- 1Department of Neurology, 12 de Octubre University Hospital, Madrid, Spain
- 2Group of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre Research Institute (imas12), Madrid, Spain
- 3Network Center for Biomedical Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases (CIBERNED), Madrid, Spain
- 4Department of Medicine, Complutense University, Madrid, Spain
- 5Department of Clinical Research, NeuroTrax Corporation, Modiin, Israel
- 6Department of Psychiatry, 12 de Octubre University Hospital, Madrid, Spain
- 7Department of Internal Medicine, 12 de Octubre University Hospital, Madrid, Spain
- 8Department of Neurology, Manipal Hospitals, Kolkata, India
- 9Centre for Neurovascular Research, Manipal Hospitals, Kolkata, India
- 10ETSIDI-Center for Automation and Robotics, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Introduction: Toxic Oil Syndrome (TOS) emerged in Spain in 1981 after ingestion of rapeseed oil adulterated with aniline derivatives. More than four decades later, survivors continue to report cognitive complaints, but objective evidence of long-term dysfunction remains limited.
Methods: In this case-control study, 47 TOS survivors and 44 matched healthy controls completed validated eye-tracking paradigms (visually guided, memory-guided, and antisaccade tasks) and a standardized neuropsychological battery. Groups did not differ significantly in age, sex, or education.
Results: TOS survivors showed preserved performance on visually guided and memory-guided saccades, with no group differences in latency, gain, peak velocity, or spatial error (all p > 0.05). In contrast, they exhibited fewer correct antisaccades (mean 3.6 vs. 5.0; p = 0.029), more reflexive saccades (mean 7.0 vs. 5.7; p = 0.033), and increased backward reflexive saccades (mean 6.3 vs. 5.1; p = 0.040). Cognitive testing revealed selective impairments in executive function, attention, and processing speed, with preserved memory. Structural equation modeling confirmed that antisaccade impairment remained significant after adjusting for confounders and demonstrated an independent contribution of attention to correct antisaccade performance.
Conclusion: Findings indicate persistent frontal-subcortical circuit dysfunction in TOS survivors, consistent with immune- or vascular-mediated injury patterns rather than progressive neurodegeneration. Eye-tracking provides a noninvasive biomarker of latent executive dysfunction and may be useful for long-term monitoring of populations exposed to environmental toxins.
Introduction
Toxic oil syndrome (TOS) is a chronic, multisystem disease that emerged in Spain in May 1981 following the ingestion of rapeseed oil adulterated with aniline derivatives and illegally distributed for human consumption. The outbreak rapidly escalated, affecting over 20,000 individuals and causing more than 300 deaths within the first year (Abaitua Borda et al., 1998; Sánchez-Porro Valadés et al., 2003; Polentinos-Castro et al., 2021). The acute phase was characterized by eosinophilia, fever, myalgia, and pulmonary edema, later progressing to a chronic condition marked by scleroderma-like skin changes, peripheral neuropathy, muscular atrophy, and both pulmonary and neuropsychiatric manifestations (Gelpí et al., 2002; De La Paz et al., 2001). Although extensive multidisciplinary investigations were conducted, the specific toxic agent was never definitively identified. Nonetheless, fatty acid anilides and other byproducts of industrial oil denaturation were implicated based on chemical and toxicological analyses (Gelpí et al., 2002).
Kaufman and Krupp (1995) proposed that TOS and eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome may be part of a spectrum of chemically induced immune-mediated disorders involving the central nervous system. Their hypothesis initiated early discussions about mechanisms such as neuroinflammation and excitotoxicity. Notably, eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome itself is linked to neurocognitive dysfunction (Armstrong et al., 1997).
One of the earliest studies of cognitive impairment in TOS was reported in a small neuropsychological study by Del Ser et al. (1986), who found deficits in episodic and semantic memory within a few years of the onset. This was followed by a 12-year follow-up study by Kaufman et al. (1995), which offered clinical evidence of chronic morbidity in TOS survivors. Among 91 individuals re-evaluated more than a decade after exposure, over half reported persistent symptoms—fatigue, muscle cramps, arthralgias, subjective cognitive difficulties, and psychiatric complaints—despite being otherwise medically stable and lacking signs of a progressive neuromuscular disorder (Kaufman et al., 1995).
Further objective evidence came from a landmark case-referent study by De La Paz et al. (2003), conducted 18 years after the outbreak. TOS patients—particularly middle-aged women—performed significantly worse than matched controls on tests of motor strength, sensory function, and several neurocognitive domains such as memory, attention, processing speed, and psychomotor performance. The pattern of deficits indicated both peripheral and central nervous system involvement, with notable disruption of frontal-subcortical circuits (De La Paz et al., 2003).
Despite earlier findings, the long-term cognitive effects of TOS remain poorly understood. A recent 43-year follow-up case–control study found subtle yet measurable deficits in executive function, attention, and processing speed among TOS survivors, assessed with modern computerized tools (Lapeña-Motilva et al., 2025). In a separate biomarker study within the same group, slightly elevated serum neurofilament light chain levels were observed in TOS patients—mainly due to a few outliers—while levels of glial fibrillary acidic protein and phosphorylated tau 217 remained within normal ranges (Ruiz-Ortiz et al., 2025). These results suggest ongoing or residual neuronal injury in some individuals, without a biomarker pattern typical of progressive neurodegenerative disease (Ruiz-Ortiz et al., 2025).
Eye tracking provides an objective, noninvasive, high-resolution readout of cognitive control, as voluntary saccades are generated by frontal-subcortical circuits that subserve attention, executive control, and working memory. In particular, the frontal eye fields, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate interact with basal ganglia loops and the superior colliculus to initiate or suppress gaze shifts; consequently, the antisaccade task—which requires suppression of a reflexive glance and generation of a willful movement in the opposite direction—is a sensitive probe of inhibitory control and working memory maintenance (Guerrero-Molina et al., 2021; García Cena et al., 2022a; García Cena et al., 2022b; Wolf et al., 2023; Benito-León et al., 2024; Leng et al., 2024; Benito-León et al., 2025). Across diverse neurological conditions, increased antisaccade error rates and prolonged latencies track frontal dysfunction, underscoring the utility of eye-movement metrics (e.g., error rates, reaction times) as surrogate markers of executive integrity (Guerrero-Molina et al., 2021; García Cena et al., 2022a; García Cena et al., 2022b; Wolf et al., 2023; Benito-León et al., 2024; Leng et al., 2024b; Benito-León et al., 2025).
To date, no studies have used eye tracking to examine cognitive dysfunction in TOS. We therefore employed three validated paradigms—visually guided saccades (basic sensorimotor function), memory-guided saccades (spatial working memory), and antisaccades (inhibitory control, executive function, and attention) to detect subtle deficits that may elude conventional testing (Guerrero-Molina et al., 2021; García Cena et al., 2022a; García Cena et al., 2022b; Wolf et al., 2023; Benito-León et al., 2024; Leng et al., 2024b; Benito-León et al., 2025). Given the chronic symptomatology of TOS, the predominance of dysexecutive features, and the absence of biomarker evidence for ongoing neurodegeneration, we hypothesized that TOS survivors would exhibit persistent oculomotor abnormalities, particularly in antisaccade performance, reflecting a long-standing disruption of frontal-subcortical circuits.
Methods
Population and recruitment procedures
Between April and June 2024, participants were recruited from the Madrid region, one of the most severely affected areas by the 1981 TOS outbreak. A case-control design was used: individuals with a confirmed diagnosis of TOS formed the exposed group, while healthy controls from the same geographic area served as the unexposed group. All assessments, including interviews and cognitive testing, were conducted at the 12 de Octubre University Hospital in Madrid.
Participant selection
TOS cases were identified based on diagnostic criteria established in earlier research (Lapeña-Motilva et al., 2025; Ruiz-Ortiz et al., 2025). Eligible individuals had experienced either the acute or chronic phase of the illness. The acute phase was characterized by alveolar–interstitial infiltrates and/or pleural effusion, along with absolute eosinophilia of more than 500 cells/mm3. The chronic phase criteria included persistent myalgia with eosinophilia and/or at least one of the following: scleroderma-like skin changes, peripheral neuropathy, pulmonary hypertension, or liver involvement.
Patients were enrolled through the 12 de Octubre University Hospital’s dedicated TOS unit—the only center in Spain specialized in the long-term follow-up of affected individuals. Recruitment was consecutive until 50 TOS cases were included. The control group consisted of 50 healthy individuals from the same community, selected from acquaintances or friends of the patients. Controls were matched by age (±5 years), sex, and educational level to ensure comparability between groups. Given the fixed size of the eligible clinic cohort and the absence of prior eye-tracking effect-size estimates in TOS, we prospectively set a pragmatic target of 50 cases and 50 matched controls based on feasibility within the recruitment window, acknowledging that a formal a priori power calculation was not possible in this rare disease context.
Exclusion criteria for both groups included any history of neurodegenerative disease (e.g., Alzheimer’s or Parkinson’s disease), stroke, chronic kidney disease, alcohol abuse, or prior traumatic injury involving the central or peripheral nervous system. Identical criteria were applied to controls to maintain methodological rigor.
Assessments and measures
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Participant data—including age, sex, educational background, medical history, and current medications—were obtained through a structured clinical interview and after revision of their medical records. Educational attainment was recorded in four categories: illiterate, primary, secondary and higher studies.
Fatigue evaluation
Fatigue was measured using the Fatigue Impact Scale for Daily Use (D-FIS; Martínez-Martín et al., 2006; Benito-León et al., 2007), a validated 8-item self-report tool designed to assess how fatigue interferes with daily activities. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert scale, from 0 (no problem) to 4 (extreme problem), yielding a total score that reflects the extent of fatigue-related impact on functioning.
Health-related quality of life
The EuroQol instrument was used to assess perceived health-related quality of life (Badia et al., 2001). This tool includes two parts: the EQ-5D descriptive system and a visual analog scale (EQ VAS). The EQ-5D has five domains—mobility, self-care, usual activities, pain/discomfort, and anxiety/depression—each rated on a three-level scale (no problems, moderate problems, severe problems; Badia et al., 2001). Responses create a health profile, from which an index score is calculated using standardized European value sets (Badia et al., 2001). This score ranges from 1, indicating perfect health, to 0, representing death, and negative scores indicate states worse than death (Badia et al., 2001). The EQ VAS enables participants to rate their overall health on a vertical scale ranging from 0 (the worst imaginable health) to 100 (the best imaginable health; Badia et al., 2001).
Assessment of depressive symptoms
Depression severity was assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (Beck et al., 1996), a 21-item self-report questionnaire widely used in clinical and research settings (Beck et al., 1996). Each item reflects a symptom commonly associated with depression and is scored from 0 (absent) to 3 (severe), based on experiences over the prior 2 weeks (Beck et al., 1996). Higher total scores indicate more severe depressive symptoms (Beck et al., 1996).
Assessment of anxiety symptoms
Anxiety was measured using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988), a validated instrument consisting of 21 items evaluating common symptoms of anxiety. Participants rate how much they have been bothered by each symptom in the past week using a 4-point scale, ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (severely bothered). Total scores range from 0 to 63, with higher scores indicating greater severity of anxiety (Beck et al., 1988).
Cognitive function testing
Cognitive performance was assessed using the NeuroTrax computerized system, which offers standardized and validated tests across multiple cognitive areas (Doniger et al., 2005; Schweiger et al., 2007; Dwolatzky et al., 2003). Due to limited time, the assessment focused on specific domains: verbal and non-verbal memory, attention (measured through Go-NoGo and Stroop Interference tasks), processing speed, and executive function (evaluated via the Go-NoGo, Stroop Interference, and Catch Game tasks; Doniger et al., 2005; Schweiger et al., 2007; Dwolatzky et al., 2003). All testing was conducted in Spanish, the native language of all participants.
Eye movement assessments
All eye-tracking evaluations were conducted in a dimly lit room, with recordings made monocularly from the dominant eye of each participant. Standardized instructions were provided, and a practice trial ensured proper understanding of the task. Saccades with latencies below 80 milliseconds and those occurring during blinks were excluded from analysis. The protocol included three well-established paradigms: visually guided saccades, memory-guided saccades, and the antisaccade task, all of which were performed on a horizontal axis (Figure 1) (Guerrero-Molina et al., 2021; García Cena et al., 2022a; García Cena et al., 2022b; Benito-León et al., 2024; Benito-León et al., 2025). We followed our published eye-tracking protocol to ensure reproducibility and enable direct comparison with earlier measurements (García Cena et al., 2020). A brief antisaccade paradigm (22 trials) was implemented to reduce fatigue and maintain calibration stability, consistent with clinical and instrument validation protocols (Hellmuth et al., 2012; Brooks et al., 2019).
• Visually guided saccades
With central fixation maintained, a peripheral target appeared at ±5°, ±10°, or ±20° along the horizontal meridian in pseudorandom order; participants were instructed to look at the target as quickly and accurately as possible and then return their gaze to the central fixation. A correct visually guided saccade was defined as the first goal-directed saccade to the target location; a backward saccade was the subsequent return movement to fixation.Measures included: number of correct saccades; number of errors; latency (ms) from stimulus onset to saccade onset; peak velocity (°/s); and gain (executed amplitude divided by target amplitude). Spatial error relative to the target was assessed separately for hypermetria (overshoot, positive degrees of visual angle) and hypometria (undershoot, negative degrees); we also counted the number of hypermetric saccades and the number of hypometric saccades. For the return movement, we recorded the number of correct backward saccades; number of backward errors; backward latency (ms); and peak backward velocity (°/s).
• Memory-guided saccades
While maintaining central fixation, a peripheral cue briefly appeared and then disappeared; after a short delay, participants made a saccade to the remembered location; then returned their gaze to the center once the fixation point reappeared. A correct memory-guided saccade was the first goal-directed saccade toward the remembered location; errors were first saccades directed elsewhere.Gain was calculated as executed amplitude divided by target amplitude; signed spatial error was the landing-position error (degrees of visual angle) relative to the cued location (positive = overshoot/hypermetria; negative = undershoot/hypometria).Measures included: number of correct memory-guided saccades; number of errors; latency; peak velocity; gain; hypermetric and hypometric spatial errors; number of correct backward memory-guided saccades (return movements to center); number of backward errors; backward latency; and peak backward velocity.
• Antisaccade task
Participants maintained central fixation; when a peripheral stimulus appeared, they were instructed to suppress the automatic glance toward it and instead generate a saccade to the mirror-symmetric location on the opposite side. A correct antisaccade was the first goal-directed saccade from fixation (screen center) to the mirror-symmetric location; a reflexive saccade was an initial saccade toward the cue, followed by a corrective movement to the mirror position; any other eye movement was considered an antisaccade error.Backward eye movements—whether backward antisaccades or backward reflexive saccades—were defined as saccades from the mirror-symmetric location back to the screen center.Measures included: number of correct antisaccades; number of antisaccade errors; antisaccade latency (ms); number of correct backward antisaccades; number of backward antisaccade errors; backward antisaccade latency (ms); number of reflexive saccades; reflexive saccade latency (ms); number of backward reflexive saccades; backward reflexive saccade latency (ms); and reflexive saccade duration (ms).
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the 12 de Octubre University Hospital in Madrid, Spain (CEIC codes: 17/035 and 23/616). It was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written (signed) informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Statistical analyses
Trial-level metrics were aggregated for each participant (means for normally distributed continuous variables, medians for skewed distributions, and totals for counts) and analyzed at the group level. All statistical analyses and figure creation were performed using Python (v3.12.2). The Python packages used included pandas (v2.2.3) for data management, TableOne (v0.9.1) for descriptive statistics, and semopy (v2.3.11) for structural equation modeling (SEM). Continuous variables were tested for normality with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Between-group comparisons of demographic, cognitive, and eye-tracking data were made using independent-sample t-tests for normally distributed variables and Mann–Whitney U tests when normality assumptions were not met. Chi-square tests were used to assess group differences in categorical variables.

Figure 1. Schematic of eye-tracking paradigms. (A) Visually guided saccades. Participants fixated a central target (I) and then made a saccade to a peripheral stimulus (II). This task indexes basic sensorimotor oculomotor function (latency, gain, peak velocity). (B) Antisaccades. Participants fixated centrally (I) and, when a peripheral stimulus appeared, suppressed the automatic response toward it and instead generated a saccade to the mirror-symmetric location on the opposite side (II). This task indexes inhibitory control, executive function, and attention. (C) Memory-guided saccades. Participants fixated centrally (I); a peripheral cue briefly appeared to be remembered (II); after a short delay, they made a saccade to the remembered location (III). This task probes spatial working memory and delayed oculomotor planning. Blue dot = visual stimulus; orange dot = gaze position. Each task comprised 22 trials.
In this study, cognitive performance data obtained from the NeuroTrax computerized assessment were standardized by transforming raw scores into z-scores using the control group’s mean and standard deviation as reference. Z-scores were computed for all participants, regardless of group assignment. Consistent with previously published NeuroTrax-based protocols (Lapeña-Motilva et al., 2025; Doniger et al., 2005; Schweiger et al., 2007), domain-level indices were derived by averaging normalized values from domain-related test scores (e.g., accuracy, response time). A global cognitive score was then calculated by averaging across all domain indices.
We developed multivariate models to evaluate the influence of clinical, psychological, and fatigue-related variables on eye-tracking measures that were significant in univariate analyses. After adjusting for age, sex, and education (Fjell et al., 2010; Roe et al., 2007; Seblova et al., 2020), we entered clinical comorbidities with established associations to cognitive decline—arterial hypertension and diabetes mellitus—because both were more prevalent in the TOS group and have recognized links to cognitive impairment (Bermejo-Pareja et al., 2010; Shome et al., 2025). In contrast, polyneuropathy was not modeled as a confounder because it is an intrinsic feature of TOS and has not been shown to contribute to cognitive dysfunction. Psychological symptoms (BDI-II and BAI) and fatigue (D-FIS) were then included as predictors. To quantify both direct and indirect pathways, we used SEM. Because BDI-II and BAI scores were highly collinear, we derived a single mood-distress composite via principal component analysis; the first component explained 90.92% of the shared variance and was carried forward as the Composite Variable for Depression and Anxiety (CVDA) in all multivariable and SEM models.
All p-values were two-tailed, and statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. Given the proof-of-concept and exploratory nature of this study, no corrections for multiple comparisons were applied. This decision is supported by established recommendations that highlight the potential for such adjustments to increase Type II error unnecessarily and emphasize that they are not essential in hypothesis-generating research (Rothman, 1990; Perneger, 1998; Bender and Lange, 2001).
Results
Of the 50 TOS patients and 50 healthy controls initially recruited, valid eye-tracking data were obtained for 47 and 44 participants, respectively. Participants were excluded due to acquisition-related issues (e.g., inadequate task compliance, calibration errors) or poor preprocessing quality (signal loss, artifacts, unstable tracking) that prevented analysis. As this was an exploratory, proof-of-concept study in a rare condition, a formal a priori sample-size calculation was not feasible. After data collection, a post-hoc power analysis (two-tailed independent-samples t-test, α = 0.05) showed that the analytic sample (N = 91) had approximately 83.8% power to detect the observed medium effect size (Cohen’s d ≈ 0.62) for the global cognitive score. Accordingly, the study was not powered to detect small between-group differences.
Demographic and neuropsychological data are summarized in Table 1. Groups were comparable in terms of age (mean age ~59 years), sex distribution (predominantly female), and educational attainment (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). However, TOS patients exhibited significantly higher prevalence of diabetes mellitus (25.5% vs. 2.3%), arterial hypertension (66.0% vs. 20.5%), and polyneuropathy (72.3% vs. 0%; all p < 0.005). TOS patients also showed markedly worse scores in health-related quality of life (EuroQol-5D index), fatigue (D-FIS), anxiety (BAI), and depression (BDI-II; all p < 0.001). Cognitively, they demonstrated significantly lower global cognitive scores (p = 0.007), with prominent deficits in executive function (p = 0.017), attention (p = 0.014), and information processing speed (p = 0.009). No significant differences were observed in memory subdomains.
Oculomotor tasks
Visually guided saccades
TOS survivors exhibited preserved performance across all visually guided saccade measures. Figure 2 shows the distribution of parameters recorded during the task: (1) number of correct saccades, (2) latency (ms), (3) peak velocity (°/s), (4) gain (executed amplitude divided by target amplitude), (5) number of hypermetric saccades, (6) number of hypometric saccades, (7) number of correct backward saccades, and (8) backward latency (ms). As detailed in Table 2, no statistically significant differences were observed between groups in any parameter (all p > 0.05). TOS participants showed subtle, non-significant trends toward slightly faster backward latencies and marginally higher peak velocities. Gain values remained tightly centered around 1.0 in both groups, indicating accurate amplitude scaling. Variability in spatial error (hypermetric and hypometric saccades) was also comparable. These findings indicate that visually guided saccades are preserved in TOS survivors, with no evidence of disrupted basic oculomotor function.

Figure 2. Violin plots of visually guided saccade measures. Violin plots show the distribution of oculomotor measures for healthy controls (blue) and patients with toxic oil syndrome (orange). From left to right, top row: number of correct saccades, latency (ms), peak velocity (°/s), and gain (executed amplitude divided by target amplitude). Bottom row: number of hypermetric saccades, number of hypometric saccades, number of correct backward saccades, and backward latency (ms). Solid lines indicate group medians; dotted lines indicate group means.
Memory-guided saccades
Performance on the memory-guided saccade task was also preserved in TOS survivors. Figure 3 displays the distribution of measures: (1) number of correct memory-guided saccades, (2) latency (ms), (3) peak velocity (°/s), (4) gain, (5) number of hypermetric saccades, (6) number of hypometric saccades, (7) number of correct backward memory-guided saccades, and (8) backward latency (ms). Compared with controls, TOS participants exhibited mild, non-significant tendencies toward longer latencies, lower peak velocities, and greater dispersion of spatial error (including hypermetric and hypometric errors). Gain values remained close to 1.0 in both groups. As summarized in Table 2, no statistically significant group differences were found in any parameter (all p > 0.05). These results indicate that memory-guided saccadic performance remains largely intact in TOS, suggesting that the integration of spatial working memory with oculomotor planning is preserved.

Figure 3. Violin plots of memory-guided saccade measures. Violin plots show the distribution of oculomotor measures for healthy controls (blue) and patients with toxic oil syndrome (orange). Top row (left to right): number of correct memory-guided saccades, latency (ms), peak velocity (°/s), and gain (executed amplitude divided by target amplitude). Bottom row (left to right): number of hypermetric saccades, number of hypometric saccades, number of correct backward memory-guided saccades, and backward latency (ms). Solid lines indicate group medians; dotted lines indicate group means.
Antisaccades
In contrast, TOS survivors demonstrated impaired antisaccade performance accompanied by increased reflexive responses, consistent with deficits in attentional and executive control. Figure 4 illustrates the distribution of eight parameters: (1) number of correct antisaccades, (2) antisaccade latency (ms), (3) number of correct backward antisaccades, (4) backward antisaccade latency (ms), (5) number of reflexive saccades, (6) reflexive saccade latency (ms), (7) number of backward reflexive saccades, and (8) backward reflexive saccade latency (ms). Compared to matched controls, TOS patients exhibited significantly fewer correct antisaccades (mean 3.6 vs. 5.0; p = 0.029). In parallel, they showed more reflexive saccades (mean 7.0 vs. 5.7; p = 0.033) and a modest increase in backward reflexive saccades (mean 6.3 vs. 5.1; p = 0.040; Table 2). No significant differences were observed in antisaccade latency (p = 0.854), backward antisaccade latency (p = 0.723), or reflexive saccade duration (p = 0.251).

Figure 4. Violin plots of antisaccade measures. Violin plots illustrate the distribution of measures for healthy controls (blue) and patients with toxic oil syndrome (orange). Top row: number of correct antisaccades, antisaccade latency (ms), number of correct backward antisaccades, and backward antisaccade latency (ms). Bottom row: number of reflexive saccades, reflexive saccade latency (ms), number of backward reflexive saccades, and backward reflexive saccade latency (ms). Solid lines indicate group medians; dotted lines indicate group means.
To further examine these associations, we constructed structural equation models (SEMs) adjusting for potential confounders. Attention was specified a priori as the sole mediator because antisaccade performance relies on top-down attentional control, and including multiple, correlated domains would over-parameterize the model in this sample (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Koçoğlu et al., 2021) We intentionally excluded formal executive function scores because of their strong conceptual and statistical overlap with antisaccade performance, which itself is a sensitive and well-established indicator of executive control (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2004; Mirsky et al., 2011). Similarly, we did not include the global cognitive score, which aggregates across multiple domains not directly related to antisaccade generation and could obscure specific associations with oculomotor measures.
Given the higher prevalence of vascular risk factors and mood/fatigue symptoms in the TOS group, these variables were incorporated into the models alongside age, sex, and education. The antisaccade impairment remained significant after full adjustment. The SEMs confirmed that the reduced number of correct antisaccades in TOS survivors remained significant after adjustment (p = 0.02; Figure 5). Attention also showed a significant direct positive influence on antisaccade performance (p = 0.01; Figure 5). By contrast, while TOS exerted significant direct effects on reflexive (p = 0.03; Figure 6) and backward reflexive saccades (p = 0.04; Figure 7), these outcomes were not significantly mediated by attention or other covariates. Overall, the models demonstrate that TOS status consistently impacted antisaccade and reflexive measures, whereas attentional control contributed independently only to antisaccade performance.

Figure 5. Structural equation model for antisaccade performance in toxic oil syndrome (TOS) survivors. Path diagram illustrating the direct and indirect effects of TOS on the number of correct antisaccades, adjusted for covariates. Predictor variables included a composite variable for depression and anxiety (CVDA), fatigue (D-FIS), diabetes mellitus, arterial hypertension, education, sex, and age. Attention was modeled as a potential mediator. Arrows represent standardized regression coefficients with associated p-values. TOS showed a significant direct negative effect on antisaccade performance (β = −1.720, p = 0.02). At the same time, attention was positively associated with performance (β = 0.928, p = 0.01), indicating that both disease status and attentional processes independently influenced antisaccade measures. CVDA, Composite Variable for Depression and Anxiety; D-FIS, Daily Fatigue Impact Scale.

Figure 6. Structural equation model for reflexive saccade performance in toxic oil syndrome (TOS) survivors. Path diagram illustrating the direct and indirect effects of TOS on the number of reflexive saccades, adjusted for covariates. Predictor variables included a composite variable for depression and anxiety (CVDA), fatigue (D-FIS), diabetes mellitus, arterial hypertension, education, sex, and age. Attention was modeled as a potential mediator. Arrows represent standardized regression coefficients with associated p-values. TOS exerted a significant direct positive effect on reflexive saccade frequency (β = 1.761, p = 0.03), while attention showed no significant mediation effect. CVDA, Composite Variable for Depression and Anxiety; D-FIS, Daily Fatigue Impact Scale.

Figure 7. Structural equation model for backward reflexive saccade performance in toxic oil syndrome (TOS) survivors. Path diagram illustrating the direct and indirect effects of TOS on the number of backward reflexive saccades, adjusted for covariates. Predictor variables included a composite variable for depression and anxiety (CVDA), fatigue (D-FIS), diabetes mellitus, arterial hypertension, education, sex, and age. Attention was modeled as a potential mediator. Arrows represent standardized regression coefficients with associated p-values. TOS exerted a significant direct positive effect on backward reflexive saccade frequency (β = 1.433, p = 0.04), while no significant indirect effects through attention or other covariates were detected. CVDA, Composite Variable for Depression and Anxiety; D-FIS, Daily Fatigue Impact Scale.
Discussion
This proof-of-concept study investigated saccadic eye movements and cognitive function in TOS survivors more than four decades after exposure. The most salient finding was a selective impairment in the antisaccade task: TOS participants generated significantly fewer correct antisaccades and exhibited increased reflexive and backward reflexive saccades. In contrast, performance in visually guided and memory-guided saccades was preserved across all parameters, including latency, gain, peak velocity, and spatial error measures. These findings suggest that basic oculomotor control and spatial working memory are preserved, whereas higher-order executive functions, particularly inhibitory control, are selectively impaired. This pattern is consistent with our neuropsychological results, which identified deficits in executive function, attention, and processing speed, with memory performance remaining relatively intact. SEM models confirmed that antisaccade impairments remained significant after adjusting for confounders. Notably, attention exerted an independent positive influence only on correct antisaccade performance (Figure 5), whereas reflexive and backward reflexive saccades (Figures 6 and 7) were driven by TOS status alone. Overall, the converging oculomotor and cognitive data indicate a long-term disruption of frontal-subcortical circuits in TOS survivors.
Our findings build upon earlier TOS studies by employing objective, high-temporal-resolution eye-tracking paradigms to detect subtle executive dysfunction. They also parallel deficits observed in other immune-mediated disorders. For instance, eosinophilia–myalgia syndrome—a condition with shared toxic and immunologic mechanisms—has been associated with similar impairments in executive function and attention, alongside magnetic resonance imaging evidence of subcortical white matter abnormalities (Armstrong et al., 1997). Both syndromes likely represent a continuum of toxic-immune encephalopathies with convergent effects on prefrontal networks (Armstrong et al., 1997).
Comparable profiles have been reported in chronic exposure to organophosphate pesticides, which selectively impair attention, executive functioning, and processing speed, while sparing language and visuospatial abilities (Jamal, 1997; Ross et al., 2013; London et al., 2012). These similarities reinforce the notion that diverse environmental toxicants can lead to common neurobehavioral outcomes via frontal-subcortical disconnection. Notably, organophosphate contamination was an early suspect in TOS etiology, although it was later discredited (Gelpí et al., 2002).
The antisaccade task is a sensitive probe of frontal dysfunction, particularly implicating the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, frontal eye fields, and basal ganglia circuits (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1995). Our finding of impaired antisaccades, alongside elevated reflexive and backward saccades, indicates a failure of inhibitory control, consistent with dysfunction in frontostriatal pathways (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1995). This profile echoes those seen in neurodegenerative disorders, including Parkinson’s disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies (Mosimann et al., 2005), Huntington disease (Patel et al., 2012) and multiple system atrophy (Brooks et al., 2017), as well as psychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia (Caldani et al., 2017; Subramaniam et al., 2018; Obyedkov et al., 2019). Similar antisaccade impairments have also been reported in prodromal or cognitive disorders, notably idiopathic REM sleep behaviour disorder (Hanuška et al., 2019) and mild cognitive impairment (Koçoğlu et al., 2021). Eye_movement abnormalities have also been noted in Alzheimer’s disease dementia (Costanzo et al., 2023) and ataxias (Szpisjak et al., 2021), underscoring the broader relevance of antisaccade deficits marker of frontal-subcortical dysfunction across diverse disorders.
While reflexive saccades are generally preserved in early Alzheimer’s disease, they tend to be impaired in subcortical and mixed dementias (Mosimann et al., 2005). The altered antisaccade pattern in TOS thus further supports selective involvement of executive networks. Functional neuroimaging and lesion studies suggest that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in response suppression, the frontal eye fields in vector inversion, and the anterior cingulate cortex in error monitoring (Funahashi, 2006; Munoz and Everling, 2004; McDowell et al., 2008; Curtis and D’Esposito, 2003).
TOS was originally linked to aniline-contaminated rapeseed oil, triggering a systemic eosinophilic and autoimmune response with central nervous system involvement (Abaitua Borda et al., 1998; Sánchez-Porro Valadés et al., 2003; Polentinos-Castro et al., 2021; Ricoy et al., 1983; Téllez et al., 1987). Neuropathological findings described central chromatolysis in anterior horn cells and brainstem nuclei, alterations in brainstem monoaminergic centers, and non-necrotizing vasculitis with focal ischemia—findings suggestive of microangiopathy and neuroinflammation. Although consistent white matter damage was not observed, the broader pattern suggests diffuse involvement rather than localized lesions (Ricoy et al., 1983; Téllez et al., 1987).
Persistent low-grade neuroinflammation may contribute to the long-term deficits observed in TOS. Animal models of environmental neurotoxicity, including exposure to manganese and organophosphates, consistently exhibit prolonged microglial activation, astrocytosis, and elevated cytokine release (e.g., IL-6, TNF-α), resulting in the disruption of frontal network function (Raison et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2023; Kirkley et al., 2017; Popichak et al., 2018; Ke et al., 2019; Sarkar et al., 2018; Conley et al., 2020; Dinamene et al., 2013). In particular, manganese neurotoxicity has been shown to induce the secretion of pro-inflammatory cytokines (TNF-α, IL-1β, IL-6) by microglia, which in turn amplifies astrocytic reactivity via NF-κB signaling and results in sustained neuroinflammation and cortical dysfunction that persists well beyond the exposure period (Kirkley et al., 2017; Popichak et al., 2018; Ke et al., 2019; Sarkar et al., 2018; Conley et al., 2020; Dinamene et al., 2013). Comparable mechanisms have been described in organophosphate models, where sustained microglia-mediated neuroinflammation amplifies neuronal hyperactivation and disrupts cortical network function long after the acute exposure (Somkhit et al., 2022). In addition, endothelial dysfunction may further compromise cerebral perfusion in metabolically demanding prefrontal regions (Iadecola, 2013).
Importantly, the absence of elevated neurodegeneration biomarkers in TOS patients argues against progressive neuronal loss, distinguishing this syndrome from Alzheimer’s disease (Ruiz-Ortiz et al., 2025). Consistent with this, preserved episodic memory and the absence of hippocampal dysfunction suggest a static or slowly progressive encephalopathy, possibly more akin to conditions such as multiple sclerosis, small vessel disease or long COVID, where frontal-subcortical networks are preferentially affected (Benito-León et al., 2025; O’Brien et al., 2003; Jolly et al., 2025; Olazarán et al., 2009). Altogether, our findings support a model of non-degenerative, immune-mediated disruption of prefrontal white matter integrity in TOS, manifesting as enduring deficits in executive function and response inhibition.
Our study’s limitations include a small sample size, which may have reduced the ability to detect subtle differences in latency or gain metrics. This limited sample size increases the risk of a Type II error and, consistent with the exploratory nature of the study, justifies not applying corrections for multiple comparisons. The cross-sectional design also restricts causal conclusions. Polyneuropathy was not treated as a confounder; although characteristic of chronic toxic oil syndrome, it is not linked to cognitive dysfunction, and adjusting for it could lead to over-adjustment and mask the true effects of the disease. Nonetheless, we acknowledge its clinical implications and report its prevalence and distribution in Table 1. Comorbidities such as hypertension, diabetes, and higher levels of anxiety, depression, and fatigue were more frequent in the TOS group. Still, these factors were incorporated into our multivariable and SEM models alongside demographic variables. Importantly, the antisaccade deficit remained significant after full adjustment, suggesting that the observed association reflects disease-related effects rather than the influence of comorbid conditions alone. However, residual confounding cannot be completely ruled out. The lack of neuroimaging data further limits structural interpretation. Future research should include both structural and functional neuroimaging to better localize and understand the observed deficits. Finally, our findings may not apply beyond the specific group of Spanish TOS survivors studied. Comparative research in other populations exposed to toxins—such as workers exposed to organophosphates or patients with eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome—could help determine whether antisaccade deficits are a common feature of chronic toxic-immune encephalopathies.
In conclusion, to our knowledge, this is the first study to apply eye-tracking to evaluate cognitive dysfunction in TOS. By combining antisaccade and related paradigms with standardized cognitive testing, we demonstrate ongoing antisaccade impairments and dysexecutive deficits consistent with disruption of frontal-subcortical control. The pattern of findings is more compatible with immune-or vascular-mediated injury than with ongoing neurodegeneration. Given that eye-tracking is objective, noninvasive, brief, and well-tolerated in older, multimorbid cohorts, antisaccade-based measures offer a practical candidate biomarker of residual executive dysfunction that now warrants validation in longitudinal, adequately powered, multimodal studies.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by 12 de Octubre University Hospital. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
JB-L: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL-M: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. MR-O: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. GMD: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. SÁ-S: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. VB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. MN: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. MM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. RM: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. SD: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. FB: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. CA: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. CG-C: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Julián Benito-León, José Lapeña-Motilva, Mariano Ruiz-Ortiz, and Verónica Giménez de Béjar are supported by the Recovery, Transformation, and Resilience Plan of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (grants TED2021-130174B-C33, NETremor, and PID2022-138585OB-C33, Resonate). Mariano Ruiz-Ortiz is also supported through a Río Hortega contract (CM22/00183). Julián Benito-León receives additional support from the National Institutes of Health (NINDS R01 NS39422 and R01 NS094607). Fernando Bartolomé is supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (CP20/00009, PI21/00183, and PI24/00099) and the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (RED2022-134774-T). Carolina Alquézar is supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (CP21/00049 and PI22/00345), the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (RED2022-134774-T and CNS2024-154198), the Eugenio Rodríguez Pascual Foundation (FERP-2022-5), and the Fundación Luzón (Ayudas Unzué Luzón 2024). Cecilia García-Cena is supported by RoboCity 2030-DIH-CM Madrid Robotics Digital Innovation Hub (Applied Robotics for Improving Citizens’ Quality of Life, Phase IV; S2018/NMT-4331), funded by the Community of Madrid and co-financed with EU Structural Funds.
Acknowledgments
We gratefully acknowledge all participating patients for their time, trust, and commitment to this study. Their collaboration made this research possible.
Conflict of interest
GMD is an employee of NeuroTrax Corporation. NeuroTrax Corporation had no role in study design; data collection, analysis, or interpretation; manuscript writing; or the decision to submit the article for publication.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Abaitua Borda, I., Philen, R. M., Posada de la Paz, M., Gómez de la Cámara, A., Díez Ruiz-Navarro, M., Giménez Ribota, O., et al. (1998). Toxic oil syndrome mortality: the first 13 years. Int. J. Epidemiol. 27, 1057–1063.
Armstrong, C., Lewis, T., D’Esposito, M., and Freundlich, B. (1997). Eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome: selective cognitive impairment, longitudinal effects, and neuroimaging findings. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 63, 633–641.
Badia, X., Roset, M., Herdman, M., and Kind, P. (2001). A comparison of United Kingdom and Spanish general population time trade-off values for EQ-5D health states. Med. Decis. Mak. 21, 7–16. doi: 10.1177/0272989x0102100102
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897.
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory -II (BDI_II) [Database record]. J. Clin. Epidemiol. doi: 10.1037/t00742-000
Bender, R., and Lange, S. (2001). Adjusting for multiple testing--when and how? J. Clin. Epidemiol. 54, 343–349. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00314-0
Benito-León, J., Lapeña, J., García-Vasco, L., Cuevas, C., Viloria-Porto, J., Calvo-Córdoba, A., et al. (2025). Exploring cognitive dysfunction in long COVID patients: eye movement abnormalities and frontal-subcortical circuits implications via eye-tracking and machine learning. Am. J. Med. 138, 550–559. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2024.04.004
Benito-León, J., Lapeña-Motilva, J., and García-Cena, C. (2024). Eye-tracking technology: a tool to enhance understanding of memory complaints in COVID-19 survivors. CNS Spectr. 29, 534–535. doi: 10.1017/s1092852924002426
Benito-León, J., Martínez-Martín, P., Frades, B., Martínez-Ginés, M. L., De Andrés, C., Meca-Lallana, J. E., et al. (2007). Impact of fatigue in multiple sclerosis: the fatigue impact scale for daily use (D-FIS). Mult. Scler. 13, 645–651. doi: 10.1177/1352458506073528
Bermejo-Pareja, F., Benito-León, J., Louis, E. D., Trincado, R., Carro, E., Villarejo, A., et al. (2010). Risk of incident dementia in drug-untreated arterial hypertension: a population-based study. J Alzheimer's Dis 22, 949–958. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-101110
Brooks, S. H., Klier, E. M., Red, S. D., Mehta, N. D., Patel, S. S., Chuang, A. Z., et al. (2017). Slowed Prosaccades and increased Antisaccade errors as a potential behavioral biomarker of multiple system atrophy. Front. Neurol. 8:261. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00261
Brooks, J. S., Smith, W. J., Webb, B. M., Heath, M. D., and Dickey, J. P. (2019). Development and validation of a high-speed video system for measuring saccadic eye movement. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 2302–2309. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01197-2
Caldani, S., Bucci, M. P., Lamy, J.-C., Seassau, M., Bendjemaa, N., Gadel, R., et al. (2017). Saccadic eye movements as markers of schizophrenia Spectrum: exploration in at-risk mental states. Schizophr. Res. 181, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.09.003
Conley, T. E., Beaudin, S. A., Lasley, S. M., Fornal, C. A., Hartman, J., Uribe, W., et al. (2020). Early postnatal manganese exposure causes arousal dysregulation and lasting Hypofunctioning of the prefrontal cortex Catecholaminergic systems. J. Neurochem. 153, 631–649. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14934
Costanzo, E., Lengyel, I., Parravano, M., Biagini, I., Veldsman, M., Badhwar, A., et al. (2023). Ocular biomarkers for Alzheimer disease dementia: an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. JAMA Ophthalmol. 141:84. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2022.4845
Curtis, C. E., and D’Esposito, M. (2003). Success and failure suppressing reflexive behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 409–418. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593126
De La Paz, M. P., Philen, R. M., and Borda, I. A. (2001). Toxic oil syndrome: the perspective after 20 years. Epidemiol. Rev. 23, 231–247. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.epirev.a000804
De La Paz, M. P., Philen, R. M., Gerr, F., Letz, R., Ferrari Arroyo, M. J., Vela, L., et al. (2003). Neurologic outcomes of toxic oil syndrome patients 18 years after the epidemic. Environ. Health Perspect. 111, 1326–1334. doi: 10.1289/ehp.111-1241614
Del Ser, T., Espasandín, P., Cabetas, I., and Arredondo, J. M. (1986). Memory disorders in the toxic oil syndrome (TOS). Arch. Neurobiol. (Madr.) 49, 19–39.
Dinamene, S., Camila, B. M., Tavares De Almeida, I., Davis Randall, L., Luisa, M. M., Vanda, A., et al. (2013). Evaluation of neurobehavioral and Neuroinflammatory end-points in the post-exposure period in rats sub-acutely exposed to manganese. Toxicology 314, 95–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tox.2013.09.008
Doniger, G., Zucker, D., Schweiger, A., Dwolatzky, T., Chertkow, H., Crystal, H., et al. (2005). Towards practical cognitive assessment for detection of early dementia: a 30-minute computerized battery discriminates as well as longer testing. CAR 2, 117–124. doi: 10.2174/1567205053585792
Dwolatzky, T., Whitehead, V., Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S., Schweiger, A., Jaffe, D., et al. (2003). Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatr. 3:4. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-3-4
Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Westlye, L. T., Østby, Y., Tamnes, C. K., Jernigan, T. L., et al. (2010). When does brain aging accelerate? Dangers of quadratic fits in cross-sectional studies. Neuro Image 50, 1376–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.061
Funahashi, S. (2006). Prefrontal cortex and working memory processes. Neuroscience 139, 251–261. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.07.003
García Cena, C. E., Andrés, D. G., and Valdeolivas, I. P. (2020). Measurement and analysis of eye movements performance to predict healthy brain aging. IEEE Access 8, 87201–87213. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2020.2992254
García Cena, C., Costa, M. C., Saltarén Pazmiño, R., Santos, C. P., Gómez-Andrés, D., and Benito-León, J. (2022a). Eye movement alterations in post-COVID-19 condition: a proof-of-concept study. Sensors 22:1481. doi: 10.3390/s22041481
García Cena, C. E., Gómez-Andrés, D., Pulido-Valdeolivas, I., Sánchez-Seco, V. G., Domingo-Santos, A., Moreno-García, S., et al. (2022b). Toward an automatic assessment of cognitive dysfunction in relapsing–remitting multiple sclerosis patients using eye movement analysis. Sensors 22:8220. doi: 10.3390/s22218220
Gelpí, E., de la Paz, M. P., Terracini, B., Abaitua, I., de la Cámara, A. G., Kilbourne, E. M., et al. (2002). The Spanish toxic oil syndrome 20 years after its onset: a multidisciplinary review of scientific knowledge. Environ. Health Perspect. 110, 457–464. doi: 10.1289/ehp.110-1240833
Guerrero-Molina, M. P., Rodríguez-López, C., Panadés-de Oliveira, L., Uriarte-Pérez De Urabayen, D., Garzo-Caldas, N., García-Cena, C. E., et al. (2021). Antisaccades and memory-guided saccades in genetic generalized epilepsy and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 123:108236. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.108236
Hanuška, J., Rusz, J., Bezdicek, O., Ulmanová, O., Bonnet, C., Dušek, P., et al. (2019). Eye movements in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: high Antisaccade error rate reflects prefrontal cortex dysfunction. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12742. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12742
Hellmuth, J., Mirsky, J., Heuer, H. W., Matlin, A., Jafari, A., Garbutt, S., et al. (2012). Multicenter validation of a bedside Antisaccade task as a measure of executive function. Neurology 78, 1824–1831. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f785
Iadecola, C. (2013). The pathobiology of vascular dementia. Neuron 80, 844–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.008
Jamal, G. A. (1997). Neurological syndromes of organophosphorus compounds. Adverse Drug React. Toxicol. Rev. 16, 133–170.
Jolly, A. A., Anyanwu, S., Koohi, F., Morris, R. G., and Markus, H. S. (2025). Prevalence of fatigue and associations with depression and cognitive impairment in patients with CADASIL. Neurology 104:e213335. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000213335
Kaufman, L. D., Izquierdo Martínez, M., Serrano, J. M., and Gómez-Reino, J. J. (1995). 12-year followup study of epidemic Spanish toxic oil syndrome. J. Rheumatol. 22, 282–288.
Kaufman, L. D., and Krupp, L. B. (1995). Eosinophilia-myalgia syndrome, toxic-oil syndrome, and diffuse fasciitis with eosinophilia. Curr. Opin. Rheumatol. 7, 560–567.
Ke, T., Sidoryk-Wegrzynowicz, M., Pajarillo, E., Rizor, A., Soares, F. A. A., Lee, E., et al. (2019). Role of astrocytes in manganese neurotoxicity revisited. Neurochem. Res. 44, 2449–2459. doi: 10.1007/s11064-019-02881-7
Kirkley, K. S., Popichak, K. A., Afzali, M. F., Legare, M. E., and Tjalkens, R. B. (2017). Microglia amplify inflammatory activation of astrocytes in manganese neurotoxicity. J. Neuroinflammation 14:99. doi: 10.1186/s12974-017-0871-0
Koçoğlu, K., Hodgson, T. L., Eraslan Boz, H., and Akdal, G. (2021). Deficits in saccadic eye movements differ between subtypes of patients with mild cognitive impairment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 43, 187–198. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2021.1900077
Lapeña-Motilva, J., Ruiz-Ortiz, M., Doniger, G. M., Nogales, M. A., Giménez De Béjar, V., Álvarez-Sesmero, S., et al. (2025). Cognitive functioning in toxic oil syndrome survivors: a case-control study four decades after the epidemic. JCM 14:3746. doi: 10.3390/jcm14113746
Leng, Q., Deng, B., and Ju, Y. (2024). Application and Progress of advanced eye movement Examinations in Cognitive Impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1377406. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1377406
London, L., Beseler, C., Bouchard, M. F., Bellinger, D. C., Colosio, C., Grandjean, P., et al. (2012). Neurobehavioral and neurodevelopmental effects of pesticide exposures. Neuro Toxicology 33, 887–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2012.01.004
Martínez-Martín, P., Catalán, M. J., Benito-León, J., Moreno, A. O., Zamarbide, I., Cubo, E., et al. (2006). Impact of fatigue in Parkinson’s disease: the fatigue impact scale for daily use (D-FIS). Qual. Life Res. 15, 597–606. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-4181-0
McDowell, J. E., Dyckman, K. A., Austin, B. P., and Clementz, B. A. (2008). Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn. 68, 255–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.016
Mirsky, J. B., Heuer, H. W., Jafari, A., Kramer, J. H., Schenk, A. K., Viskontas, I. V., et al. (2011). Anti-saccade performance predicts executive function and brain structure in Normal elders. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 24, 50–58. doi: 10.1097/wnn.0b013e318223f6c6
Mosimann, U. P., Müri, R. M., Burn, D. J., Felblinger, J., O’Brien, J. T., and McKeith, I. G. (2005). Saccadic eye movement changes in Parkinson’s disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain 128, 1267–1276. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh484
Munoz, D. P., and Everling, S. (2004). Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 218–228. doi: 10.1038/nrn1345
Nieuwenhuis, S., Broerse, A., Nielen, M. M. A., and Jong, R. D. (2004). A goal activation approach to the study of executive function: an application to Antisaccade tasks. Brain Cogn. 56, 198–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2003.12.002
O’Brien, J. T., Erkinjuntti, T., Reisberg, B., Roman, G., Sawada, T., Pantoni, L., et al. (2003). Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(03)00305-3
Obyedkov, I., Skuhareuskaya, M., Skugarevsky, O., Obyedkov, V., Buslauski, P., Skuhareuskaya, T., et al. (2019). Saccadic eye movements in different dimensions of schizophrenia and in clinical high-risk state for psychosis. BMC Psychiatry 19:110. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2093-8
Olazarán, J., Cruz, I., Benito-León, J., Morales, J. M., Duque, P., and Rivera-Navarro, J. (2009). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: methods and prevalence from the GEDMA study. Eur. Neurol. 61, 87–93. doi: 10.1159/000177940
Patel, S. S., Jankovic, J., Hood, A. J., Jeter, C. B., and Sereno, A. B. (2012). Reflexive and volitional saccades: biomarkers of Huntington disease severity and progression. J. Neurol. Sci. 313, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.09.035
Perneger, T. V. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ 316, 1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236
Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., Müri, R., and Vermersch, A. (1995). Cortical control of saccades. Ann. Neurol. 37, 557–567.
Polentinos-Castro, E., Biec-Amigo, T., Delgado-Magdalena, M., Flores-Acosta, J. M., Sánchez-Perruca, L., Rabanal-Carrera, A., et al. (2021). Chronic diseases and multimorbidiy in patients with toxic oil syndrome: a comparative study with general population. Rev. Esp. Salud Publica 95:e202104047.
Popichak, K. A., Afzali, M. F., Kirkley, K. S., and Tjalkens, R. B. (2018). Glial-neuronal signaling mechanisms underlying the Neuroinflammatory effects of manganese. J. Neuroinflammation 15:324. doi: 10.1186/s12974-018-1349-4
Raison, C. L., Capuron, L., and Miller, A. H. (2006). Cytokines sing the blues: inflammation and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends Immunol. 27, 24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.it.2005.11.006
Ricoy, J. R., Cabello, A., Rodriguez, J., and Téllez, I. (1983). Neuropathological studies on the toxic syndrome related to adulterated rapeseed oil in Spain. Brain 106, 817–835.
Roe, C. M., Xiong, C., Miller, J. P., and Morris, J. C. (2007). Education and Alzheimer disease without dementia: support for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neurology 68, 223–228. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000251303.50459.8a
Ross, S. M., McManus, I. C., Harrison, V., and Mason, O. (2013). Neurobehavioral problems following low-level exposure to organophosphate pesticides: a systematic and Meta-analytic review. Crit. Rev. Toxicol. 43, 21–44. doi: 10.3109/10408444.2012.738645
Rothman, K. J. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology 1, 43–46. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199001000-00010
Ruiz-Ortiz, M., Lapeña-Motilva, J., Giménez De Bejar, V., Bartolomé, F., Alquézar, C., Martínez-Castillo, M., et al. (2025). Blood biomarkers of neurodegeneration over four decades after toxic oil syndrome: a case-control study. IJMS 26:5122. doi: 10.3390/ijms26115122
Sánchez-Porro Valadés, P., Posada de la Paz, M., de Andrés Copa, P., Gimenez Ribota, O., and Abaitua Borda, I. (2003). Toxic oil syndrome: survival in the whole cohort between 1981 and 1995. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 56, 701–708. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(03)00119-7
Sarkar, S., Malovic, E., Harischandra, D. S., Ngwa, H. A., Ghosh, A., Hogan, C., et al. (2018). Manganese exposure induces Neuroinflammation by impairing mitochondrial dynamics in astrocytes. Neuro Toxicology 64, 204–218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2017.05.009
Schweiger, A., Abramovitch, A., Doniger, G. M., and Simon, E. S. (2007). A clinical construct validity study of a novel computerized battery for the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 100–111. doi: 10.1080/13803390500519738
Seblova, D., Berggren, R., and Lövdén, M. (2020). Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: systematic review and Meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 58:101005. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.101005
Shome, G., Mondal, R., Deb, S., Roy, J., Mandal, A. K., and Benito-León, J. (2025). Bridging pancreatic amyloidosis and neurodegeneration: the emerging role of amylin in diabetic dementia. IJMS 26:5021. doi: 10.3390/ijms26115021
Somkhit, J., Yanicostas, C., and Soussi-Yanicostas, N. (2022). Microglia Remodelling and Neuroinflammation parallel neuronal Hyperactivation following acute organophosphate poisoning. IJMS 23:8240. doi: 10.3390/ijms23158240
Subramaniam, A., Danivas, V., Mahavir Agarwal, S., Kalmady, S., Shivakumar, V., Amaresha, A. C., et al. (2018). Clinical correlates of saccadic eye movement in antipsychotic-naïve schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 259, 154–159. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.011
Szpisjak, L., Szaraz, G., Salamon, A., Nemeth, V. L., Szepfalusi, N., Veres, G., et al. (2021). Eye-tracking-aided characterization of saccades and Antisaccades in SYNE1 Ataxia patients: a pilot study. BMC Neurosci. 22:7. doi: 10.1186/s12868-021-00612-9
Téllez, I., Cabello, A., Franch, O., and Ricoy, J. R. (1987). Chromatolytic changes in the central nervous system of patients with the toxic oil syndrome. Acta Neuropathol. 74, 354–361.
Wolf, A., Tripanpitak, K., Umeda, S., and Otake-Matsuura, M. (2023). Eye-tracking paradigms for the assessment of mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1197567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1197567
Keywords: toxic oil syndrome, frontal-subcortical dysfunction, saccadic eye movements, cognitive impairment, eye-tracker
Citation: Benito-León J, Lapeña-Motilva J, Ruiz-Ortiz M, Doniger GM, Álvarez-Sesmero S, de Béjar VG, Nogales MA, Morales M, Mondal R, Deb S, Bartolomé F, Alquézar C and García-Cena C (2025) Frontal-subcortical dysfunction in toxic oil syndrome: a proof-of-concept eye-tracking and cognitive study four decades after exposure. Front. Neurosci. 19:1666809. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2025.1666809
Edited by:
Ran Wang, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, ChinaReviewed by:
Junyi Zhou, Fujian Normal University, ChinaGustavo De Sá Oliveira Lima, Universidade Federal da Bahia, Brazil
Copyright © 2025 Benito-León, Lapeña-Motilva, Ruiz-Ortiz, Doniger, Álvarez-Sesmero, de Béjar, Nogales, Morales, Mondal, Deb, Bartolomé, Alquézar and García-Cena. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Julián Benito-León, amJlbml0b2w2N0BnbWFpbC5jb20=