- 1Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, GA, United States
- 2Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, United States
- 3Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States
- 4Department of Human Genetics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, United States
The startle response is a reflexive contraction of skeletal musculature in response to a strong acoustic stimulus that is evolutionarily preserved across species. There is a broad and comprehensive literature connecting components of the startle response such as latency, magnitude and pre-pulse inhibition, to psychosis status and risk. In this review, we examine the startle response in human subjects with 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome (22qDel) and in analogous animal models. 22qDel is a copy number variant disorder typically involving ~1.5 to 3 Mb of DNA on the proximal 22q region, which occurs in approximately 1 in 2000–6,000 births, and serves as the most robust single genetic predictor of psychosis risk (conferring ~30x higher risk). By comparing the human literature directly to the genetic mouse models, we identify areas of convergence and divergence between human and animal results and highlight gaps related to differences in neurodevelopmental stages, experimental design, stimulus outcome measurements, and genetic deletion areas in each animal model. We then highlight the translational power of the acoustic startle response and how it can be studied in conjunction with more basic cellular investigations related to basic neural function and responsiveness. Because the acoustic startle response is seen across vertebrate species with well characterized circuitry, we argue for using the acoustic startle response as a translational biological probe of underlying neurobiology relevant to 22qDel and by extension, psychosis and psychosis risk.
Introduction
22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22qDel) is a chromosomal interstitial-deletion disorder that occurs in 1 in 2000–6,000 live births (Botto et al., 2003; Olsen et al., 2018; Blagojevic et al., 2021). The deletion confers risk for a variety of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders, the most documented of which are psychotic disorders. Twenty to 30 % of individuals with 22qDel develop SCZ by early adulthood, making 22qDel one of the most robust genetic predictors of schizophrenia (SCZ; Jonas et al., 2014; Olsen et al., 2018). SCZ associated with 22qDel is phenotypically comparable to idiopathic SCZ at the level of both symptoms and brain morphology (Bassett et al., 2003, 2005; Sun et al., 2020; Supekar et al., 2024; Zinkstok et al., 2019).
The acoustic startle response (ASR) is a promising translational target for studying the neurobiological pathways that connect the genetic changes in 22qDel with global symptoms in SCZ. The ASR is the reflexive contraction of skeletal musculature that is mediated by a pontine-based, three-synapse subcortical neural circuit in humans, non-human primates, and rodents on exposure to a strong acoustic stimulus (Koch, 1999). It has been studied extensively in SCZ and other psychiatric disorders. Changes in the ASR, such as greater startle latency, associate with psychosis symptoms and can predict conversion to psychosis for clinical high risk groups (Braff et al., 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2008, 2016; Cadenhead et al., 2020).
The 22q11.2 region includes several genes that might affect neuronal processing related to ASR. The PRODH, COMT, DGCR8, TBX1, ZDHHC8, and CXCR4 genes are involved in neuronal development, synaptic function, and dopaminergic, GABAergic and glutamatergic signaling (Boot et al., 2008; Squarcione et al., 2013; de Koning et al., 2015). The PRODH gene affects proline metabolism and the balance between glutamate and GABA synthesis, impacting neuronal activity and sensory processing in 22qDel. The COMT gene, affecting dopamine metabolism, indirectly influences GABAergic function by altering dopamine levels, impacting cognitive functions and sensory processing. These genes may affect ASR through their roles in neurotransmitter signaling within startle circuits, particularly PRODH’s influence on excitatory/inhibitory balance in brainstem nuclei and COMT’s effects on dopaminergic modulation of startle magnitude and habituation. The developmental roles of TBX1 and DGCR8 may also impact the proper formation of the three-synapse pontine circuit underlying the startle response.
As a promising target for translational research, the ASR has been studied in humans with 22qDel and in mouse models of 22qDel. This mini review will summarize and compare findings to date in human subjects with this deletion syndrome and mouse deletion models, and it will identify areas in need of further research.
ASR
The ASR is a fast, involuntary motor reaction to sudden and intense stimuli, and its core neural architecture is highly conserved across mammalian species (Koch, 1999; Zheng and Schmid, 2023) The response is generated from a simple, three-synapse brainstem circuit: auditory input reaches cochlear root neurons (CRNs), which project directly to giant neurons in the caudal pontine reticular nucleus (PnC), which then activate spinal motor neurons to produce the reflexive motor response (Davis et al., 1982; Lee et al., 1996; Davis, 1997; Koch, 1999; Fendt et al., 2001; Zheng and Schmid, 2023). This circuit enables startle responses within ~10–20 ms of stimulus onset depending on the species. The latency and magnitude of response depend on glutamatergic excitation, particularly via AMPA and NMDA receptors. Blocking these receptors in the PnC has been shown to reduce or abolish ASR in rodent models (Miserendino and Davis, 1993). GluA4 knockout mice that lack a key AMPA receptor subunit essentially lack ASR (García-Hernández and Rubio, 2022). GABA is implicated in prepulse inhibition (Yeomans et al., 2010) but does not directly mediate the ASR.
In humans, the ASR is studied by attaching electrodes to the face to measure the electromyographic signal from the eyeblink response elicited by the startling stimulus (Landis and Hunt, 1939). In rodents, the ASR is studied by movement as captured by a piezoelectric movement-sensitive platform.
There are two ASR variables that have been studied in 22qDel humans and 22qDel mouse models. The first is startle magnitude, which is assessed in humans by measuring the magnitude of the eyeblink response that the auditory stimulus elicits (Graham, 1975). It is measured in rodents via the animal’s movement in response to the stimulus (Hoffman and Searle, 1968; Saito et al., 2020). Human findings on the relationship between startle magnitude and SCZ are mixed; some studies have found reduced magnitude in SCZ vs. control participants (Kumari et al., 2005; Quednow et al., 2006; Minassian et al., 2007; Greenwood et al., 2010; Matsuo et al., 2016), but most studies have found no between-group difference (Braff et al., 1992, 2005; Cadenhead et al., 2000; Parwani et al., 2000; Ludewig et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2002; Wynn et al., 2004; Swerdlow et al., 2006; Takahashi et al., 2008; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Storozheva et al., 2016).
The second variable is prepulse inhibition (PPI). PPI indicates the reduction in startle magnitude that occurs when the startling sound is preceded by a non-startling tone (Hoffman and Searle, 1968; Graham, 1975). It is used to index sensorimotor gating (San-Martin et al., 2020). PPI impairment has been consistently found in non-22qDel SCZ [Braff et al., 1978, 1992; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Swerdlow and Light, 2018; Massa et al., 2020; and see reviews in Braff et al., 2001 (human studies of prepulse inhibition); Swerdlow et al., 2008; San-Martin et al., 2020] but not in first-degree relatives of individuals with SCZ (review in Li et al., 2021).
A third variable, startle latency, has been measured in human 22qDel studies but not animal studies. Startle latency is the time between the startling stimulus and the startle response. Latency serves as a marker for speed of neural processing, and SCZ is associated with slower latency in several studies (Braff et al., 1978, 1999; Swerdlow et al., 2006; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2017; Massa et al., 2020) but not all (Braff et al., 1992; Parwani et al., 2000).
ASR PPI, magnitude, and latency are highly heritable, making them attractive investigation targets for research on the genetics of SCZ (Anokhin et al., 2003; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Quednow et al., 2011; Greenwood et al., 2020; Massa et al., 2020).
The ASR is affected by many variables. Startle paradigm factors include background noise; the volume, duration, and rise-and-fall time of the startling tone; and whether a non-startling tone precedes the startling (Hoffman and Searle, 1968; Ison and Hoffman, 1983; Zheng and Schmid, 2023). Startle responses are also affected by exposure to the startling stimulus over time; responsivity can be decreased by habituation (Hoffman and Searle, 1968), where the subject acclimates to the stimulus, and increased by fear potentiation, where the startling tone is linked to an aversive stimulus (Davis et al., 1993; Grillon and Baas, 2003). In humans, ASR is affected by age, gender, menstrual cycle phase, medication status, genetic background, and smoking status (Swerdlow et al., 1997, 2006; Braff et al., 2001; Duncan et al., 2001; Ludewig et al., 2002; Jovanovic et al., 2004; Gebhardt et al., 2012; Fargotstein et al., 2018; Swerdlow and Light, 2018). In a meta-analysis, PPI was also related to education level and continent (potentially indicating a relationship between ethnicity and PPI) (San-Martin et al., 2020). Neurobiologically, startle magnitude is decreased by NMDA and non-NMDA glutamate antagonists (Miserendino and Davis, 1993) and increased by direct and indirect dopamine agonists (Kehne and Sorenson, 1978; Davis, 1980; Johansson et al., 1995; Martinez et al., 1999; Swerdlow et al., 2002).
Psychosis research has focused on several genes in the human 22q11.2 region. The catechol-O-methyl-transferase (COMT) gene and the proline dehydrogenase 1 (PRODH) gene mediate the ASR and potentially both mediate dopamine levels (Parker et al., 2024). The COMT gene encodes an enzyme critical to dopamine catabolism in the prefrontal cortex. The COMT-Val allele codes for a high-activity enzyme, and the COMT-Met allele codes for a low-activity enzyme (Paylor and Lindsay, 2006). COMT-Met associates with higher cortical dopamine and lower midbrain dopamine levels (Chen et al., 2004; Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2005). In human 22qDel studies, PPI has been lower in COMT-Met than COMT-Val (Vorstman et al., 2009; De Koning et al., 2012). Another 22q11.2 region gene, PRODH, affects proline levels, and higher proline levels can increase dopamine release in the prefrontal cortex (Stark et al., 2009; Vorstman et al., 2009). The COMT and PRODH genes may have an interaction that indicates a homeostatic response to elevated dopaminergic signaling in the frontal cortex (Paterlini et al., 2005). De Koning et al. found that COMT-Met interacted with high proline levels in 22qDel to yield further reduced startle magnitude (De Koning et al., 2015). A recent mouse paper has found that a third gene in the region syntenic to human 22q11.2, Tbx1, may selectively affect acoustic sensorimotor gating (Hiramoto et al., 2025).
ASR in 22qDel humans
Previous 22qDel research has examined ASR in children (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; Vorstman et al., 2009) as well as in adolescents and adults (De Koning et al., 2012, 2015; McCabe et al., 2014; Parker et al., 2024).
There have been mixed findings on startle magnitude in 22qDel. Most studies have found no difference in startle magnitude between 22qDel and control groups (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; Vorstman et al., 2009; De Koning et al., 2012; McCabe et al., 2014), but a more recent study found decreased startle magnitude in 22qDel subjects (Parker et al., 2024). This difference was further evidenced by the significantly higher number of 22qDel subjects classified as non-startlers (i.e., did not have a measurable blink response to at least four of the first six startling stimuli): while 25% of control subjects were non-startlers, 64.5% of 22qDel subjects were non-startlers. McCabe et al. (2014) also found a non-significant trend (p = 0.07) toward more no-response pulse trials in the 22qDel group compared to their control group, and Sobin et al. (2005b) removed three 22qDel participants from PPI analysis for being non-startlers (compared to no control participants). While de Koning et al. (2012, 2015) did not report between-group magnitude differences, they found lower startle magnitude and PPI in participants with the COMT-Met allele than the COMT-Val allele. The magnitude was further decreased in hyperprolinemic subjects (2015).
Findings on PPI in 22qDel have also been mixed, with studies reporting reduced PPI in 22qDel children (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; Vorstman et al., 2009) but not adults (De Koning et al., 2012). Sobin et al. (2005a, 2005b) report that reduced PPI in both their cohorts was largely driven by decreased PPI in 22qDel boys vs. control boys; the PPI difference in 22qDel vs. control girls was much smaller. 22qDel PPI could not be meaningfully computed in the Parker et al. cohort (2024) due to the number of prepulse trials that elicited no startle response.
Startle latency has been studied less than magnitude or PPI in human studies. Sobin et al. (2005a, 2005b) found no difference in latency between children with and without 22qDel, while Parker et al. (2024) found that latency was prolonged in adolescents and adults with 22qDel.
These studies are summarized in Table 1. The inconsistency of 22qDel ASR findings could be from differences in participant age, paradigm design, or the relatively small sample sizes of studies. Additional research will need to determine whether differences in 22qDel ASR are marked from a young age or if they emerge in the psychosis risk window from late adolescence to early adulthood.
ASR in 22qDel mouse models
Mouse models are useful for 22qDel research because mouse genomic region 16qA13 (MMU16) is thought to be syntenic to the human 22q11.2 region (Paylor and Lindsay, 2006). Mouse models used to study 22qDel vary in the size and location of the MMU16 deletion as well as in mouse genetic background. There is one strain modeling the most prevalent 3.0 mb deletion in humans (Del(3.0 Mb) in Saito et al., 2020), five strains approximating the less-common 1.5mB deletion (1. Df(16)1/+ in Lindsay et al., 1999; 2. Lgdel/+ in Merscher et al., 2001; 3. Df(16)A+/− in Stark et al., 2009; 4. Df(h22q11)/+ in Didriksen et al., 2017; 5. Del(1.5 Mb)/+ in Saito et al., 2021), and one strain modeling a 1.4 Mb deletion on a section of 22q11 that is rarely deleted on its own in humans (Del(1.4 Mb) + in Saito et al., 2021). There are also mouse lines modeling deletions of smaller sections of the 22q11 region or with single gene knockouts from the deletion region (Kimber et al., 1999; Paylor et al., 2006). This review addresses mouse models with deletions covering a substantial part of the 16qA13 region. See Tables 2, 3 for information on the mouse lines reviewed. For more detailed information about 22qDel mouse models, see Ellegood et al., 2014; Meechan et al., 2015; Paylor and Lindsay, 2006; and Saito et al., 2020, 2021. For insight into the opportunities and limits of using mouse models in the context of schizophrenia, see Villarin and Kellendonk, 2025.
As shown in Tables 4, 5, PPI has been consistently reduced in 22qDel mouse models (Paylor et al., 2001, 2006; Long et al., 2006; Stark et al., 2009; Sigurdsson, 2016; Didriksen et al., 2017; Scarborough et al., 2019; Saito et al., 2020, 2021; Tripathi et al., 2020). The only model that has been tested for PPI that has not shown reduced PPI is Del(1.4 Mb)/+, which intentionally isolates a part of the deletion region that is not ever deleted in isolation in humans (Saito et al., 2021). Conversely, ASR magnitude has been increased in most mouse models [Del(3.0 Mb)/+; Df(h22q11)/+; LgDel; Df(16)1/+], though there was no difference found in two models [Del(1.4 Mb)/+, which does not replicate human 22qDel (Saito et al., 2021); and Df(16)A+/− (Stark et al., 2009)]. With the exception of a 150 kb deletion model (Kimber et al., 1999), partial models of the deletion have either shown increased magnitude and decreased latency or no difference (Paylor et al., 2006). Kimber et al.’s model demonstrated no difference in magnitude and increased PPI. Notably, no mouse data have been published on startle response latency in 22qDel.
Discussion of 22qDel translational findings
Current ASR findings between mouse and human 22qDel research show similar overall trends, with greater internal consistency among mouse findings. PPI is reduced in all mouse models of 22qDel (Paylor et al., 2001, 2006; Long et al., 2006; Stark et al., 2009; Sigurdsson, 2016; Didriksen et al., 2017; Scarborough et al., 2019; Saito et al., 2020, 2021; Tripathi et al., 2020), and PPI in people with 22qDel has been either reduced (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; Vorstman et al., 2009) or no significant PPI difference was found (De Koning et al., 2012; McCabe et al., 2014). Startle magnitude findings have been more divergent. Most human studies showed no difference in startle magnitude between 22qDel and control groups (reduced magnitude in 22qDel in Parker et al., 2024; no difference in Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; De Koning et al., 2012; McCabe et al., 2014). While two mouse studies found no magnitude difference in 22qDel models (Stark et al., 2009; Saito et al., 2021), most found a significant startle magnitude increase in 22qDel mice (Paylor et al., 2001, 2006; Long et al., 2006; Didriksen et al., 2017; Scarborough et al., 2019; Saito et al., 2020; Tripathi et al., 2020). To our knowledge, startle latency has not yet been studied in mice.
There are several factors that could inform the inconsistencies within human data and between human and mouse studies. First, the ASR is affected by estrogen. Menstrual cycle phase significantly affects PPI in adult humans (Swerdlow et al., 1997; Jovanovic et al., 2004), and estrogen levels modulate PPI and startle latency in mice (Charitidi et al., 2012). It is thought that the estrogen effect on PPI is mediated by estrogen effects on dopaminergic and possibly serotonergic signaling (see Koch, 1998; and discussions in Swerdlow et al., 1997; Jovanovic et al., 2004). Two human studies have found that differences in PPI in 22qDel have been largely driven by decreased PPI in 22qDel boys vs. control boys (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b). In contrast, another study found greater startle latency in CHR participants who later converted to psychosis, but only in females (Cadenhead et al., 2020). Only two out of 10 experiments on 22qDel model mice have included female mice; the greater consistency in mouse results, especially PPI, could potentially be due to more stable sample estrogen levels.
The human studies have varied in participant age, which complicates cross comparison. The human startle response changes significantly through childhood, and PPI matures around age 9–10 (Gebhardt et al., 2012). Adolescent PPI changes in healthy boys are more pronounced than in healthy girls (Giannopoulos et al., 2022). PPI also changes more slowly throughout the course of adulthood (Ellwanger et al., 2003). It is unknown to what extent ASR differences between individuals with and without 22qDel may be influenced by age. At this point, the only studies reporting decreased PPI in humans with 22qDel have been on children (Sobin et al., 2005a, 2005b; Vorstman et al., 2009). Mouse age has varied across studies, though most mice have been post-puberty (except possibly Didriksen et al., 2017). Hearing loss is a 22qDel phenotype that must be addressed in mouse and human ASR research. 22qDel mouse models generally have poor hearing (Fuchs et al., 2013), and they also display delayed-onset auditory thalamocortical projections that emerge between 2 and 4 months of age (Chun et al., 2014, 2017).
Mouse ASR is also influenced by genetic background (Pietropaolo and Crusio, 2009), and the genetic background of human 22qDel carriers affects their likelihood of developing psychiatric symptoms (Bassett et al., 2017; Cleynen et al., 2021). Obviously, genetic background varies widely in human 22qDel research. While several different genetic lines have been used for 22qDel mouse models, genetic background is far more controlled, which may also explain the consistency across mouse results. Finally, studies on both humans and mice have had relatively small sample sizes (see Tables 1, 4, 5).
It is noteworthy that the increased startle magnitude observed in 22qDel mouse models does not align with startle magnitude findings in the context of psychosis. While some studies on startle magnitude in psychosis have found reduced magnitude in SCZ vs. control participants (Kumari et al., 2005; Quednow et al., 2006; Minassian et al., 2007; Matsuo et al., 2016; Greenwood et al., 2020), most have found no significant difference between groups (Braff et al., 1992, 2005; Cadenhead et al., 2000; Parwani et al., 2000; Ludewig et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2002; Wynn et al., 2004; Swerdlow et al., 2006; Takahashi et al., 2008; Hasenkamp et al., 2010; Storozheva et al., 2016). This is consistent with human 22qDel research findings, but inconsistent with the reliably increased startle magnitude in 22qDel mice models. On the other hand, the decrease in PPI found in all mouse models of 22qDel and in three of the five human 22qDel studies on PPI is consistent with PPI reduction as a SCZ endophenotype (as reviewed in Swerdlow and Light, 2018).
Finally, it is important to acknowledge the role of medication as a confounding factor in research on 22qDel patients. Because 22qDel patients are at elevated risk for a variety of psychiatric syndromes, including ADHD, they are more likely to be prescribed a variety of psychotropic medications (Fiksinski et al., 2021). This includes antipsychotics which tend to normalize startle parameters, and psychostimulants which may amplify them (Ashare et al., 2010). Protocols for research with clinical samples typically do not include restriction of prescribed medications, thus we are not aware of any published studies of 22qDel samples that include only nonmedicated patients. Further, subgrouping 22q Del patients based on medication status diminishes statistical power for detecting differences from healthy controls. While this limitation is unavoidable in human studies, it is not relevant to animal models, and likely explains the greater consistency in studies of ASR based on animal models of 22qDel.
Conclusion
Translational ASR research has advanced our understanding of the neurobiology underlying several psychiatric disorders (Davis et al., 1993; Braff et al., 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2001, 2016). The ASR can be easily studied across species using analogous paradigms. Since startle circuitry and neurochemistry have been established in animal models (Lee et al., 1996; Koch, 1999; Fendt et al., 2001; Zheng and Schmid, 2023), ASR abnormalities in human conditions can provide insight into the neurobiology of those conditions. 22qDel research matters in its own right, and, given the high risk of SCZ development that 22qDel confers, this research will provide important inroads into the neurobiology of psychosis. To date, there are areas of convergence and divergence in ASR variables between human and mouse 22qDel studies. Further translational study of the ASR in 22qDel holds promise for identifying underlying neurobiology and potential treatment targets for 22qDel and SCZ.
Author contributions
SI: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DAP: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JC: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. EFW: Writing – review & editing. ED: Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. NIH/NIMH R01MH117315 to JC and ED; NIH/NCATS TL1TR002382 to DP. ED has received funding not related to this manuscript from the Department of Veterans Affairs and the Department of Defense. ED is a full-time attending psychiatrist in the Mental Health Service Line at the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Decatur, GA. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Department of Veterans Affairs.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Anokhin, A. P., Heath, A. C., Myers, E., Ralano, A., and Wood, S. (2003). Genetic influences on prepulse inhibition of startle reflex in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 353, 45–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.09.014
Ashare, R. L., Hawk, L. W. Jr., Shiels, K., Rhodes, J. D., Pelham, W. E. Jr., and Waxmonsky, J. G. (2010). Methylphenidate enhances prepulse inhibition during processing of task-relevant stimuli in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychophysiology 47, 838–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01001.x
Bassett, A. S., Chow, E. W. C., AbdelMalik, P., Gheorghiu, M., Husted, J., and Weksberg, R. (2003). The schizophrenia phenotype in 22q11 deletion syndrome. AJP 160, 1580–1586. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1580
Bassett, A. S., Chow, E. W. C., Husted, J., Weksberg, R., Caluseriu, O., Webb, G. D., et al. (2005). Clinical features of 78 adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 138A, 307–313. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30984
Bassett, A. S., Lowther, C., Merico, D., Costain, G., Chow, E. W. C., Van Amelsvoort, T., et al. (2017). Rare genome-wide copy number variation and expression of schizophrenia in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. AJP 174, 1054–1063. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16121417
Blagojevic, C., Heung, T., Theriault, M., Tomita-Mitchell, A., Chakraborty, P., Kernohan, K., et al. (2021). Estimate of the contemporary live-birth prevalence of recurrent 22q11.2 deletions: a cross-sectional analysis from population-based newborn screening. CMAJ Open 9, E802–E809. doi: 10.9778/cmajo.20200294
Boot, E., Booij, J., Zinkstok, J., Abeling, N., de Haan, L., Baas, F., et al. (2008). Disrupted dopaminergic neurotransmission in 22q11 deletion syndrome. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 1252–1258. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301508
Botto, L. D., May, K., Fernhoff, P. M., Correa, A., Coleman, K., Rasmussen, S. A., et al. (2003). A population-based study of the 22q11.2 deletion: phenotype, incidence, and contribution to major birth defects in the population. Pediatrics 112, 101–107. doi: 10.1542/peds.112.1.101
Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology 156, 234–258. doi: 10.1007/s002130100810
Braff, D. L., Grillon, C., and Geyer, M. A. (1992). Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 206–215.
Braff, D. L., Light, G. A., Ellwanger, J., Sprock, J., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2005). Female schizophrenia patients have prepulse inhibition deficits. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 817–820. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.030
Braff, D., Stone, C., Callaway, E., Geyer, M., Glick, I., and Bali, L. (1978). Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in Normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology 15, 339–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01390.x
Braff, D. L., Swerdlow, N. R., and Geyer, M. A. (1999). Symptom correlates of prepulse inhibition deficits in male schizophrenic patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 596–602.
Cadenhead, K. S., Duncan, E., Addington, J., Bearden, C., Cannon, T. D., Cornblatt, B. A., et al. (2020). Evidence of slow neural processing, developmental differences and sensitivity to Cannabis effects in a sample at clinical high risk for psychosis from the NAPLS consortium assessed with the human startle paradigm. Front. Psychol. 11:833. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00833
Cadenhead, K. S., Swerdlow, N. R., Shafer, K. M., Diaz, M., and Braff, D. L. (2000). Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. AJP 157, 1660–1668. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1660
Charitidi, K., Meltser, I., and Canlon, B. (2012). Estradiol treatment and hormonal fluctuations during the estrous cycle modulate the expression of estrogen receptors in the auditory system and the prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response. Endocrinology 153, 4412–4421. doi: 10.1210/en.2012-1416
Chen, J., Lipska, B. K., Halim, N., Ma, Q. D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., et al. (2004). Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 75, 807–821. doi: 10.1086/425589
Chun, S., Du, F., Westmoreland, J. J., Han, S. B., Wang, Y.-D., Eddins, D., et al. (2017). Thalamic miR-338-3p mediates auditory thalamocortical disruption and its late onset in models of 22q11.2 microdeletion. Nat. Med. 23, 39–48. doi: 10.1038/nm.4240
Chun, S., Westmoreland, J. J., Bayazitov, I. T., Eddins, D., Pani, A. K., Smeyne, R. J., et al. (2014). Specific disruption of thalamic inputs to the auditory cortex in schizophrenia models. Science 344, 1178–1182. doi: 10.1126/science.1253895
Cleynen, I., Engchuan, W., Hestand, M. S., Heung, T., Holleman, A. M., Johnston, H. R., et al. (2021). Genetic contributors to risk of schizophrenia in the presence of a 22q11.2 deletion. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 4496–4510. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0654-3
Davis, M. (1980). Neurochemical modulation of sensory-motor reactivity: acoustic and tactile startle reflexes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4, 241–263.
Davis, M. (1997). “The neurophysiological basis of acoustic startle modulation: research on fear motivation and sensory gating” in Attention and orienting. Eds. P. Lang, R. Simons, & M. Balaban. (Psychology Press).
Davis, M., Falls, W. A., Campeau, S., and Kim, M. (1993). Fear-potentiated startle: a neural and pharmacological analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 58, 175–198.
Davis, M., Gendelman, D., Tischler, M., and Gendelman, P. (1982). A primary acoustic startle circuit: lesion and stimulation studies. J. Neurosci. 2, 791–805. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-06-00791.1982
De Koning, M. B., Boot, E., Bloemen, O. J., Van Duin, E. D., Abel, K. M., De Haan, L., et al. (2012). Startle reactivity and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response are modulated by catechol- O -methyl-transferase Val 158 met polymorphism in adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome. J. Psychopharmacol. 26, 1548–1560. doi: 10.1177/0269881112456610
De Koning, M. B., Van Duin, E. D. A., Boot, E., Bloemen, O. J. N., Bakker, J. A., Abel, K. M., et al. (2015). PRODH rs450046 and proline x COMT Val158Met interaction effects on intelligence and startle in adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome. Psychopharmacology 232, 3111–3122. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3971-5
Didriksen, M., Fejgin, K., Nilsson, S. R. O., Birknow, M. R., Grayton, H. M., Larsen, P. H., et al. (2017). Persistent gating deficit and increased sensitivity to NMDA receptor antagonism after puberty in a new mouse model of the human 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: a study in male mice. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 42, 48–58. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150381
Duncan, E., Madonick, S., Chakravorty, S., Parwani, A., Szilagyi, S., Efferen, T., et al. (2001). Effects of smoking on acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in humans. Psychopharmacology 156, 266–272. doi: 10.1007/s002130100719
Ellegood, J., Markx, S., Lerch, J. P., Steadman, P. E., Genç, C., Provenzano, F., et al. (2014). Neuroanatomical phenotypes in a mouse model of the 22q11.2 microdeletion. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 99–107. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.112
Ellwanger, J., Geyer, M. A., and Braff, D. L. (2003). The relationship of age to prepulse inhibition and habituation of the acoustic startle response. Biol. Psychol. 62, 175–195. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00126-6
Fargotstein, M., Hasenkamp, W., Gross, R., Cuthbert, B., Green, A., Swails, L., et al. (2018). The effect of antipsychotic medications on acoustic startle latency in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 198, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.030
Fendt, M., Li, L., and Yeomans, J. S. (2001). Brain stem circuits mediating prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Psychopharmacology 156, 216–224. doi: 10.1007/s002130100794
Fiksinski, A. M., Schneider, M., Zinkstok, J., Baribeau, D., Chawner, S. J. R. A., and Vorstman, J. A. S. (2021). Neurodevelopmental trajectories and psychiatric morbidity: lessons learned from the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 23:13. doi: 10.1007/s11920-021-01225-z
Fuchs, J. C., Zinnamon, F. A., Taylor, R. R., Ivins, S., Scambler, P. J., Forge, A., et al. (2013). Hearing loss in a mouse model of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. PLoS One 8:e80104. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080104
García-Hernández, S., and Rubio, M. E. (2022). Role of GluA4 in the acoustic and tactile startle responses. Hear. Res. 414:108410. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2021.108410
Gebhardt, J., Schulz-Juergensen, S., and Eggert, P. (2012). Maturation of prepulse inhibition (PPI) in childhood. Psychophysiology 49, 484–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01323.x
Giannopoulos, A. E., Zioga, I., Papageorgiou, P., Pervanidou, P., Makris, G., Chrousos, G. P., et al. (2022). Evaluating the modulation of the acoustic startle reflex in children and adolescents via vertical EOG and EEG: sex, age, and behavioral effects. Front. Neurosci. 16:798667. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.798667
Graham, F. (1975). The more or less startling effects of weak prestimuli. Psychophysiology 12, 238–248.
Greenwood, A. K., Peichel, C. L., and Zottoli, S. J. (2010). Distinct startle responses are associated with neuroanatomical differences in pufferfishes. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 613–620. doi: 10.1242/jeb.037085
Greenwood, T. A., Swerdlow, N. R., Sprock, J., Calkins, M. E., Freedman, R., Green, M. F., et al. (2020). Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude and latency from the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 224, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.11.003
Grillon, C., and Baas, J. (2003). A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application in psychiatry. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 1557–1579. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00202-5
Hasenkamp, W., Epstein, M. P., Green, A., Wilcox, L., Boshoven, W., Lewison, B., et al. (2010). Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Res. 178, 236–243. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.11.012
Hiramoto, T., Sumiyoshi, A., Kato, R., Yamauchi, T., Takano, T., Kang, G., et al. (2025). Highly demarcated structural alterations in the brain and impaired social incentive learning in Tbx1 heterozygous mice. Mol. Psychiatry 30, 1876–1886. doi: 10.1038/s41380-024-02797-x
Hoffman, H. S., and Searle, J. L. (1968). Acoustic and temporal factors in the evocation of startle. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 43, 269–282.
Ison, J. R., and Hoffman, H. S. (1983). Reflex modification in the domain of startle: II. The anomalous history of a robust and ubiquitous phenomenon. Psychol. Bull. 94, 3–17.
Johansson, C., Jackson, D. M., Zhang, J., and Svensson, L. (1995). Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle, a measure of sensorimotor gating: effects of antipsychotics and other agents in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 52, 649–654.
Jonas, R. K., Montojo, C. A., and Bearden, C. E. (2014). The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome as a window into complex neuropsychiatric disorders over the lifespan. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 351–360. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.019
Jovanovic, T., Szilagyi, S., Chakravorty, S., Fiallos, A. M., Lewison, B. J., Parwani, A., et al. (2004). Menstrual cycle phase effects on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Psychophysiology 41, 401–406. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00166.x
Kehne, J. H., and Sorenson, C. A. (1978). The effects of pimozide and phenoxybenzamine pretreatments on amphetamine and apomorphine potentiation of the acoustic startle response in rats. Psychopharmacology 58, 137–144. doi: 10.1007/BF00426896
Kimber, W. L., Hsieh, P., Hirotsune, S., Yuva-Paylor, L., Sutherland, H. F., Chen, A., et al. (1999). Deletion of 150 kb in the minimal DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome critical region in mouse. Hum. Mol. Genet. 8, 2229–2237.
Koch, M. (1998). Sensorimotor gating changes across the estrous cycle in female rats. Physiol. Behav. 64, 625–628.
Kumari, V., Das, M., Zachariah, E., Ettinger, U., and Sharma, T. (2005). Reduced prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Psychophysiology 42, 588–594. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00346.x
Lee, Y., López, D. E., Meloni, E. G., and Davis, M. (1996). A primary acoustic startle pathway: obligatory role of Cochlear root neurons and the nucleus reticularis Pontis Caudalis. J. Neurosci. 16, 3775–3789.
Li, W., Mao, Z., Bo, Q., Sun, Y., Wang, Z., and Wang, C. (2021). Prepulse inhibition in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a systematic review. Early Interv. Psychiatry 15, 652–661. doi: 10.1111/eip.13003
Lindsay, E. A., Botta, A., Jurecic, V., Carattini-Rivera, S., Cheah, Y.-C., Rosenblatt, H. M., et al. (1999). Congenital heart disease in mice deficient for the DiGeorge syndrome region. Nature 401, 379–383. doi: 10.1038/43900
Lindsay, E. A., Vitelli, F., Su, H., Morishima, M., Huynh, T., Pramparo, T., et al. (2001). Tbx1 haploinsufficiency in the DiGeorge syndrome region causes aortic arch defects in mice. Nature 410, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/35065105
Long, J. M., LaPorte, P., Merscher, S., Funke, B., Saint-Jore, B., Puech, A., et al. (2006). Behavior of mice with mutations in the conserved region deleted in velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome. Neurogenetics 7, 247–257. doi: 10.1007/s10048-006-0054-0
Ludewig, K., Geyer, M. A., Etzensberger, M., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2002). Stability of the acoustic startle reflex, prepulse inhibition, and habituation in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 55, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00198-0
Martinez, Z. A., Ellison, G. D., Geyer, M. A., and Swerdlow, N. R. (1999). Effects of sustained cocaine exposure on sensorimotor gating of startle in rats. Psychopharmacology 142, 253–260.
Massa, N., Owens, A. V., Harmon, W., Bhattacharya, A., Ivleva, E. I., Keedy, S., et al. (2020). Relationship of prolonged acoustic startle latency to diagnosis and biotype in the bipolar-schizophrenia network on intermediate phenotypes (B–SNIP) cohort. Schizophr. Res. 216, 357–366. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.11.013
Matsuo, J., Ota, M., Hori, H., Hidese, S., Teraishi, T., Ishida, I., et al. (2016). A large single ethnicity study of prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia: separate analysis by sex focusing on effect of symptoms. J. Psychiatr. Res. 82, 155–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.07.026
McCabe, K. L., Atkinson, R. J., Cooper, G., Melville, J. L., Harris, J., Schall, U., et al. (2014). Pre-pulse inhibition and antisaccade performance indicate impaired attention modulation of cognitive inhibition in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS). J. Neurodev. Disord. 6:38. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-38
Meechan, D. W., Maynard, T. M., Tucker, E. S., Fernandez, A., Karpinski, B. A., Rothblat, L. A., et al. (2015). Modeling a model: mouse genetics, 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, and disorders of cortical circuit development. Prog. Neurobiol. 130, 1–28. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.004
Merscher, S., Funke, B., Epstein, J. A., Heyer, J., Puech, A., Lu, M. M., et al. (2001). TBX1 is responsible for cardiovascular defects in Velo-cardio-facial/DiGeorge syndrome. Cell 104, 619–629. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00247-1
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Kohn, P. D., Kolachana, B., Kippenhan, S., McInerney-Leo, A., Nussbaum, R., et al. (2005). Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 594–596. doi: 10.1038/nn1438
Minassian, A., Feifel, D., and Perry, W. (2007). The relationship between sensorimotor gating and clinical improvement in acutely ill schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 89, 225–231. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.006
Miserendino, M. J. D., and Davis, M. (1993). NMDA and non-NMDA antagonists infused into the nucleus reticularis pontis caudalis depress the acoustic startle reflex. Brain Res. 623, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91430-Z
Olsen, L., Sparsø, T., Weinsheimer, S. M., Dos Santos, M. B. Q., Mazin, W., Rosengren, A., et al. (2018). Prevalence of rearrangements in the 22q11.2 region and population-based risk of neuropsychiatric and developmental disorders in a Danish population: a case-cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 573–580. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30168-8
Parker, D. A., Imes, S., Ruban, G., Ousley, O. Y., Henshey, B., Massa, N. M., et al. (2024). Reduced amplitude and slowed latency of the acoustic startle response in adolescents and adults with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Schizophr. Res. 269, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2024.04.022
Parwani, A., Duncan, E. J., Bartlett, E., Madonick, S. H., Efferen, T. R., Rajan, R., et al. (2000). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 662–669. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00148-1
Paterlini, M., Zakharenko, S. S., Lai, W.-S., Qin, J., Zhang, H., Mukai, J., et al. (2005). Transcriptional and behavioral interaction between 22q11.2 orthologs modulates schizophrenia-related phenotypes in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1586–1594. doi: 10.1038/nn1562
Paylor, R., Glaser, B., Mupo, A., Ataliotis, P., Spencer, C., Sobotka, A., et al. (2006). Tbx1 haploinsufficiency is linked to behavioral disorders in mice and humans: implications for 22q11 deletion syndrome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 7729–7734. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600206103
Paylor, R., and Lindsay, E. (2006). Mouse models of 22q11 deletion syndrome. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 1172–1179. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.01.018
Paylor, R., McIlwain, K. L., McAninch, R., Nellis, A., Yuva-Paylor, L. A., Baldini, A., et al. (2001). Mice deleted for the DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome region show abnormal sensorimotor gating and learning and memory impairments. Hum. Mol. Genet. 10, 2645–2650. doi: 10.1093/hmg/10.23.2645
Perry, W., Feifel, D., Minassian, A., Bhattacharjie, I., and Braff, D. L. (2002). Information processing deficits in acutely psychotic schizophrenia patients medicated and Unmedicated at the time of admission. AJP 159, 1375–1381. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1375
Pietropaolo, S., and Crusio, W. E. (2009). Strain-dependent changes in acoustic startle response and its plasticity across adolescence in mice. Behav. Genet. 39, 623–631. doi: 10.1007/s10519-009-9291-y
Quednow, B. B., Ettinger, U., Mössner, R., Rujescu, D., Giegling, I., Collier, D. A., et al. (2011). The schizophrenia risk allele C of the TCF4 rs9960767 polymorphism disrupts sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia Spectrum and healthy volunteers. J. Neurosci. 31, 6684–6691. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0526-11.2011
Quednow, B. B., Wagner, M., Westheide, J., Beckmann, K., Bliesener, N., Maier, W., et al. (2006). Sensorimotor gating and habituation of the startle response in schizophrenic patients randomly treated with Amisulpride or olanzapine. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 536–545. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.012
Saito, R., Koebis, M., Nagai, T., Shimizu, K., Liao, J., Wulaer, B., et al. (2020). Comprehensive analysis of a novel mouse model of the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: a model with the most common 3.0-Mb deletion at the human 22q11.2 locus. Transl. Psychiatry 10:35. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0723-z
Saito, R., Miyoshi, C., Koebis, M., Kushima, I., Nakao, K., Mori, D., et al. (2021). Two novel mouse models mimicking minor deletions in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome revealed the contribution of each deleted region to psychiatric disorders. Mol. Brain 14:68. doi: 10.1186/s13041-021-00778-7
San-Martin, R., Castro, L. A., Menezes, P. R., Fraga, F. J., Simões, P. W., and Salum, C. (2020). Meta-analysis of sensorimotor gating deficits in patients with schizophrenia evaluated by Prepulse inhibition test. Schizophr. Bull. 46, 1482–1497. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa059
Scarborough, J., Mueller, F., Weber-Stadlbauer, U., Richetto, J., and Meyer, U. (2019). Dependency of prepulse inhibition deficits on baseline startle reactivity in a mouse model of the human 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome. Genes Brain Behav. 18:e12523. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12523
Sigurdsson, T. (2016). Neural circuit dysfunction in schizophrenia: insights from animal models. Neuroscience 321, 42–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.059
Smith, A. K., Jovanovic, T., Kilaru, V., Lori, A., Gensler, L., Lee, S. S., et al. (2017). A gene-based analysis of acoustic startle latency. Front. Psychol. 8:117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00117
Sobin, C., Kiley-Brabeck, K., and Karayiorgou, M. (2005a). Associations between prepulse inhibition and executive visual attention in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 553–562. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001609
Sobin, C., Kiley-Brabeck, K., and Karayiorgou, M. (2005b). Lower Prepulse inhibition in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. AJP 162, 1090–1099. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1090
Squarcione, C., Torti, M. C., Di Fabio, F., and Biondi, M. (2013). 22q11 deletion syndrome: a review of the neuropsychiatric features and their neurobiological basis. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 9, 1873–1884. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S52188
Stark, K. L., Burt, R. A., Gogos, J. A., and Karayiorgou, M. (2009). Analysis of prepulse inhibition in mouse lines overexpressing 22q11.2 orthologues. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 983–989. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709000492
Storozheva, Z. I., Kirenskaya, A. V., Novototsky-Vlasov, V. Y., Telesheva, K. Y., and Pletnikov, M. (2016). Startle modification and P50 gating in schizophrenia patients and controls: Russian population. Span. J. Psychol. 19:E8. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2016.1
Sun, D., Ching, C. R. K., Lin, A., Forsyth, J. K., Kushan, L., Vajdi, A., et al. (2020). Large-scale mapping of cortical alterations in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: convergence with idiopathic psychosis and effects of deletion size. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 1822–1834. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0078-5
Supekar, K., de Los Angeles, C., Ryali, S., Kushan, L., Schleifer, C., Repetto, G., et al. (2024). Robust and replicable functional brain signatures of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and associated psychosis: a deep neural network-based multi-cohort study. Mol. Psychiatry 29, 2951–2966. doi: 10.1038/s41380-024-02495-8
Swerdlow, N. R., Braff, D. L., and Geyer, M. A. (2016). Sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: what we said 25 years ago, what has happened since then, and what comes next. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1072–1081. doi: 10.1177/0269881116661075
Swerdlow, N., Eastvold, A., Karban, B., Ploum, Y., Stephany, N., Geyer, M., et al. (2002). Dopamine agonist effects on startle and sensorimotor gating in normal male subjects: time course studies. Psychopharmacology 161, 189–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1040-3
Swerdlow, N., Geyer, M., and Braff, D. (2001). Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology 156, 194–215. doi: 10.1007/s002130100799
Swerdlow, N. R., Hartman, P. L., and Auerbach, P. P. (1997). Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual cycle: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 41, 452–460.
Swerdlow, N. R., and Light, G. A. (2018). Sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: advancing our understanding of the phenotype, its neural circuitry and genetic substrates. Schizophr. Res. 198, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.02.042
Swerdlow, N. R., Light, G. A., Cadenhead, K. S., Sprock, J., Hsieh, M. H., and Braff, D. L. (2006). Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325
Swerdlow, N. R., Weber, M., Qu, Y., Light, G. A., and Braff, D. L. (2008). Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology 199, 331–388. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1072-4
Takahashi, H., Iwase, M., Ishii, R., Ohi, K., Fukumoto, M., Azechi, M., et al. (2008). Impaired prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in Japanese patients with schizophrenia. Neurosci. Res. 62, 187–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.08.006
Tripathi, A., Spedding, M., Schenker, E., Didriksen, M., Cressant, A., and Jay, T. M. (2020). Cognition- and circuit-based dysfunction in a mouse model of 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: effects of stress. Transl. Psychiatry 10:41. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0687-z
Villarin, J. M., and Kellendonk, C. (2025). An ace in the hole? Opportunities and limits of using mice to understand schizophrenia neurobiology. Mol. Psychiatry, 1–5. doi: 10.1038/s41380-025-03060-7
Vorstman, J. A., Turetsky, B. I., and Sijmens-Morcus, M. E. (2009). Proline affects brain function in 22q11.2DS children with the low activity COMT 158 allele. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 739–746. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.132
Wynn, J. K., Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., McGee, M., Salveson, D., and Green, M. F. (2004). Prepulse facilitation and prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 518–523. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.018
Yeomans, J. S., Bosch, D., Alves, N., Daros, A., Ure, R. J., and Schmid, S. (2010). GABA receptors and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in mice and rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 2053–2061. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07236.x
Zheng, A., and Schmid, S. (2023). A review of the neural basis underlying the acoustic startle response with a focus on recent developments in mammals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 148:105129. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105129
Keywords: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, acoustic startle response, psychosis risk, genetic high risk for psychosis, psychophysiology
Citation: Imes S, Parker DA, Chen M, Cubells JF, Walker EF and Duncan E (2025) The acoustic startle response in 22q11 deletion syndrome: from animal models to humans. Front. Neurosci. 19:1630109. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1630109
Edited by:
Ioanna Zioga, Radboud University, NetherlandsReviewed by:
Munekazu Komada, Kindai University, JapanCopyright © 2025 Imes, Parker, Chen, Cubells, Walker and Duncan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sidney Imes, c2ltZXNAZW1vcnkuZWR1