- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, United States
Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) are relatively newly discovered photoreceptors other than rods and cones. For the last decade, people have been considered ipRGCs to be primarily in charge of non-image-forming and cognitive functions. However, an increasing body of evidence has pointed out that ipRGCs also play a role in visual processing, such as contrast, brightness and color perception. In this mini-review, I listed what the caveats about those studies discussing how ipRGCs affect cognitive functions and how ipRGCs serve as image-forming functions under well-controlled condition.
Introduction
Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) are relatively newly discovered photoreceptors other than rods and cones. They are characterized by the photopigment—melanopsin, which makes them sensitive to light wavelength around 480 nm (Allen and Baño-Otálora, 2022; Do and Yau, 2010; Pickard and Sollars, 2011; Provencio et al., 1998; Provencio et al., 2000). Over the last decade, abundant of studies have proposed how ipRGCs serve as a “non-image-forming” photoreceptor and do not involve in visual functions (see Mahoney and Schmidt, 2024; Meng et al., 2025 for review). However, recently, increasing evidence obtained under better controlled light conditions suggests that ipRGCs contribute to human visual perception as well.
Blue light, which ipRGCs are most sensitive to, has been indicated to delay circadian rhythm (Chang et al., 2015; Daneault et al., 2016; Vandewalle et al., 2006), increase alertness (Beaven and Ekström, 2013; Phipps-Nelson et al., 2009; Souman et al., 2018), improve working memory (Suzuki et al., 2025; Vandewalle et al., 2013; Vandewalle et al., 2007; Vandewalle et al., 2009), improve task switching ability (Ferlazzo et al., 2014; but see Lee et al., 2021 showing no impacts), enhance creativity (Abdullah et al., 2016), expand time perception (Yang et al., 2018) and other cognitive functions (see Mahoney and Schmidt, 2024; Meng et al., 2025 for review). However, effects from blue light do not necessarily indicate that they originate from ipRGCs, especially when studies examining cognitive functions did not properly control luminance, background colors, and cone activation levels (Lucas et al., 2014; Mahoney and Schmidt, 2024; Yang et al., 2023). Specifically, most studies used different background light types (e.g., green or orange light, Chen and Yeh, 2019; Lee and Yeh, 2021) while failing to control luminance and the stimulation levels of cones and ipRGCs (Yang et al., 2023). This can possibly be a reason for mixed findings in several studies. A better way to control luminance would be using flicker photometry so that when the target color (i.e., blue) and the control color (e.g., green or orange) alternate at a frequency of around 10–20 Hz, the parvocellular pathway (P-pathway), which is primarily responsible for colors and sustained spatial response, will be suppressed (silenced). In the meantime, the magnocellular pathway (M-pathway) is in charge of the luminance and brightness for the two light sources, minimizing the perception of flicker and leads to identical luminance of the target and control colors (Bone and Landrum, 2004; Lee et al., 1988).
The techniques for controlling the background lights have significantly improved recently: By using silent substitution to create a metamer of the control light and stimulating ipRGCs with high versus low energy levels, researchers were able to dissociate the effects of cones and ipRGCs by using four-primary (e.g., Chien et al., 2020; Chien et al., 2023; Horiguchi et al., 2013; Tsujimura et al., 2010; Woelders et al., 2023; Yamakawa et al., 2019; Yang et al., 2023; Yang et al., 2018; Zele et al., 2018a) or five-primary lights (e.g., Cao et al., 2015; Uprety et al., 2022; Zele et al., 2018b), depending on the luminance level and if rods have saturated at the photopic level. Indeed, compared to studies examining the effects of ipRGCs on cognitive functions which have several caveats, studies examining the effects of ipRGCs on animal vision (e.g., Aranda and Schmidt, 2021; Barrionuevo and Cao, 2019; Hu et al., 2022; Patterson et al., 2020; Schmidt et al., 2011; Shi et al., 2025) and low-level human vision (e.g., Chien et al., 2023; Spitschan et al., 2017; Uprety et al., 2022) are under better controlled conditions.
ipRGCs contribute to color vision. More evidence has pointed out that ipRGCs are involved in color threshold (Barrionuevo and Cao, 2019) and color processing (Zele et al., 2018b), (see Barrionuevo et al., 2024) for review but Woelders et al. (2023) showing no effects of ipRGCs on color processing. If ipRGCs are changing human color vision, this could change how the trichromatic theory of color vision – the fundamental theory describing how three types of cones in the human retina can modulate the perception of a full range of colors – is mathematically computed, and the color matching functions should take this into account when examining the relations between cones and the spectral power distribution (SPD). Indeed, an experiment has revealed that traditional trichotomy can only explain perceptual sensitivity at the fovea but not in the periphery, because ipRGCs are primarily distributed in the periphery, thereby modulating the visibility there which was not account by the traditional trichotomy (Horiguchi et al., 2013). Future studies should carefully examine trichromatic theory of color vision at different eccentricities while taking ipRGCs’ distribution into consideration.
In addition to color vision, ipRGCs also contribute to contrast (Chien et al., 2023; Schmidt et al., 2014; Zele et al., 2019) and brightness perception (Besenecker et al., 2016; Brown et al., 2012; Cao et al., 2018; DeLawyer et al., 2020; Joyce et al., 2022; Lucas et al., 2020; Yamakawa et al., 2019; Zele et al., 2018b), but see Vincent et al. (2021) showing that ipRGCs do not impact luminance detection. For example, two blind patients without outer retina could report brightness percept and had pupillary light response when given short-wavelength light stimuli (Zaidi et al., 2007), indicating that rods and cones are not the only determining factors mediating visual processing. Indeed, Nugent and Zele (2024) have shown that ipRGCs alone can produce visual percept in response to both spatial and temporal patterns. Additionally, by using silent substitution, Chien et al. (2023) have shown that increased ipRGCs stimulation led to a higher contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies, and this effect varied across eccentricities according to the distribution of ipRGCs on the retina. Animal models also provided supporting evidence to this finding given that mice lacking melanopsin showed deficits in contrast sensitivity (Schmidt et al., 2014). Recently, Shi et al. (2025) also demonstrated that ipRGC activation can enhance the orientation selectivity in the primary visual cortex of mice by increasing preferred-orientation responses and narrowing tuning bandwidth.
Why do ipRGCs play a role in contrast, brightness, and color perception? The visual pathways mediated by ipRGCs are still perplexed (Joyce et al., 2022). Despite this, mice lacking rods and cones could still do the light detection task through melanopsin (Ecker et al., 2010). Animal models have shown that ipRGCs project extensively to the superior colliculus (SC) and dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN), both of which play significant roles in visual perception rather than solely mediating non-image-forming functions (Allen and Baño-Otálora, 2022; Ellis et al., 2016).
For human, superior colliculus can process light information bypassing the cortical pathways and plays a critical role in controlling human eye movements (Binns, 1999; Liu X. et al., 2022; Manger, 2020). Interestingly, in human studies, melanopsin can stimulate the human homolog of frontal eye fields (Hung et al., 2017), which also engage in eye movements planning (Grosbras et al., 2005; Paus, 1996; Rivaud et al., 1994) and endogenous attention (Fernández et al., 2023). The neuroimaging evidence pointed out that ipRGCs are sending signals to neural substrates which are directly or indirectly connected to visual processing (Hung et al., 2017). More neuroimaging and modeling work with good quality of control are needed to verify ipRGCs’ mechanisms between brain structures and how they are shaping human low-level vision.
Contrary to previous arguments that ipRGCs are primarily in charge of non-image-forming functions (e.g., Mahoney and Schmidt, 2024; Meng et al., 2025), I argued that a variety of studies examining the effects of ipRGCs on cognition have serious caveats in controlling the light conditions. For example, due to the sluggishness and the receptive field properties of ipRGCs (Procyk et al., 2015), when examining their effects on cognition, researchers should carefully control the background lights (Mahoney and Schmidt, 2024), stimuli location (Barrionuevo et al., 2024), environmental lights (Webster et al., 2007) and the adaptation time needed to stimulate ipRGCs (Procyk et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2023). In vision studies, these factors are usually well-controlled, and ipRGCs undoubtfully play a role in human visual processing, such as contrast, brightness, and color processing (Allen et al., 2019; Joyce et al., 2022; Lucas et al., 2020). It is unknown, however, whether and how much are people metacognitively aware of the effects of ipRGCs on their cognition and perception (Cheng et al., 2023). Future studies could also consider how ipRGCs affect mid-level visual tasks, such as crowding, texture segregation, object localization, given the closely relationship between low- and mid-level visual processing in humans (Anderson, 2020; Jennings and Martinovic, 2014; Jones et al., 1997). Additionally, understanding how animal models’ findings can be applied to humans could further clarify the evolutionary conservation of visual and non-visual pathways, revealing their functional relevance in human physiology and behaviors (Emanuel and Do, 2015; Liu A. L. et al., 2022; Tünçok et al., 2025). Most importantly, whether and how the image-forming and non-image-forming functions of ipRGCs interact could further unveil how human visual and cognitive functions integrated to support our daily lives.
Author contributions
H-HL: Project administration, Conceptualization, Writing – review and editing, Investigation, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
I thank Pei-Ling Yang for helpful feedback and comments on the manuscript.
Conflict of interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author declares that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Abdullah, S., Czerwinski, M., Mark, G., and Johns, P. (2016). “Shining (blue) light on creative ability,” in Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing, (Heidelberg).
Allen, A. E., and Baño-Otálora, B. (2022). Slow vision: Measuring melanopsin-mediated light effects in animal models. Prog. Brain Res. 273, 117–143. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2022.04.009
Allen, A. E., Martial, F. P., and Lucas, R. J. (2019). Form vision from melanopsin in humans. Nat. Commun. 10:2274. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.11.088
Aranda, M. L., and Schmidt, T. M. (2021). Diversity of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: Circuits and functions. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 78, 889–907. doi: 10.1007/s00018-020-03641-5
Barrionuevo, P. A., Salinas, M. L. S., and Fanchini, J. M. (2024). Are ipRGCs involved in human color vision? Hints from physiology, psychophysics, and natural image statistics. Vis. Res. 217:108378. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108378
Barrionuevo, P., and Cao, D. (2019). “Does melanopsin help to explain color constancy in natural environments?,” in Proceedings of the International Color Association (AIC) Conference, (Taipei).
Beaven, C. M., and Ekström, J. (2013). A comparison of blue light and caffeine effects on cognitive function and alertness in humans. PLoS One 8:e76707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076707
Besenecker, U., Bullough, J., and Radetsky, L. C. (2016). Spectral sensitivity and scene brightness at low to moderate photopic light levels. Light. Res. Technol. 48, 676–688. doi: 10.1177/1477153515575767
Binns, K. (1999). The synaptic pharmacology underlying sensory processing in the superior colliculus. Prog. Neurobiol. 59, 129–159. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00099-9
Bone, R. A., and Landrum, J. T. (2004). Heterochromatic flicker photometry. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 430, 137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.abb.2004.04.003
Brown, T. M., Tsujimura, S.-I., Allen, A. E., Wynne, J., Bedford, R., Vickery, G., et al. (2012). Melanopsin-based brightness discrimination in mice and humans. Curr. Biol. 22, 1134–1141. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.039
Cao, D., Chang, A., and Gai, S. (2018). Evidence for an impact of melanopsin activation on unique white perception. J. Optic. Soc. A A 35, B287–B291. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.35.00B287
Cao, D., Nicandro, N., and Barrionuevo, P. A. (2015). A five-primary photostimulator suitable for studying intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell functions in humans. J. Vis. 15:27. doi: 10.1167/15.1.27
Chang, A.-M., Aeschbach, D., Duffy, J. F., and Czeisler, C. A. (2015). Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 1232–1237. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418490112
Chen, H.-W., and Yeh, S.-L. (2019). Effects of blue light on dynamic vision. Front. Psychol. 10:497. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00497
Cheng, T., Chiu, L., Huang, L., Lin, Y.-T., Lee, H.-H., Chen, Y.-C., et al. (2023). “Predictive processing in the “Second Brain”: From gut complex to meta-awareness,” in Expected Experiences, eds. T. Cheng, R. Sato, and J. Hohwy (London: Routledge), 170–194.
Chien, S.-E., Chen, Y.-C., Matsumoto, A., Yamashita, W., Shih, K.-T., Tsujimura, S.-I., et al. (2020). The modulation of background color on perceiving audiovisual simultaneity. Vis. Res. 172, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.04.009
Chien, S.-E., Yeh, S.-L., Yamashita, W., and Tsujimura, S.-I. (2023). Enhanced human contrast sensitivity with increased stimulation of melanopsin in intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Vis. Res. 209:108271. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2023.108271
Daneault, V., Dumont, M., Massé, É, Vandewalle, G., and Carrier, J. (2016). Light-sensitive brain pathways and aging. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 35:9. doi: 10.1186/s40101-016-0091-9
DeLawyer, T., Tsujimura, S.-I., and Shinomori, K. (2020). Relative contributions of melanopsin to brightness discrimination when hue and luminance also vary. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 37, A81–A88. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.382349
Do, M. T. H., and Yau, K.-W. (2010). Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Physiol. Rev. 90, 1547–1581. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00013.2010
Ecker, J. L., Dumitrescu, O. N., Wong, K. Y., Alam, N. M., Chen, S.-K., LeGates, T., et al. (2010). Melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion-cell photoreceptors: Cellular diversity and role in pattern vision. Neuron 67, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.023
Ellis, E. M., Gauvain, G., Sivyer, B., and Murphy, G. J. (2016). Shared and distinct retinal input to the mouse superior colliculus and dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 602–610. doi: 10.1152/jn.00227.2016
Emanuel, A. J., and Do, M. T. H. (2015). Melanopsin tristability for sustained and broadband phototransduction. Neuron 85, 1043–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.011
Ferlazzo, F., Piccardi, L., Burattini, C., Barbalace, M., Giannini, A. M., and Bisegna, F. (2014). Effects of new light sources on task switching and mental rotation performance. J. Environ. Psychol. 39, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.03.005
Fernández, A., Hanning, N. M., and Carrasco, M. (2023). Transcranial magnetic stimulation to frontal but not occipital cortex disrupts endogenous attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 120:e2219635120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2219635120
Grosbras, M. H., Laird, A. R., and Paus, T. (2005). Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 140–154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20145
Horiguchi, H., Winawer, J., Dougherty, R. F., and Wandell, B. A. (2013). Human trichromacy revisited. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, E260–E269. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214240110
Hu, J., Shi, Y., Zhang, J., Huang, X., Wang, Q., Zhao, H., et al. (2022). Melanopsin retinal ganglion cells mediate light-promoted brain development. Cell 185, 3124–3137. e15. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2022.07.009
Hung, S.-M., Milea, D., Rukmini, A. V., Najjar, R. P., Tan, J. H., Viénot, F., et al. (2017). Cerebral neural correlates of differential melanopic photic stimulation in humans. Neuroimage 146, 763–769. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.061
Jennings, B. J., and Martinovic, J. (2014). Luminance and color inputs to mid-level and high-level vision. J. Vis. 14:9. doi: 10.1167/14.2.9
Jones, M. J., Sinha, P., Vetter, T., and Poggio, T. (1997). Top–down learning of low-level vision tasks. Curr. Biol. 7, 991–994. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(06)00419-2
Joyce, D. S., Houser, K. W., Peirson, S. N., Zeitzer, J. M., and Zele, A. J. (2022). Melanopsin Vision: Sensation and Perception Through Intrinsically Photosensitive Retinal Ganglion Cells. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lee, B., Martin, P., and Valberg, A. (1988). The physiological basis of heterochromatic flicker photometry demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the macaque retina. J. Physiol. 404, 323–347. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017292
Lee, H.-H., and Yeh, S.-L. (2021). Blue-light effects on saccadic eye movements and attentional disengagement. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 83, 1713–1728. doi: 10.3758/s13414-021-02250-z
Lee, H.-H., Tu, Y.-C., and Yeh, S.-L. (2021). In search of blue-light effects on cognitive control. Sci. Rep. 11:15505. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-94989-6
Liu, A. L., Liu, Y.-F., Wang, G., Shao, Y.-Q., Yu, C.-X., Yang, Z., et al. (2022). The role of ipRGCs in ocular growth and myopia development. Sci. Adv. 8:eabm9027. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abm9027
Liu, X., Huang, H., Snutch, T. P., Cao, P., Wang, L., and Wang, F. (2022). The superior colliculus: Cell types, connectivity, and behavior. Neurosci. Bull. 38, 1519–1540. doi: 10.1007/s12264-022-00858-1
Lucas, R. J., Allen, A. E., Milosavljevic, N., Storchi, R., and Woelders, T. (2020). Can we see with melanopsin? Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 6, 453–468. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-030320-041239
Lucas, R. J., Peirson, S. N., Berson, D. M., Brown, T. M., Cooper, H. M., Czeisler, C. A., et al. (2014). Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 37, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.004
Mahoney, H. L., and Schmidt, T. M. (2024). The cognitive impact of light: Illuminating ipRGC circuit mechanisms. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 159–175. doi: 10.1038/s41583-023-00788-5
Manger, P. (2020). “Consistencies and variances in the anatomical organization of aspects of the mammalian brain stem,” in Evolutionary Neuroscience, ed. S. V. Shepherd (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 377–396.
Meng, J., Huang, X., Ren, C., and Xue, T. (2025). Non-image-forming functions of Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-112723-035532
Nugent, T. W., and Zele, A. J. (2024). What can the eye see with melanopsin? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 121:e2411151121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2411151121
Patterson, S. S., Kuchenbecker, J. A., Anderson, J. R., Neitz, M., and Neitz, J. (2020). A color vision circuit for non-image-forming vision in the primate retina. Curr. Biol. 30, 1269–1274.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.040
Paus, T. (1996). Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: A selective review. Neuropsychologia 34, 475–483. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00134-4
Phipps-Nelson, J., Redman, J. R., Schlangen, L. J., and Rajaratnam, S. M. (2009). Blue light exposure reduces objective measures of sleepiness during prolonged nighttime performance testing. Chronobiol. Int. 26, 891–912. doi: 10.1080/07420520903044364
Pickard, G. E., and Sollars, P. J. (2011). Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Rev. Physiol. Biochem. Pharmacol. 162, 59–90. doi: 10.1007/112_2011_4
Procyk, C. A., Eleftheriou, C. G., Storchi, R., Allen, A. E., Milosavljevic, N., Brown, T. M., et al. (2015). Spatial receptive fields in the retina and dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of mice lacking rods and cones. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 1321–1330. doi: 10.1152/jn.00368.2015
Provencio, I., Jiang, G., De Grip, W. J., Hayes, W. P., and Rollag, M. D. (1998). Melanopsin: An opsin in melanophores, brain, and eye. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 340–345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.1.340
Provencio, I., Rodriguez, I. R., Jiang, G., Hayes, W. P., Moreira, E. F., and Rollag, M. D. (2000). A novel human opsin in the inner retina. J. Neurosci. 20, 600–605. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00600.2000
Rivaud, S., Müri, R., Gaymard, B., Vermersch, A., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1994). Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 102, 110–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00232443
Schmidt, T. M., Alam, N. M., Chen, S., Kofuji, P., Li, W., Prusky, G. T., et al. (2014). A role for melanopsin in alpha retinal ganglion cells and contrast detection. Neuron 82, 781–788. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.022
Schmidt, T. M., Chen, S.-K., and Hattar, S. (2011). Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: Many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends Neurosci. 34, 572–580. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.001
Shi, Y., Zhang, J., Li, X., Han, Y., Guan, J., Li, Y., et al. (2025). Non-image-forming photoreceptors improve visual orientation selectivity and image perception. Neuron 113, 486–500.e13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2024.11.015
Souman, J. L., Tinga, A. M., Te Pas, S. F., Van Ee, R., and Vlaskamp, B. N. (2018). Acute alerting effects of light: A systematic literature review. Behav. Brain Res. 337, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.016
Spitschan, M., Bock, A. S., Ryan, J., Frazzetta, G., Brainard, D. H., and Aguirre, G. K. (2017). The human visual cortex response to melanopsin-directed stimulation is accompanied by a distinct perceptual experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 12291–12296. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711522114
Suzuki, Y., Nakauchi, S., and Liao, H.-I. (2025). Selective activation of ipRGC modulates working memory performance. PLoS One 20:e0327349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0327349
Tsujimura, S.-I., Ukai, K., Ohama, D., Nuruki, A., and Yunokuchi, K. (2010). Contribution of human melanopsin retinal ganglion cells to steady-state pupil responses. Proc. R. Soc B Biol. Sci. 277, 2485–2492. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0330
Tünçok, E., Kiorpes, L., and Carrasco, M. (2025). Opposite asymmetry in visual perception of humans and macaques. Curr. Biol. 35, 681–687.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.12.024
Uprety, S., Adhikari, P., Feigl, B., and Zele, A. J. (2022). Melanopsin photoreception differentially modulates rod-mediated and cone-mediated human temporal vision. Iscience 25:104529. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104529
Vandewalle, G., Balteau, E., Phillips, C., Degueldre, C., Moreau, V., Sterpenich, V., et al. (2006). Daytime light exposure dynamically enhances brain responses. Curr. Biol. 16, 1616–1621. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.031
Vandewalle, G., Collignon, O., Hull, J. T., Daneault, V., Albouy, G., Lepore, F., et al. (2013). Blue light stimulates cognitive brain activity in visually blind individuals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2072–2085. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00450
Vandewalle, G., Gais, S., Schabus, M., Balteau, E., Carrier, J., Darsaud, A., et al. (2007). Wavelength-dependent modulation of brain responses to a working memory task by daytime light exposure. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2788–2795. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm007
Vandewalle, G., Maquet, P., and Dijk, D.-J. (2009). Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.004
Vincent, J., Haggerty, E. B., Brainard, D. H., and Aguirre, G. K. (2021). Melanopic stimulation does not alter psychophysical threshold sensitivity for luminance flicker. Sci. Rep. 11:20167. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-99684-0
Webster, M. A., Mizokami, Y., and Webster, S. M. (2007). Seasonal variations in the color statistics of natural images. Network 18, 213–233. doi: 10.1080/09548980701654405
Woelders, T., Allen, A. E., and Lucas, R. J. (2023). Melanopsin enhances image persistence. Curr. Biol. 33, 5048–5056.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.10.039
Yamakawa, M., Tsujimura, S.-I., and Okajima, K. (2019). A quantitative analysis of the contribution of melanopsin to brightness perception. Sci. Rep. 9:7568. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44035-3
Yang, C.-C., Tsujimura, S.-I., and Yeh, S.-L. (2023). Blue-light background impairs visual exogenous attention shift. Sci. Rep. 13:3794. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-24862-7
Yang, P.-L., Tsujimura, S.-I., Matsumoto, A., Yamashita, W., and Yeh, S.-L. (2018). Subjective time expansion with increased stimulation of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Sci. Rep. 8:11693. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29613-1
Zaidi, F. H., Hull, J. T., Peirson, S. N., Wulff, K., Aeschbach, D., Gooley, J. J., et al. (2007). Short-wavelength light sensitivity of circadian, pupillary, and visual awareness in humans lacking an outer retina. Curr. Biol. 17, 2122–2128. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.034
Zele, A. J., Adhikari, P., Cao, D., and Feigl, B. (2019). Melanopsin driven enhancement of cone-mediated visual processing. Vis. Res. 160, 72–81. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2019.04.009
Zele, A. J., Adhikari, P., Feigl, B., and Cao, D. (2018a). Cone and melanopsin contributions to human brightness estimation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 35, B19–B25. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.35.000B19
Keywords: intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), blue light, visual processing, non-image-forming functions, melanopsin
Citation: Lee H-H (2025) Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells and visual processing: ipRGCs beyond non-image-forming functions. Front. Neurosci. 19:1635101. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1635101
Received: 26 May 2025; Accepted: 15 July 2025;
Published: 05 August 2025.
Edited by:
Mario Senden, Maastricht University, NetherlandsReviewed by:
Birke Benedikter, Philipps-Universität Marburg, GermanyCopyright © 2025 Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hsing-Hao Lee, aHNpbmdoYW9sZWVAbnl1LmVkdQ==