Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 05 August 2025

Sec. Decision Neuroscience

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2025.1644485

Decision-making impairments in epilepsy: behavioral and EEG evidence of event-related potential

Sijie An&#x;Sijie An1Qing Chen&#x;Qing Chen1Yuwei ZhouYuwei Zhou1Xiangyu GaoXiangyu Gao2Siyu GongSiyu Gong2Minghao AnMinghao An2Yimin LiuYimin Liu2Chengjuan XieChengjuan Xie1Yubao Jiang
Yubao Jiang1*Kai Wang,
Kai Wang1,3*Xingui Chen,
Xingui Chen1,4*
  • 1Department of Neurology, The First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
  • 2First School of Clinical Medicine Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
  • 3Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China
  • 4Department of Neurology, Yuexi County Hospital, Anqing, China

Objective: Decision-making is impaired in patients with epilepsy; however, the changes in decision-making in patients with new-onset and seizure-remission epilepsy remain unclear. The aim of this study was to examine decision-making differences in patients with new-onset and seizure-remission epilepsy, as well as the neurophysiological mechanisms.

Methods: This study included 32 patients with new-onset epilepsy, 31 with seizure-remission epilepsy with generalized tonic–clonic seizures, and 34 matched healthy individuals. Simultaneous electroencephalogram was performed using the Iowa gambling task (IGT). Behavioral performance in the IGT was assessed among the three groups, and P300 and theta oscillations were used as electrophysiological indicators to observe decision-making ability.

Results: Patients with new-onset and seizure-remission epilepsy had significantly lower net scores, lower accounts, and lesser P300 amplitudes and theta oscillation power than did healthy individuals. The percent use of negative feedback was positively correlated with the P300 amplitude.

Conclusion: Impaired decision-making in persons with epilepsy is associated with decreased P300 amplitude and attenuated theta oscillations. Decision-making function was impaired despite clinical seizure-remission.

Significance: This study is the first to compare the behavioral differences in decision-making ability between patients with new-onset and seizure-remission epilepsy. The combination of electroencephalographic features reveals neural mechanisms and improves the understanding of epilepsy decision-making.

1 Introduction

Epilepsy is a prevalent neurological disorder characterized by recurring seizures and unpredictable disturbances in brain function, leading to neurobiological, cognitive, psychological, and social ramifications (Fisher et al., 2005). According to the International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE), generalized seizures rapidly involve bilateral hemispheric networks, including absence seizures, myoclonic seizures, and generalized tonic–clonic seizures (GTCS). GTCS is a common form of epileptic seizure (Fisher et al., 2017; Hirsch et al., 2022). Epilepsy affects approximately 70 million people worldwide (Loscher et al., 2020); however, it has received relatively little scholarly attention despite the importance of understanding the mechanisms behind its onset and progression.

Cognitive impairment is frequently observed in people with epilepsy, with an incidence rate of up to 70%. The study indicates that patients with epilepsy are prone to cognitive and behavioral deficits. Factors such as epilepsy type, underlying etiology, age of onset, seizure frequency, and disease course are all considered important influencers of cognition (Operto et al., 2023). A prolonged history of epileptic seizures and repeated episodes of status epilepticus may induce progressive alterations in brain connectivity, which could lead to cognitive deterioration over time (Novak et al., 2022). Individuals with epilepsy may experience attention deficits, and impairments in memory, language, and executive functions associated with frontal brain regions (Hamed, 2009; Li et al., 2020). People with epilepsy exhibit inferior performance compared to healthy counterparts in immediate memory, delayed memory, and learning activities (Tedrus et al., 2020). A high frequency of seizures is associated with adverse outcomes; functional impairments in brain regions are more severe in patients with interictal phases lasting less than 1 year (Vollmar et al., 2011). Although the exact cause of cognitive impairment in epilepsy remains unknown, the effect of epilepsy on cognitive function is both clinically and neuroscientifically significant.

Decision-making is a fundamental human behavior characterized by the maximization of expected rewards (Biernacki et al., 2016). It encompasses computational analysis, risk assessment, and consequence evaluation. Cognition impairments may appear early in epilepsy (Witt and Helmstaedter, 2015). Individuals with inadequately managed epilepsy exhibit impaired decision-making capabilities (Labudda et al., 2009). There is a lack of research on decision-making ability during the remission phase of epilepsy. Investigating decision-making during new-onset and seizure-remission is essential to better understand epilepsy. The Iowa gambling task (IGT) is a widely used tool for assessing ambiguous situations with implicit outcomes and probabilities (Buelow and Suhr, 2009; Bechara et al., 1994). Its probabilities are implicit, and it shows sensitivity in identifying impulse control disorder diseases (Gleichgerrcht et al., 2010).

The P300 component has been widely used in psychological and neurological studies as an indicator of conscious error recognition and response adjustment (Gokcay et al., 2006). Event-related potentials (ERP) research commonly regards the P300 as a marker for decision-making in ambiguous contexts (Zheng et al., 2020). Additionally, neural oscillations, particularly theta oscillations, are essential for various neurophysiological and cognitive functions. Theta signals, especially in the frontal cortex, facilitate communication between the frontal executive and parietal attentional control areas and support higher-order cognitive processes (Rajan et al., 2019). When errors or negative feedback occur, theta oscillation power increases in the middle frontal region (Nurislamova et al., 2019). These findings suggest that P300 and theta oscillations are valuable electrophysiological markers for monitoring alterations in decision-making abilities. Therefore, using these metrics to investigate the electrophysiological mechanisms underlying GTCA-related decision-making impairments is a promising approach.

Although many studies have identified behavioral indicators of decision-making dysfunction in patients with epilepsy, few have explored electrophysiological mechanisms underlying these deficits (Donoghue and Voytek, 2022). In new-onset epilepsy patients with recent frequent seizures, the brain neuronal network may be in a stage of plasticity remodeling mediated by abnormal electrical activity (Xing et al., 2024). Studies have shown that the left thalamic volume in children with new-onset epilepsy can be significantly reduced (Perani et al., 2018), and the functional connectivity abnormality of this structure with the prefrontal cognitive regulatory network may directly lead to decision-making related cognitive impairment. Although seizure control is achieved in remission epilepsy patients, persistent abnormal electrical activity may exist in the brain (Clemens et al., 2013), and long-term disease course combined with neurotoxicity of antiepileptic drugs may lead to neural structure remodeling and metabolic reduction in the prefrontal cortex (Majeed et al., 2022), which can still decrease the regulatory efficiency of the executive function network. Comparing the cognitive characteristics of the two groups clarifies the dynamic impact of epilepsy on brain function at the mechanistic level, while providing a basis for clinical hierarchical management. This holds certain significance for improving the overall prevention and treatment level of epilepsy and ameliorating long-term prognosis of patients.

This study focuses on the assessment of cognitive functions in patients with epilepsy. The IGT was employed to quantitatively analyze decision-making abilities. By including drug-naive new-onset patients (to exclude the interference of antiepileptic drugs on cognition), we systematically compared the differences in decision-making functions among new-onset patients, those in remission, and healthy controls. The study integrated electrophysiological analyses of event-related potentials (P300) and theta wave oscillations in the prefrontal cortex. We hypothesized that remission patients would show slightly better performance than new-onset patients, but when compared with healthy controls, the decision-making ability of both epileptic groups was found to decline. Electrophysiological results showed decreased P300 amplitude and prefrontal theta wave power. This study aims to clarify the behavioral characteristics of decision-making disorders in different stages of epilepsy and reveal their underlying neuroelectrophysiological mechanisms.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

All patients were recruited from the outpatient clinic and ward of the Department of Neurology at the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) age of 18–65 years; (2) Patients demonstrating GTCS during video-EEG monitoring, with clinical manifestations consistent with typical tonic–clonic seizure characteristics, as diagnosed by certified epileptologists according to the International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE) classification criteria (Fisher et al., 2017); and (3) new-onset epilepsy characterized by the initial epileptic seizure diagnosed as epilepsy within 6 months and not previously treated with antiepileptic medication (Hermann et al., 2006). Seizure-remission epilepsy is characterized by a seizure-free period of at least 12 months (Itamura et al., 2023), and no anti-seizure medication adjustments have been made within 1 year. The exclusion criteria were (1) the existence of other organic diseases, (2) a history of alcohol-related disorders or the consumption of any substance affecting the central nervous system, and (3) comorbid anxiety or depression.

The study ultimately included 32 individuals with new-onset diagnosis (18 male individuals) and 31 patients in seizure-remission (20 male individuals). Thirty-four healthy individuals (16 male individuals) were matched for age and sex. 18 patients were on anti-seizure medication monotherapy, while 13 patients were on polytherapy.

No substantial differences were observed among the three groups regarding sex (χ2 = 3.871, p = 0.424), age (F = 2.277, p = 0.108), or education (F = 2.242, p = 0.112), with specific demographic details indicated in Table 1. All individuals underwent standardized neuropsychological assessments to evaluate their cognitive abilities at baseline. The Beijing variant of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment Test was used to evaluate overall cognitive performance (Yu et al., 2012). The Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAMA) (Price et al., 2011) and the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD) (Möller, 2001) were used to exclude participants with comorbid anxiety and depression (exclusion criteria: HAMA > 17, HAMD > 14). The Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University approved all study protocols (Ethical approval number: 2019H022, date: 2020.12.28). All participants provided informed consent before participating in the study. The experiment is in accordance with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic background information of participants and IGT performance.

2.2 IGT

A computerized variant of the IGT was used in this experiment. Participants were notified that they would engage in a gambling game, with a selection of two decks of cards displayed on the computer screen, allowing the user to opt for either a “small” (50 RMB) or a “large” (100 RMB) bet. The win/loss sequence for each betting type is randomized, exhibiting a win/loss probability of 0.6/0.4 for a 50-point wager and 0.4/0.6 for a 100-point wager. The most effective technique is to select the “small,” low-risk wager type to optimize the end outcome. The initial investment was 1,000 RMB. After each option, a 200–400 ms pause ensued, followed by feedback, in the form of a cartoon face. The cartoon-based visage endured for 1,000 ms. Subsequently, a message with text and numbers appeared on the computer screen, notifying the participants of the results of their selection. They were instructed to maximize their monetary gain (Figure 1). Furthermore, “high-risk loss” was delineated as when a member selected a high-risk choice and incurred a loss. A “low-risk win” was defined as a scenario in which the player selects the low-risk option and achieves a profit. The percent use of negative feedback was calculated by dividing the number of times a participant transitioned to a lower risk after selecting a high-risk loss by the number of times the participant accepted a high-risk loss. The activity spanned approximately 15 min and had 300 trials. All patients were compensated based on a fixed rate of cash at the end of the experiment according to the final account.

Figure 1
Flowchart depicting a decision-making process. It starts with a choice between 50 and 100, during a period labeled

Figure 1. Flowchart of the Iowa gambling task (IGT), with the key F representing 50 and key J representing 100. After each selection, the screen exhibited 200–400 ms of fixation, succeeded by feedback in the form of a cartoon face, which persisted for 1,000 ms until a letter appeared on the computer screen, alerting the participant of the outcome of the choice. The experiment concluded after 300 trials.

2.3 EEG data recording and analysis

EEG recordings were performed in a tranquil and comfortable setting, with the participants encouraged to cleanse their scalps before the experiment and remain awake and attentive. Electrode scalp elastic caps were positioned according to the international 10–20 system, and EEG was performed using Neuro Scan equipment (Neuro Scan, Sterling, VA, United States). The electrode on the forehead was the grounding electrode, and all EEG channels were referenced to the left mastoid (Choi et al., 2022). A vertical electrooculogram was placed 1 cm above the middle of the left eyebrow and 1 cm below the left pupil, whereas a horizontal electrooculogram was placed 1 cm lateral to the two ocular lobes to assess the open and closed states of the eyes. All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 KΩ. EEG activity was enhanced by band-pass filtering from 0.01 to 100 Hz and continuously captured at 1,000 Hz per channel.

The EEG data were processed and analyzed using MATLAB scripts within the EEGLAB framework (Edwards et al., 2013). The average values of the right and left mastoids were re-referenced and down-sampled to 500 Hz. Eye movements, electromyographic activity, and other non-artifactual components were eliminated by independent component analysis using the EEGLAB toolbox. Midline loci (FZ, CZ, and CPZ) exhibit greater P300 amplitudes (Helfrich and Knight, 2019). A waveform period of [−0.2, 1] was selected to generate group-level ERP plots, and the mean P300 amplitude was calculated over a 350–450 ms time window for statistical analysis. Theta oscillations were analyzed using the STUDY panel within the EEGLAB framework. A frequency range of 4–6 Hz and a temporal window of 350–450 ms were designated as the criteria for theta oscillations based on prior research and current time-frequency representations. The mean power (dB) within this time frame was extracted for statistical evaluation and subsequently illustrated (Soltani Zangbar et al., 2020).

2.4 Statistical analyses

All behavioral and electrophysiological analyses were conducted using SPSS software (version 17.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Comparisons across the sexes were conducted using the chi-square test. The aggregate of the net scores from the five block groups constituted the overall net scores, whereas the final account at the conclusion of the game represented the IGT account. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine the disparities among the three groups in the IGT total net scores, IGT accounts, and the percent use of negative feedback. The behavioral experiments were segmented into five blocks, with net scores calculated as the difference between the number of favorable and unfavorable choices in each block. The block served as a within-patient factor, whereas the group functioned as a between-patient factor. The decision-making performance of the three groups was assessed using a 3 (group) × 5 (block) repeated-measures ANOVA. Multiple comparisons among the three groups were conducted using Bonferroni adjustment. The mean amplitude of the event-related potential P300 and the mean power of theta oscillations were examined using repeated-measures ANOVA, with the type of feedback (win/loss), intensity of risk (50 for low risk and 100 for high risk), and electrodes (Fz, FCZ, CZ, and CPZ) as within-patient factors and the group as a between-patient factor. Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated to examine the strength of the associations between P300 amplitude and the percent use of negative feedback. A p-value of < 0.05 was considered significant.

3 Results

3.1 Demographics and behavioral performance

The IGT net scores [F = 5.107, p = 0.008] exhibited significant variation among the three groups (Figure 2). The IGT net scores of the healthy individuals surpassed those of the epilepsy group. Furthermore, multiple comparisons revealed significant differences in the IGT net scores between the new-onset epilepsy and healthy groups (p = 0.003), as well as between the seizure-remission and healthy groups (p = 0.019). No significant difference was observed in the IGT net scores between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.550). The money account of the IGT exhibited significant variation among the three groups, being higher in healthy individuals than in the two epilepsy groups [F = 5.271, p = 0.007]. Multiple comparisons revealed significant differences between the new-onset epilepsy and healthy groups (p = 0.004), as well as between the seizure-remission and healthy groups (p = 0.010). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.800). The percent use of negative feedback [F = 3.085, p = 0.05] differed significantly among the three groups, and multiple comparisons of the difference between new-onset epilepsy and healthy individuals were significant (p = 0.017). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.106), nor between the healthy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.447).

Figure 2
Set of four graphs labeled a) to d). a) Box plot comparing net scores of three groups: New-onset, Seizure-remission, and HC, showing higher scores for HC. b) Box plot of money accounts for the same groups, indicating significant differences, with HC highest. c) Box plot of negative feedback usage ratios, again showing HC with a notably different pattern. d) Line graph illustrating scores over five blocks for each group, with HC generally scoring higher consistently. Each plot uses unique patterns for group identification.

Figure 2. Performance of the three groups on the Iowa gambling task (IGT). The IGT net scores were compared among the three groups. (a) The IGT net scores. (b) The money account. (c) The percent use of negative feedback. (d) Comparison of the net scores of the three patient groups in the five blocks. Bars indicate the standard error of the mean. * p < 0.05. HC, healthy control.

A significant within-patient effect of block [F = 15.614, p = 0.001] and a significant between-patient effect of group [F = 5.107, p = 0.008] was observed, suggesting a dynamic process in IGT performance. Paired comparisons showed no significant difference between the new-onset and remission groups (p = 1.000), a significant difference between the new-onset and healthy groups (p = 0.010), and between the remission and healthy groups (p = 0.058). Additionally, there was a significant interaction between block and group [F = 2.125, p = 0.036], indicating that decision-making strategies varied among different groups. Analysis of the simple effects of group and block showed a significant difference in the net scores between patients with new-onset epilepsy and healthy individuals (p = 0.01) and a borderline significant difference between patients in seizure-remission and healthy individuals (p = 0.058). In blocks 1 and 3, a significant difference was observed in the net scores between the healthy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.039 and p = 0.034, respectively), and in block 2, a significant difference was observed between the healthy and new-onset epilepsy groups (p = 0.003).

3.2 P300 amplitude

Significant effects were observed for the intensity [F = 87.979, p = 0.001], feedback type [F = 9.645, p = 0.003], and electrode [F = 38.221, p = 0.001]. Furthermore, significant interactions were observed between feedback type and group [F = 3.737, p = 0.027], intensity and group [F = 8.184, p = 0.001], and electrode and group [F = 2.162, p = 0.048]. Univariate tests revealed that an intensity of 100 elicited a significantly higher wave amplitude, compared with an intensity of 50. A significant difference was observed among the three groups in the loss-100 condition (F = 3.476, p = 0.035), and further analysis revealed a significant difference between the healthy and new-onset epilepsy groups (p = 0.04). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and remission groups (p = 1.000), nor between the remission and healthy groups (p = 0.186). Wave amplitudes differed significantly between the healthy and new-onset epilepsy groups at the FCZ in the loss-100 condition (p = 0.024). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and remission groups (p = 1.000), nor between the remission and healthy groups (p = 0.123). A notable disparity was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and healthy groups (p = 0.004), as well as between the seizure-remission and healthy groups (p = 0.05). By contrast, no significant difference was found between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 1.000) at the FZ in the loss-100 condition. Table 2 presents the analysis of the simple effects of feedback type, intensity, electrode, and group. The average ERP waveforms of the three groups at FCZ and CZ for the loss-100 conditions are shown in Figure 3.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Repeated measures ANOVA for P300 amplitude (μV) across the three groups.

Figure 3
Graphs show EEG waveform data and bar charts comparing amplitude across three groups: new-onset (red), seizure-remission (green), and healthy controls (blue). Panels a and b display line graphs for FZ and FCZ channels, illustrating latency (200-800 ms) vs. amplitude (microvolts). Panels c and d present bar charts for FZ and FCZ, showing mean amplitudes with error bars. Asterisks denote statistically significant differences between groups.

Figure 3. The event-related potential graphs for the electrodes (a) FZ and (b) FCZ in the loss-100 condition illustrate significant amplitude increases within the defined time frames marked by the black boxes. The P300 mean amplitude analysis for the corresponding extracted time windows is presented in (c,d), with bars representing the standard error of the mean. * p < 0.05. HC, healthy control.

3.3 Theta oscillations

A significant effect was observed for intensity [F = 15.306, p = 0.001] but not for the electrode [F = 1.207, p = 0.312] or feedback type [F = 0.059, p = 0.809]. Significant between-patient group effects [F = 4.574, p = 0.013] were observed, and post hoc comparisons revealed significant differences between the healthy and new-onset epilepsy groups (p = 0.026) and between the healthy and seizure-remission groups (p = 0.041). No significant difference was found between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 1.000). A notable interaction was observed between feedback type and intensity (F = 69.810, p = 0.001) and among feedback type, intensity, and group (F = 3.959, p = 0.022). Univariate analyses revealed significant differences among the three groups regarding the type of feedback for loss [F = 6.645, p = 0.002]. Significant differences were found in the comparisons of the healthy group with the new-onset group (p = 0.006) and with the seizure-remission group (p = 0.007). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy group and the seizure-remission group (p = 1.000). There were significant difference among the three groups in the 100 intensity condition [F = 4.609, p = 0.012]. Further analysis revealed significant differences between the healthy and seizure-remission groups, as well as between the healthy individuals and new-onset epilepsy groups (p = 0.032 and p = 0.031, respectively). No significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p = 1.000). Additionally, theta oscillation induced at the intensity of 100 was higher than at the intensity of 50. Post hoc 2 × 2 comparison showed that, at the FZ, FCZ, CZ, and CPZ electrodes, the healthy group differed significantly from both the seizure-remission and new-onset epilepsy groups (p < 0.05). However, no significant difference was observed between the new-onset epilepsy and seizure-remission groups (p > 0.05). The results of the specific analyses are presented in Table 3. The average theta oscillations power of the three groups at the FZ, FCZ, and CZ electrodes under the loss-100 condition is shown in Figure 4.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Repeated measures ANOVA for theta oscillations (dB) across the three groups.

Figure 4
Heat maps and bar graphs compare theta power across three brain regions: FZ, FCZ, and CZ. Each region is shown for new-onset epilepsy, seizure-remission, and healthy controls (HC). Heat maps depict frequency in Hertz against time, with intensity in decibels. Bar graphs show mean theta power, indicating significant differences among groups with asterisks.

Figure 4. Time-frequency representations of theta oscillations at electrodes (a) FZ, (b) FCZ, and (c) CZ were generated using MATLAB, with the black box delineating the time-frequency region of interest where power exhibits a significant increase. Differences in theta oscillatory activity among the three electrodes are illustrated (bars represent the standard error of the mean; * p < 0.05). HC, healthy control.

3.4 Correlation analysis

The relationship between behavioral markers of decision-making and electrophysiological indicators was investigated in all individuals (Figure 5). In all the three groups, there was a significant correlation between the percentage use of negative feedback and the P300 amplitude at the FZ electrode in the loss-100 condition (new-onset group: r = 0.371, p = 0.031; seizure-remission group: r = 0.439, p = 0.013; healthy group: F = 0.350, p = 0.042).

Figure 5
Scatter plot showing the relationship between P300 values and the percent use of negative feedback. Three groups are represented: HC, new-onset, and seizure-remission. Each group is depicted with different colored data points and trend lines. The x-axis represents the percent use of negative feedback, and the y-axis represents P300 values. Gray shaded areas indicate confidence intervals around each trend line.

Figure 5. Scatterplot illustrating the relationship between the percentage use of negative feedback and P300 amplitude elicited by a high chance of loss at FZ across three groups, demonstrating a substantial positive association (grey-shaded regions represent 95% confidence intervals). HC, healthy control.

4 Discussion

This study examined disparities in decision-making ability and electrophysiological mechanisms among individuals with new-onset GTCS, those in seizure-remission and healthy controls under ambiguous risk conditions. The study results indicated inferior decision-making in patients with new-onset epilepsy and those in seizure-remission compared with their healthy counterparts, and the decline in decision-making ability may be associated with reduced P300 amplitude and theta oscillations. This study also observed that patients with epilepsy had lower P300 amplitude and theta oscillation power, and the percent use of negative feedback loss than did the healthy group. Furthermore, the percent use of negative feedback demonstrated a significant positive correlation with P300 amplitude. These results suggest that patients continue to experience lingering deficits in their decision-making capacities despite clinical seizure-remission.

Consistent with prior studies on juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME), JME patients exhibiting deficient executive function are more prone to engage in risky decision-making compared to healthy controls (Zamarian et al., 2013). Research has revealed impaired decision-making capabilities in persons with cryptogenic frontal lobe epilepsy and temporal lobe epilepsy. In the present study, people with GTCS performed worse, compared with healthy individuals, in terms of the IGT net scores, IGT accounts, and percent use of negative feedback (Delazer et al., 2010; Yamano et al., 2011). In the new-onset epilepsy group, performance initially increased in the second block but then leveled off, whereas healthy individuals displayed an overall upward trend. Across all blocks, the people with epilepsy group performed worse than the healthy group did, showing challenges in developing and maintaining favorable long-term decision-making strategies. Emotional regulation and decision-making processes appear to be affected when patients face risky situations. The orbital frontal cortical (OFC), anterior cingulate, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), plays a crucial role in decision-making behavior. Patients with inadequately managed seizures often show poorer performance on the IGT and other executive function assessments, likely owing to cortical dysfunction from prolonged seizures and reduced connectivity in neural networks (Kanner and Bicchi, 2022; Brand et al., 2007). The OFC is involved in decision-making, risk aversion, and relearning (Balewski et al., 2023) and conveys external information to the hypothalamus and amygdala through the VMPFC. The thalamus activates subnetworks in the frontal cortex that influence various aspects of decision-making (Yang et al., 2022). Imaging studies have shown that changes in subcortical areas linked to epilepsy and weakened connectivity in the frontal-thalamic network may impair decision-making (Zhang et al., 2011). Reduced connectivity between the OFC and VMPFC may also impair contingency relearning, as observed in patients who struggle to adjust their strategies after failure, contributing to their compromised decision-making ability. From a neurobiological perspective, the weakened regulation of the prefrontal cortex over the limbic system makes individuals more susceptible to emotional drives, leading to impulsive behaviors. This reduction in neural regulation directly manifests as a decline in decision-making ability in behavioral terms (Jones et al., 2021). The low percent use of negative feedback in the epilepsy group indicates slower strategy adjustment or difficulty in effectively using negative feedback to optimize decision-making, which aligns with studies on decision-making impairment in addiction models suggesting an inability to convert negative feedback into a motivation for “impulse inhibition” (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2018). When the prefrontal cortex is damaged, the failure of decision-making network integration causes individuals to be dominated by emotions, triggering negative feedback processing disorders and leading to repeated erroneous choices (Bechara et al., 1997). In this study, epileptic patients exhibited reduced sensitivity to negative outcomes and delayed strategy adjustment, which corresponds to the clinical observations of impulsive behaviors and decreased daily decision-making ability in epileptic patients.

This study found that patients with epilepsy showed reduced P300 amplitude and theta oscillation power and lower the percent use of negative feedback compared to the healthy group. Furthermore, the percent use of negative feedback was significantly positively correlated with P300 amplitude. This finding suggests that impaired decision-making of patients with epilepsy prevents them from recognizing errors, processing feedback, or adapting their strategies accordingly. Theta oscillations facilitate coordinated neuronal activity, contributing to P300 generation (Popp et al., 2019). Diminished dopamine levels during decision-making result in reduced activity of the midbrain limbic pathway (Christie and Tata, 2009; van Holst et al., 2010). Lower dopamine levels may be associated with decreased P300 amplitudes and theta oscillations during decision-making in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy models show reduced hippocampal theta oscillations and poor spatial learning, whereas healthy rats exhibit higher theta coherence during spatial working memory and decision-making tasks (Benchenane et al., 2010). Oscillations support the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in coordinating activity, enabling efficient information processing and integration. Therefore, P300 and theta oscillations are indicators of compromised decision-making in patients with epilepsy.

Although there was no statistically significant difference between the epilepsy subgroups, the remission group showed better performance than the new-onset epilepsy group in IGT behavioral indicators and P300 amplitude. However, the decision-making capacity of patients with epilepsy in seizure-remission is lower than that of healthy individuals. Electrophysiological data indicate that even after achieving clinical seizure control, patients in seizure-remission still show deficits in error feedback detection and brain oscillatory function. The persistent impact of chronic neural remodeling may alter neuronal connections and functions, impacting decision-making ability even in seizure-remission (Sutula, 2004; Deng et al., 2024).

Clinically, IGT and EEG can be used in combination as cognitive screening tools. Evaluating the decision-making ability of newly diagnosed patients and monitoring P300 amplitude can help early identify individuals at high risk of cognitive decline. For patients in the remission period, early cognitive training combined with cognitive rehabilitation therapy can guide clinical medication adjustment for targeted improvement. This study has certain limitations. First, the sample sizes of the groups were limited and the study did not include multicenter institutional data. Second, The disease course varied between patients with epilepsy in seizure-remission and those with new-onset epilepsy. The course of epilepsy onset is less strongly correlated with decision-making impairments; however, the experimental design of this study could not eliminate the influence of disease course. Third, the influence of medication could not be disregarded in this trial. Therefore, In subsequent studies, we will systematically collect medication-related data for all patients and integrate these variables into statistical analyses to comprehensively investigate the potential impact of medications on research outcomes.

5 Conclusion

Patients with GTCS exhibit diminished decision-making capabilities, and those experiencing new-onset epilepsy perform worse, compared with those in seizure-remission. This decline may be associated with a reduced P300 amplitude and diminished theta oscillations. Furthermore, even in the absence of clinical seizures, patients in the seizure-remission phase of epilepsy display some level of decision-making impairment, indicating underlying neurocognitive deficits. This study is constrained by its sample size, and future investigations with large multicenter cohorts are imperative to validate these findings and explore nuanced subgroup differences in decision-making abilities.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University approved all study protocols (Ethical approval number: 2019H022, date: 2020.12.28). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SA: Methodology, Conceptualization, Project administration, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Software, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Data curation. QC: Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization, Supervision. YZ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Data curation. XG: Validation, Investigation, Data curation, Writing – review & editing. SG: Writing – review & editing, Validation, Investigation, Project administration. MA: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. YL: Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis. CX: Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis. YJ: Visualization, Resources, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. KW: Visualization, Conceptualization, Resources, Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Methodology. XC: Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Research Fund of the Anhui Institute of Translational Medicine (grant numbers 2023zhyx-C34 and 2023zhyx-C65), the Natural Science Foundation of China (grant numbers 81803103 and 82090034), and the Anhui Province Clinical Medical Research Transformation Special Project (202204295107020024).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

ANOVA, analysis of variance; DMN, default mode network; EEG, electroencephalogram; ERP, event-related potentials; GTCS, generalized tonic–clonic seizures; HAMA, Hamilton Anxiety Scale; HAMD, Hamilton Depression Scale; IGT, Iowa Gambling Task; ILAE, International League Against Epilepsy; OFC, orbital frontal cortical; VMPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

References

Balewski, Z. Z., Elston, T. W., Knudsen, E. B., and Wallis, J. D. (2023). Value dynamics affect choice preparation during decision-making. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 1575–1583. doi: 10.1038/s41593-023-01407-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275, 1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benchenane, K., Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Tierney, P. L., Gioanni, Y., Battaglia, F. P., et al. (2010). Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal-prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 66, 921–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Biernacki, K., McLennan, S. N., Terrett, G., Labuschagne, I., and Rendell, P. G. (2016). Decision-making ability in current and past users of opiates: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 342–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M., Grabenhorst, F., Starcke, K., Vandekerckhove, M. M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2007). Role of the amygdala in decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: evidence from patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuropsychologia 45, 1305–1317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buelow, M. T., and Suhr, J. A. (2009). Construct validity of the Iowa gambling task. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 102–114. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9083-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, H., Park, J., and Yang, Y. M. (2022). Whitening technique based on gram-Schmidt Orthogonalization for motor imagery classification of brain-computer Interface applications. Sensors (Basel) 22:6042. doi: 10.3390/s22166042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, G. J., and Tata, M. S. (2009). Right frontal cortex generates reward-related theta-band oscillatory activity. NeuroImage 48, 415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemens, B., Puskás, S., Besenyei, M., Spisák, T., Emri, M., and Fekete, I. (2013). Remission of benign epilepsy with rolandic spikes: an EEG-based connectivity study at the onset of the disease and at remission. Epilepsy Res. 106, 128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2013.04.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delazer, M., Zamarian, L., Bonatti, E., Kuchukhidze, G., Koppelstätter, F., Bodner, T., et al. (2010). Decision making under ambiguity and under risk in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 48, 194–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, D., Sun, H., Wang, Y., Guo, X., Yuan, Y., Wang, J., et al. (2024). Structural and functional abnormalities in first-episode drug-naïve pediatric idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Cereb. Cortex 34:bhae021. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhae021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Donoghue, T., and Voytek, B. (2022). Automated meta-analysis of the event-related potential (ERP) literature. Sci. Rep. 12:1867. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05939-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, D., Cortes, M., Datta, A., Minhas, P., Wassermann, E. M., and Bikson, M. (2013). Physiological and modeling evidence for focal transcranial electrical brain stimulation in humans: a basis for high-definition tDCS. NeuroImage 74, 266–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. S., Cross, J. H., French, J. A., Higurashi, N., Hirsch, E., Jansen, F. E., et al. (2017). Operational classification of seizure types by the international league against epilepsy: position paper of the ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia 58, 522–530. doi: 10.1111/epi.13670

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. S., van Emde Boas, W., Blume, W., Elger, C., Genton, P., Lee, P., et al. (2005). Epileptic seizures and epilepsy: definitions proposed by the international league against epilepsy (ILAE) and the International Bureau for Epilepsy (IBE). Epilepsia 46, 470–472. doi: 10.1111/j.0013-9580.2005.66104.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibáñez, A., Roca, M., Torralva, T., and Manes, F. (2010). Decision-making cognition in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 6, 611–623. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2010.148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gokcay, A., Celebisoy, N., Gokcay, F., and Atac, C. (2006). Cognitive functions evaluated by P 300 and visual and auditory number assays in children with childhood epilepsy with occipital paroxysms (CEOP). Seizure 15, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2005.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamed, S. A. (2009). The aspects and mechanisms of cognitive alterations in epilepsy: the role of antiepileptic medications. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 15, 134–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2008.00062.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., and Knight, R. T. (2019). Cognitive neurophysiology: event-related potentials. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 160, 543–558. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-64032-1.00036-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermann, B., Jones, J., Sheth, R., Dow, C., Koehn, M., and Seidenberg, M. (2006). Children with new-onset epilepsy: neuropsychological status and brain structure. Brain 129, 2609–2619. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, E., French, J., Scheffer, I. E., Bogacz, A., Alsaadi, T., Sperling, M. R., et al. (2022). ILAE definition of the idiopathic generalized epilepsy syndromes: position statement by the ILAE task force on nosology and definitions. Epilepsia 63, 1475–1499. doi: 10.1111/epi.17236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Itamura, S., Sasaki, K., Fujii, Y., and Okano, R. (2023). Antiseizure medication treatment outcomes in new-onset pediatric epilepsy. Pediatr. Int. 65:e15523. doi: 10.1111/ped.15523

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, J. A., Zuhlsdorff, K., and Dalley, J. W. (2021). Neurochemical substrates linked to impulsive and compulsive phenotypes in addiction: a preclinical perspective. J. Neurochem. 157, 1525–1546. doi: 10.1111/jnc.15380

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanner, A. M., and Bicchi, M. M. (2022). Antiseizure medications for adults with epilepsy: a review. JAMA 327, 1269–1281. doi: 10.1001/jama.2022.3880

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Labudda, K., Frigge, K., Horstmann, S., Aengenendt, J., Woermann, F. G., Ebner, A., et al. (2009). Decision making in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 47, 50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Sun, Y., Zhang, T., Shi, Q., Sun, J., Xiang, J., et al. (2020). The relationship between epilepsy and cognitive function in benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Brain Behav. 10:e01854. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1854

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loscher, W., Potschka, H., Sisodiya, S. M., and Vezzani, A. (2020). Drug resistance in epilepsy: clinical impact, potential mechanisms, and new innovative treatment options. Pharmacol. Rev. 72, 606–638. doi: 10.1124/pr.120.019539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Majeed, C. N., Rudnick, S. R., and Bonkovsky, H. L. (2022). Review of Antiseizure medications for adults with epilepsy. JAMA 328:680. doi: 10.1001/jama.2022.10594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Möller, H. J. (2001). Methodological aspects in the assessment of severity of depression by the Hamilton depression scale. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 251, Ii13–Ii20. doi: 10.1007/BF03035121

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Novak, A., Vizjak, K., and Rakusa, M. (2022). Cognitive impairment in people with epilepsy. J. Clin. Med. 11:267. doi: 10.3390/jcm11010267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nurislamova, Y. M., Novikov, N. A., Zhozhikashvili, N. A., and Chernyshev, B. V. (2019). Enhanced Theta-band coherence between Midfrontal and posterior parietal areas reflects post-feedback adjustments in the state of outcome uncertainty. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 13:14. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Operto, F. F., Pastorino, G. M. G., Viggiano, A., Dell'Isola, G. B., Dini, G., Verrotti, A., et al. (2023). Epilepsy and cognitive impairment in childhood and adolescence: a Mini-review. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 21, 1646–1665. doi: 10.2174/1570159X20666220706102708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, S., Tierney, T. M., Centeno, M., Shamshiri, E. A., Yaakub, S. N., O'Muircheartaigh, J., et al. (2018). Thalamic volume reduction in drug-naive patients with new-onset genetic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsia 59, 226–234. doi: 10.1111/epi.13955

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Popp, F., Dallmer-Zerbe, I., Philipsen, A., and Herrmann, C. S. (2019). Challenges of P 300 modulation using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Front. Psychol. 10:476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00476

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, R. B., Eldreth, D. A., and Mohlman, J. (2011). Deficient prefrontal attentional control in late-life generalized anxiety disorder: an fMRI investigation. Transl. Psychiatry 1:e46. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.46

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajan, A., Siegel, S. N., Liu, Y., Bengson, J., Mangun, G. R., and Ding, M. (2019). Theta oscillations index frontal decision-making and mediate reciprocal frontal-parietal interactions in willed attention. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2832–2843. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltani Zangbar, H., Ghadiri, T., Seyedi Vafaee, M., Ebrahimi Kalan, A., Fallahi, S., Ghorbani, M., et al. (2020). Theta oscillations through hippocampal/prefrontal pathway: importance in cognitive performances. Brain Connect. 10, 157–169. doi: 10.1089/brain.2019.0733

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutula, T. P. (2004). Mechanisms of epilepsy progression: current theories and perspectives from neuroplasticity in adulthood and development. Epilepsy Res. 60, 161–171. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2004.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedrus, G., Passos, M., Vargas, L. M., and Menezes, L. (2020). Cognition and epilepsy: cognitive screening test. Dement. Neuropsychol. 14, 186–193. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642020dn14-020013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Holst, R. J., van den Brink, W., Veltman, D. J., and Goudriaan, A. E. (2010). Brain imaging studies in pathological gambling. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 12, 418–425. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0141-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdejo-Garcia, A., Chong, T. T., Stout, J. C., Yücel, M., and London, E. D. (2018). Stages of dysfunctional decision-making in addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 164, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2017.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollmar, C., O'Muircheartaigh, J., Barker, G. J., Symms, M. R., Thompson, P., Kumari, V., et al. (2011). Motor system hyperconnectivity in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: a cognitive functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain 134, 1710–1719. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Witt, J. A., and Helmstaedter, C. (2015). Cognition in the early stages of adult epilepsy. Seizure 26, 65–68. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2015.01.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, B., Barbour, A. J., Vithayathil, J., Li, X., Dutko, S., Fawcett-Patel, J., et al. (2024). Reversible synaptic adaptations in a subpopulation of murine hippocampal neurons following early-life seizures. J. Clin. Invest. 134:e175167. doi: 10.1172/JCI175167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamano, M., Akamatsu, N., Tsuji, S., Kobayakawa, M., and Kawamura, M. (2011). Decision-making in temporal lobe epilepsy examined with the Iowa gambling task. Epilepsy Res. 93, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, W., Tipparaju, S. L., Chen, G., and Li, N. (2022). Thalamus-driven functional populations in frontal cortex support decision-making. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1339–1352. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01171-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J., Li, J., and Huang, X. (2012). The Beijing version of the Montreal cognitive assessment as a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment: a community-based study. BMC Psychiatry 12:156. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamarian, L., Höfler, J., Kuchukhidze, G., Delazer, M., Bonatti, E., Kemmler, G., et al. (2013). Decision making in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. J. Neurol. 260, 839–846. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6715-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Liao, W., Chen, H., Mantini, D., Ding, J. R., Xu, Q., et al. (2011). Altered functional-structural coupling of large-scale brain networks in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Brain 134, 2912–2928. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Y., Yi, W., Cheng, J., and Li, Q. (2020). Common and distinct electrophysiological correlates of feedback processing during risky and ambiguous decision making. Neuropsychologia 146:107526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107526

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: epilepsy, decision-making, Iowa gambling task (IGT), event-related potentials (ERP), event-related spectral power

Citation: An S, Chen Q, Zhou Y, Gao X, Gong S, An M, Liu Y, Xie C, Jiang Y, Wang K and Chen X (2025) Decision-making impairments in epilepsy: behavioral and EEG evidence of event-related potential. Front. Neurosci. 19:1644485. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1644485

Received: 10 June 2025; Accepted: 22 July 2025;
Published: 05 August 2025.

Edited by:

Peter Bossaerts, The University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Gianluca Dini, University of Perugia, Italy
Lana Omari, University of Milano Bicocca, Italy

Copyright © 2025 An, Chen, Zhou, Gao, Gong, An, Liu, Xie, Jiang, Wang and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yubao Jiang, amlhbmd5dWJhbzE5ODJAMTI2LmNvbQ==; Kai Wang, d2FuZ2thaTE5NjRAMTI2LmNvbQ==; Xingui Chen, Y2hlbl94aW5fZ3VpQDEyNi5jb20=

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.