memory impairment (Short and Louca, 2015; Stickgold and Walker, 2007). On the other hand, it may also increase the risk of cardiovascular diseases, diabetes, obesity (Tobaldini et al., 2017), and even elevate the likelihood of developing cancer (Huang et al., 2021). Therefore, the National Sleep Foundation (NSF) recommends that young adults aged 18 to 25 should sleep at least 7 to 9 h per day, while older adults aged 65 and above are advised to sleep at least 7 to 8 h daily (Hirshkowitz et al., 2015).
A growing body of research indicates a strong link between sleep deprivation and cognitive decline (Guarnieri, 2019; Jee et al., 2020). Total sleep deprivation exceeding 24 h significantly impairs performance in executive functions (García et al., 2021). Cognitive flexibility, as a core component of executive functions, undergoes significant changes under the influence of sleep deprivation. Given the critical role of cognitive flexibility in adapting to dynamic environments and making quick decisions, it is particularly important in specialized professions such as athletes, clinicians, and astronauts. For individuals in these professions, maintaining a high level of cognitive flexibility is not only a professional requirement but also a critical factor in ensuring workplace safety and efficiency (Whitney et al., 2015). Therefore, research in this area holds significant value. It is noteworthy that most studies have reported connections between sleep deprivation and various cognitive components such as attention, memory, processing speed, and complex reasoning (Hudson et al., 2020; Cousins and Fernández, 2019). However, only a few studies have investigated the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility, and the results have been inconsistent (Lim and Dinges, 2010).
Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to consciously adapt cognitive strategies in response to environmental changes, enabling individuals to adjust to new situations and solve novel problems. At its core, it involves the switching between rules, reflecting an individual's cognitive shifting and inhibitory control abilities (Zühlsdorff et al., 2023). Currently, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and task-switching paradigms are commonly used to measure cognitive flexibility (Nyhus and Barceló, 2009; Schmitter-Edgecombe and Langill, 2006). However, these methods primarily rely on behavioral data analysis, making it difficult to explore the underlying neural mechanisms. There is limited research utilizing objective techniques, such as electroencephalography (EEG), to investigate cognitive flexibility under conditions of sleep deprivation. Previous studies have reported that the prefrontal cortex plays a critical role in cognitive flexibility. Sleep deprivation reduces blood flow in the prefrontal regions, leading to impaired prefrontal cortex function (Verweij et al., 2014). Sleep deprivation disrupts the balance of glutamate-GABA neurocircuitry in the prefrontal cortex, resulting in altered neuronal firing patterns (Kamal, 2010). Recent research highlights the critical role of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in neuronal development, survival, and synaptic plasticity enhancement. Genetic variations in BDNF may indicate susceptibility to the effects of sleep deprivation (Murer et al., 2001; Bachmann et al., 2012). Research using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has found that after sleep deprivation, individuals exhibit significantly reduced activation in the prefrontal cortex when performing cognitive flexibility tasks (Drummond et al., 1999). Additionally, sleep deprivation can trigger oxidative stress, resulting in the accumulation of free radicals that damage brain cells and neural connections, thereby affecting the neural foundations required for cognitive flexibility (Ramanathan and Siegel, 2011). However, there is still no consensus on the relationship between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility. For example, a prospective cross-sectional study found that emergency department physicians did not show impaired cognitive flexibility despite experiencing sleep deprivation due to shift work (Persico et al., 2018).
Sleep deprivation, as a potentially significant factor influencing cognitive flexibility, makes it crucial to study the relationship between the two in order to gain a deeper understanding of the underlying mechanisms. Such insights are essential for developing interventions or recommendations aimed at enhancing cognitive flexibility and improving individual performance in learning, work, and daily life. Therefore, this scoping review aims to synthesize existing evidence on the effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility and explore the relationship between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility.
Materials and methods
The study protocol was registered on the Open Science Framework with the registration number (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YKHBT).
Research question
The research question encompasses several sub-questions: Firstly, does sleep deprivation affect cognitive flexibility in humans? If so, are there differences in the domains of cognitive flexibility that are impacted? Lastly, does the literature suggest potential mechanisms linking sleep deprivation to impaired cognitive flexibility?
Literature search
The literature search included articles from the establishment of the databases up to February 28, 2025. These searches were organized and conducted by the librarians. According to the pre-established retrieval strategy, PubMed, Web of Science, ClinicalKey, Cochrane, Scopus, SinoMed, and CNKI databases were performed to identify relevant literature. The main concepts searched included “sleep deprivation” and “cognitive flexibility”. Taking PubMed as an example, the search formula is shown in Table 1.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Our study included observational studies with no restrictions on participants' age, gender, race, or other demographic characteristics. These studies were designed to explore the relationship between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility. Among them, sleep deprivation could be acute, transient, or chronic. If the literature was only available in the form of an abstract, or was a systematic review in any form, or was written in a language other than English, it would be excluded. Studies assessing the impact of sleep deprivation on other cognitive domains were also not eligible for inclusion. See the Table 2.
Study selection
Use Zotero software to manage and screen literature. After removing duplicates, one researcher screened titles and abstracts. Documents describing sleep deprivation as an exposure factor were temporarily retained for full-text review. Another researcher conducted the full-text review, determining inclusion based on predefined criteria. Disagreements were resolved through joint review and consensus.
Quality appraisal
The Mixed Method Appraisal Tool (MMAT, 2018 version) was used to evaluate the quality of the literature, enabling appropriate weighting and interpretation. The MMAT is designed for systematic reviews and assesses five research categories: qualitative research, randomized controlled trials, quantitative non-randomized studies, quantitative descriptive studies, and mixed methods studies (Hong et al., 2018). Two initial questions must be addressed during evaluation: “S1: Are there clear research questions?” and “S2: Do the collected data address the research questions?” Only if both answers are “yes” can the quality assessment proceed. Each study was evaluated against five criteria, with each met criterion marked by an asterisk (*). A study meeting all criteria received five asterisks (*****). Two researchers independently scored the studies, resolving discrepancies through discussion. To ensure comprehensiveness, no low-quality studies were excluded (Appendix 1).
Data extraction
Microsoft Excel was used to manage the collected data. Through group discussions, we created a data extraction form, including fields such as author, publication year, country, study design, participant type, age, sample size, measurement tools for sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility, and main findings. We also extracted data on potential mechanisms linking sleep deprivation to cognitive flexibility. To clarify this relationship, we categorized and organized the extracted content, highlighting results from individual studies and common patterns across the literature.
Results
Search results
This review followed the systematic review and meta-analysis (PRISMA 2020) framework (Page et al., 2021). The initial search identified 410 studies. After the automatic and manual removal of duplicate records, 271 studies remained eligible for title and abstract screening. Upon further review of titles and abstracts, systematic reviews, literature reviews, editorials, and similar types of studies were excluded, resulting in 41 studies advancing to the full-text review stage. Ultimately, 17 studies fully met the inclusion criteria (Figure 1). During the data extraction process of these 16 articles, it was discovered that several articles pertained to the same studies but presented outcomes with distinct focal points. During the full-text review stage, the primary reasons for excluding 24 studies included missing critical data and mismatched study contexts.
Study characteristics
The included articles were published between 1998 and 2024, with 87.5% of them published after 2018. These articles originated from the United States (Randazzo et al., 1998; Whitney et al., 2017; Grant et al., 2018; Honn et al., 2019), France (Maltese et al., 2016; Persico et al., 2018), China (Zhang et al., 2024), Canada (Chan et al., 2024), the United Kingdom (Stager et al., 2024), Ireland (O'Hagan et al., 2018), Egypt (Abdelhamid et al., 2020), Mexico (García et al., 2021), Belgium (Slama et al., 2018), Italy (Ballesio et al., 2018), Turkey (Kiriş, 2022; Sen et al., 2023), and Australia (Pourhassan et al., 2023). Among the 17 studies, six were randomized controlled trials (Randazzo et al., 1998; Honn et al., 2019; Kiriş, 2022; Zhang et al., 2024; Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024), while the remaining were quantitative non-randomized studies (Maltese et al., 2016; Whitney et al., 2017; Persico et al., 2018; O'Hagan et al., 2018; Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; Ballesio et al., 2018; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; García et al., 2021; Sen et al., 2023; Pourhassan et al., 2023). It is evident that the sample sizes of these studies varied (ranging from n = 7 to n = 66), with the included studies generally featuring small sample sizes, which may be influenced by factors such as study duration, exposure variables, and ethical considerations. The average age of participants ranged from 11.6 to 38.2 years. The study participants included children, adolescents, undergraduate, clinical physicians (anesthesiology residents and emergency department doctors), football players, pilots, and other adults (Table 3).
Quality of the included studies
The quality of the included studies was generally high (Randazzo et al., 1998; Maltese et al., 2016; Whitney et al., 2017; Persico et al., 2018; O'Hagan et al., 2018; Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; Ballesio et al., 2018; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; García et al., 2021; Kiriş, 2022; Sen et al., 2023; Pourhassan et al., 2023; Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024), with one study rated as moderate quality (Zhang et al., 2024), and one study rated as low quality (Honn et al., 2019) (Appendix 1).
Summary of findings
Methods and measures of sleep deprivation
Five studies employed total sleep deprivation to observe its effects on cognitive flexibility (Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; Honn et al., 2019; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2024), seven studies utilized partial sleep deprivation (Randazzo et al., 1998; Maltese et al., 2016; Persico et al., 2018; Ballesio et al., 2018; Pourhassan et al., 2023; Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024), four studies adopted a form of alternating between regular sleep schedules and total sleep deprivation (Slama et al., 2018; O'Hagan et al., 2018; García et al., 2021; Sen et al., 2023). Another study investigated the effects of sleep restriction vs. sleep extension on adolescents (Kiriş, 2022). Among the included studies, nine utilized monitoring devices or scales to assess the sleep quality of participants under sleep deprivation conditions. Specifically, two studies employed polysomnography (PSG) (Randazzo et al., 1998; Grant et al., 2018), six studies used wrist actigraphy (Slama et al., 2018; Ballesio et al., 2018; Kiriş, 2022; Pourhassan et al., 2023; Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024), three studies mentioned the use of sleep diary to record sleep conditions (Pourhassan et al., 2023; Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024), three studies applied the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) (Slama et al., 2018; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; Sen et al., 2023), and only one study utilized the Visual Analog Scale (VAS) for sleep assessment (Whitney et al., 2017). The remaining seven studies did not mention any sleep-related evaluation tools (Table 3).
Cognitive flexibility outcome measurement
Among these, five studies utilized the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) (O'Hagan et al., 2018; Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; García et al., 2021; Sen et al., 2023), three studies employed the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Maltese et al., 2016; Persico et al., 2018; Pourhassan et al., 2023), two studies adopted the task-switching paradigm (Ballesio et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2024), and two studies applied the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test (DDCS) (Chan et al., 2024; Stager et al., 2024). The remaining studies respectively utilized the Trail Making Test (TMT) (Abdelhamid et al., 2020), the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) (Randazzo et al., 1998), the AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT-s) (Whitney et al., 2017), the Three-phase Reversal Learning Decision Task (Honn et al., 2019), and Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Kiriş, 2022). It is noteworthy that only two studies employed objective detection techniques such as EEG and MRI (Zhang et al., 2024; Chan et al., 2024) (Table 3).
Relationship between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility
Among the 17 studies included in this review, all reported an association between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility. Based on the relevant data extracted from Table 3, eight studies indicated that sleep deprivation affects cognitive flexibility (Whitney et al., 2017; O'Hagan et al., 2018; Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; Honn et al., 2019; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2024; Chan et al., 2024). Participants with longer duration of sleep deprivation exhibited higher inhibitory costs, task-switching costs, and switch error rates, along with prolonged reaction times. Six studies showed that sleep deprivation did not lead to a decline in cognitive flexibility but significantly impaired cognitive processes such as verbal fluency, working memory, alertness, selective attention, processing speed, and perceptual reasoning (Randazzo et al., 1998; Maltese et al., 2016; Persico et al., 2018; García et al., 2021; Kiriş, 2022; Pourhassan et al., 2023). Surprisingly, two studies conducted in Italy and Turkey, respectively, revealed that cognitive flexibility improved to some extent after sleep deprivation (Ballesio et al., 2018; Sen et al., 2023). One study focused on the cognitive flexibility performance of adolescents in different weight groups following sleep deprivation (Stager et al., 2024). The results demonstrated that overweight or obese adolescents scored significantly lower in cognitive flexibility after sleep deprivation compared to the well-rested group, while adolescents with healthy weight showed improved cognitive flexibility scores after sleep deprivation.
Potential biological mechanisms underlying impaired cognitive flexibility
We also extracted and summarized the mechanisms by which sleep deprivation affects cognitive flexibility as mentioned in the included studies. Eight studies briefly explained the potential mechanisms, which primarily include five types. The first is changes in brain region activity and neurotransmitters: Sleep deprivation reduces insular activity, impairs memory capacity, and affects the expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). A deficiency in BDNF leads to decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (O'Hagan et al., 2018; Grant et al., 2018; Honn et al., 2019). The second is related to genes and dopamine: Sleep deprivation affects the binding potential of dopamine D2 receptors (DRD2) (Whitney et al., 2017). The third is the neuroendocrine mechanism: acute sleep deprivation places the body in a state of stress, altering neurotransmitter and hormone secretion (Sen et al., 2023). The fourth is reduced cerebral blood flow: Sleep deprivation decreases cerebrovascular reactivity in specific brain regions, reducing oxygen flow (Chan et al., 2024). Additionally, it may also be associated with overweight and obesity (Stager et al., 2024) (Table 4 and Figure 2).
Discussion
While some studies suggest that sleep deprivation does not affect certain executive functions (Binks et al., 1999), cognitive flexibility, as a core component, has been closely linked to sleep deprivation in multiple studies (Stenson et al., 2023). This review highlights extensive research on cognitive flexibility in children, adolescents, and young adults. Honn et al. (2019) found that sleep deprivation reduces sensitivity to neurofeedback, impairing individuals' ability to adjust behavior or decisions based on new information, leading to blunted responses and reduced cognitive flexibility. Three studies consistently demonstrated that sleep deprivation primarily reduces accuracy rather than reaction time, particularly under total sleep deprivation (Slama et al., 2018; García et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2024). This aligns with prior research indicating that sleep deprivation affects accuracy more than speed (Van Dongen et al., 2003), suggesting that while reaction speed may be maintained, the brain's ability to accurately process information is significantly compromised under sleep deprivation. Similarly, Chan et al. (2024) found that adolescents experienced reduced cognitive flexibility after five nights of sleep restriction. MRI results revealed a significant decrease in cerebrovascular reactivity (CVR) in the fusiform cortex and occipital lobe, suggesting that even short-term sleep restriction may impair cognitive flexibility by affecting cerebrovascular function. Another study utilized EEG to explore the neural mechanisms underlying sleep deprivation's impact on cognitive flexibility in adolescents, employing event-related potentials and time-frequency analysis (Zhang et al., 2024). These studies innovatively applied neuroimaging techniques to objectively examine sleep deprivation's effects on cognitive flexibility at the brain's physiological level, providing valuable insights.
In contrast to previous findings, Ballesio et al. (2018) and Sen et al. (2023) reported increased cognitive flexibility in participants. This may be attributed to short-term, acute sleep deprivation temporarily enhancing alertness and arousal, indirectly boosting cognitive flexibility. However, prolonged sleep deprivation is likely to impair cognitive function. While Randazzo et al. (1998) found no significant impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility in children, they noted that even one night of sleep restriction could impair abstract thinking, language processing, and other higher-order cognitive functions. A UK study highlighted the role of body weight in adolescents' cognitive flexibility after sleep restriction (Stager et al., 2024). Overweight or obese adolescents exhibited poorer cognitive flexibility and processing speed, while those with a healthy weight remained unaffected, suggesting an interaction between body weight and sleep deprivation in influencing cognitive flexibility.
Second, research on the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility in special occupational groups, such as healthcare (Weinger and Ancoli-Israel, 2002) and aerospace (Basner et al., 2014), has gained attention. A survey of pilots found that cognitive flexibility is among the earliest indicators affected by sleep deprivation (O'Hagan et al., 2018). Even short-term sleep deficiency can impair the brain's ability to adapt to new tasks, particularly in high-demand work or study scenarios. Ensuring sufficient sleep is crucial, as it not only affects daily performance but also impacts safety and efficiency in specific occupations. For instance, clinical doctors working night shifts may experience significant cognitive impairments, increasing the risk of misdiagnosis or missed diagnoses. Studies show that sleep deprivation prolongs surgeons' operation times and raises the likelihood of surgical complications (Taffinder et al., 1998). Additionally, physical and mental fatigue is linked to major medical errors, such as syringe confusion and incorrect drug dosages (Barger et al., 2006). Three studies in this review focused on clinical doctors. One found that anesthesiologists experienced significant declines in cognitive flexibility and alertness after 24-h shifts (Abdelhamid et al., 2020). However, the other two studies showed no significant decline in cognitive flexibility after partial sleep deprivation, although processing speed, working memory, and perceptual reasoning were affected (Maltese et al., 2016; Persico et al., 2018). These differences may stem from variations in study populations. For instance, compared to doctors in France, anesthesiologists in Egypt face longer working hours, staff shortages, and greater work pressure, potentially making their cognitive flexibility more vulnerable to sleep deprivation. Similarly, a study of Australian football players found no impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility (Pourhassan et al., 2023). This may be due to athletes' unique ability to maintain cognitive flexibility under sleep deprivation, possibly through superior compensatory mechanisms. Research suggests that athletes can better allocate attention resources, filter key information, and respond accurately and quickly to cognitive stress caused by sleep deprivation (Souissi et al., 2013).
Furthermore, the review results indicate that the method of sleep deprivation appears to play a significant role in determining the outcomes of cognitive flexibility. Five studies employed total sleep deprivation (Grant et al., 2018; Slama et al., 2018; Honn et al., 2019; Abdelhamid et al., 2020; Hauglund et al., 2025), with participants remaining awake for 24 h, or even up to 38 h, and the results for cognitive flexibility showed a highly consistent trend, all indicating a significant decline. In contrast, studies involving partial sleep deprivation or alternating between normal sleep and total sleep deprivation exhibited inconsistent results in cognitive flexibility tests. This phenomenon suggests that prolonged total sleep deprivation severely impacts the brain's cognitive functions, ultimately leading to a deterioration in cognitive flexibility. However, under conditions of partial sleep deprivation or alternating sleep states, the brain may attempt to maintain cognitive flexibility by reallocating neural resources and enhancing activity in certain key brain regions (Kim et al., 2001; Kiriş, 2022). Nevertheless, partial sleep deprivation could potentially have long-term adverse effects on cognitive flexibility, which warrants further investigation. This also underscores the indispensable role of sleep in maintaining normal cognitive functions of the brain, highlighting that adequate sleep is fundamental to ensuring optimal cognitive flexibility (Raven et al., 2018).
Sleep deprivation has varying effects on cognitive functions at different developmental stages due to differences in brain maturation, physiological characteristics, and compensatory capacities (Sullan et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2022). In childhood, short-term sleep deprivation may not directly impair cognitive flexibility but can disrupt neurodevelopmental processes and compromise higher-order cognitive functions like language processing (Randazzo et al., 1998). Adolescence is a critical period for neural reorganization, particularly in prefrontal-limbic connectivity, influencing cognitive flexibility through the interplay of sleep, cognition, and emotion (Lo et al., 2016). Despite the relatively mature adult brain, sleep deprivation can still impact cognitive flexibility through diverse neural mechanisms. The impact of partial sleep deprivation differs across occupations (Abdelhamid et al., 2020), while total sleep deprivation may result in a “resource depletion” pattern in the brain.
This study also investigated sleep deprivation's effects on other cognitive components, including working memory, alertness, selective attention, sustained attention, inhibitory control, and processing speed. Sleep deprivation typically leads to declines in these functions. Cognitive flexibility depends on the coordination of these processes (Dajani and Uddin, 2015); for example, working memory manages task-related information, while inhibitory control minimizes distractions and errors (Friedman and Robbins, 2022). Alertness and sustained attention also support focus and adaptability during complex tasks (Bastos et al., 2018). Mechanistically, only two studies explored genetic factors (Whitney et al., 2017; Grant et al., 2018), linking the C957T polymorphism of the dopamine receptor D2 gene and the BDNF Met allele to sleep deprivation-induced cognitive flexibility impairments. BDNF Met allele carriers exhibited greater deficits, with higher error rates in Stroop tasks. However, findings from these two genetic loci are insufficient to fully explain the underlying mechanisms.
This review offers valuable insights into the effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility but is subject to several limitations. The studies included in the review utilized a variety of measurement tools, posing challenges in drawing definitive conclusions despite reporting statistically significant findings. Small sample sizes may restrict the generalizability of results, with most studies relying on task-based assessments and only two incorporating objective detection methods, potentially introducing bias. Moreover, the overrepresentation of young participants warrants caution in extrapolating findings to other age demographics. The brief duration of sleep deprivation experiments may not fully capture the dynamic alterations in cognitive flexibility, resulting in incomplete data. Notably, there is a lack of comparative studies examining partial vs. total sleep deprivation effects on cognitive flexibility. Future investigations should aim to increase sample sizes, prolong study durations, and employ more objective methodologies, such as functional near-infrared spectroscopy, to elucidate the neural mechanisms underpinning the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility. Additionally, cognitive flexibility is a multifaceted process influenced by various brain regions, emotional states, and stress levels. Inadequate control of these confounding variables may compromise the accuracy and reliability of study outcomes. In essence, further comprehensive and methodologically rigorous research is imperative to explore the ramifications of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility, its underlying mechanisms, and strategies to ameliorate adverse effects and optimize performance in conditions of inadequate sleep.
Conclusion
These studies reveal that the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility varies depending on factors such as demographics, occupations, and experimental setups. While most studies show that sleep deprivation hampers task-switching accuracy and cognitive flexibility, especially evident under total sleep deprivation conditions with consistent outcomes, the effects of partial sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility and its lasting consequences remain uncertain. The review underscores the necessity for more precise assessment tools, extended study durations, larger participant pools, and longitudinal investigations to elucidate the link between sleep deprivation and cognitive flexibility. The confirmation of the adverse effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility underscores the potential value of personalized interventions, such as educating individuals on sleep hygiene and providing cognitive training. Prioritizing interventions for adolescents and high-risk professional groups is essential. Implementing a comprehensive system integrating monitoring, intervention, and assessment could effectively enhance cognitive performance, ultimately enhancing overall quality of life.
Author contributions
XS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing –review & editing. ZQ: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XiaZ: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YZ: Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XinZ: Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HZhao: Writing – review & editing. HZhan: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the Jilin Province Science and Technology Development Plan Project (Grant No. 232662SF0103109942).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2025.1626309/full#supplementary-material
References
Abdelhamid, B. M., Omar, H., Hassan, M. M., Embaby, S. A., Rady, A., and Mohamed Aly, H. (2020). Effects of partial sleep deprivation following night shift on cognitive functions of Egyptian anesthesiologists: prospective observational study. Egypt. J. Anaesth. 36, 61–68. doi: 10.1080/11101849.2020.1768630
Bachmann, V., Klein, C., Bodenmann, S., Schäfer, N., Berger, W., Brugger, P., et al. (2012). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism modulates sleep intensity: EEG frequency- and state-specificity. Sleep 35, 335–344. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1690
Ballesio, A., Cerolini, S., Ferlazzo, F., Cellini, N., and Lombardo, C. (2018). The effects of one night of partial sleep deprivation on executive functions in individuals reporting chronic insomnia and good sleepers. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 60, 42–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.02.002
Barger, L. K., Ayas, N. T., Cade, B. E., Cronin, J. W., Rosner, B., Speizer, F. E., et al. (2006). Impact of extended-duration shifts on medical errors, adverse events, and attentional failures. PLoS Med. 3:e487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030487
Basner, M., Dinges, D. F., Mollicone, D. J., Savelev, I., Ecker, A. J., Di Antonio, A., et al. (2014). Psychological and behavioral changes during confinement in a 520-day simulated interplanetary mission to Mars. PLoS ONE 9:e93298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093298
Bastos, A. M., Loonis, R., Kornblith, S., Lundqvist, M., and Miller, E. K. (2018). Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 115, 1117–1122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1710323115
Binks, P. G., Waters, W. F., and Hurry, M. (1999). Short-term total sleep deprivation does not selectively impair higher cortical functioning. Sleep 22, 328–334. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.3.328
Chan, A., Au, C. T., Reyna, M. E., Robertson, A., Walker, K., Westmacott, R., et al. (2024). The impact of sleep restriction on cerebrovascular reactivity and cognitive outcomes in healthy adolescents: a pilot crossover trial. Sleep Med. 124, 717–726. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2024.11.005
Choshen-Hillel, S., Ishqer, A., Mahameed, F., Reiter, J., Gozal, D., Gileles-Hillel, A., et al. (2021). Acute and chronic sleep deprivation in residents: cognition and stress biomarkers. Med. Educ. 55, 174–184. doi: 10.1111/medu.14296
Cousins, J. N., and Fernández, G. (2019). The impact of sleep deprivation on declarative memory. Prog. Brain Res. 246, 27–53. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.01.007
Dajani, D. R., and Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 38, 571–578. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.07.003
Drummond, S. P., Brown, G. G., Stricker, J. L., Buxton, R. B., Wong, E. C., and Gillin, J. C. (1999). Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport 10, 3745–3748. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199912160-00004
Friedman, N. P., and Robbins, T. W. (2022). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 72–89. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0
García, A., Angel, J. D., Borrani, J., Ramirez, C., and Valdez, P. (2021). Sleep deprivation effects on basic cognitive processes: which components of attention, working memory, and executive functions are more susceptible to the lack of sleep? Sleep Sci. 14, 107–118. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20200049
Grant, L. K., Cain, S. W., Chang, A. M., Saxena, R., Czeisler, C. A., and Anderson, C. (2018). Impaired cognitive flexibility during sleep deprivation among carriers of the brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met allele. Behav. Brain Res. 338, 51–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.025
Guarnieri, B. (2019). Sleep disorders and cognitive alterations in women. Maturitas 126, 25–27. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2019.04.214
Hauglund, N. L., Andersen, M., Tokarska, K., Radovanovic, T., Kjaerby, C., Sørensen, F. L., et al. (2025). Norepinephrine-mediated slow vasomotion drives glymphatic clearance during sleep. Cell 188, 606–622.e17. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2024.11.027
Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., et al. (2015). National Sleep Foundation's sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health 1, 40–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010
Hong, Q. N., Fàbregues, S., Bartlett, G., Boardman, F., Cargo, M., Dagenais, P., et al. (2018). The Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT) version 2018 for information professionals and researchers. Educ. Inf. 34, 285–291. doi: 10.3233/EFI-180221
Honn, K. A., Hinson, J. M., Whitney, P., and Van Dongen, H. P. A. (2019). Cognitive flexibility: a distinct element of performance impairment due to sleep deprivation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 126, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.02.013
Huang, J., Song, P., Hang, K., Chen, Z., Zhu, Z., Zhang, Y., et al. (2021). Sleep deprivation disturbs immune surveillance and promotes the progression of hepatocellular carcinoma. Front. Immunol. 12:727959. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2021.727959
Hudson, A. N., Van Dongen, H. P. A., and Honn, K. A. (2020). Sleep deprivation, vigilant attention, and brain function: a review. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 21–30. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0432-6
Jee, H. J., Shin, W., Jung, H. J., Kim, B., Lee, B. K., and Jung, Y. S. (2020). Impact of sleep disorder as a risk factor for dementia in men and women. Biomol. Ther. 28, 58–73. doi: 10.4062/biomolther.2019.192
Kamal, S. M. (2010). Pharmacological modulation of brain levels of glutamate and GABA in rats exposed to total sleep deprivation. J. Exp. Pharmacol. 2:65–71. doi: 10.2147/JEP.S11143
Khan, M. A., and Al-Jahdali, H. (2023). The consequences of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance. Neurosciences 28, 91–99. doi: 10.17712/nsj.2023.2.20220108
Kiley, J. P., Twery, M. J., and Gibbons, G. H. (2019). The National Center on Sleep Disorders Research: progress and promise. Sleep 42:z105. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz105
Kim, D. J., Lee, H. P., Kim, M. S., Park, Y. J., Go, H. J., Kim, K. S., et al. (2001). The effect of total sleep deprivation on cognitive functions in normal adult male subjects. Int. J. Neurosci. 109, 127–137. doi: 10.3109/00207450108986529
Kiriş, N. (2022). Effects of partial sleep deprivation on prefrontal cognitive functions in adolescents. Sleep Biol. Rhythm. 20, 499–508. doi: 10.1007/s41105-022-00396-3
Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol. Bull. 136, 375–389. doi: 10.1037/a0018883
Lo, J. C., Ong, J. L., Leong, R. L., Gooley, J. J., and Chee, M. W. (2016). Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep deprived adolescents: the need for sleep study. Sleep 39, 687–698. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5552
Madan Jha, V. (2022). The prevalence of sleep loss and sleep disorders in young and old adults. Aging Brain 3:100057. doi: 10.1016/j.nbas.2022.100057
Maltese, F., Adda, M., Bablon, A., Hraeich, S., Guervilly, C., Lehingue, S., et al. (2016). Night shift decreases cognitive performance of ICU physicians. Intensive Care Med. 42, 393–400. doi: 10.1007/s00134-015-4115-4
Murer, M. G., Yan, Q., and Raisman-Vozari, R. (2001). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the control human brain, and in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 63, 71–124. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00014-9
Nyhus, E., and Barceló, F. (2009). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a critical update. Brain Cogn. 71, 437–451. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.03.005
O'Hagan, A. D., Issartel, J., McGinley, E., and Warrington, G. (2018). A pilot study exploring the effects of sleep deprivation on analogue measures of pilot competencies. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 89, 609–615. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5056.2018
Page, M. J., Moher, D., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n160
Persico, N., Maltese, F., Ferrigno, C., Bablon, A., Marmillot, C., Papazian, L., et al. (2018). Influence of shift duration on cognitive performance of emergency physicians: a prospective cross-sectional study. Ann. Emerg. Med. 72, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2017.10.005
Pourhassan, J., Sarginson, J., Hitzl, W., and Richter, K. (2023). Cognitive function in soccer athletes determined by sleep disruption and self-reported health, yet not by decision-reinvestment. Front. Neurol. 13:872761. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.872761
Ramanathan, L., and Siegel, J. M. (2011). Sleep deprivation under sustained hypoxia protects against oxidative stress. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 51, 1842–1848. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2011.08.016
Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., and Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep 21, 861–868.
Raven, F., Van der Zee, E. A., Meerlo, P., and Havekes, R. (2018). The role of sleep in regulating structural plasticity and synaptic strength: implications for memory and cognitive function. Sleep Med. Rev. 39, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.05.002
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., and Langill, M. (2006). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. Neuropsychology 20, 675–684. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.6.675
Sen, B., Kurtaran, N. E., and Öztürk, L. (2023). The effect of 24-hour sleep deprivation on subjective time perception. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 192, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.08.011
Short, M. A., and Louca, M. (2015). Sleep deprivation leads to mood deficits in healthy adolescents. Sleep Med. 16, 987–993. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.007
Slama, H., Chylinski, D. O., Deliens, G., Leproult, R., Schmitz, R., and Peigneux, P. (2018). Sleep deprivation triggers cognitive control impairments in task-goal switching. Sleep 41:zsx200. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx200
Souissi, N., Chtourou, H., Aloui, A., Hammouda, O., Dogui, M., Chaouachi, A., et al. (2013). Effects of time-of-day and partial sleep deprivation on short-term maximal performances of judo competitors. J. Strength Cond. Res. 27, 2473–2480. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e31827f4792
Stager, L. M., Watson, C. S., Cook, E. W. 3rd, and Fobian, A. D. (2024). Effect of sleep restriction on adolescent cognition by adiposity: a randomized crossover trial. JAMA Neurol. 81, 712–721. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2024.1332
Stenson, A. R., Whitney, P., Hinson, J. M., Hansen, D. A., Lawrence-Sidebottom, D., Skeiky, L., et al. (2023). Effects of total sleep deprivation on components of top-down attentional control using a flexible attentional control task. J. Sleep Res. 32:e13744. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13744
Stickgold, R., and Walker, M. P. (2007). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Med. 8, 331–343. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.011
Sullan, M. J., Drummond, S. P. A., and Granholm, E. (2021). Sleep deprivation and compensatory cognitive effort on a visual information processing task. Sleep 44:zsaa177. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa177
Taffinder, N. J., McManus, I. C., Gul, Y., Russell, R. C., and Darzi, A. (1998). Effect of sleep deprivation on surgeons' dexterity on laparoscopy simulator. Lancet 352:1191. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)00034-8
Tobaldini, E., Costantino, G., Solbiati, M., Cogliati, C., Kara, T., Nobili, L., et al. (2017). Sleep, sleep deprivation, autonomic nervous system and cardiovascular diseases. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 321–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.004
Underwood, E. (2013). Neuroscience. Sleep: the brain's housekeeper? Science 342:301. doi: 10.1126/science.342.6156.301
Van Dongen, H. P., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M., and Dinges, D. F. (2003). The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 26, 117–126. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117
Verweij, I. M., Romeijn, N., Smit, D. J., Piantoni, G., Van Someren, E. J., and van der Werf, Y. D. (2014). Sleep deprivation leads to a loss of functional connectivity in frontal brain regions. BMC Neurosci. 15:88. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-15-88
Weinger, M. B., and Ancoli-Israel, S. (2002). Sleep deprivation and clinical performance. JAMA 287, 955–957. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.8.955
Whitney, P., Hinson, J. M., Jackson, M. L., and Van Dongen, H. P. (2015). Feedback blunting: total sleep deprivation impairs decision making that requires updating based on feedback. Sleep 38, 745–754. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4668
Whitney, P., Hinson, J. M., Satterfield, B. C., Grant, D. A., Honn, K. A., and Van Dongen, H. P. A. (2017). Sleep deprivation diminishes attentional control effectiveness and impairs flexible adaptation to changing conditions. Sci. Rep. 7:16020. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16165-z
Xie, L., Kang, H., Xu, Q., Chen, M. J., Liao, Y., Thiyagarajan, M., et al. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science 342, 373–377. doi: 10.1126/science.1241224
Yang, F. N., Xie, W., and Wang, Z. (2022). Effects of sleep duration on neurocognitive development in early adolescents in the USA: a propensity score matched, longitudinal, observational study. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 6, 705–712. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(22)00188-2
Zhang, X., Feng, S., Yang, X., Peng, Y., Du, M., Zhang, R., et al. (2024). Neuroelectrophysiological alteration associated with cognitive flexibility after 24 h sleep deprivation in adolescents. Conscious. Cogn. 124:103734. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103734
Keywords: sleep deprivation, cognitive flexibility, biological mechanisms, cognitive performance, executive function
Citation: Sun X, Qu Z, Zhang X, Zhang Y, Zhang X, Zhao H and Zhang H (2025) The effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive flexibility: a scoping review of outcomes and biological mechanisms. Front. Neurosci. 19:1626309. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1626309
Received: 10 May 2025; Accepted: 30 June 2025;
Published: 22 July 2025.
Edited by:
Cristina Lanni, University of Pavia, ItalyReviewed by:
Massimo Grilli, University of Genoa, ItalyNurcihan Kiriş, Izmir Kâtip Çelebi University, Türkiye
Copyright © 2025 Sun, Qu, Zhang, Zhang, Zhang, Zhao and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hongshi Zhang, NTUwMzU3NkBxcS5jb20=