Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 May 2018
Sec. Schizophrenia

Differential Resting-State Connectivity Patterns of the Right Anterior and Posterior Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortices (DLPFC) in Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 2JARA BRAIN, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 3Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • 4Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Brain and Behaviour (INM-7), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
  • 6Iowa Neuroscience Institute, Carver College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
  • 7Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical School, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 8Werner Reichardt Center for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 9LEAD Graduate School and Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 10Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 11Univ Lille, CNRS UMR 9193, SCALab and CHU Lille, Division of Psychiatry, CURE platform, Fontan Hospital, Lille, France
  • 12Neuroscience Division, University Medical Centre Utrecht and Rudolf Magnus Institute for Neuroscience, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 13Section for Experimental Psychopathology and Neuroimaging, Department of General Psychiatry, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany

In schizophrenia (SCZ), dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been linked to the deficits in executive functions and attention. It has been suggested that, instead of considering the right DLPFC as a cohesive functional entity, it can be divided into two parts (anterior and posterior) based on its whole-brain connectivity patterns. Given these two subregions' differential association with cognitive processes, we investigated the functional connectivity (FC) profile of both subregions through resting-state data to determine whether they are differentially affected in SCZ. Resting-state magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans were obtained from 120 patients and 172 healthy controls (HC) at 6 different MRI sites. The results showed differential FC patterns for the anterior and posterior parts of the right executive control-related DLPFC in SCZ with the parietal, the temporal and the cerebellar regions, along with a convergent reduction of connectivity with the striatum and the occipital cortex. An increased psychopathology level was linked to a higher difference in posterior vs. anterior FC for the left IFG/anterior insula, regions involved in higher-order cognitive processes. In sum, the current analysis demonstrated that even between two neighboring clusters connectivity could be differentially disrupted in SCZ. Lacking the necessary anatomical specificity, such notions may in fact be detrimental to a proper understanding of SCZ pathophysiology.

Introduction

In neuroimaging, the most widely used method of characterizing functional neuroanatomy is based on the integration of larger anatomical brain regions. Based on this methodology, the DLPFC is frequently treated as a unified region controlling function through a top-down modulation of task-relevant information processing in the premotor and posterior parietal associative cortices (1, 2).

The DLPFC is reciprocally interconnected with motor areas in the medial frontal lobe, the rostral cingulate cortex, the premotor cortices as well as the cerebellum and the superior colliculus (36). While there is consensus regarding the role of the DLPFC in executive control, the varied results with respect to the location and extent of activation sites [e.g., (79)] beg the question of whether this variability is due to the region's functional heterogeneity.

Deficits in executive function, working memory, and attention in schizophrenia [stable and common symptoms observed during the lifespan of schizophrenia patients (1012) are thought to be linked to reduced activity in the bilateral] DLPFC and the dorsal parietal cortex (13). However, findings pertaining to DLPFC involvement in schizophrenia are not consistent with studies that do not reveal any differences in the DLPFC between HC (14) and others despite reporting patterns of hyperactivation (15). Moreover, there is considerable variability in terms of the location and extent of DLPFC impairment across functional neuroimaging experiments investigating executive functions in both healthy controls (16, 17) and patients (13, 18, 19). For instance, when healthy controls showed involvement of the BA44 and BA 40 in response to working memory challenge (17) the differences between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls during working memory task were seen more posterior (BA 45, BA 46, BA 47) (18).

The inconclusive results of the studies mentioned above may be due not only to the variations in the experimental context but also to the lack of an understanding of the functional heterogeneity of the DLPFC region. This most likely indicates that the executive control of behavior relies on distinct DLPFC subregions involved in differentiable neural networks and cognitive functions.

The results of our previous parcellation study involving data from healthy individuals suggest that a single region of interest within the right DLPFC is not a functional entity but is organized hierarchically with one anterior ventral and one posterior dorsal DLPFC sub-region (Figure 1) (16). Our previous study showed an increased connectivity between the posterior (vs. anterior) DLPFC and the posterior parietal cortex, thereby indicating a possible network for cognitive control related to stimulus processing and selection of behavior-relevant information. In contrast, for the anterior (vs. posterior) DLPFC cluster, an increased functional connectivity with the ACC was seen. This resonates well with the results from a range of fMRI studies investigating cognitive control, suggesting that the DLPFC and the ACC (BAs 24 and 32) are specifically activated with an increase in demands for cognitive control and monitoring due to conflict in information processing and competing response plans (2022). Thus, while the posterior cluster of the region is likely changed to be more strongly involved in action control processes, depending on the interaction with stimulus processing and working changed memory, the anterior region is likely responsible for higher-order control processes such as motor response monitoring and action inhibition (16).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Posterior (red) and anterior (green) right DLPFC seed regions as derived from a previous co-activation-based parcellation study as described in Cieslik et al. (16).

Based on these observations, we sought to explore whether SCZ is linked to a differential connectivity disruption in the right anterior and posterior DLPFC (aDLPFC/pDLPFC) sub-regions, hypothesizing that a connectivity imbalance in the DLPFC sub-regions is part of the SCZ pathophysiology.

Methods

Definition of Volume of Interest (VOI)

The DLPFC VOIs were based on previous work (16), separating the right DLPFC region into two clusters by using hierarchical cluster analysis: a more anterior-ventral (center of gravity MNI coordinates: 30/43/23) one and a more posterior-dorsal one (center of gravity MNI coordinates: 37/33/32) (Figure 1). Henceforth, for the sake of simplicity, the regions will be referred to as anterior and posterior seeds or sub-regions. In the study by Cieslik et al. (16), volume of Interest was defined by merging the DLPFC activation sites from 4 previous studies investigating motor control. The first 3 studies (23, 24, 25) used manual stimulus–response tasks requiring a speeded response to a visual stimulus by a button press with either the left or right index finger, whereas the fourth study (26) used a manual sequence reproduction task. All 4 studies, which differed in the specific demands for executive motor control, showed activation in the right DLPFC with partially overlapping but slightly different locations. Following thresholding at P < 0.05 (cluster-level family-wise error [FWE]-corrected) of the individual contrasts, the ensuing 4 DLPFC clusters were combined into a single VOI (cluster size: 674 voxels). That is, every single voxel in the VOI region showed activation in at least one of the 4 studies. Subsequently, it was assessed whether this seed region could be divided into subregions based on similarities and differences between co-activation patterns of the individual seed voxels across neuroimaging experiments.

Sample Description

We investigated resting-state functional connectivity of the two right DLPFC sub-regions in 120 patients with a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia [recruited at six sites: Aachen, Alberquerque (COBRE), Göttingen, Groningen, Lille, and Utrecht] and 172 healthy controls, with patients and controls not differing with respect to age Symptom severity was assessed by use of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale [PANSS, (27)] (for detailed demographic and clinical characteristics, please refer to Table 1 and Supporting Information Table S1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics.

The diagnosis was ascertained by clinical examination of the attending psychiatrist according to the International Classification of Diseases (28) or the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (29) criteria.

At the time of scanning, all patients were under antipsychotic medications, except for 6 (7%), who were not on any medication. Chlorpromazine-equivalents (CPZ-equivalents) were estimated as described in (Table 2) (30).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Information on how many of the patients were medicated or unmedicated at time of measurement and mean medication (CPZ-equivalents) per site.

All subjects provided written informed consent to participate in the study prior to inclusion as approved by the ethics committees of the participating universities. Joint re-analysis was approved by the ethics committee at the Heinrich-Heine University Düsseldorf.

Resting State fMRI Data: Imaging and Preprocessing

For each subject resting state EPI (echo-planar-imaging) images were acquired using standard blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast [gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence] (Please see Table S2 for details on EPI sequence for each site).

Prior to further processing (using SPM8, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) the first four images were discarded allowing for magnetic field saturation. The EPI images were corrected for head movement by affine registration in a two-pass procedure realigning EPI volumes to its mean image. The mean EPI image for each subject was spatially normalized to the MNI ICBM-152 subject template using the “unified segmentation” approach (31). The ensuing deformation field was applied to the individual EPI volumes and smoothed with a 5-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.

Neither the patient and control subsamples (sites) nor the overall disease cohorts showed group differences (t-tests with p > 0.2) in the three movement parameters (DVARS, FD, and RMD), indicating reasonably similar head motion during the scanning process (32).

To minimize spurious correlations between BOLD time courses through confounds such as physiological noise and motion, any variance that could be explained by the following nuisance variables was removed from each voxel's time series: (i) the six motion parameters derived from image realignment and (ii) their first derivative. According to published evaluations, motion regressors entered the model as first- and second-order terms resulting in 24 movement regressors (33) Given the evidence of group comparisons being distorted by correcting for the global mean signal (34, 35), the whole-brain resting-state functional connectivity of each of the two seeds (for all subjects) was calculated without Global Signal regression.

Finally, the data were band-pass filtered preserving BOLD frequencies between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz (36).

Individual and Group Level Analyses

For each subject and VOI the first eigenvariate of the VOIs time-series were calculated separately for each DLPFC seed and supplied to further whole-brain functional connectivity analysis. Linear (Pearson) correlation coefficients between the time series of the seed regions and those of all other gray-matter voxels in the brain were computed to quantify resting-state functional connectivity. These voxel-wise correlation coefficients were then transformed into Fisher's z-scores and then fed into a second-level analysis of variance (ANOVA).

Next, any variances from each voxel of the two seeds of the resting state functional connectivity (RSFC) data, which could be explained by age, gender, site and amount of within-scanner movement, were removed. To do so, voxel-wise effects of age, gender, site, and amount of within-scanner movement were included as predictors in a regression model based on only the HC data. This model was then used to adjust the FC data of each patient into relative scores. Therefore, these scores reflect hyper- or hypoconnectivity at each voxel relative to what would have been expected based on those covariates in HC. Thus, the deviations from the expected value calculated on the basis of HC values was used to express hyper- or hypoconnectivity of the DLPFC seeds in patients. The main problem that arises when estimating the coefficients for confounder adjustment from the entire group of subjects (i.e., patients and controls) is the potential multi-collinearity between socio-demographic confounders (age, gender as well as measurement site) and clinical characteristics and hence the neurobiological features of schizophrenia. Thus, the estimated effects of the confounders also include pathological features that are likely to introduce a bias to the estimation. In view of this, we resorted to the strategy described in Rozycki et al. (37), i.e., to estimate the effects of socio-demographic covariates only in the healthy control population and apply the ensuing model to adjust the data from all participants. Given that this approach permits an unbiased estimation of the effects of, e.g., age and gender, when it is applied to the patients, only the effects of the covariates (but not the disease process itself) ought to be removed. Similarly, the coefficients estimated from the model based on the HC data were applied to the whole sample including the patients. This effectively removed the influence of the demographic and, most importantly, any clinical effects on the difference between patients and controls. These adjusted data formed the basis of the between-group analysis (ANOVA) created to test for seed (aDLPFC/pDLPFC) × diagnostic group interaction.

Resting State Functional Connectivity of the Seed Regions in HC

In a first step, we aimed to replicate the specific RSFC profile of the two DLPFC subregions that was found in our previous study (16). To do so, in the present healthy subsample we first tested for increased FC of the anterior vs. posterior seed (aDLPFC_HC > pDLPFC_HC) in conjunction with the main effect of the anterior seed's positive correlation (aDLPFC_HC). The same rational was done to test for the specific RSFC of the posterior seed (i.e., pDLPFC_HC > aDLPFC_HC ∩ pDLPFC_HC) in HC. The results were p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level.

Group Differences in Whole-Brain Functional Connectivity

In a conjunction analysis we tested for disorder-related changes in the connectivity of both seeds. To this end, we first identified regions to which both seed regions are connected in HC and for which both seeds show reduced connectivity in SCZ. To test this, a conjunction analysis across the difference in RSFC between HC and SCZ (HC > SCZ) for both seeds and the main effect of both seeds' positive correlation in HC (i.e., aDLPFC_HC > aDLPFC_SCZ ∩ pDLPFC_HC > pDLPFC_SCZ ∩ aDLPFC_HC ∩ pDLPFC_HC) was performed. Next, again using conjunction analysis, we looked for regions being significantly connected in HC and showing increased RSFC for SCZ compared to HC (i.e., aDLPFC_HC < aDLPFC_SCZ ∩ pDLPFC_HC < pDLPFC_SCZ ∩ aDLPFC_HC ∩ pDLPFC_HC). The results of both analyses, unless noted otherwise, were p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level.

Finally, we assessed seed × group interactions to reveal a specific disconnectivity of both DLPFC sub-regions. We tested for the regions showing a relatively increased positive RSFC with the posterior DLPFC cluster (and relatively decreased positive RSFC with the anterior cluster), and then visa-versa.

Results of the interaction were regarded as significant if they passed the threshold of a cluster-level FWE (cFWE) rate of p < 0.05 (cluster-forming threshold at voxel level: p < 0.001). The use of cFWE correction was on account of the fact that no significant clusters were left after FWE correction on the voxel level. The significant effects of the interaction were tested with t-tests. The results of the t-test were p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level.

Correlations Between Functional Connectivity and Clinical Parameters

At the next step we analyzed the relationship between the relative RSFC shifts between both seed regions and psychopathology in the schizophrenic sample. To this extend, the differences of adjusted RSFC between the anterior and posterior DLPFC seeds were calculated (pDLPFC > aDLPFC) in the SCZ sample. For this reason, positive differential values indicate a RSFC shift from the anterior (and toward the posterior) seed, while negative differential values indicate a RSFC shift from the posterior (and toward the anterior) seed. The calculated amount of differential disconnectivity was then correlated with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) total scores and all individual subscales. Again, the results were cFWE-corrected at p < 0.05 (Figures S1, S2, cluster-forming threshold at voxel level: p < 0.001). No significant clusters were left following additional Bonferroni correction. For exploratory reasons, results prior to Bonferroni correction have been provided.

Anatomical Allocation of Results

Brain regions were anatomically allocated to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps using v2.0 of the SPM Anatomy Toolbox [http://www.fz-juelich.de/ime/spm_anatomy_toolbox; (38, 39)].

Results

Resting-State Functional Connectivity in HC

In HC, a significantly stronger RSFC of the posterior (compared to the anterior) seed was found with the left homotope (geometrically corresponding) region (peak MNI: −28/56/16; T = 9.48; 1,578 voxels), the bilateral IPS (peak MNI: 56, −34, 46/−44, −42, 42; areas hIP1, hIP2, and hIP3), the right inferior temporal gyrus (peak MNI: 60/–54/−16; T = 6.20; 123 voxels) and the left precentral gyrus (BA 44; peak MNI: −46/6/32; T = 6.90; 88 voxels). Conversely, significantly stronger RSFC of the anterior (compared to the posterior) seed was observed with the left homotope region (MNI-coordinates: −28/56/16 T = 9.48; 1,578 voxels), the left ACC (MNI-coordinates: −2/30/20; T = 7.18; 87 voxels), the left PCC (MNI-coordinates: −8/−46/28; T = 5.44; 66 voxels), the right inferior frontal gyrus (p. orbitalis, MNI-coordinates: 30/18/−22; T = 6.03; 64 voxels) and the right frontopolar region (Area Fp1, MNI-coordinates: 32/50/18; T = 40.58; 1,067 voxels).

Taken together, in a new sample of healthy controls we could replicate our previous results (16).

Group Differences in Resting-State Functional Connectivity: Conjunction Analysis

Compared to the HC, SCZ patients showed significantly reduced RSFC of the two seed regions (conjunction analysis) with the bilateral caudate, the right putamen, bilateral inferior occipital gyrus (V4) extending into middle occipital gyrus (Figure 2). For further details, please refer to Table 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significantly decreased functional connectivity of the posterior and anterior right DLPFC seeds (conjunction analysis) with the bilateral striatum (A) as well as the bilateral visual cortex (B) in SCZ patients as compared to HC. Results are projected onto the MNI single-subject template on the left side and contrast estimates for the effect in the point maximum respective region are shown on the right.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regions with significantly decreased functional connectivity with both DLPFC seeds.

Results of the individual contrasts [p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level] of SCZ-related reduction in RSFC with the anterior and posterior clusters respectively are presented in Tables S3, S4. Please also refer to Table S5 which shows for exploratory reasons the conjunction analysis results after cFWE correction. In contrast with these marked reductions, no significant RSFC increases were observed for either seed in SCZ patients relative to HC.

Group Differences in Resting-State Functional Connectivity: Group × Seed Interaction

Seed (aDLPFC/pDLPFC) × diagnostic group (SCZ/HC) interactions indicated sub-regional DLPFC disconnectivity with the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) (peak MNI: 50/−54/42; T = 3.74; 239 voxels) and the left cerebellum (peak MNI: −46/−58/−40; T = 4.62; 134 voxels) in patients (Figures 3A,D).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regions showing an SCZ-related (relative) decreased functional connectivity with the anterior DLPFC compared to the posterior seed were (A) the right IPL (Area PGa) and (D) the left cerebellum. Contrast estimates for the effect in the respective region (B,E) and the difference (SCZ—HC) in RSFC between HC and SCZ for the posterior and anterior DLPFC seeds respectively (C,F) are demonstrated below. *indicates the significant effects of the interaction assessed with t-tests (p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level).

According to the parameter estimates (Figure 3B), the IPL of both groups had a weaker connectivity with the aDLPFC compared to the pDLPFC, although the difference was significant only in the patient group (peak MNI: 54/−34/60; T = 6.27) (Figure 3C).

In the second cluster located in the cerebellum, HC showed stronger FC in the anterior DLPFC compared to the posterior DLPFC, while a reverse pattern was observed in patients (Figure 3E). The group difference in parameter estimates (Figure 3F) indicated a shift of the cerebellum's RSFC away from the anterior (toward the posterior) DLPFC subregion among patients. None of the group comparisons revealed any significant differences in the region (Figure 3E).

The reverse interaction indicated further subregional DLPFC disconnectivity with the right precentral gyrus (Area 4a, peak MNI: 38/−22/56; T = 4.31; 127 voxels) and the right middle temporal gyrus (peak MNI: 50/−32/−6; T = 4.45; 115 voxels) (Figures 4A,D).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Regions showing an SCZ-related (relative) decreased functional connectivity with the posterior compared to the anterior DLPFC seed were (A) the right precentral gyrus and (D) the right middle temporal gyrus. Contrast estimates for the effect in the respective region are shown under (B,E). Parameter estimates depict the difference in RSFC between HC and SCZ for the posterior and anterior DLPFC seeds respectively and are shown under (C,F). *indicates the significant effects of the interaction assessed with t-tests (p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)-corrected on the voxel level).

In HC, a significantly increased positive RSFC with the posterior DLPFC cluster (and decreased RSFC with the anterior cluster) was observed in Area 4a (peak MNI: 38/−22/56; T = 6.37), whereas a reverse non-significant relationship (relatively decreased positive RSFC with the posterior cluster and an increased RSFC with the anterior cluster) was observed in SCZ (Figure 4B).

The group difference in parameter estimates (Figure 4C) revealed an RSFC shift in patients from Area 4a toward the anterior and away from the posterior DLPFC subregion.

In the cluster located in the right middle temporal gyrus (Figure 4E), the SCZ group showed a relatively decreased positive RSFC with the posterior DLPFC cluster (and relatively increased positive RSFC with the anterior cluster), with these subregional differences in RSFC being statistically significant (peak MNI: 52/−30/−10; T = 7.23). The group difference in terms of parameter estimates (Figures 4E,F) revealed a significantly reduced positive RSFC between the right middle temporal gyrus and the posterior DLPFC (peak MNI: 48/−32/−6; T = 5.45; 20 voxels) in patients as compared to controls.

Correlation With Clinical Parameters

With increasing symptom severity, several cortical regions showed a relatively decreased RSFC with the anterior DLPFC sub-region, paralleled by a relatively increased RSFC with the posterior sub-region.

An increase in the difference in adjusted RSFC (calculated as pDLPFC > aDLPFC) in the left IFG/anterior insula lobe (MNI: −34/22/12; T = 4.35; 161 voxels) was found to go along with higher total PANSS scores (Figure 5A), while in the left operculum (MNI: −44/−6/18; T = 4.31; 124 voxels) a stronger difference was related to higher general psychopathology PANSS scores (Figure 5B).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation with symptomatology. Correlation of the difference between posterior and anterior DLPFC's FC with (A) the left IFG/anterior insula region and total PANSS score, and (B) the left operculum and the general PANSS score general PANSS score.

The correlation analysis with the total negative symptoms scale and the age of onset did not reveal any significant effects.

Discussion

We investigated RSFC in HC and patients with SCZ, targeting the connectivity for the right anterior and posterior executive control-related DLPFC sub-regions (16). In SCZ patients, those two neighboring sub-regions demonstrated diametrically altered functional connectivity affecting the right IPL, the left cerebellum, the right precentral gyrus and the right middle temporal gyrus. Correlation analysis indicated furthermore that the connectivity imbalance of the DLPFC sub-regions with the left IFG/anterior insula and the left operculum may be linked to symptom severity.

We believe that such small, albeit not well understood, effects have an enormous impact on the results (and their interpretation) of neuroimaging studies. The common practice of studying the DLPFC region as a whole obfuscates the differential effects, thus stymieing the progress of our understanding of DLPFC functions and their pathological effects in patient collectives.

Reduction of RSFC With the Striatum and the Occipital Cortex in Both DLPFC Sub-Regions

Reduced corticostriatal connectivity, common among patients with chronic and adolescent-onset schizophrenia [(40, 41)] as well as patients with first-episode psychosis (42) and their relatives (43), is not only a trait characteristic, but also, as indicated by an improvement of psychosis symptoms with increased RSFC between the right striatum and the right DLPFC (44), a state characteristic of psychosis (44).

Reduced RSFC with the bilateral striatum in SCZ patients (compared to HC) was found for both DLPFC seeds. As corticostriatal dysconnectivity accounts for psychotic symptoms of SZC, it is not surprising that it likely affects rather large areas of the right DLPFC.

A reduced RSFC between the DLPFC and the bilateral V4 (a region crucial for visual object recognition and visual attention (45) is a less common finding in schizophrenia patients and is likely to be linked to disruptions in the visual attention network (46, 47).

Attention and Action Inhibition: Connectivity Shifts From the Anterior (Toward the Posterior) DLPFC Subregion

Among patients, we observed a subregional DLPFC dysconnectivity with the right IPL and the left cerebellum. As previously shown by our group, the anterior DLPFC subregion in particular is likely involved in higher-order control processes of motor behavior such as attention and subsequent behavioral adjustments (16). As part of the frontoparietal executive-control network, the IPL is strongly interconnected with the DLPFC (48) and is also involved in attentional selection of sensory contents (49) and sustained attention (50). Our research (the presented work as well as the previous study by Cieslik et al. (16) has consistently shown an increased connectivity between the posterior (vs. anterior) DLPFC and the IPL in HC. In the present study, the IPL-aDLPFC connectivity was found to be weaker in patients compared to HC, and, only in patients, the IPL showed a significantly weaker connectivity to the aDLPFC compared to the pDLPFC. Apart from being linked to thought disorder and disruption of sensory integration, the reduced connection between the DLPFC (in particular the anterior DLPFC seed) and the IPL likely underlies the pronounced deficits in attention and other executive functions seen in psychosis (51). In addition, the connectivity shift from the anterior (toward the posterior) DLPFC region may underlie particular deficits in attentive stimulus processing and selection of behavior-relevant information. The connectivity shift may also explain why the involvement of the parietal cortex in SCZ strongly depends on the chosen task. For instance, compared to controls, patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia have shown significantly lower activation in the posterior parietal cortices along with decreased frontoparietal functional connectivity during a working-memory task (52). The link is underscored by the fact that selective attention plays a central role in working memory (53).

With cerebellar hypoactivation being commonplace in SCZ (54, 55), a few studies also indicate changes in the RSFC between the cerebellum and the DLPFC in SCZ (56, 57). The cerebellum, which has been known to be linked to the motor system, is also likely involved in attention, cognitive learning, and multisensory integration (58). It has also been seen to have a lower activation level during working-memory tasks in patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia (52). Habas et al. (59) have shown that the cerebellum, as part of cortico-cerebellar loops, is involved in executive control and salience detection. Our findings are also consistent with the notion of a misconnection in the cortico-cerebellar-thalamo-cortical (CCTC) network (60) underlying the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

Taken together, our observations suggest that the cortico-cerebellar and cortico-parietal RSFC dysfunction in SCZ may not only be network-specific (61), but also strongly dependent on the exact localization of the DLPFC clusters. Given the involvement of the anterior DLPFC seed in higher-order control processes of motor behavior and attention (16), these findings can help broaden our understanding with respect to the task-based aberrant activity patterns in those regions.

Action Execution: Connectivity Shifts From the Posterior (Toward the Anterior) DLPFC Subregion

Here, we observed effects in the patients' primary motor cortex (Area 4a) and the right MTG.

The right DLPFC has been seen to be strongly interconnected with the motor cortex (4, 61, 62), modulating its activity during selection, planning, and execution of motor behavior (63). In HC, we saw a significantly increased positive RSFC with the posterior DLPFC cluster (and decreased RSFC with the anterior cluster) in Area 4a. This effect, likely associated in HC with a stronger emphasis of the posterior DLPFC cluster in action execution (16), was missing in patients. Connectivity shifts from the posterior (toward the anterior) DLPFC subregion and the Area 4a may be a correlate of reduced capacities in action execution (64) and prolonged motor planning and execution (65) seen in schizophrenia. The primary motor cortex, which is activated not only during execution (66, 67) but also during observation of others' actions (68), the latter putatively reflecting mirror neuron activity (69), is linked to a wide range of important social behaviors, from speech to imitation and empathy (69) and is reduced in schizophrenia (70, 71). The activation of the primary motor cortex is also closely linked to automatic motor potentiation: e.g., the viewing of a right-facing cup handle activates the left hemispheric motor areas (72), triggering potentiation of the right hand (73). The regulatory relationship between the frontal and primary motor cortices is also evident in what is known as “utilization behavior” (74), which is believed to be linked to intrusive, disruptive, and inappropriate motor behaviors seen in individuals with frontal system damage or dysfunction (e.g., dementias, schizophrenia, depressive disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) (75).

In the cluster located in the right MTG, only patients showed a connectivity shift from the posterior (toward the anterior) DLPFC. That this effect was not seen in HC, indicates its association with functional aberrations. Thus, the right MTG is linked to the perception of biological motion [movements generated by living beings; (76)] as well as different aspects of face perception (77, 78). In SCZ, deficits in face perception (79) and biological motion perception (80), processes crucial to the interpretation of social information, have already been elucidated (81). As demonstrated by our results, deficits in action execution in schizophrenia may be related in particular to the disturbed RSFC between the right DLPFC subregion and the primary motor cortices. Specifically, given the involvement of the posterior DLPFC seed in action execution (16), a connectivity shift from the posterior (toward the anterior) DLPFC may underlie impairments (in SCZ) with respect to prolonged motor planning and execution, or utilization behavior.

The Link Between Pathophysiology and Psychopathology

The correlation analysis with psychopathology indicated that the sub-regional DLPFC disconnectivity with the left IFG/anterior insula and the left operculum correlated positively with increasing total PANSS and general psychopathology scores.

The ACC, the DLPFC, the insula and the posterior parietal cortex form a regulatory network for high-level cognitive control and attentional processes (82, 83). In particular, decreased gray matter in the anterior insula has been linked to executive function deficits observed in e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and depression (84).

Patients with schizophrenia also experience difficulties in appropriately attributing self-generated sensory stimuli. The anterior insula/IFC region and the adjoining frontal operculum have been suggested to be involved in the processing of self-generated sensory stimuli (81). The regulatory influence of the right DLPFC on the anterior insula has been observed during the regulation of pain intensity (85) as well as visual and auditory awareness of the moment (81). Taken together, our findings suggest that symptoms linked to misperception of the self as a distinct entity, and also deficits in high-level cognitive control and attentional processes, are likely to be dependent on symptom severity.

Limitations

Despite its careful design, and the fact that it reached its goal, the study had a few limitations that ought to be mentioned. First, the data were collected from 6 different sites, and thus the heterogeneity of scanners and sequences might potentially have affected the results. Second, given that the diagnosis was based (depending on the site where the subjects were recruited) on the DSM-5 or ICD-10 criteria, the procedure, despite the overlap in diagnostic criteria, could have led to diagnostic inconsistencies. Third, at the time of assessment, most patients (93%) were under psychopharmacological treatment, with about 69% (n = 83) of the patient's group taking multiple compounds and no more than six having the same combination of drugs. Therefore, the potential effect of medication on the results cannot be ruled out. Finally, another limitation of the study is the lack of neuropsychological assessment of patients, owing to which the examination of the DLPFC subregions in relation to executive functions was rendered impossible.

Conclusion

As summarized, the current analysis involving a large, representative sample of SCZ patients has demonstrated that even the connectivity of two neighboring clusters identified by advanced in-vivo mapping may be differentially disrupted in SCZ. Thus, we conclude that the imbalanced connectivity of the sub-regions (anterior and posterior DLPFC seeds), rather than that of the DLPFC as a whole, characterizes the connectional disruption of the DLPFC in SCZ. We would also argue that prevalent concepts such as “prefrontal dysfunction” may be far too broad to help explain the apparently region-specific disturbances in SCZ.

Author Contributions

NC and SE: substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work; TN-J, BD, LK, AA, RJ, IS and OG: the acquisition of data for the work; NC, SE, EC, and VM: analysis and interpretation of data for the work; NC, SE, EC, and VM: drafting the work; NC, SE, EC, VM, TN-J, BD, LK, AA, RJ, IS, and OG: revising it critically for important intellectual content; NC, SE, EC, VM, TN-J, BD, LK, AA, RJ, IS, and OG: final approval of the version to be published; NC, SE, TN-J, BD, LK, AA, RJ, IS, and OG: agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) via the Clinical Research Group 241 Genotype-phenotype relationships and neurobiology of the longitudinal course of psychosis, TP2 (PI Gruber; http://www.kfo241.de; grant number GR 1950/5-1).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00211/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science (2003) 302:1181–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. MacDonald GM, Gervais BR, Snyder JA, Tarasov GA, Borisova OK. Radiocarbon dated Pinus sylvestris L. wood from beyond tree-line on the Kola Peninsula, Russia. Holocene (2000) 10:143–7. doi: 10.1191/095968300667807510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. (1993) 336:211–28. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lu MT, Preston JB, Strick PL. Interconnections between the prefrontal cortex and the premotor areas in the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol. (1994) 341:375–92. doi: 10.1002/cne.903410308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. The cerebrocerebellar system. Int Rev Neurobiol. (1997) 41:31–60. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(08)60346-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Petrides M, Pandya DN. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur J Neurosci. (1999) 11:1011–36. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nee DE, Jonides J, Berman MG. Neural mechanisms of proactive interference-resolution. Neuroimage (2007) 38:40–751. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Vogt P, Riitters KH, Estreguil C, Kozak J, Wade TG, Wickham JD. Mapping spatial patterns with morphological image processing. Landsc Ecol. (2007) 22:171–77. doi: 10.1007/s10980-006-9013-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Yamaguchi S, Zheng D, Oka T, Bokura H. The key locus of common response inhibition network for no-go and stop signals. J Cogn Neurosci. (2008) 20:1434–42. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Birkett P, Brindley A, Norman P, Harrisoon G, Baddeley A. Control of attention in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. (2006) 40:579–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Everett J, Laplante L, Thomas J. The selective attention deficit in schizophrenia. Limited resources or cognitive fatigue? J Nerv Ment Dis. (1989) 177:735–8. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198912000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Orellana G, Slachevsky A, Peña M. Executive attention impairment in first episode schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry (2012) 12:154. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Barch DM, Ceaser A. Cognition in schizophrenia: core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Guillozet-Bongaarts AL, Hyde TM, Dalley RA, Hawrylycz MJ, Henry A, Hof PR, et al. Altered gene expression in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry (2014) 19:478–85. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJP, Duyn J, Coppola R, et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex (2000) 10:1078–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Cieslik EC, Zilles K, Caspers S, Roski C, Kellermann TS, Jakobs O, et al. Is there “One” DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cereb Cortex (2013) 23:2677–89. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn PD, Kolachana B, Kippenhan S, McInerney-Leo A, Nussbaum R, et al. Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:594–6. doi: 10.1038/nn1438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, Holt JL, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, et al. Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1809–17. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Van Snellenberg JX, Girgis RR, Horga G, Van de Giessen E, Slifstein M, Ojeil N, et al. Mechanisms of working memory impairment in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 80:617–26. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Liston C, Matalon S, Hare TA, Davidson MC, Casey BJ. Anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortices are sensitive to dissociable forms of conflict in a task switching paradigm. Neuron (2006) 50:643–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cole MW, Schneider W. The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage (2007) 37:343–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sohn MH, Albert MV, Jung K, Carter CS, Anderson JR. Anticipation of conflict monitoring in the anterior cingulate cortex and the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:10330–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703225104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Jakobs O, Wang LE, Dafotakis M, Grefkes C, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB. Effects of timing and movement uncertainty implicate the temporo-parietal junction in the prediction of forthcoming motor actions. Neuroimage (2009) 47:667–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Cieslik EC, Zilles K, Kurth F, Eickhoff SB. Dissociating bottom up and top-down processes in a manual stimulus–response compatibility task. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 104:1472–83. doi: 10.1152/jn.00261.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Eickhoff SB, Pomjanski W, Jakobs O, Zilles K, Langner R. Neural correlates of developing and adapting behavioral biases in speeded choice reactions—an fMRI study on predictive motor coding. Cereb Cortex (2011) 21:1178–91. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kellermann TS, Sternkopf MA, Schneider F, Habel U, Turetsky BI, Zilles K, et al. Modulating the processing of emotional stimuli by cognitive demand. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2012) 7:263–73. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dilling H, Freyberger HJ. (2016). Taschenführer zur ICD-10-Klassifikation Psychischer Störungen. Bern: Hogrefe.

29. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

30. Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos R, Miller D, Ho BC. Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67:255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage (2005) 26:839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Van Dijk KRA, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage (2012) 59:431–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, et al. An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage (2013) 64:240–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gotts SJ, Saad ZS, Jo HJ, Wallace GL, Cox RW, Martin A. The perils of global signal regression for group comparisons: a case study of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:356. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Hang JJ, Martin A, et al. Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect. (2012) 2:25–32. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2007) 8:700–11. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rozycki M, Satterthwaite TD, Koutsouleris N, Erus G, Doshi J, Wolf DH, et al. Multisite machine learning analysis provides a robust structural imaging signature of schizophrenia detectable across diverse patient populations and within individuals. Schizophr Bull. (2017). doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx137. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Eickhoff SB, Grefkes C, Zilles K, Fink GR. The somatotopic organization of cytoarchitectonic areas on the human parietal operculum. Cereb Cortex (2007) 17:1800–11. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Gereon RF, Amunts K, et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage (2005) 25:1325–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Peters H, Riedl V, Manoliu A, Scherr M, Schwerthöffer D, Zimmer C, et al. Changes in extra-striatal functional connectivity in patients with schizophrenia in a psychotic episode. Br J Psychiatry (2017) 210:75–82. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.151928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

41. James A, Joyce E, Lunn D, Hough M, Kenny L, Ghataorhe R, et al. Abnormal frontostriatal connectivity in adolescent-onset schizophrenia and its relationship to cognitive functioning. Eur Psychiatry (2016) 35:32–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.2426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zhou Y, Liang M, Jiang T, Tian L, Liu Y, Liu Z, et al. Functional dysconnectivity of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in first-episode schizophrenia using resting-state fMRI. Neurosci Lett. (2007) 417:297–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fornito A, Harrison BJ, Goodby E, Dean A, Ooi C, Nathan RJ, et al. Functional dysconnectivity of corticostriatal circuitry as a risk phenotype for psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry (2013) 70:1143. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sarpal DK, Robinson DG, Lencz T, Argyelan M, Ikuta T, Karlsgodt K, et al. Antipsychotic treatment and functional connectivity of the striatum in first episode schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:5. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Roe AW, Chelazzi L, Connor CE, Conway BR, Fujita I, Gallant JL, et al. Toward a unified theory of visual area V4. Neuron (2012) 74:12–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Potts GF, O'Donnell BF, Hirayasu Y, McCarley RW. Disruption of neural systems of visual attention in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2002) 59:418–24. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.5.418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Wood SM, Potts GF, Martin LE, Kothmann D, Hall JF, Ulanday JB. Disruption of auditory and visual attention in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2007) 156:105–16. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Gary HG, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:2349–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ptak R. The frontoparietal attention network of the human brain: action, saliency, and a priority map of the environment. Neuroscientist (2012) 18:502–15. doi: 10.1177/1073858411409051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Singh-Curry V, Husain M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia (2009) 47:1434–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Torrey EF. Schizophrenia and the inferior parietal lobule. Schizophr Res. (2007) 97:215–25. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Loeb FF, Zhou X, Craddock KE, Shora L, Broadnax DD, Gochman P, et al. Reduced functional brain activation and connectivity during a working memory task in childhood-onset schizophrenia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 57:166–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Vandierendonck A. Symbiosis of executive and selective attention in working memory. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:588. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lungu O, Barakat M, Laventure S, Debas K, Proulx S, Luck D, et al. The incidence and nature of cerebellar findings in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of fMRI literature. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:797–806. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mothersill O, Knee-Zaska C, Donohoe G. Emotion and theory of mind in schizophrenia investigating the role of the cerebellum. Cerebellum (2016) 15:357–68. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0696-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Chen YL, Tu PC, Lee YC, Chen YS, Li CT, Su TP. Resting-state fMRI mapping of cerebellar functional dysconnections involving multiple large-scale networks in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2013) 149:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Wang L, Zou F, Shao Y, Ye E, Jin X, Tan S, et al. Disruptive changes of cerebellar functional connectivity with the default mode network in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2014) 160:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ronconi L, Casartelli L, Carnal S, Molten M, Aragon F, Borgata R. When one is enough: impaired multisensory integration in cerebellar agenesis. Cereb Cortex (2016) 27:2041–51. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Habas C, Kadar N, Nguyen D, Prater K, Beckmann CF, et al. Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:8586–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Andreasen NC. A unitary model of schizophrenia: Bleeder's fragmented preen as schizencephaly. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:781–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Connections of the ventral granular frontal cortex of macaques with perisylvian premotor and somatosensory areas: anatomical evidence for somatic representation in primate frontal association cortex. J Comp Neurol. (1989) 282:293–316. doi: 10.1002/cne.902820210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Takada M, Nambu A, Hatanaka N, Tachibana Y, Miyachi M, Taira M, et al. Organization of prefrontal outflow toward frontal motor-related areas in macaque monkeys. Eur J Neurosci. (2004) 19:3328–42. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03425.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Sreenivasan KK, Curtis CE, D'Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. (2003) 2:82–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001

CrossRef Full Text

64. Gold JM, Robinson B, Leonard CJ, Hahn B, Chen S, McMahon RF, et al. Selective attention, working memory, and executive function as potential independent source of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2017). doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx155. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Dutschke LL, Stegmayer K, Ramseyer F, Bohlhalter S, Vanbellingen T, Strik W, et al. Gesture impairments in schizophrenia are linked to increased movement and prolonged motor planning and execution. Schizophr Res. (2017) 8:30413–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Terumitsu M, Ikeda K, Kwee IL, Nakada T. Participation of primary motor cortex area 4a in complex sensory processing: 3.0-T fMRI study. Neuroreport (2009) 20:679–83. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832a1820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Geyer S, Ledberg A, Schleicher A, Roland PE. Two different areas within the primary motor cortex of man. Nature (1996) 382:805–7. doi: 10.1038/382805a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1998) 95:15061–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Iacoboni M, Dapretto M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2006) 7:942–51. doi: 10.1038/nrn2024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Enticott P, Hoy K, Herring S, Johnston P, Daskalakis Z, Fitzgerald P. Reduced motor facilitation during action observation in schizophrenia: a mirror neuron deficit? Schizophr Res. (2008) 102:116–21. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Mehta UM, Thirthalli J, Aneelraj D, Jadhav P, Gangadhar BN, Keshavan MS. Mirror neuron dysfunction in schizophrenia and its functional implications: a systematic review. Schizophr Res. (2014) 160:9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Grafton ST, Fadiga L, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G. Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage (1997) 6:231–6. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. McBride J, Boy F, Husain M, Sumner P. Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action. Front Hum Neurosci. (2012) 6:82. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Freeman SM, Itthipuripat S, Aron AR. High working memory load increases intracortical inhibition in primary motor cortex and diminishes the motor affordance effect J Neurosci. (2016) 36:5544–55. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0284-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Archibald SJ, Mateer CA, Kerns KA. Utilization behavior: clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychol Rev. (2001) 11:117–30. doi: 10.1023/A:1016673807158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Puce A, Perrett D. Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos Trans R Soc B (2003) 358:435–45. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Ramon M, Vizioli L, Liu-Shuang J, Rossion B. Neural microgenesis of personally familiar face recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:E4835–44. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414929112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Vartanian O, Goel V, Lam E, Fisher M, Granic J. Middle temporal gyrus encodes individual differences in perceived facial attractiveness. Psychol Aesthet Creativ Arts (2013) 7:38–47. doi: 10.1037/a0031591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Kohler CG, Walker JG, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg RJ. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Bull. (2010) 36:1009–19. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Kim J, Doop ML, Blake R, Park S. Impaired visual recognition of biological motion in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2005) 77:299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Craig AD. How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Goodkind M, Eickhoff SB, Oathes DJ, Jiang Y, Chang A, Jones-Hagata LB, et al. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:305. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Wylie KP, Tregellas JR. The role of the insula in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2010) 123:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Lorenz J, Minoshima S, Casey KL. Keeping pain out of mind: the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in pain modulation. Brain (2003) 126:1079–91. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI neuroimaging, Schizophrenia, DLPFC, resting-state fMRI, parcellation

Citation: Chechko N, Cieslik EC, Müller VI, Nickl-Jockschat T, Derntl B, Kogler L, Aleman A, Jardri R, Sommer IE, Gruber O and Eickhoff SB (2018) Differential Resting-State Connectivity Patterns of the Right Anterior and Posterior Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortices (DLPFC) in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 9:211. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00211

Received: 17 October 2017; Accepted: 03 May 2018;
Published: 28 May 2018.

Edited by:

Thomas W. Weickert, University of New South Wales, Australia

Reviewed by:

Fabio Sambataro, University of Udine, Italy
Caroline F. Zink, Lieber Institute for Brain Development, United States
Vishnu Murty, Temple University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Chechko, Cieslik, Müller, Nickl-Jockschat, Derntl, Kogler, Aleman, Jardri, Sommer, Gruber and Eickhoff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Natalia Chechko, nchechko@ukaachen.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.