ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Polit. Sci., 14 July 2025

Sec. Elections and Representation

Volume 7 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2025.1616180

This article is part of the Research TopicPolitical Communication Strategies and the Instrumentalization of Hate Speech in Electoral CampaignsView all articles

Asymmetric and sexist polarization: political self-positioning as a moderator of the relationship between extremism and the justification of violence among adolescents


Francisco Villegas LirolaFrancisco Villegas LirolaPilar Rodriguez Martinez
Pilar Rodriguez Martinez*Antonio Jesus Segura SnchezAntonio Jesus Segura SánchezLucia Martinez JoyaLucia Martinez Joya
  • University of Almeria, Almería, Andalusia, Spain

In this paper, we present the results of a study on violent extremism and political self-positioning of adolescents in the province of Almerí (Spain). This research is based on a questionnaire conducted in secondary schools in Almerí (4) and Madrid (1) in May 2022. We tried to answer the following questions: are social extremism and the justification of violent illegal acts related? To what extent does political self-positioning moderate the relationship between extremism and the justification of violence among Alpha Generation adolescents? Does this occur in the same way for boys and girls? We proceeded to adapt and validate a scale of violent extremism. For a sample of 1,170 students aged 12–19 years, a hierarchical regression analysis was conducted for a multicategorical moderator variable. The results indicate that political polarization among adolescents does not manifest itself in the same way for those who identify with the most radical ideologies on the political spectrum. The endorsement of pro-violence attitudes is a notable phenomenon, particularly among boys who align with the political extreme right.

Introduction

Adolescents born around 2013 onwards are part of a generation that has been labeled the iPad generation, the Alpha generation, or the Polars (Jaiswal, 2023). This last term refers to two significant challenges that this generation will likely face: the melting of the polar ice caps due to climate change and political polarization (Twenge, 2023, p. 2). In this article, we will focus on the latter problem, that of political polarization.

Specifically, we will seek to explore the relationship between political self-positioning and violent extremism in a sample of adolescents born around 2013, and currently in secondary education in Almeria (Spain). We will begin by alluding to the general characteristics of this new generation of adolescents.

While it is not clear that they constitute a generation different from Generation Z (Nagy and Köllcsey, 2017), it seems that the most characteristic features of this Alpha generation would be the fact that they have been socialized in the digital world and in the new technological trends, apps, tweets, memes, social networks, tablets, and in the colloquial slang of these media (McCrindle and Wolfinger, 2009).

These adolescents are highly connected, with a preference for video formats. They are independent in making their own decisions, and they are diverse in demographics (such as ethnicity and gender), as well as in terms of their tastes and points of view (Iberdrola, 2023). However, it is important to note that one in three adolescents may engage in problematic use of the Internet and social media (Iberdrola, 2023).

Additionally, one in five may have a certain addiction to video games, while one in five may be suffering from cyberbullying. Furthermore, 29% indicate that their caregivers do not set limits or rules on the use of the Internet and screens (Andrade et al., 2021). The authors believe that it is important to encourage this generation to develop skills such as managing emotions, forming stable relationships, and integrating technology into their daily lives (Jha, 2020, p. 9). It would seem that happiness, satisfaction with life and expectations have all decreased somewhat (Twenge, 2023). However, it would be a mistake to assume that this generation is homogeneous.

In the Spanish case, the INJUVE study on political ideology of young people pointed out in 1917 that young people tended to place themselves near the center of the ideological scale, slightly to the left, while they placed their parents/mothers more to the right (INJUVE, 2017). The 2020 report (INJUVE, 2021) pointed out that it should be studied where young people are getting political information, since the consumption of political information through social networks or internet had reached the levels of television, which had been until that moment the most used media to get informed. And that, this trend is being observed in all the countries of our environment and will be especially pronounced among the youngest people (INJUVE, 2021, p. 188).

We live in polarized, or perhaps asymmetrically polarized, environments, as noted by authors who note that “polarization is mainly due to a sharp retreat of moderation on the right side of the political spectrum” (Hacker and Pierson, 2017, p. 59). In these environments, the lack of trust in democratic processes, the political detachment of the citizenry—exacerbated in the case of young people—, and the rise of violent extremism constitute serious threats against the values of freedom, citizenship and tolerance that should define a democratic society (Azqueta and Merino-Arribas, 2022). It is possible that adolescents may be more susceptible to the processes of extremism and radicalization (Tamayo Sáez et al., 2021), particularly through their exposure to hate speech (Wachs et al., 2022), as they are mainly informed through social media. In fact, Rodríguez Martínez et al. (2024) found that the amount and type of daily social media use moderated the relationship between extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts among adolescents aged 12 to 19. Greater social media use does not correlate with being more extremist; rather, it correlates with being more supportive of violence.

In recent years, an increasing number of articles in the media have also highlighted that there may be a growing gender gap occurring among high school seniors. While teenage boys appear to be becoming more conservative, girls seem to be leaning more toward liberal or progressive views (De-Visé, 2023), which highlights the differences in the gender gap over political issues.

The gender gap persists with regard to the use of violence. This may indicate the rise of toxic masculinity, which involves a stronger commitment to traditional male gender roles that stigmatize and restrict the expression of emotions typically associated with femininity, while promoting anger and other similar emotions (Liu, 2016). This closely links to the defense of violent attitudes. Indeed, a review of the literature on the subject reveals that several studies conducted in Spain have demonstrated that the content of children's television programs in 2021 is 2fold more violent than that of the preceding year. This is evidenced by the prevalence of violence against women and other men, perpetrated by boys, and violence against women and other women, perpetrated by girls (CIMA, 2023). These data are significant in light of the fact that in 2021 Spain occupied the first position in terms of hours of television consumption (219 min per day) in comparison to the rest of the European Union countries. With regard to behaviors, data on bullying and cyberbullying in childhood (12 to 16 years) indicate that there is a notable disparity between boys and girls in Spain. In particular, a higher percentage of boys (6.3%) than girls (3.5%) have been identified as aggressors. This discrepancy persists with regard to cyberbullying. A 2016 study by Save the Children found that 4.5% of boys had engaged in cyberaggression, compared to 3% of girls. A recent study on cyber violence in Spain concludes that women, especially young women, are the main victims of cyberbullying (Sousa et al., 2024).

In any case, when referring to violent extremism, it is important to notice that we can refer to either attitudes or behaviors. When referring to attitudes, we usually use the term cognitive radicalization. When referring to behaviors, we use the term behavioral radicalization. However, attitudes and behaviors are not always linked. Many people, including adolescents, who hold radical (or extremist) views, will never resort to violent behavior to defend their beliefs (Wolfowicz et al., 2021). There is no single profile or path for a person to undergo behavioral radicalization, which does not necessarily follow cognitive radicalization. Furthermore, most adolescents (ages 12–19) do not express extremist or pro-violent attitudes (Nivette et al., 2022).

In this study we aim to answer these questions: to what extent does political self-positioning moderate the relationship between extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts among adolescents of the Alpha generation? Does it occur in the same way for both sexes? Assuming that extremist positions are those that are going to justify violence to a greater extent, it is also worth asking whether pro-violence attitudes are associated with left-wing or right-wing extremism.

We will present the results of a study on violent extremism and political self-positioning of adolescents based on a questionnaire conducted in secondary schools in the province of Almería in May 2022. We will begin by presenting the process that leads to the development of violent extremist attitudes and the concept of political self-positioning. We will then present our hypotheses. We will report on how we collected the data and highlight the main results of our analysis.

Socialization, environments and violent extremism

During adolescence, there are three primary environments influence socialization: the family, school, and peer groups. The family plays a fundamental role in the socialization of young people. Positive family experiences can foster resilience in the face of violence (Spalek, 2016). Conversely, an inadequate family environment can make adolescents feel detached and expose them to behavioral models based on poor conflict management (Farrington et al., 2009). Harsh treatment, the imposition of rules, and parental violence can lead to violent extremist attitudes or behaviors (Baier et al., 2016).

The school environment can redirect extremist attitudes toward activism and improve young people's resilience (Koirikivi et al., 2021). However, it can also increase feelings of failure and frustration, leading to violence. Adolescents often distance themselves emotionally from their parents, meaning their peer group plays a significant role in shaping their ideologies and attitudes (Greve, 2007). Linked to family breakdown and school failure, this phenomenon can facilitate identification with extremist groups that engage in violent behavior and identify as victims of established social structures (Eldor et al., 2022). Participation in shared spaces and leisure activities where aggressive and violent behavior is common can contribute to the normalization of such behavior. These dynamics can cause adolescents to view violence as a justified means of achieving their ideological goals and accept the culture of aggression in these social contexts.

The development of violent extremist attitudes

The term violent extremist attitudes encompasses the acceptance of a radical ideology at the cognitive level and a pattern of behavior that includes engaging in, participating in, sustaining, or supporting violent actions (Bazaga-Fernández and Tamayo-Sáez, 2021). The development of these attitudes can be explained by three factors, as outlined in the 3N radicalization model (Kruglanski et al., 2019). Firstly, the need to feel respected and to imbue one's life with meaning is a significant factor (Kruglanski et al., 2013). Consequently, when the adolescent self-perceives humiliation, oppression, oblivion, social alienation or uncertainty, one option is the search for meaning through the formation of a group identity (N1). The group serves as a social network (N2), offering a source of empowerment through mere membership (Jost et al., 2008). Bonds based on opposition to those outside the group are strengthened (Swann et al., 2012). Consequently, there is a distinction between the in-group and the out-group (Webber and Kruglanski, 2017). One indicator of identity is the utilization of shared narratives or interpretations (N3) of a polarized society (Jost et al., 2008). This discourse is predicated on the condemnation of actions deemed wrong (García-Juanatey et al., 2020). Under this meaning of identity, the dichotomy of friend and foe is postulated (Talshir, 2005).

One's own positions are elevated, and violence and aggression toward opposing groups are legitimized (Lobato et al., 2020). This construction of us serves as the foundation for justifying violence and illegal acts as a legitimate form of defense against perceived aggression from the them (Ozer and Bertelsen, 2018; Lobato et al., 2020). It is often observed that violent extremist groups elaborate their messages on the basis of perceived grievances directed toward the target groups (Schils and Verhage, 2017). For example, “the H7.-group, which is associated with the VOX political party, has been known to espouse views that are perceived as hateful and xenophobic. [...] an extreme right-wing political party [...] of hatred of foreigners, hatred of the MENA region, LGTB regulation, and other similar sentiments. Ultimately, many individuals espouse these views and utilize the flag of Spain as a symbol of their affiliation. They are inciting hatred among the general population” (CIS, 2023a).

In Rodriguez Martinez et al. (2023), the authors present a comprehensive review of instruments designed to measure violent extremist attitudes among adolescents. Indeed, one of the most effective instruments is the one developed by Ozer and Bertelsen (2018). The authors emphasized the necessity of developing and validating two generic scales for empirical research on violent radicalization, specifically concerning (1) approval of extremism and (2) acceptance of violent and/or illegal means. They posit that extremist attitudes are related to two interdependent dimensions: (1) approval of global personal and social change and (2) intolerance toward other groups of citizens who oppose these social changes. The construction of the two scales is based on the assumption that extremist attitudes per se need not be accompanied by pro-violence attitudes. Ozer and Bertelsen (2018) designated their scales as the Extremism Scale and the Pro-violence and Illegal Acts in Relation to Extremism Scale. The initial scale was designed to assess attitudes toward comprehensive sociocultural transformation and intolerance toward others through group dynamics. The first component included lifestyle and culture, socioeconomic foundations, and the governmental system. The second component concerned the us-them distinction, devaluation of others, breakdown of deliberation, and inability to coexist. The second scale was designed to measure attitudes of pro-violence and acceptance of the use of illegal means in relation to extremism, with a focus on various domains, including society, a higher cause, family and friends, and the group.

Both scales were tested with high school students from Denmark and the United States and yielded consistent results; in addition to information on age, gender, and country of birth, the measures contained five self-report measurement scales: The Extremism Scale consisted of 14 items scored on a 7-point scale, and the Pro-violence and Illegal Acts in Relation to Extremism Scale included 6 items relating to acceptance of the use of violence and 6 relating to acceptance of the use of illegal means. Both scales ranged from (1) Strongly Disagree to (7) Strongly Agree. According to the authors, both scales confirmed the initially assumed distinction between attitudes related to activism and those referring to radicalism; moreover, they considered that the Extremism Scale was able to identify extremism through the criterion of a deeply felt desire to change the conditions of life and leaving aside concerns for human coexistence.

As we argued earlier, the processes that lead to the development of violent extremist attitudes can be expected to be associated with both far-left and far-right political positions, although, as the authors point out, right-wing and left-wing populisms may not coincide on the same goals: “although a wide range of politicians advocate for the people, left-wing populists prefer to attack the economic elite. Attacks on the media elite and ostracism of others, however, are predominantly made by right-wing speakers” (Engesser et al., 2017, p. 1109). We will now turn our attention to the tools that can be used to measure adolescents' political self-positioning.

Political self-positioning

The authors highlight the utility of the political self-positioning scale as a tool for identifying the ideological positions of citizens in advanced democracies (Peral and Calvo, 2023; Bauer et al., 2017). In fact, this scale is included in the question asked in the barometers of the Centro de Investigaciones Sociológicas (CIS). It should be noted that while the CIS repository (https://www.cis.es/) contains questionnaires dating back to June 1979, the first instance of a specific question on political self-positioning appears in March 1981 (CIS, 2023b). From March 1981 to May 1983 (CIS, 2023c), the scale had seven options and the question was formulated as follows: “When thinking about political tendencies, the words left and right are normally used. Please place yourself on the following scale according to your political tendency: 1 extreme left, 2 left, 3 center left, 4 center, 5 center right, 6 right, 7 extreme right, 8 don't know, 9 no answer.” (CIS, 2023c). Since June 1983 (CIS, 2023d), up to the present, the scale has ten options, from extreme left (1) to extreme right (10), although authors such as Camino (2022) propose a simplification that entails the recategorization of the variable into five categories: 1 and 2 for extreme left, 3 and 4 for left, 5 and 6 for center, 7 and 8 for right, 9 and 10 for extreme right.

In general, those who define themselves as left-wing are concerned with issues associated with social justice, global inequality, climate change or women's rights, while those who define themselves as right-wing are concerned with social order, criminality, capitalism, national symbols or immigration (García-Sánchez et al., 2022). On the other hand, Peral and Calvo (2023) found that, although “citizens place themselves on the left-right scale according to their values or social status, they do so more significantly according to their proximity to political parties” (Peral and Calvo, 2023, p. 36). Few studies analyze political ideology and the use of violence for ideological reasons. Jasko et al. (2022) point out that data on extremists in the United States indicates that left-wing extremists are less likely to use violence than right-wing extremists and Islamists. Using global databases, the authors demonstrate that attacks motivated by left-wing groups were less deadly than those motivated by right-wing and Islamist groups. Nevertheless, the percentage of extremists who use violence remains very low. For example, in the United States, the probability was 0.33% for left-wing extremists, 0.61% for right-wing extremists, and 0.62% for Islamists.

Research hypothesis

The following research hypotheses were proposed:

[H1] Attitudes about social extremism are directly related to those related to the justification of violence and illegal acts.

[H2] The perception of aggressive or violent behavior in different social settings significantly correlates with political self-positioning.

[H3] Perception of aggressive or violent behavior in different social settings significantly moderates the relationship between social extremism (ES) and justification of violence—illegal acts (PIARES).

[H4] Political self-positioning exerts an influence on the relationship between attitudes about social extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts.

[H5] There is a significant difference between the influence of far-left political self-positioning and that of the far left on the relationship between social extremism and attitudes toward the justification of violence and illegal acts.

[H6] The relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence and illegal acts is moderated by sex.

Method

Participants

The data for this research were collected from students in public and private secondary schools located in Almería (4) and Madrid (1) during May 2022. This research project was conducted in the context of a larger study on the violent radicalization of adolescents. A total of 1,175 participants completed the questionnaire. Questionnaires from participants who did not indicate their gender (12 participants), did not complete any items related to calculating the Social Extremism Scale or the Pro-Violence and Illegal Acts in Relation to Social Extremism Scale (9 participants), or answered all items on these scales with the same score (19 participants, due to acquiescence bias) were excluded. After eliminating incomplete questionnaires, the sample consisted of 1,135 participants. The participants' ages ranged from 12 to 19 years, with a mean age of 15.6 years. The sample consisted of 581 females (51.2%) and 554 males. The data can be accessed via the following link: https://repositorio.ual.es/handle/10835/14093 (Rodríguez Martínez et al., 2020).

The questionnaire was answered by all the adolescents from five secondary schools present in the classrooms with the prior consent of the School Councils of the schools. In the process, the researchers explained the general guidelines of the questionnaire, assuring the adolescents that their responses would be treated anonymously. To answer the questionnaire, the research team provided the adolescents with a QR code that they used to answer the questionnaire on their cell phones for approximately 30 min. In case they did not have a cell phone, they were provided with the questionnaire on paper.

The questionnaire included sociodemographic questions (sex, age, parents' nationality, family savings level, mother's highest level of education, frequency of attendance at religious services and self-positioning on the political ideological scale) and questions on the adolescent's integration into the school, perception of discrimination and violence, attitudes toward violence and use of violence. All items of the Ozer and Bertelsen scales were included to reproduce the scales (SE and PIARES).

Variables

In a democratic system, having a radical point of view on how society should be organized is legitimate, but accepting violence—illegal acts committed to impose one's point of view—is not. Our concern was not so much with assessing whether the students exhibited a profile of social extremism as it was with their relationship to the justification of violence. For this reason, in the present study, the index of social extremism (SE) was taken as an independent variable, and the index of justification of violence and illegal acts (PIARES) as a dependent variable. Political self-positioning and sex were used as moderating variables.

The variables social extremism (SE) and justification of violence-illegal acts (PIARES) were obtained from the sum of the corresponding items of the SE and PIARES scales of Ozer and Bertelsen (2018). Scores were standardized to improve comparability of results. To facilitate the description of both variables, indicators based on percentiles and total scores were calculated. Four level were established: low, medium, high and very high (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Indicators of the Extremism Scale (SE), justification of violence-illegal acts (PIARES).

The variable political self-positioning refers to the participants' placement on the left-right ideological spectrum (A36). In accordance with the specifications set forth by the Centro de Investigaciones Sociológicas (CIS), an ordinal variable was employed, comprising 10 response options. The scale ranged from 1 (extreme left) to 10 (extreme right).

This variable was recategorized into two nominal variables with five categories (A36_5I and A36_5D; Table 2). This subdivision was undertaken with the objective of utilizing the extreme-left positioning (A36_5I) and extreme-right positioning (A36_5D) as reference categories.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Coding of the political self-positioning variable.

Respondents were asked to what extent they perceive aggressive or violent behavior in the following settings: (a) their family environment, (b) their immediate environment, (c) leisure spaces and (d) their school environment.

Instruments

To assess social extremism (SE) and the justification of violence and illegal acts (PIARES), the scales developed by Ozer and Bertelsen (2018) were utilized. The questionnaire was translated and adapted to align with the characteristics of the sample under study. Subsequently, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted. The principal component method was used to extract the factors. A commonality value of <60% was used as the criterion for excluding variables. Items 1.5 (Extr = 0.429) and 1.6 (Extr = 0.591) in the factor “Justification of violence,” and items 2.1 (Extr = 0.591) and 2.6 (Extr = 0.578) in the factor “Justification of illegal acts,” had extraction values below the established threshold of 60%. In other words, these items did not share sufficient variance with the other items in each factor, which compromised their ability to adequately represent the theoretical dimension being evaluated. Consequently, these four items were removed from the PIARES scale, thereby preserving the test's theoretical meaning and enhancing its statistical fit, with a total explained variance of 65.84%. This improves the instrument's internal consistency and factorial validity (Figure 1). CFA was performed again to verify the good fit of both scales to the sample used (Table 3).

Figure 1
Diagram showing three analyses related to the PIARES Scale. On the left, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) Model 1 with 12 items connects three factors: R1, R2, and R3. In the center, an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) scree plot with an explained variance of 65.839 shows eigenvalues decreasing with component numbers. On the right, CFA Model 2 depicts 8 items, highlighting connections between factors and specific items with varying path coefficients.

Figure 1. Validation process of the justification of violence and illegal acts subscale.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Fit indices confirmatory factorial analysis.

Measurement invariance was tested for both scales: (M1) metric (control of factor loadings), (M2) scalar (control of intercepts), and (M3) strict (control of residuals; Table 4).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Configurational invariance of SE Scale and PIARES with respect to sex variable.

Statistical analysis

[H1] Social extremism (Independent variable) has a direct relationship with the justification of violence and illegal acts (Dependent variable).

The association between the two scales was analyzed (δ Somers and Chi-square), followed by logistic regression analysis. Subsequently, linear regression analysis was carried out taking as dependent variable the typical scores of the PIARES scale (zPIARES) and as independent the typical scores of the Social Extremism scale (zSE).

[H2] To determine the association between the perception of violent or aggressive behavior in different social settings and political self-positioning, a Pearson correlation was calculated.

[H3] The influence of the perception of aggressive or violent behavior in different socialization settings on the relationship between ES and PIARES was determined using simple moderation analysis (PROCESS model 1).

[H4] Political self-positioning (Moderator variable) influences the relationship between social extremism (Independent Variable) and justification of violence-illegal acts (Dependent Variable).

Hierarchical regression analysis was performed using the PROCESS algorithm (Hayes, 2022), taking zPIARES as the dependent variable, zSE as the independent variable, and political self-positioning as a moderator on an ordinal scale with 10 categories, where 1 is extreme left and 10 is extreme right (Figure 2).

[H5] The political self-positioning of the extreme left differs significantly from that of the extreme left in how it influences the relationship between social extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts. Following the proposal of Camino (2022), the political self-positioning variable (10 categories) was recoded into five. In the two models, zPIARES was taken as the dependent variable and zSE as the independent variable. In the first model, the extreme left category was taken as a reference, and in the second, the extreme right category (Figure 3).

[H6] The relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence and illegal acts is moderated by sex.

Figure 2
Diagram illustrating a model with three variables. The independent variable (IV) is Extremism Social (zES), influencing the dependent variable (DV), Justification of violence-illegal acts (zPIARES), denoted by \(b_1\) (IV → DV). Political self-positioning, a moderating variable (W) with 10 categories, influences the DV directly (\(b_2\) (W → DV)) and interacts with the IV for an additional effect (\(b_3\) (X*W → DV)). Arrows indicate the direction of influence between variables.

Figure 2. Political positioning moderation model of the relationship between extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts.

Figure 3
Diagram showing a model with three variables. The independent variable is Social Extremism (zES), the moderating variable is Political self-positioning (five categories), and the dependent variable is Justification of violence and illegal acts (zPIARES). Arrows indicate interactions, with coefficients b1 for the independent variable, b2 to b5 for the moderator, and b6 to b9 for their interaction. Two models are referenced, one with far-left and another with far-right as the reference category for the moderator variable.

Figure 3. Comparison of moderation of extreme left and extreme right positioning on the relationship between extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts.

The initial step in the sequence of analyses was to ascertain whether there were significant differences in the justification of violence and illegal acts between boys and girls. Having verified the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence (H1), we proceeded to investigate whether the sex variable moderated this relationship. Ultimately, given that political self-positioning influenced the relationship between extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts, we deemed it necessary to analyze whether sex moderated the relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence-illegal acts. A comparison of means with an independent samples t-test, a segmented hierarchical regression analysis with the PROCESS algorithm, and an univariate ANOVA were conducted (Figure 4).

Figure 4
Flowchart depicting hypotheses and statistical analyses related to justifying violent illegal acts. H4.1 suggests significant gender differences. If yes, H4.2 examines gender moderation between social extremism and justification. If no, an independent t-test and hierarchical regression are used. For affirmative H4.2, political self-positioning is analyzed in H3. If yes, a generalized linear model considers political self-positioning with gender. H4.3 investigates gender's role in moderating political self-positioning and justification.

Figure 4. Moderation analysis of the sex variable in the relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence-illegal acts.

Programs used

The following software programs were utilized in this study: SPSS v. 28 and AMOS v. 28.

Results

Descriptive

In the case of the violent extremism variable, the mean was 38.82 (σ: 14.60), with a minimum of 14 and a maximum of 98. 20.4% (231) showed low levels, 54.6% (620) medium and 25% (284; high or very high). For the variable justification of violence-illegal acts, the mean was 17.19 (σ: 9.73), with a minimum of 8 and a maximum of 56. 20.8% (236) showed low levels, 48.8% (554) low, 23.9% (271) high, and 6.5% (very high).

As for political self-positioning, 135 (11.9%) did so as far left, 200 (17.6%) left, 525 (46.3%) center, 161 (14.2%) right, and 114 (10%) far right.

Regarding the perception of aggressive or violent behavior in different social environments, the following was found: (a) In the family environment, 71% reported nothing, 22.1% reported a little, 5.1% reported quite a lot and 1.4% reported a lot (mean: 1.36, SD: 0.65); (b) In their immediate environment (e.g., the street, neighborhood or town), 33.6% reported nothing, 43.2% reported a little, 19% reported quite a lot and 4.2% reported a lot (mean: 1.74, SD: 0.78); (c) In leisure spaces (e.g., squares, parks or places of entertainment), 27.7% reported nothing, 40.7% reported a little, 24.5% reported quite a lot and 7.7% reported a lot (mean: 1.93, SD: 0.82) 0.5% reported quite a lot and 3.8% reported very much (mean: 1.93, SD: 0.82); and (d) in their school environment (secondary school), 28.0% reported nothing at all, 41.0% reported a little, 24.2% reported quite a lot, and 6.8% reported a lot (mean: 2.10, SD: 0.88).

[H1] Attitudes about social extremism (independent variable) are directly related to attitudes about the justification of violence and illegal acts (dependent variable).

The relationship between attitudes about social extremism and attitudes about the justification of violence and illegal acts.

A Pearson's Chi-Square value of 216.46 (p-value: < 0.001) was obtained, with δ Somers for justification of violence-illegal acts as the dependent variable of 0.475, and for social extremism as the dependent variable of 0.402. In other words, the two variables are significantly associated in both directions and with a similar magnitude of association (Göktas and Isci, 2011).

Linear regression analysis

A correlation was found between zSE and zPIARES of 0.61 (standardized B), with intercept tending to 0. The R-squared value of 0.37 indicated that social extremism explained 37% of the variability in the justification of violence-illegal acts. A Durbin-Watson statistic value of 1.97 was obtained, so there is no risk of autocorrelation. That is, the regression equation: zY^=zX·0.61. The values of x have to be typed (Equation 1) and the values of zy, transformed into direct scores (σy = 9.73, y¯=17.9) (Equation 2).

zX=x-38.2/14.6    (1)
y^=(zY^·σy)+y¯    (2)

[H2] The perception of aggressive or violent behavior differs significantly in different social settings and correlates with political self-positioning.

Pearson's correlation was not significant in any of the socialization settings considered. In the family environment, r = −0.07, p = 0.20; in close environments, r = −0.026, p = 0.39; in leisure spaces, r = 0.034, p = 0.25; and in school environments, r = 0.036, p = 0.23.

[H3] The perception of aggressive or violent behavior in different social settings significantly moderates the relationship between social extremism (ES) and the justification of illegal acts of violence (PIARES).

In the family environment

For the interaction between the independent variable (ES) and the moderating variable (family environment) to explain the dependent variable (PIARES), a coefficient of 0.03 and a p-value of 0.39 were found. This was not significant.

In the immediate environment

For the interaction between the independent variable (ES) and the moderating variable (close environment) to explain the dependent variable (PIARES), a coefficient of 0.06 and a p-value of 0.03 were found, which was significant. Although very small, this was significant.

Leisure environments

For the interaction between the independent variable (ES) and the moderating variable (leisure environments) to explain the dependent variable (PIARES), a coefficient of 0.03 and a p-value of 0.29 were found. This was not significant.

School environment

For the interaction between the independent variable (ES) and the moderating variable (school environment) to explain the dependent variable (PIARES), a coefficient of −0.01 and a p-value of 0.67 were found. This was also not significant.

[H3] Political self-positioning influences the relationship between attitudes about social extremism and attitudes about justification of violence-illegal acts.

The correlation between the observed value (zY) and the estimated value (zY^) was 0.62, with an R-squared of 0.38, with F(3,1,131)=235.28 (p-value: 0.000). That is, the interaction model was significant, obtaining in the unconditional interaction test an R-squared increase of 0.005, with F(1,1,131) = 8.070 (p-value: 0.0046).

Model coefficients (W, X, XW → Y) were significant, with b2 (W) = 0.022, p-value: 0.042 [CI 95%: 0.001, 0.042]; b1 (X) = 0.46 [CI 95%: 0.347, 0.569], p-value: 0.000, b3 (W*X): 0.027 [CI 95%: 0.008, 0.045], p-value: 0.005, b0: −0.120 [CI 95%: −0.235, −0.004], p-value: 0.04.

For the moderator variable, cut-off points were taken as those corresponding to the 16th (−1σ), 50th (0σ), and 84th (+1σ) percentiles, which corresponded to 3 (extreme left and left), 5 (center), and 7 (right and extreme right). The effect conditioned by political self-positioning on the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence was statistically significant for all categories of the moderator variable (Table 5).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Conditional effects of violent extremism for categories of political self-positioning.

The general regression equation for the proposed interaction model will be (Anguinis, 2003).

zy^=b0+b1·zX+b2·W+b3·zX·W    (3)

Grouping terms, we will have:

zy^=(b1+b3·W)·X+(b0+b2·W)    (4)

Substituting, we will have for W = 3 (extreme left-left):

zy^=[0.458+(0.0266·3)]·zX+[-0.1195+(0.4580·3)]      =0.538 ·zX+1.25

For W = 5 (center), zy^=0.591 zX+2.17and for W = 7 (extreme right-right), zy^=0.644zX+ 3.087.

[H4] Far-left political self-positioning differs significantly from far-left political self-positioning in how it influences the relationship between attitudes about social extremism and attitudes about justification of violence and illegal acts.

Political self-positioning with 5 categories was used as a moderating variable. Darlington and Hayes (2017) posited that it is possible to use a categorical variable with k categories as a predictor in a regression model, representing it with k-1 variables (Table 6).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Coding of the moderator variable political self-positioning, for hierarchical regression analysis (Darlington and Hayes, 2017).

The general regression equation relating social extremism (zX), with justification for violence (zY^), being moderated by political self-positioning (W1 ...W4) would be (Hayes and Montoya, 2017):

zY^=b0+b1·zX+b2·W1+b3·W2+b4·W3+b5·W4       +b6·W1·zX+b7·W2·zX+b8·W3·zX+b9·W4·zX    (5)

Grouping terms (intercept and slope):

zY^=[b0+b2·W1+b3·W2+b4·W3+b5·W4]       +[b1+b6·W1+b7·W2+b8·W3+b9·W4]·zX    (6)

Reference category: far left

The R-squared value was 0.40, F(9,1,125) = 81.56, and p-value: 0.000. That is, the model was significant and improved the explained variance of the previous model.

Taking as reference category (1: W1 = W2 = W3 = W3 = W4 = 0) extreme left, it was observed that the coefficient of justification of violence-illegal acts obtained for W1 (left with respect to extreme left) was −0.392 (CI95%: −0.21; 0.13), not significant, with p-value: 0.656, for W2 (center with respect to extreme left) of −0.096 (CI 95%: −0.244; 0.053), not significant, with p-value: 0.207, W3 (right relative to extreme left) of −0.045 (CI 95%: −0.225; 0.134), not significant, with p-value: 0.623. In the case of W4 (extreme-right relative to extreme-left) of 0.251 (CI 95%: 0.045; 0.457), significant, with p-value: 0.017.

Reference category: far right

Taking right-wing extremism as a reference category, we find that the rest of the categories justify less the use of violence-illegal acts and that this difference is significant (Table 7). As for the interactions, all have a negative sign, that is, for the interactions between the variable extremism and the rest of the categories, the justification of violence is also lower than in the case of the interaction extremism with the extreme-right category. In this case, the difference is only significant with the center category (W2).

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Coefficients in the hierarchical regression model relating social extremism to the justification of violence, moderated by political self-positioning, taking the extreme right as the reference category.

Substituting the values into the general regression (Equation 6), we obtained for the extreme-right category (1) with W1 = W2 = W3 = W4 = 0:

zY^extreme right=[b0+b2·W1+b3·W2+b4·W3+b5·W4]                                    +[b1+b6·W1+b7·W2+b8·W3+b9·W4]                                    ·zX=[-0.270-0.296·(0)-0.347·(0)                                    -0.290·(0)-0.251·(0)]+[0.712-0.018·(0)                                    -0.189·(0)-0.174·(0)-0.098·(0)]                                    =-0.270+0.712·zX

For a right-wing ideological position (2), with W1 = 1, W2 = W3 = W4 = 0:

zY^right=[-0.270-0.296·(1)-0.347·(0)-0.290·(0)                  -0.251·(0)]+[0.712-0.018·(1)-0.189·(0)                  -0.174·(0)-0.098·(0)]= -0.566+0.694·zX

A center position (3), with W2 = 1, W1 = W3 = W4 = 0:

zY^center=[-0.270-0.296·(0)-0.347·(1)-0.290·(0)                    -0.251(0)]+[0.712-0.018·(0)-0.189·(1)-0.174                     ·(0)-0.098·(0)]= -0.617+0.523·zX

A left position (4), with W3 = 1, W1 = W2 = W4 = 0:

zY^left=[-0.270-0.296·(0)-0.347·(0)-0.290·(1)               -0.251(0)]+[0.712-0.018·(0)-0.189·(0)-0.174                ·(1)-0.098·(0)]= -0.560+0.538·zX

A far left position (5), with W4 = 1, W1 = W2 = W3 = 0:

zY^far left=[-0.270-0.296·(0)-0.347·(0)-0.290·(0)                       -0.251(1)]+[0.712-0.018·(0)-0.189·(0)-0.174                        ·(0)-0.098·(1)]= -0.521+0.614·zX

[H5] Sex moderates the relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence and illegal acts.

To the question of whether the differences in the justification of violence-illegal acts are significant as a function of sex, we found a typed mean of 1.74 (SD: 1.08) for boys and −1.65 (SD: 0.88) for girls. It was possible to assume equal variances, with F (32.65, p-value: < 0.001), and mean difference of 0.34 (95%CI: 0.22; 0.45). The sample is similar for boys (554) and girls (581), so the use of the standardized Cohen's d effect size estimator (Equation 7) was appropriate, with a value of 0.99 (Baguley, 2012). That is, the difference was significant and with a large effect size.

Stadardized Cohen d= Mean Boys-Mean GirlsCombined standard deviation    (7)

Sex differences moderated the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence, finding an R-squared value of 0.39, with F(3, 1, 131) = 244.47 (p-value: 0.000), obtaining in the unconditional interaction test an R-squared increase of 0.009, with F(1,1,131) = 16.38 (p-value: 0.0001).

The overall interaction coefficient (ZSE x Sex → ZPIARES) of the variable social extremism and sex as predictors of justification of violence was 0.19 (CI 95%: 0.098; 0.282), with t = 4.05 (p-value: 0.0001). The conditional effects of social extremism, on the moderator in its prediction of justification of violence-illegal acts was 0.49 (CI 95%: 0.42; 0.56), with t = 14.15 (p-value: 0.000) for females, and 0.69 (CI 95%: 0.62; 0.75), with t = 21.90 (p-value: 0.000) for males.

For the analysis of how sex moderates the relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence-illegal acts, we found an R-squared of 0.049, with F (3,1,131)= 19.37 (p-value: 0.000), and an increased R-squared with the interaction of 0.01 (p-value: 0.0004).

The regression coefficients were significant for all components (Table 8).

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Sex moderation regression coefficients of the relationship of political self-positioning with justification of violence-illegal acts.

Discussion

Consequently, attitudes pertaining to social extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts are interrelated. An increase in attitudes about extremism is associated with an increased probability of justifying violence and illegal acts, and vice versa. The regression model, which considers the level of social extremism as the sole predictor, explains 37% of the variability in justifying violence and illegal acts. The ratio of these two phenomena is 0.61, indicating that for each unit of social extremism, there is an increase of 0.61 units in justifying violence and illegal acts.

With regard to socialization environments, our sample found no difference in the perception of aggressive or violent behavior in the family environment, immediate surroundings, leisure spaces or school environment between those who identify more to the right and those who identify more to the left.

The results revealed that, except for slight moderation in the close environment, perception of aggressive or violent behavior is not significantly associated with political self-positioning. Similarly, when analyzing its moderating role in the relationship between ES and PIARES, only interaction in the close environment reached statistical significance, but with such a small coefficient that its practical relevance is questionable. Based on the available data, it has not been possible to conclude that differential perceptions of aggressive or violent behavior significantly influence political self-positioning or the relationship between social extremism and the justification of violent or illegal acts.

With regard to the second hypothesis, the moderated regression model yielded statistically significant results. Attitudes toward social extremism exert a significant influence on the justification of violence and illegal acts (b1: 0.46), while political self-positioning also has a notable impact on this phenomenon (b2: 0.022). Furthermore, the interaction between social extremism and political self-positioning also plays a role in shaping the justification of violence and illegal acts. Notably, the relationship between social extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts varies depending on the political self-positioning of the individual in question. For individuals with similar levels of extremism, the justification of violence-illegal acts is lower for those with a left-extreme left positioning and higher for those with a right-extreme right positioning (Figure 5).

Figure 5
Line graph showing the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence or illegal acts. Three lines represent political self-positioning: far left-left (blue), center (green), and extreme right-right (red). All lines show positive correlation, with the extreme right-right line having the steepest slope. The x-axis represents social extremism, ranging from -1.00 to 1.50, and the y-axis represents justification of violence-illegal acts, ranging from -0.50 to 0.75.

Figure 5. Regression equations for extreme left-left, center, and extreme right-right.

This aspect is of great importance, since the political self-positioning can be gradually radicalized by the incentives that the group itself receives on social networks for expressing its indignation, and above all by the normalization and celebration of violence as a means of conflict resolution (Love and Sharman, 2024; Rodríguez Martínez et al., 2024).

In our third hypothesis, we postulated that far-left political self-positioning differs significantly from far-right self-positioning in its influence on the relationship between social extremism and the justification of violence and illegal acts.

Reference category: far left

In the regression equation that relates extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts, when taking as a reference a self-positioning of the extreme left, a higher level is observed for this category with respect to the left, the center and the right, although this difference is not significant. However, in the case of the extreme right, it is observed that the extreme left presents a lower level of justification of violence-illegal acts, and that this difference is significant (Figure 6).

Figure 6
Line graph showing the estimated marginal means of ZPIARES based on political self-positioning. Values decrease from Far-Left to Left, then increase through Center, Right, reaching the highest point at Far-right.

Figure 6. Justification of violence-illegal acts according to political positioning.

Reference category: far right

When the reference category is an extreme right-wing positioning, the conditional effects of the focal predictor (ES) on the values of the moderator variable (political self-positioning) are significant in all cases for the prediction of the justification of violence-illegal acts (PIARES). The regression equations for each category indicate that the intercept corresponding to the extreme right category (−0.270) is greater than those of the other categories (ranging from −0.521 for the extreme left to −0.617 for the center). This means that the extreme right category will exhibit higher levels of justification for illegal acts of violence than the other categories.

As for the slopes (Table 9), the highest (0.712) is for the far-right category. This means that, for the same level of social extremism, the justification of illegal acts will be higher (Table 9).

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Conditional effects and regression equations.

The symmetry property of statistical interactions in moderation analysis implies that if the effect of social extremism on the justification of violence-illegal acts is significant, the effect of political self-positioning on the justification of violence-illegal acts, moderated by social extremism, will also be significant (Montoya, 2016). With regard to justification of violence, the extreme left political positioning is not equivalent to the extreme right. The highest levels of social extremism justifying violence-illegal acts are found among those who self-position themselves on the extreme right (Figure 7).

Figure 7
Line graph showing the relationship between social extremism (zES) on the x-axis and justification of violence or illegal acts (zPIARES) on the y-axis, with data points and trend lines for different political positions: Far right, Right, Center, Left, and Far Left. The trend lines indicate varying degrees of correlation between these variables for each political position.

Figure 7. Regression equations of the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence, moderated by political self-positioning.

For the fourth hypothesis, it was confirmed that for similar positions of social extremism, girls justified less violence-illegal acts than boys (Figure 8).

Figure 8
Scatter plot showing the relationship between social extremism (zES) and justification of violence-illegal acts (zPIARES) for men and women. Two trend lines represent men with a positive slope of 0.69 and an intercept of 0.09, and women with a slope of 0.5 and an intercept of -0.11. The solid line is for men, and the dashed line is for women.

Figure 8. Gender moderation in the relationship between social extremism and justification of violence-illegal acts.

Moreover, that for similar values of political self-positioning, girls justified significantly less violence than boys (Figure 9).

Figure 9
Line graph depicting estimated averages of justifying violence and illegal acts (zPIARES) by political self-positioning, from far-right to far-left, for men and women. Men are represented by a dotted line and women by a solid line. Men show higher justification on both political extremes, while women show less variation across the spectrum.

Figure 9. Gender moderation in the relationship between political self-positioning and justification of violence-illegal acts.

Consequently, the political polarization of adolescents does not manifest in the same manner for those who align themselves with the most radical ideologies on the political spectrum, whether to the left or to the right. The endorsement of pro-violence attitudes is a notable phenomenon, particularly among adolescents who align with the political extreme right. This study corroborates the findings of those who have proposed an alternative approach to the study of political polarization (Twenge, 2023), namely that there is an asymmetric polarization (Hacker and Pierson, 2017), in which a sexist component can be seen, with relevant differences in the legitimization of violence as a means of political action (Frazer and Hutchings, 2014).

Furthermore, gendered socialization persists, with boys exhibiting a significantly higher proclivity for advocating violent attitudes within the context of toxic masculinity (Liu, 2016).

Limitations

The age of the participants, between 12 and 18 years old, without a reference of what happens in the next age bracket, 19–24, generates doubts about the changes in the relationship between the degree of social extremism and the justification of violence-illegal acts to achieve their objectives, as well as the influence of political self-positioning in this relationship.

Future lines of research

It would be beneficial to replicate the procedure longitudinally with the same group of students and to increase the number of participants in order to conduct a more comprehensive study.

This research suggests that youth organizations that self-identify as right-wing extremist may serve as a conduit for the propagation of hate speech. Verification of this hypothesis is necessary, as is characterization of the target groups, as well as the instruments and procedures of generation and dissemination.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary material.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comite de Bioetica de la Universidad de Almeria. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

FV: Funding acquisition, Resources, Visualization, Formal analysis, Validation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Data curation, Investigation, Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Software. PR: Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis, Resources, Validation, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Visualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Data curation, Supervision, Investigation, Software, Conceptualization. AS: Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Resources, Writing – original draft. LM: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Investigation.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by [Programa Estatal para Impulsar la Investigación Científico -Técnica y su Transferencia, del Plan Estatal de Investigación Científica, Técnica y de Innovación 2021–2023, Spain] grant number [PID2021-127113OB-I00].

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpos.2025.1616180/full#supplementary-material

References

Andrade, B., Guadix, I., and Rial, A. Y Suárez, F. (2021). Impacto de la tecnología en la adolescencia. Relaciones, riesgos y oportunidades. Madrid: UNICEF España.

Google Scholar

Anguinis, H. (2003). Regression Analysis for Categorial Moderators. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Azqueta, A., and Merino-Arribas, A. (2022). Education in the prevention of radicalism: a review for Europe. Rev. Educ. 397, 209–238. Available online at: https://recyt.fecyt.es/index.php/Redu/article/view/94824/69072

Google Scholar

Baguley, T. (2012). Standardized or simple effect size: what should be reported? Br. J. Psychol. 100, 603–617. doi: 10.1348/000712608X377117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baier, D., Manzoni, P., and Bergmann, M. C. (2016). Einflussfaktoren des politischen extremismus im jugendalter—rechtsextremismus, linksextremismus und islamischer extremismus im vergleich [influencing factors of political extremism in adolescence—right-wing extremism, left-wing extremism and islamic extremism compared]. Monatsschr. Kriminol. Strafrechtsreform 3, 171–198. doi: 10.1515/mkr-2016-0302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, P. C., Barberá, P., Ackermann, K., and Venetz, A. (2017). Is the left-right scale a valid measure of ideology: individual-level variation in associations with 'left' and 'right' and left-right self-placement. Polit. Behav. 39, 553–583. doi: 10.1007/s11109-016-9368-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bazaga-Fernández, I., and Tamayo-Sáez, M. (2021). Violent radicalization. Eunomia. Revista en Cultura de la Legalidad 20, 322–333. doi: 10.20318/eunomia.2021.6079

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camino, E. (2022). Nonresponse to ideological self-location: a social distribution of silence. J. Polit. Sociol. Res. 21, 1–18. doi: 10.15304/rips.21.1.7695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

CIMA (2023). The audiovisual diet offered in children's television programming. Available online at: https://cimamujerescineastas.es/wp-content/uploads/2023/02/2022_Informe-contenido-series-infantiles_completo.pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

CIS (2023a). Study no. 11003. Living conditions of young people: Training, employment, emancipation and participation. Qualitative sociological study. Transcripts. Focus group 11. Available online at: https://www.cis.es/descarga-fichero-datos?anio=2022andcodEstudio=11003andfichero=CL11003.zip (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

CIS (2023b). Study 1277. March 1981 barometer. Available online at: https://www.cis.es/documents/d/cis/cues1277spdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

CIS (2023c). Study 1350. May 1983 barometer. Available online at: https://www.cis.es/documents/d/cis/cues1350pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

CIS (2023d). Study 1360. Barómetro de junio 1983. Available online at: https://www.cis.es/documents/d/cis/cues1360spdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Darlington, R. B., and Hayes, A. F. (2017). Regression Analysis and Linear Models: Concepts, Applications, and Implementation. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

De-Visé, D. (2023). High School Boys are Trending Conservative. The Hill. Available online at: https://thehill.com/blogs/blog-briefing-room/4125661-high-school-boys-are-trending-conservative/ (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Eldor, D. S., Lindholm, K., Chavez, M. H., Vassanyi, S., Badiane, M. O. I., Yaldizli, K., et al. (2022). Resilience against radicalization and extremism in schools: development of a psychometric scale. Front. Psychol. 13:980180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.980180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Engesser, S., Ernst, N., Esser, F., and Büchel, F. (2017). Populism and social media: how politicians spread a fragmented ideology. Inf. Commun. Soc. 20, 1109–1126. doi: 10.1080/1369118X.2016.1207697

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrington, D. P., Coid, J. W., and Murray, J. (2009). Family factors in the intergenerational transmission of offending. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health 19, 109–124. doi: 10.1002/cbm.717

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazer, E., and Hutchings, K. (2014). Feminism and the critique of violence: negotiating feminist political agency. J. Polit. Ideol. 19, 143–163. doi: 10.1080/13569317.2014.909263

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Juanatey, A., Miranda-Leibe, L., Steible, B., Díaz-Pagés, A. L., and Sueiro-Monje, N. (2020). Right-Wing Extremism Among Spanish Youth: Current Situation and Perspectives. INJUVE. Government of Spain. Available online at: https://www.injuve.es/sites/default/files/adjuntos/2021/05/estudio_injuve_el_extremismo_de_derecha.pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

García-Sánchez, E., García-Castro, J. D., Willis, G. B., and Rodríguez-Bailón, R. (2022). Perceptions of economic inequality in everyday life and political ideology: a study with young people in Spain. Rev. Estud. Soc. 79, 2–21. doi: 10.7440/res79.2022.01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Göktas, A., and Isci, O. (2011). A comparison of the most commonly used measures of association for doubly ordered square contingency tables via simulation. Adv. Methodol. Stat. 8, 17–37. doi: 10.51936/milh5641

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greve, W. (2007). “Selbst und Identität im Lebenslauf [El yo y la identidad en el curso de la vida],” in Entwicklungspsychologie der Lebensspanne, eds. En J. Brandstädter y U. Lindenberger (Kohlhammer), 305–336.

Google Scholar

Hacker, J. S., and Pierson, P. (2017). “Confronting asymmetric polarization,” in Solutions to Political Polarization in America, ed. N. Persily (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 59–70. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781316091906.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process (3rd Ed). Available online at: http://www.guilford.com/p/hayes3 (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., and Montoya, A. K. (2017). A tutorial on testing, visualizing, and testing an interaction involving a multicategorical variable in linear regression analysis. Commun. Methods Meas. 11, 1–30. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2016.1271116

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L.-T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iberdrola (2023). Alfa Generation. Available online at: https://www.iberdrola.com/talento/generacion-alfa (Accessed June 12, 2023).

Google Scholar

INJUVE (2017). Informe de resultados sondeo de opinión. Available online at: https://www.injuve.es/sites/default/files/2018/27/publicaciones/sondeo_2017-1_informe.pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

INJUVE (2021). Informe Juventud en España 2020. Madrid: Injuve. Available online at: https://www.injuve.es/sites/default/files/adjuntos/2021/11/informe_juventud_espana_2020_0_1_wdb_v1.pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Jaiswal, R. (2023). HR strategies for generation ALPHA: what would it look like? Int. J. Creat. Res. Thought. 11, c592–c598. Available online at: https://ijcrt.org/viewfull.php?&p_id=IJCRT2307301

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jasko, K., LaFree, G., Piazza, J., and Becker, M. H. (2022). A comparison of political violence by left-wing, right-wing, and Islamist extremists in the United States and the world. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 119, 1–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2122593119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jha, A. K. (2020). Understanding Generation Alpha. Available online at: https://osf.io/d2e8g/ (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., Ledgerwood, A., and Hardin, C. D. (2008). Shared reality, system justification, and the relational basis of ideological beliefs. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 171–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00056.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koirikivi, P., Benjamin, S., Hietajärvi, L., Kuusisto, A., and Gearon, L. (2021). Resourcing resilience: educational considerations for supporting well-being and preventing violent extremism amongst Finnish youth. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 26, 553–569. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2021.2010578

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W., Bélanger, J. J., Gelfand, M., Gunaratna, R., Hettiarachchi, M., Reinares, F., et al. (2013). Terrorism-a (self) love story: redirecting the significance quest can end violence. Am. Psychol. 68, 559–575. doi: 10.1037/a0032615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W., Bélanger, J. J., and Gunaratna, R. (2019). The Three Pillars of Radicalization: Needs, Narratives, and Networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Liu, W. M. (2016). How Trump's ‘Toxic Masculinity' Is Bad for Other Men. Motto (Time). New York.

Google Scholar

Lobato, R. M., Moyano, M., Bélanger, J. J., and Trujillo, H. M. (2020). The role of vulnerable in support for homegown terrorism: fieldwork using the 3N model. Aggress. Behav. 47, 50–57. doi: 10.1002/ab.21933

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Love, S., and Sharman, R. (2024). Perceived grievance and individualising moralities: exploring the psychological structure of left-wing authoritarianism. J. Polit. Ideol. 1–26. doi: 10.1080/13569317.2024.2344755

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrindle, M., and Wolfinger, E. (2009). The ABCs of XYZ: Understanding the Global Generations. New South Wales: McCrindle Publication.

Google Scholar

Montoya, A. K. (2016). Extending the Jhonson-Neyman Procedure to Categorical Independent Variable: Mathematical Derivations and Computational ToolsK [doctoral thesis]. The Oiao State University. Available online at: https://www.akmontoya.com/_files/ugd/0a182f_6f44045fe73a457da04a6b018688dd6b.pdf (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Nagy, A., and Köllcsey, A. (2017). Generation alpha: marketing or science? Acta Technologica Dubnicae 7, 107–115. doi: 10.1515/atd-2017-0007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nivette, A., Echelmeyer, L., Weerman, F., Eisner, M., and Ribeaud, D. (2022). Understanding changes in violent extremist attitudes during the transition to early adulthood. J. Quant. Criminol. 38, 949–978. doi: 10.1007/s10940-021-09522-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozer, S., and Bertelsen, P. (2018). Capturing violent radicalization: developing and validating scales measuring central aspects of radicalization. Scand. J. Psychol. 59, 653–660. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12484

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peral, E. B., and Calvo, R. L. (2023). The hegemony of political parties. Partisanship as a key factor explaining self-placement on the left-right scale: evidence from Spain. Span. J. Soc. Res. 179, 21–38. doi: 10.5477/cis/reis.179.21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez Martínez, P., Roith, C., Segura-Sánchez, A., Lozano-Diaz, A., López-Berlanga, M. C., Sáez-Castro, L. M., et al. (2020). Database on radicalization and violent extremism in secondary schools, Spain (Almeria and Madrid). Available online at: https://repositorio.ual.es/handle/10835/14093 (Accessed February 10, 2024).

Google Scholar

Rodriguez Martinez, P., Roith, C., and Segura-Sanchez, Lopez-Narbona, A. (2023). Extremist and pro-violence attitudes of Spanish adolescents in secondary schools. Cogent Soc. Sci. 9. doi: 10.1080/23311886.2023.2239542

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez Martínez, P., Villegas Lirola, F., and Sousa, J. (2024). Gender effects of social network use among secondary school adolescents in spain: extremist and pro-violence attitudes. Fem. Criminol. 19, 329–354. doi: 10.1177/15570851241240825

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schils, N., and Verhage, A. (2017). Understanding how and why young people enter radical or violent extremist groups. Int. J. Confl. Violence 11:a473. doi: 10.4119/ijcv-3084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sousa, J., Ávila, N. R., and Rodríguez Martínez, P. (2024).Cyberviolence in Spain: types, victims and aggressors [Ciberviolencia en España: tipos, víctimas y agresores]. Revista de Comunicación de la SEECI. 57, 1–22. doi: 10.15198/seeci.2024.57.e877

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spalek, B. (2016). Radicalisation, de-radicalisation and counter-radicalisation in relation to families: key challenges for research, policy, and practice. Secur. J. 29, 39–52. doi: 10.1057/sj.2015.43

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swann, W. B., Jetten, J., Gomez, A., Whitehouse, A., and Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal: a theory of identity fusion. Psychol. Rev. 119, 441–456. doi: 10.1037/a0028589

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Talshir, G. (2005). Knowing right from left: the politics of identity between the radical left and far right. J. Polit. Ideol. 10, 311–335. doi: 10.1080/13569310500244339

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamayo Sáez, M., Bazaga Fernández, I., and Bermejo Casado, R. (2021). Violent radicalization of young people a challenge in the construction of safe societies: a proposal for a typology and scale of young people vulnerable to violent radicalization processes. Cuad. Gob. Adm. Pública 8, 119–130. doi: 10.5209/cgap.78366

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M. (2023). Generations: The Real Differences Between Gen Z, Millennials, Gen X, Boomers, and Silents-and What They Mean for America's Future. NewYork: Atria Books. doi: 10.56315/PSCF12-23Twenge

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wachs, S., WeJEtstein, A., Bilz, L., and Gámez-Guadix, M. (2022). Adolescents' motivations to perpetrate hate speech and links with social norms [Adolescents' motivations to perpetrate hate speech and links with social norms]. Comunicar 71, 9–20. doi: 10.3916/C71-2022-01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webber, D., and Kruglanski, A. W. (2017). “Psychological factors in radicalization: A “3N” approach,” in The Handbook of the Criminology of Terrorism, eds. G. LaFree and J. Freilich (West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell), 33–46. doi: 10.1002/9781118923986.ch2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfowicz, M., Litmanovitz, Y., Weisburd, D., and Hasisi, B. (2021). Cognitive and behavioral radicalisation: a systematic review of the putative risk and protective factors. Campbell Syst. Rev. 17, 1–90. doi: 10.1002/cl2.1174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: asymmetric and sexist polarization, social extremism, pro-violence attitudes, adolescents, political self-positioning, analysis of moderation, Spain

Citation: Villegas Lirola F, Rodriguez Martinez P, Segura Sánchez AJ and Martinez Joya L (2025) Asymmetric and sexist polarization: political self-positioning as a moderator of the relationship between extremism and the justification of violence among adolescents. Front. Polit. Sci. 7:1616180. doi: 10.3389/fpos.2025.1616180

Received: 22 April 2025; Accepted: 25 June 2025;
Published: 14 July 2025.

Edited by:

Patricia Sánchez-Holgado, University of Salamanca, Spain

Reviewed by:

Rashmi Gopi, University of Delhi, India
Michael Becker, University of Nebraska Omaha, United States

Copyright © 2025 Villegas Lirola, Rodriguez Martinez, Segura Sánchez and Martinez Joya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pilar Rodriguez Martinez, cGlsYXJyQHVhbC5lcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.