Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Pharmacol., 03 May 2019
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Antipsychotics of a New Generation: Where Are We Now? View all 6 articles

Trace Amine-Associated Receptor 1 Agonist Modulates Mismatch Negativity-Like Responses in Mice

  • 1Department of Higher Nervous Activity and Psychophysiology, Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia
  • 2Department of General Physiology, Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia
  • 3Institute of Translational Biomedicine and Saint Petersburg University Hospital, Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia

The trace amine-associated receptor 1 (TAAR1) is a G protein-coupled receptor widely expressed in the mammalian brain, particularly in limbic system and monoaminergic areas. It has proven to be an important modulator of dopaminergic, serotoninergic, and glutamatergic neurotransmission and is considered to be a potential useful target for the pharmacotherapy of neuropsychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia. One of the promising schizophrenia endophenotypes is a deficit in neurocognitive abilities manifested as mismatch negativity (MMN) deficit. This study examines the effect of TAAR1 partial agonist RO5263397 on the MMN-like response in freely moving C57BL/6 mice. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from awake mice in the oddball paradigm before and after RO5263397 administration. The RO5263397 (but not saline) administration increased the N40 amplitude in response to deviant stimuli. That provided the MMN-like difference at the 36–44 ms interval after the injection. The pitch deviance-elicited changes before the injection and in the control paradigm were established for the P68 component. After TAAR1 agonist administration the P68 amplitude in response both to standard and deviant stimuli was increased. These results suggest that the MMN-like response in mice may be modulated through TAAR1-dependent processes (possibly acting through the direct or indirect glutamate NMDA receptor modulation), indicating the TAAR1 agonists potential antipsychotic and pro-cognitive activity.

Introduction

Trace amines (tyramine, tryptamine, phenylethylamine, and octopamine) are related by structure to classical biogenic amines and have long been known to be present in low concentrations (0.1–10 nM) in the mammalian nervous system. Trace amines (TAs) dysregulation is associated with psychiatric impairments, including the affective behavior, schizophrenia, depression, Parkinson’s disease, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Branchek and Blackburn, 2003; Burchett and Hicks, 2006; Berry et al., 2017; Gainetdinov et al., 2018). For example, the increased levels of phenylethylamine, tyramine, and tryptamine levels in urinary and plasma of schizophrenic patients suggest the TA role in the pathophysiology of the disease (Davis, 1989; Berry, 2007).

Although TAs have been investigated for a long time (Boulton, 1974), their receptors in vertebrates are relatively recently discovered (Borowsky et al., 2001). The so-called trace amine associated receptors (TAARs) belong to the family of G protein-coupled receptors and are divided into three subgroups: TAAR 1–4, TAAR 5 and TAAR 6–9 (Lindemann et al., 2005).

Trace amine associated receptor1 is the best studied member of the TA receptor family (Lindemann et al., 2008; Bradaia et al., 2009; Revel et al., 2011; Leo et al., 2014). It is found in the central nervous system of rodents, monkeys and humans and is activated by trace amines, such as phenylethylamine and tyramine (Miller, 2011). TAAR1 is mainly expressed in the limbic system and monoaminergic systems regions, including the hypothalamus, substantia nigra, dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), and ventral tegmental area (VTA), amygdala, nucleus of the solitary tract, rhinal cortices and subiculum (Lindemann et al., 2008; Panas et al., 2010).

Several studies have established that TAAR1 is localized in dopaminergic and serotonergic regions of the brain and acts as a neuromodulator for dopaminergic, serotonergic and glutamatergic systems. Experiments in vitro with TAAR1 agonists and antagonists have revealed that TAAR1 reduces the dopamine (DA) and serotonin [5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT)] neurons firing frequency in limbic and monoaminergic systems, such as VTA and DRN (Bradaia et al., 2009; Revel et al., 2011; Espinoza et al., 2011). TAAR1 agonists administration in vivo have been also known to reduce the firing rate of DA neurons and inhibit the behavioral and electrophysiological effects of psychostimulants (Revel et al., 2011; Revel et al., 2013). Indeed, Taar1 knockout mice have no overt phenotype, however, exhibiting schizophrenia-like properties. This is manifested in an elevated amphetamine-induced hyperactivity and an increased striatal release of DA, norepinephrine and 5-HT (Wolinsky et al., 2007; Lindemann et al., 2008); a D2 DA receptor supersensitivity in the striatum (Hirvonen et al., 2005; Corripio et al., 2006, 2011); a decreased function of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors in the prefrontal cortex (Espinoza et al., 2015) and a deficit in the prepulse inhibition test (Wolinsky et al., 2007; Polyakova et al., 2018). Taken together, these data suggest that TAAR1 agonist is an important negative monoaminergic and positive glutamatergic neurotransmission modulator, which makes it a potential useful target for the pharmacotherapy of neuropsychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia.

Common schizophrenia symptoms include a deficit in several neurocognitive abilities, which is manifested in an impaired prepulse inhibition, a P50 event-related potential (ERP) suppression, a deficit in mismatch negativity (MMN) and P300. Disturbances in these processes reflect the deficient ability to regulate the environmental information inflow and are highly replicated schizophrenia biomarkers (Turetsky et al., 2007; Light and Swerdlow, 2015).

Mismatch negativity is a differential auditory ERP response to stimuli that deviate from repeatedly presented standard stimuli and reflect automatic and pre-attentive auditory stimulus-discrimination processes (Näätänen et al., 1978, 2011). A gradual decrease in MMN amplitude reflects cognitive and functional impairments that depend on the schizophrenia disease endurance (Javitt et al., 1993; Michie, 2001; Light and Braff, 2005; Näätänen and Kähkönen, 2009). Neuropharmacological basis of these violations is not fully understood. Among the major neurotransmitter systems glutamate-, 5-HT- and dopaminergic systems are known to be involved in the MMN generation. Therefore, the study of TAAR1 contribution to the pharmacological regulation of these processes may help to understand the neurophysiological basis of schizophrenia and develop new treatments to this debilitating disorder.

Here, we investigated the selective TAAR1 partial agonist RO5263397 effects on the MMN-like response in the oddball paradigm in freely moving C57BL/6 mice.

Materials and Methods

Animal Preparation

Twenty-two male C57BL/6 mice (weight range 25–40 g) were obtained from the vivarium of the Institute of Translational Biomedicine (Saint Petersburg State University, Saint Petersburg, Russia).

All animal studies were carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Ministry of Health of Russian Federation and the principles of the Basel Declaration. All experiments were approved by the Saint Petersburg State University Ethical Committee for Animal Research (approval number 131-03-4 from March 14, 2016). All surgical procedures were performed under anesthesia, and all efforts were made to minimize suffering.

Mice were anesthetized with Zoletil (150 mg/kg i.p.) and xylazine (0.2 mg/kg i.m.). Epidural active electrodes were implanted above the two auditory cortices (bregma AP = -3.5; L = ± 3.5). Two reference electrodes were located at AP = + 2.0, L = ± 0.5; the ground electrode was located at AP = -1.0, L = -0.5. The leads were fixed with dental cold-cured plastic Acrodent (JSC Ctoma, Ukraine). The operation field was treated with Baneocin. Baytril® (enrofloxacin, 2.5 mg/kg i.m.) that was administered preoperatively as an antibiotic. The animals were allowed to recover for at least 4 days after surgery. The electrode placement was confirmed after the experiment.

Experimental Design

Electrocorticogram (ECoG) was recorded continuously during the experiment by a Mitsar-EEG-05/70-201 amplifier with WinEEG software (v. 2.4, Mitsar Co., Ltd., Russia) within the experimental chamber located between the speakers. Each animal experiment was conducted with a 1-day break. The awake mice were placed in the experimental chamber for 15 min before the ERPs session and were free to explore during recordings. After that, the first ERP recording in the oddball paradigm was performed; RO5263397 (1 mg/kg i.p.) or 0.9% NaCl were administered 15 min prior to the start of the second ERP recording session. RO5263397 was prepared in 0.9% saline solution before the injection. The drug dose was selected based on the previous studies of the RO5263397 dose-dependent effect (Revel et al., 2013). The order of conditions was counter balanced across the animals.

Data Analysis

Electrocorticogram was filtered (bandpass 0.32–70 Hz), and artifacts were removed by visual inspection during offline analysis. High frequency (100–200 Hz) and high amplitude waves (amplitude exceeds ± 500 μV on any of the channels) occurred primarily during the grooming (Roger et al., 2009) and were rejected.

Stimuli were presented in oddball and reverse oddball paradigms via speakers by the Psytask software (v. 2.4, Mitsar Co., Ltd., Russia). Standard and deviant sinusoidal tones were 100 ms in length (including 5 ms of rise/fall time), and the intensity was 86 dB SPL averaged across the experimental chamber. Standards were 6 or 8 kHz frequency tones (probability 85%), and deviants were 8 or 6 kHz frequency tones (probability 15%) presented in a pseudo-random order with 500 ISI such that before each deviant stimulus, at least one standard stimulus occurred. The order of paradigms was counter balanced across animals. Data analysis was performed according to Featherstone et al. (2015). ERP to standard and deviant tones were quantified. Baseline data were collected from – 100 to 0 ms before stimulus onset. To receive resulting ERPs from the equal number of standard and deviant tones, only the last standard tone prior to the deviant was averaged. Finally, a difference wave was calculated as the deviant minus standard ERP. Mean latency and amplitude measures (baseline to peak) were extracted over 36–44 ms and 68–100 ms latency windows corresponding to the N40 and P68 ERP peaks. Mean latencies and amplitudes were obtained for standard, deviant, and difference responses.

All data were normally distributed according to the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Mean latencies and amplitudes of the ERP components were analyzed using three-way repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA) with Fisher LSD post hoc test (IBM SPSS Statistic v. 21, IBM Corporation, New York, United States) with the factors Injection (before and after), Stimulus type (deviant and standard), and Electrode location (left and right). The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used where sphericity assumptions were violated. In addition, paired sample t-test was applied. Difference MMN-like responses were analyzed using two-way rmANOVA with the factors Injection (before and after) and Electrode location (left and right).

Results

Figure 1 illustrates the averaged difference responses obtained during the experiment. For the N40 component a significant Stimulus type main effect [F(1,21) = 7.059; p = 0.015] and Injection ×Stimulus type interaction [F(1,21) = 9.087; p = 0.007] in the RO5263397 administration condition was found. Fisher LSD post hoc test indicated significantly more negative response to the deviant stimuli after the TAAR1 agonist injection (p = 0.029) The difference between deviant and standard stimuli responses also became significant after the RO5263397 administration (p < 0.001) (Figure 2). MMN-like responses analysis revealed the significant Injection main effect [F(1,21) = 9.087; p = 0.007] indicating an increased difference between deviant and standard responses after the RO5263397 injection (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. MMN-like response (differential component) above the left and right auditory cortex before (gray line) and after (black line) 0.9% NaCl or 1 mg/kg RO 5263397. The black line on the x-axis shows a stimulus duration. The dashed lines are the intervals taken for the amplitudes and latencies analysis. Stars denote statistical significance (p ≤ 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. N40 and P68 amplitude in response to standard (gray columns) and deviant (black columns) stimuli before and after the TAAR1 agonist (RO 5263397) injection in left (LH AC) and right-hemisphere auditory cortex (RH AC). Error bars are standard error of the mean. Stars denote statistical significance (p ≤ 0.05).

In saline condition a significant Stimulus type main effect [F(1,21) = 6.310; p = 0.020] was found. The paired sample t-test showed significantly larger mean amplitude to the deviant than to the standard stimulus after [left hemisphere: t(21) = -2.357, p = 0.028; right hemisphere: t(21) = -2.703, p = 0.013], but not before saline injection (Figure 2). It should be noted that there is a slight but not significant decrease [F(1,21) = 2.168; n.s.] in the response amplitude to the standard stimulus, which may reflect the stress effect caused by the fact of injection (Karl et al., 2006).

For the P68 component the significant main effects for Stimulus type [F(1,21) = 11.588; p = 0.003] and Injection [F(1,21) = 11.422; p = 0.003] were found. Paired t-tests confirmed larger amplitude in response to the deviant stimuli before [left hemisphere: t(21) = 2.103, p = 0.048] and after [left hemisphere: t(21) = 2.564, p = 0.018; right hemisphere: t(21) = 3.571, p = 0.002] the injection and an increased positivity in response to the standard [left hemisphere: t(21) = 2.376, p = 0.027; right hemisphere: t(21) = 3.772, p = 0.001], and deviant stimuli [left hemisphere: t(21) = 2.200, p = 0.039; right hemisphere: t(21) = 2.590, p = 0.017] after the RO5263397 administration (Figure 2). No difference in MMN-like responses before and after injection were found [F(1,21) < 1; n.s.]. No significant Injection or Stimulus type effects were revealed in saline condition.

No effects on the latency of any described components after injections after saline or RO5263397 were found.

Discussion

The current study was aimed to evaluate the selective TAAR1 partial agonist RO5263397 effect on MMN-like responses in freely moving mice. Recorded ERPs contained two main components: negative – with 40 ms latency (N40) and positive – with 68 ms latency (P68). The pitch deviance-elicited changes in the control paradigm and before the injection were established for the P68 component. The MMN-like difference for the N40 component was also found after the TAAR1 agonist injection.

Currently, there is no consensus about the MMN-like response component in rodents that is considered to be analogous to human MMN. Auditory ERPs in humans consist of three main components that are defined by peak latencies and polarity: P50, N100, and P200. MMN arises in approximately 150–200 ms after the deviant stimulus in oddball paradigm and partly overlapped with the N100 component (Näätänen et al., 2011). The latency and polarity of rodent auditory ERPs are quite variable and strongly depend on the reference electrode position and anesthetic agents (Harms et al., 2016). Some authors consider that mouse auditory ERP latencies are approximately 40% of latencies seen in humans (Siegel et al., 2003) and that P1, N1, and P2 components in mice are analogous to the human P50, N100, and P200, according to the ISI response relationship (Umbricht et al., 2002, 2004; Maxwell et al., 2004). Many researchers report an MMN-like response in rodents (positive or negative), reaching a maximum between 50 and 100 ms (Ahmed et al., 2011; Astikainen et al., 2011; Nakamura et al., 2011; Jung et al., 2013; Shiramatsu et al., 2013; Harms et al., 2014; Sivarao et al., 2014; Ruusuvirta et al., 2015).

Event-related potentials registered in our study were highly similar in polarity and peak latencies to components obtained in rats (Jung et al., 2013; Aleksandrov et al., 2018) and mice (Umbricht et al., 2004; Ehrlichman et al., 2008). After the RO5263397 administration the N40 amplitude increased in response to deviant stimuli, which led to the MMN-like difference in the 36–44 ms time interval. Besides the P68 amplitude was increased in response both to standard and deviant stimuli and no difference for the MMN-like response was found.

Mismatch negativity is considered a correlate of pre-attentive processes and associated with a change detection due to a mismatch in echoic memory traces (Näätänen et al., 2011). The human MMN is a well-known schizophrenia biomarker (Turetsky et al., 2007; Light and Swerdlow, 2015). In patients with schizophrenia, the MMN reduced amplitude is a stable indicator of anomaly in hearing and attention (Javitt et al., 1993; Michie, 2001; Light and Braff, 2005). A gradual decrease in the MMN amplitude reflects cognitive and functional dysfunctions, depending on the disease duration (Näätänen and Kähkönen, 2009).

Impaired MMN generation is often associated with glutamatergic NMDA receptor system deficiency. Some research demonstrate that the competitive and non-competitive NMDA antagonists reduce MMN amplitude in humans (Javitt et al., 1996; Umbricht et al., 2000; Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 2001) and rats (Tikhonravov et al., 2008; Harms et al., 2018). Deviance-elicited change in N40 amplitude in mice is also decreased under the NMDA antagonist ketamine action that makes it similar to MMN in humans (Ehrlichman et al., 2008). Furthermore, other compounds with NMDA antagonist properties (alcohol and nitric oxide) also reduced MMN (Jääskeläinen et al., 1995; Pang and Fowler, 1999).

Recent studies provide evidence that the NMDA receptors composition and functions may depend on TAAR1. Taar1 knockout mice demonstrate the reduced NMDA receptor subunits expression and phosphorylation in prefrontal cortex and striatum (Espinoza et al., 2015; Sukhanov et al., 2016). In addition, TAAR1 agonists are able to suppress the hyperlocomotion induced by the NMDA antagonist (Revel et al., 2011, 2013), decrease impulsive behavior in mice (Espinoza et al., 2015) and reverse the cognitive impairments in rats treated with the glutamate NMDA receptor antagonist (Revel et al., 2013). Thus, the TAAR1 activation is able to modulate directly or indirectly the NMDA-mediated glutamatergic activity, likely ameliorating the hypoglutamatergic state. That may be the possible explanation of increased deviance-elicited change in N40 amplitude after the TAAR1 agonist administration.

The monoaminergic systems, being the negative TAAR1 neuromodulation targets, may also modulate the NMDA activity, thus playing a potential role in MMN generation (Espinoza et al., 2011; Revel et al., 2011). Otherwise, numerous studies have shown the multidirectional effect of dophaminergic and serotonergic systems on MMN. As reported by Kähkönen et al. (2001) a decrease in the dopaminergic activity after the D2 receptor antagonist (haloperidol) administration in humans leads to an increase in the MMN amplitude. However, some other studies have found no effects of haloperidol (Kähkönen et al., 2002; Pekkonen et al., 2002) or selective D2 agonist (bromocriptine) and D1/D2 agonist (pergolide) (Leung et al., 2007) on MMN. In a number of studies, a 5-HT synthesis decrease by the acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) has been found to impair the MMN amplitude (Ahveninen et al., 2002). A negative component decrease in the oddball paradigm has also been found in the 50% serotonin depletion condition after the para-chlorophenylalanine administration in rats (Ehlers et al., 1991). On the contrary, some studies in humans indicate an improved attention and executive functions (Coull et al., 1995; Schmitt et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2002), an increased MMN amplitude and a decreased MMNm latency (Kähkönen et al., 2005) after the ATD. Although some studies show increased MMN amplitude with monoaminergic systems activity decrease, but the role of this system in the MMN generation remains unclear. Therefore, it is difficult to conclude that an increase in the MMN-like response after the TAAR1 agonist administration may be associated with dopaminergic or serotonergic activity neuromodulation.

The ERPs modulation can also be associated with the increased intensity dependence due to the reduced serotonergic neurotransmission (Hegerl and Juckel, 1993; Hegerl et al., 2001). Thus, an increase in the P68 amplitude in response to standard and deviant stimuli after the TAAR1 agonist administration may be the result of new afferents activation associated with the modulation of monoaminergic system.

Therefore, the TAAR1 activation, most likely acting thorough the NMDA receptor modulation, plays the critical role in the N40 MMN-like response increase. Taken together these data indicate that TAAR1 activation can significantly increase the MMN response suggesting the potential antipsychotic and pro-cognitive activity of TAAR1 agonists.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

All animal studies were carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Ministry of Health of Russian Federation and the principles of the Basel Declaration. All experiments were approved by the Saint Petersburg State University Ethical Committee for Animal Research (approval number 131-03-4 from March 14, 2016). All surgical procedures were performed under anesthesia, and all efforts were made to minimize suffering.

Author Contributions

AA and RG provided the concept of the study. AA, RG, VK, and AV provided design of the study. VK, ED, AV, and NP contributed to the data acquisition and analysis. All authors contributed to the interpretation and discussion of the results, drafted, and critically revised the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Russian Foundation for Basic Research (Grant No. 17-04-00082).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ahmed, M., Mällo, T., Leppänen, P. H. T., Hämäläinen, J., Äyräväinen, L., Ruusuvirta, T., et al. (2011). Mismatch brain response to speech sound changes in rats. Front. Psychol. 2:283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahveninen, J., Kähkönen, S., Pennanen, S., Liesivuori, J., Ilmoniemi, R. J., and Jääskeläinen, I. P. (2002). Tryptophan depletion effects on EEG and MEG responses suggest serotonergic modulation of auditory involuntary attention in humans. Neuroimage 16, 1052–1061. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aleksandrov, A. A., Knyazeva, V. M., Volnova, A. B., Dmitrieva, E. S., Korenkova, O., Espinoza, S., et al. (2018). Identification of TAAR5 Agonist Activity of Alpha-NETA and Its Effect on Mismatch Negativity Amplitude in Awake Rats. Neurotox. Res. 34, 442–451. doi: 10.1007/s12640-018-9902-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astikainen, P., Stefanics, G., Nokia, M., Lipponen, A., Cong, F., Penttonen, M., et al. (2011). Memory-based mismatch response to frequency changes in rats. PLoS One 6:e0024208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, M. D. (2007). The potential of trace amines and their receptors for treating neurological and psychiatric diseases. Rev. Recent Clin. Trials 2, 3–19. doi: 10.2174/157488707779318107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, M. D., Gainetdinov, R. R., Hoener, M. C., and Shahid, M. (2017). Pharmacology of human trace amine-associated receptors: therapeutic opportunities and challenges. Pharmacol. Ther. 180, 161–180. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2017.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borowsky, B., Adham, N., Jones, K. A., Raddatz, R., Artymyshyn, R., Ogozalek, K. L., et al. (2001). Trace amines: identification of a family of mammalian G protein-coupled receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 8966–8971. doi: 10.1073/pnas.151105198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulton, A. A. (1974). Amines and Theories in Psychiatry. Lancet 304, 52–53. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91390-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradaia, A., Trube, G., Stalder, H., Norcross, R. D., Ozmen, L., Wettstein, J. G., et al. (2009). The selective antagonist EPPTB reveals TAAR1-mediated regulatory mechanisms in dopaminergic neurons of the mesolimbic system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 20081–20086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906522106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branchek, T. A., and Blackburn, T. P. (2003). Trace amine receptors as targets for novel therapeutics: legend, myth and fact. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 3, 90–97. doi: 10.1016/S1471-4892(02)00028-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchett, S. A., and Hicks, T. P. (2006). The mysterious trace amines: protean neuromodulators of synaptic transmission in mammalian brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 79, 223–246. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corripio, I., Escartí, M. J., Portella, M. J., Pérez, V., Grasa, E., Sauras, R. B., et al. (2011). Density of striatal D2 receptors in untreated first-episode psychosis: an I123-IBZM SPECT study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 861–866. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corripio, I., Pérez, V., Catafau, A. M., Mena, E., Carrió, I., and Alvarez, E. (2006). Striatal D2 receptor binding as a marker of prognosis and outcome in untreated first-episode psychosis. Neuroimage 29, 662–666. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coull, J. T., Sahakian, B. J., Middleton, H. C., Young, A. H., Park, S. B., McShane, R. H., et al. (1995). Differential effects of clonidine, haloperidol, diazepam and tryptophan depletion on focused attention and attentional search. Psychopharmacology 121, 222–230. doi: 10.1007/BF02245633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, B. A. (1989). Biogenic amines and their metabolites in body fluids of normal, psychiatric and neurological subjects. J. Chromatogr. A 466, 89–218. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9673(01)84617-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, C. L., Wall, T. L., and Chaplin, R. I. (1991). Long latency event-related potentials in rats: effects of dopaminergic and serotonergic depletions. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 38, 789–793. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90243-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrlichman, R. S., Maxwell, C. R., Majumdar, S., and Siegel, S. J. (2008). Deviance-elicited changes in event-related potentials are attenuated by ketamine in mice. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1403–1414. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinoza, S., Lignani, G., Caffino, L., Maggi, S., Sukhanov, I., Leo, D., et al. (2015). TAAR1 modulates cortical glutamate NMDA receptor function. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2217–2227. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinoza, S., Salahpour, A., Masri, B., Sotnikova, T. D., Messa, M., Barak, L. S., et al. (2011). Functional interaction between trace amine-associated receptor 1 and dopamine D2 receptor. Mol. Pharmacol. 80, 416–425. doi: 10.1124/mol.111.073304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Featherstone, R. E., Shin, R., Kogan, J. H., Liang, Y., Matsumoto, M., and Siegel, S. J. (2015). Mice with subtle reduction of NMDA NR1 receptor subunit expression have a selective decrease in mismatch negativity: implications for schizophrenia prodromal population. Neurobiol. Dis. 73, 289–295. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainetdinov, R. R., Hoener, M. C., and Berry, M. D. (2018). Trace amines and their receptors. Pharmacol. Rev. 70, 549–620. doi: 10.1124/pr.117.015305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, L., Fulham, W. R., Todd, J., Budd, T. W., Hunter, M., Meehan, C., et al. (2014). Mismatch negativity (MMN) in freely-moving rats with several experimental controls. PLoS One 9:e0110892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, L., Fulham, W. R., Todd, J., Meehan, C., Schall, U., Hodgson, D. M., et al. (2018). Late deviance detection in rats is reduced, while early deviance detection is augmented by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801. Schizophr. Res. 191, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, L., Michie, P. T., and Näätänen, R. (2016). Criteria for determining whether mismatch responses exist in animal models: focus on rodents. Biol. Psychol. 116, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegerl, U., Gallinat, J., and Juckel, G. (2001). Event-related potentials: do they reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission and do they predict clinical response to serotonin agonists? J. Affect. Disord. 62, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00353-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegerl, U., and Juckel, G. (1993). Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission: a new hypothesis. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 173–187. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90137-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirvonen, J., Van Erp, T. G. M., Huttunen, J., Aalto, S., Någren, K., Huttunen, M., et al. (2005). Increased caudate dopamine D 2 receptor availability as a genetic marker for schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 371–378. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jääskeläinen, I. P., Lehtokoski, A., Alho, K., Kujala, T., Pekkonen, E., Sinclair, J. D., et al. (1995). Low dose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-related potentials. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 607–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01555.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Javitt, D. C., Doneshka, P., Zylberman, I., Ritter, W., and Vaughan, H. G. (1993). Impairment of early cortical processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential confirmation study. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 513–519. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90005-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Javitt, D. C., Steinschneider, M., Schroeder, C. E., and Arezzo, J. C. (1996). Role of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 11962–11967. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.21.11962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, F., Stephan, K. E., Backes, H., Moran, R., Gramer, M., Kumagai, T., et al. (2013). Mismatch responses in the awake rat: evidence from epidural recordings of auditory cortical fields. PLoS One 8:e0063203. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kähkönen, S., Ahveninen, J., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Kaakkola, S., Näätänen, R., Huttunen, J., et al. (2001). Effects of haloperidol on selective attention: a combined whole-head MEG and high-resolution EEG study. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 498–504. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00255-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kähkönen, S., Ahveninen, J., Pekkonen, E., Kaakkola, S., Huttunen, J., Ilmoniemi, R. J., et al. (2002). Dopamine modulates involuntary attention shifting and reorienting: an electromagnetic study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1894–1902. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00305-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kähkönen, S., Mäkinen, V., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Pennanen, S., Liesivuori, J., and Ahveninen, J. (2005). Serotonergic modulation of mismatch negativity. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 138, 61–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karl, A., Malta, L. S., and Maercker, A. (2006). Meta-analytic review of event-related potential studies in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychol. 71, 123–147. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreitschmann-Andermahr, I., Rosburg, T., Demme, U., Gaser, E., Nowak, H., and Sauer, H. (2001). Effect of ketamine on the neuromagnetic mismatch field in healthy humans. Cogn. Brain Res. 12, 109–116. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00043-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leo, D., Mus, L., Espinoza, S., Hoener, M. C., Sotnikova, T. D., and Gainetdinov, R. R. (2014). Taar1-mediated modulation of presynaptic dopaminergic neurotransmission: role of D2 dopamine autoreceptors. Neuropharmacology 81, 283–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, S., Croft, R. J., Baldeweg, T., and Nathan, P. J. (2007). Acute dopamine D1 and D2 receptor stimulation does not modulate mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology 194, 443–451. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0865-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, G. A., and Braff, D. L. (2005). Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 127–136. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, G. A., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2015). Future clinical uses of neurophysiological biomarkers to predict and monitor treatment response for schizophrenia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1344, 105–119. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann, L., Ebeling, M., Kratochwil, N. A., Bunzow, J. R., Grandy, D. K., and Hoener, M. C. (2005). Trace amine-associated receptors form structurally and functionally distinct subfamilies of novel G protein-coupled receptors. Genomics 85, 372–385. doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2004.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann, L., Meyer, C., Jeanneau, K., Bradaia, A., Ozmen, L., Bluethmann, H., et al. (2008). Trace amine-associated receptor 1 modulates dopaminergic activity. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 324, 948–956. doi: 10.1124/jpet.107.132647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, C. R., Liang, Y., Weightman, B. D., Kanes, S. J., Abel, T., Gur, R. E., et al. (2004). Effects of chronic olanzapine and haloperidol differ on the mouse N1 auditory evoked potential. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:739. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, P. T. (2001). What has MMN revealed about the auditory system in schizophrenia? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 177–194. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00166-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, G. M. (2011). The emerging role of trace amine-associated receptor 1 in the functional regulation of monoamine transporters and dopaminergic activity. J. Neurochem. 116, 164–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07109.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, F., Smith, K., Cowen, P., Robbins, T., and Sahakian, B. (2002). The effects of tryptophan depletion on cognitive and affective processing in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 163, 42–53. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1128-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W. K., and Mäntysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. 42, 313–329. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., and Kähkönen, S. (2009). Central auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia as revealed by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm: a review. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 125–135. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708009322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., Kujala, T., and Winkler, I. (2011). Auditory processing that leads to conscious perception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by the mismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology 48, 4–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01114.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, T., Michie, P. T., Fulham, W. R., Todd, J., Budd, T. W., Schall, U., et al. (2011). Epidural auditory event-related potentials in the rat to frequency and duration deviants: evidence of mismatch negativity? Front. Psychol. 2:367. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panas, H. N., Lynch, L. J., Vallender, E. J., Xie, Z., Chen, G. L., Lynn, S. K., et al. (2010). Normal thermoregulatory responses to 3-iodothyronamine, trace amines and amphetamine-like psychostimulants in trace amine associated receptor 1 knockout mice. J. Neurosci. Res. 88, 1962–1969. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, E. W., and Fowler, B. (1999). Dissociation of the mismatch negativity and processing negativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide. Psychophysiology 36, 552–558. doi: 10.1017/breakS0048577299971172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekkonen, E., Hirvonen, J., Ahveninen, J., Kähkönen, S., Kaakkola, S., Huttunen, J., et al. (2002). Memory-based comparison process not attenuated by haloperidol: a combined MEG and EEG study. Neuroreport 13, 177–181. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polyakova, N. V., Vinogradova, E. P., Aleksandrov, A. A., and Gainetdinov, R. R. (2018). Prepulse inhibition in the TAAR1 knockout mice. Rossiiskii fiziologicheskii zhurnal imeni I.M. Sechenova / Rossiiskaia akademiia nauk 104, 1098–1105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revel, F. G., Moreau, J.-L., Gainetdinov, R. R., Bradaia, A., Sotnikova, T. D., Mory, R., et al. (2011). TAAR1 activation modulates monoaminergic neurotransmission, preventing hyperdopaminergic and hypoglutamatergic activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 8485–8490. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103029108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revel, F. G., Moreau, J. L., Pouzet, B., Mory, R., Bradaia, A., Buchy, D., et al. (2013). A new perspective for schizophrenia: TAAR1 agonists reveal antipsychotic- and antidepressant-like activity, improve cognition and control body weight. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 543–556. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roger, C., Hasbroucq, T., Rabat, A., Vidal, F., and Burle, B. (2009). Neurophysics of temporal discrimination in the rat: a mismatch negativity study. Neurotox Res. Psychophysiol. 46, 1028–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00840.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruusuvirta, T., Lipponen, A., Pellinen, E. K., Penttonen, M., and Astikainen, P. (2015). Auditory cortical and hippocampal local-field potentials to frequency deviant tones in urethane-anesthetized rats: an unexpected role of the sound frequencies themselves. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 96, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, J. A. J., Jorissen, B. L., Sobczak, S., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Hogervorst, E., Deutz, N. E. P., et al. (2000). Tryptophan depletion impairs memory consolidation but improves focussed attention in healthy young volunteers. J. Psychopharmacol. 14, 21–29. doi: 10.1177/026988110001400102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiramatsu, T. I., Kanzaki, R., and Takahashi, H. (2013). Cortical mapping of mismatch negativity with deviance detection property in rat. PLoS One 8:e0082663. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, S. J., Connolly, P., Liang, Y., Lenox, R. H., Gur, R. E., Bilker, W. B., et al. (2003). Effects of strain, novelty, and NMDA blockade on auditory-evoked potentials in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:675. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sivarao, D. V., Chen, P., Yang, Y., Li, Y. W., Pieschl, R., and Ahlijanian, M. K. (2014). NR2B antagonist CP-101,606 abolishes pitch-mediated deviance detection in awake rats. Front. Psychiatry 5:96. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sukhanov, I., Caffino, L., Efimova, E. V., Espinoza, S., Sotnikova, T. D., Cervo, L., et al. (2016). Increased context-dependent conditioning to amphetamine in mice lacking TAAR1. Pharmacol. Res. 103, 206–214. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2015.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tikhonravov, D., Neuvonen, T., Pertovaara, A., Savioja, K., Ruusuvirta, T., Näätänen, R., et al. (2008). Effects of an NMDA-receptor antagonist MK-801 on an MMN-like response recorded in anesthetized rats. Brain Res. 1203, 97–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. E., Light, G. A., Olincy, A., Radant, A. D., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 69–94. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umbricht, D., Latanov, A., Vissotksi, D., Nitsch, R., and Lipp, H. P. (2002). Development of a mouse model of deficits in preattentive auditory processing in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 51:64S.

Google Scholar

Umbricht, D., Schmid, L., Koller, R., Vollenweider, F. X., Hell, D., and Javitt, D. C. (2000). Ketamine-induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthy volunteers: implications for models of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 1139–1147. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.12.1139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umbricht, D., Vyssotky, D., Latanov, A., Nitsch, R., Brambilla, R., D’Adamo, P., et al. (2004). Midlatency auditory event-related potentials in mice: comparison to midlatency auditory ERPs in humans. Brain Res. 1019, 189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolinsky, T. D., Swanson, C. J., Smith, K. E., Zhong, H., Borowsky, B., Seeman, P., et al. (2007). The Trace Amine 1 receptor knockout mouse: an animal model with relevance to schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav. 6, 628–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00292.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: trace amine-associated receptors, TAAR1, RO5263397, mismatch negativity, event-related potentials, oddball paradigm, schizophrenia biomarkers

Citation: Aleksandrov AA, Knyazeva VM, Volnova AB, Dmitrieva ES, Polyakova NV and Gainetdinov RR (2019) Trace Amine-Associated Receptor 1 Agonist Modulates Mismatch Negativity-Like Responses in Mice. Front. Pharmacol. 10:470. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00470

Received: 13 February 2019; Accepted: 15 April 2019;
Published: 03 May 2019.

Edited by:

Tatiana Lipina, University of Toronto, Canada

Reviewed by:

Giovanni Laviola, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Italy
Stephen Morairty, SRI International, United States

Copyright © 2019 Aleksandrov, Knyazeva, Volnova, Dmitrieva, Polyakova and Gainetdinov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Veronika M. Knyazeva, v.m.knyazeva@spbu.ru; knyazeva.vero@yandex.ru

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.