Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 10 May 2019
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Identifying Neuroimaging-Based Markers for Distinguishing Brain Disorders View all 25 articles

Disrupted Resting Frontal–Parietal Attention Network Topology Is Associated With a Clinical Measure in Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Yanpei Wang,,Yanpei Wang1,2,3Fuxiang Tao,*Fuxiang Tao1,2*Chenyi ZuoChenyi Zuo3Maihefulaiti KanjiMaihefulaiti Kanji4Mingming Hu,Mingming Hu1,2Daoyang Wang*Daoyang Wang3*
  • 1State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3College of Educational Science, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, China
  • 4The Key Laboratory of Mental Development and Learning Science, Xinjiang Normal University, Urumqi, China

Purpose: Although alterations in resting-state functional connectivity between brain regions have been reported in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), the spatial organization of these changes remains largely unknown. Here, we studied frontal–parietal attention network topology in children with ADHD, and related topology to a clinical measure of disease progression.

Methods: Resting-state fMRI scans were obtained from New York University Child Study Center, including 119 children with ADHD (male n = 89; female n = 30) and 69 typically developing controls (male n = 33; female n = 36). We characterized frontal–parietal functional networks using standard graph analysis (clustering coefficient and shortest path length) and the construction of a minimum spanning tree, a novel approach that allows a unique and unbiased characterization of brain networks.

Results: Clustering coefficient and path length in the frontal–parietal attention network were similar in children with ADHD and typically developing controls; however, diameter was greater and leaf number, tree hierarchy, and kappa were lower in children with ADHD, and were significantly correlated with ADHD symptom score. There were significant alterations in nodal eccentricity in children with ADHD, involving prefrontal and occipital cortex regions, which are compatible with the results of previous ADHD studies.

Conclusions: Our results indicate the tendency to deviate from a more centralized organization (star-like topology) towards a more decentralized organization (line-like topology) in the frontal–parietal attention network of children with ADHD. This represents a more random network that is associated with impaired global efficiency and network decentralization. These changes appear to reflect clinically relevant phenomena and hold promise as markers of disease progression.

Introduction

Even when in the resting state and not performing any task processing, the brain is still working. The spontaneous brain activity present in the resting state is not random and usually shows high temporal coupling across different brain regions. This creates a set of brain networks that are densely interconnected and distinct from others. These resting-state networks are not a reaction to any task, but are intrinsically generated by the brain itself (1, 2). To date, at least seven brain networks have been identified by a variety of functional connectivity analysis approaches, and they show a high stability across individuals (16). The frontal–parietal attention network (FPAN) is involved cognitive process, especially attention in these networks (79). The FPAN not only has been studied in task-related activation studies involving sustained attention, but has also been confirmed by functional connectivity at rest that directly relates to attention performance (8, 10).

Graph theory has been used to study the architecture of brain networks (11, 12) and has revealed an economical and highly efficient organization of functional connectivity that combines global efficiency and local integration. This is called small-world (SW) topology and is characterized by limited long-distance and dense local connections (13, 14). Many brain diseases have been related to disrupted organization of brain networks. The study of brain networks has increased our understanding of the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms for many brain diseases such as epilepsy and schizophrenia (1517). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common psychiatric disorders during childhood and persists into adolescence and adulthood (18). Several whole-brain studies using graph theory analysis have reported a shift from a SW topology towards a more regular organization in ADHD, which results in increased local integration and loss of global network efficiency (19, 20). In addition, a vulnerability of some hub regions has been reported (21). As a neurodevelopmental disorder, ADHD is characterized by developmentally inappropriate symptoms of excessive inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. Many studies have found that ADHD is a developmental disorder and associated with developmental delay (22). Recently, graph analysis studies have confirmed a shift from more random to more regular SW topological structure during maturation (2325). Smit and colleagues have confirmed connectivity alteration that reflected increased network randomness, or decreased order (26). These results suggest that the maturational delay in ADHD is reflected by more random brain connectivity, but not more regular (2325).

It is difficult to compare networks reported in graph theory studies across different groups and conditions. A normalization step is required to allow comparison. Common approaches are thresholding and/or comparing the observed network with randomized networks generated from the observed network; however, these do not provide a unique or consistent solution (27). One potential solution is minimum spanning tree (MST), which is derived from a weighted network (28). MST is an acyclic subnetwork that connects the same number of nodes and connections, and therefore not only makes the comparison of network topology easier across conditions but also avoids potential deviations that may be introduced through normalization steps. Several studies have used the MST approach to investigate brain networks and have shown that this approach is sensitive to brain disease, such as Alzheimer’s disease (29), epilepsy (30), and maturation from childhood into adulthood (23).

The aim of the present study was to explore the alteration of the FPAN connectivity or topology in children with ADHD. Increasing evidence has demonstrated that ADHD was a developmental disorder and associated with developmental delay. Typical maturation during childhood involves a shift from a random towards more regular networks (31). We hypothesized that, in youth with ADHD, functional networks would shift towards being more random, evidenced by decreased local integration and global efficiency. Although some previous studies report a regular topology in ADHD, with increased local integration and decreased global efficiency, we believe that these studies have some shortcomings. First, they analyzed the whole brain network, but different brain networks mainly took on different cognitive task (32), which relied on coactivation of executive network (e.g., frontal–parietal control network) and reciprocal suppression of the nonexecutive network (e.g., default mode network, DMN). The whole brain network analysis may have confused the role of different brain networks. Fair and colleagues found reduced spontaneous activity within the DMN in ADHD (33), and a follow-up study found decreased connectivity in DMN and dorsal attention networks, and enhanced connectivity within reward-motivation regions in the resting-state in young adults with ADHD (34). These previous findings suggest the presence of altered functional brain networks associated with attention and cognitive processing in ADHD. However, the topological features of functional brain networks in FPAN have yet to be extensively investigated. The FPAN is a critical module in attention processing (8), and exploring its alteration in ADHD may be helpful for understanding the pathological mechanism of disease. Second, usually, they used a range of thresholds to construct the SW topology, and the difference between ADHD and typically developing controls (TDCs) mainly exists at some threshold, which was not robust and lost many low signals. In addition, they did not compare with the real random networks. We used the connectivity strength between each pair of brain regions as the edge to construct the SW topology and compared the observed network with randomized networks generated from the observed network to normalize. We also construct MST to explore the alteration of brain networks in ADHD. Conventional network measures may give inaccurate differences in connectivity strength, density, and graph size between subjects. MST overcomes these problems and provides an elegant solution, which, up to this point, has not received much attention in the neuroscience literature. MST is an unbiased approach, and the diameter and leaf number of MST were strongly related with the path length of SW topology (24). MST captured changes in FPAN topology, supporting results derived from conventional network analysis (24). In addition, MST successfully captured alterations in the properties of the whole-brain network during maturation in children (23) and supported the finding that the randomness of the topology reduced with age, as shown by conventional network analysis (24). Finally, although many studies have reported group differences between children with ADHD and TDC, they have not associated these differences with clinical features. In the present study, we computed the Pearson’s correlation between the properties of SW topology and clinical features.

In the present study, we used concepts from graph theory to examine resting-state functional connectivity within the FPAN. We hypothesized that the functional networks in the FPAN would shift towards being more random in children with ADHD than in TDC. We used graph theory analysis to quantify publically available resting functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from 119 children with ADHD and 69 TDC. We calculated several measures derived from the SW and MST to assess local integration, global efficiency, and relative node importance within the networks and hypothesized that brain networks of participants with ADHD would display lower global efficiency and local integration than brain networks of TDC, and that this would be accompanied by a loss of centrality of individual brain regions within the FPAN. An analysis was then conducted to determine correlations between SW and MST parameters and ADHD-related disability, as measured using the ADHD symptom score. We hypothesized that, in children with ADHD, SW and MST would be associated with ADHD symptom score.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Data Acquisition

The data we used in this study are publicly available from the ADHD-200 Consortium (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/adhd200/). We first selected 191 participants between the ages of 7 and 14 years from New York University Child Study Center and excluded 7 participants whose IQ (Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, WASI), gender, or diagnosis information were missing, resulting in the 188 participants for further analysis, including 119 children with ADHD (male n = 89; female n = 30) and 69 TDC children (male n = 33; female n = 36), detailed in Table 1. All participants provided signed informed consent as approved by the IRBs of NYU and the NYU School of Medicine and were compensated, and the institutional review boards approved the research protocols.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of ADHD and TDC groups.

ADHD Symptoms Measures

Dimensional ratings of ADHD symptoms (Inattention; Hyperactivity/Impulsivity) were assessed using Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised, Long Version (CPRS-LV).

Magnetic Resonance Imaging Dataset and Processing

Magnetic Resonance Imaging Dataset

High-resolution T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE images covering the whole brain were acquired for each participant on a Siemens 3.0-Tesla Allegra MRI scanner at the NYU Center for Brain Imaging [time repetition (TR) = 2,530 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.25 ms, T1 = 1,100 ms; flip angle = 7°, voxel size = 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.3 mm, field of vision (FOV) = 256 mm]. Functional imaging was performed in a single run using a blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast sensitive gradient echo-planar sequence (TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 15 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 240 mm, 33 slices per volume, 176 volumes, acquisition voxel size = 3.0 × 3.0 × 4.0 mm). During this scan, participants were asked to relax with their eyes open.

Data Processing

Image preprocessing was performed using the DPARSF data processing assistant for resting functional MRI (rsfMRI) (35). Preprocessing comprised the following steps: 1) discarding the first 10 volumes; 2) slice timing to correct for temporal offsets; 3) realignment of each volume for head movement; 4) spatial normalization to MNI space (New Segment + DARTEL) and then resampled to 3-mm isotropic voxels; 5) spatial smoothing with a 4-mm 3D full width at half-maximum kernel; 6) detrending to remove linear trends due to scanner drift; 7) temporal band-pass filtering (0.01–0.1 Hz) to remove low-frequency drift and high-frequency physiological noises; and 8) regressing whole brain and white matter signals out of the 24 motion parameters.

Graph and Functional Connectivity Analysis

Graph analysis was performed using Gretna software (36) for BOLD time series extraction (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/gretna) and Brain connectivity toolbox (37) for SW and MST topology (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/bct/). The functional connectivity derived from 16 brain regions forming FPAN (detailed in Table 2), which come from previous literature and were transformed to The Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates (7, 9). Regions of interesting (ROIs) were defined as 6-mm-radius spheres around these MNI coordinates (8). We extracted BOLD time series from each of the voxels in each ROI, and averaged all voxels in the respective ROI as the signal. The functional connectivity between each pair of ROIs was then computed by a Pearson’s correlation and formed a 16×16 matrix, which were z-standardized by Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to approximate a Gaussian distribution. Typical graph analyses of weighted networks ignored negative correlation (1), and we followed the traditional approach. We used the matrix to construct SW networks and to compute the network properties. Graph and functional connectivity analysis pipelines are shown in Figure 1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 MNI coordinates of the 16 nodes in the FPAN.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Graph and functional connectivity analysis pipeline. Schematic overview of the formation of individual functional brain networks using two different methods, i.e., construction of weighted graphs and minimum spanning trees (MST). After MRI recording (A), data from the MRI (B) were projected onto a functional framework of 16 frontal–parietal attention network (FPAN regions) (C). Functional connectivity between each pair of brain regions was assessed by means of the Pearson’s correlation (Fisher’s r-to-z transformation) (D). For weighted network analysis, a weighted graph (E) was constructed from the Pearson’s correlation (z-standardized). For minimum spanning tree analysis, the minimum spanning tree matrix also was derived from the correlation (z-standardized) matrix by Kruskal’s algorithm (D’), with concurrent MST construction (E’). Finally, network measures were computed for both the weighted graph (F) and the minimum spanning tree (F’) (28).

Small-World Properties

In order to approximate a Gaussian distribution, the Pearson’s correlation coefficients in the resulting 16×16 matrix were transformed by Fisher’s r-to-z. This matrix represents the strength of the functional connectivity between all 16 regions in the whole brain and served as an adjacency matrix for graph analysis. SW parameters clustering coefficient (C) and path length (L) were calculated in terms of Watts and Strogat (1998). Briefly, characteristic path length is defined as the average shortest path connecting any two nodes in the graph. The path length is used to measure how well a network is connected; a small value indicates an average short distance between any two nodes. The cluster coefficient is defined as the number of actual edges connecting the neighbors of a node divided by the maximum number of edges possible between neighboring nodes. The cluster coefficient of a network is used to measure how many local clusters exist in the network. A high cluster coefficient indicates that the neighbors of a node are often also directly connected to each other, that is, they form a cluster.

To determine whether a network has SW properties, the values of L and C must be normalized by generated random networks (12). SW networks are characterized by path lengths that are similar to those of comparable random networks (Lrandom) but with increased cluster coefficients (Crandom):λ = L/Lrandom≈1 and γ = C/Crandom > 1 (38). Random clustering coefficient and path length derived from the mean of those values from 100 random networks. Each random network was generated by randomly reshuffling the edge weights in the original network (39), which ensures that the node degree and node distribution of the random network are similar to those of the original network.

Minimum Spanning Tree Reconstruction

The MST of an undirected weighted network is a unique acyclic subgraph that connects all the nodes with the minimum possible link weight. In our analysis, we used the maximum connection strength (correlation matrix) as the edge to construct an acyclic subnetwork, equivalent to a MST as obtained by using the Kruskal algorithm (40). Briefly, all connections are arranged in descending order, then starting from the strongest strength edge, consecutive high strength links were added until all nodes (n) were connected and formed an acyclic subnetwork consisted with n-1 edges (Figure 1). If adding a link resulted in the formation of a cycle, this link was skipped.

In terms of the information about the topological properties of the MST, we can compute several measures to characterize the topology of the tree, including the diameter, normalized leaf fraction, kappa (degree of divergence), betweenness centrality, and hierarchy. The diameter, which is the largest distance between any two nodes, is defined as the longest shortest path in the network. The normalized leaf number is defined as the number of nodes with a degree of 1, divided by the maximum number of leaves possible given the size of the tree, and is used to measure the integration in the network (41). A decreased value of the normalized leaf number indicates a decreased global efficiency. Previous studies have found leaf number to be an important network parameter during development, and it is sensitive to the changes in aging (25), autism (42), and Parkinson’s disease (28). Kappa, also called degree of divergence, is used to measure the broadness of the degree distribution. A decreased value of kappa indicates a decreased number of highly connected nodes or “hubs.” Betweenness centrality (BC) of a node is defined as the number of shortest paths between any two nodes passing it, divided by the total number of shortest paths in the network. If BC = 0, the node is a leaf node; if BC = 1, the node is a central node in a star-like network. The BC of a node ranges between 0 and 1. Usually, we used BCmax, which is the BC of the node with the highest BC in the tree to measure the BC of the tree. A decreased value of BCmax in the tree indicates a decreased global efficiency and a decreased “hub” strength. Hierarchy is an indicator of the balance between efficient communication paths and overload of hub nodes, which is defined as

TH=L2mBCmax

where L is the leaf number, m is the number of vertices −1, and BCmax is the maximum value of betweenness centrality. The value of hierarchy ranges between 0 and 1. If leaf number = 2, tree is a line-like topology, and hierarchy approaches 0. If leaf number = m, tree is a star-like topology, and tree hierarchy approaches 0.5. When the number is between 2 and m, tree hierarchy can have higher values (28).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical differences in age, handedness, gender, verbal, and performance IQ were evaluated using T test (Table 1). Due to the difference only in verbal IQ and gender between TDC and ADHD children, all analyses were also conducted with verbal IQ and gender as covariates. Group differences in graph theory analysis and functional connectivity were examined using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in which the main effect of diagnosis was tested with verbal IQ and gender as covariates. Moreover, a partial correlation coefficient was used to assess the relation between network topology (in terms of SW and MST parameters) and ADHD symptom score. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 21 (IBM, Armonk, NY). Multiple comparisons were controlled using the false discovery rate (q < 0.05) (43).

Results

Group Characteristics

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of ADHD and TDC children. No significant group differences were observed in age (t = 0.217, p = 0.828), handedness (t = −1.745, p = 0.082), and performance IQ (t = 1.566, p = 0.119); gender (t = −3.715, p < 0.001) and verbal (t = 2.604, p = 0.010) IQ showed differences between ADHD and TDC children. In the subsequent analysis, gender and verbal IQ were used as covariates.

Functional Connectivity

After controlling for gender and verbal IQ, no significant group differences were observed for the FPAN mean strength [ADHD: 0.155 ± 0.034; TDC: 0.153 ± 0.035, F(1,184) = 0.472, p = 0.493], detailed in Table 3. No significant correlation (r = 0.023; p = 0.754) was observed between the FPAN mean strength and the ADHD scores, detailed in Table 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Group differences in network parameters.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Correlations between network parameters and disability score.

Small-World Topology

No significant group differences were observed in SW topology clustering coefficient [ADHD: 1.254 ± 0.193; TDC: 1.221 ± 0.219, F(1,184) = 0.328, p = 0.568] or path length [ADHD: 1.118 ± 0.066; TDC: 1.104 ± 0.072, F(1,184) = 1.209, p = 0.273], detailed in Table 3. No significant correlation was observed between SW topology clustering coefficient and ADHD symptom score (r = 0.040, p = 0.590) or path length and ADHD symptom score (r = 0.097, p = 0.186), detailed in Table 4.

Minimum Spanning Tree Topology

A significant group difference was observed for diameter [ADHD: 0.667 ± 0.089; TDC: 0.632 ± 0.099, F(1,184) = 7.387, p = 0.007], leaf number [ADHD: 0.410 ± 0.071; TDC: 0.428 ± 0.084, F(1,184) = 6.098, p = 0.014], tree hierarchy [ADHD: 0.021 ± 0.004; TDC: 0.022 ± 0.004, F(1,184) = 5.505, p = 0.020], and kappa [ADHD: 2.264 ± 0.123; TDC: 2.308 ± 0.157, F(1,184) = 7.780, p = 0.006], detailed in Table 3. These variables were significantly related to ADHD symptom score (diameter: r = 0.175, p = 0.017; leaf number: r = −0.208, p = 0.004; tree hierarchy: r = −0.218, p = 0.003; kappa: r = −0.212, p = 0.004), detailed in Table 4. No significant group difference was observed for betweenness centrality [ADHD: 0.649 ± 0.058; TDC: 0.656 ± 0.060, F(1,184) = 0.724, p = 0.396] or eccentricity [ADHD: 0.535 ± 0.028; TDC: 2.308 ± 0.157, F(1,184) = 2.071, p = 0.152], detailed in Table 3, and these variables had no significant correlation with ADHD symptom score (betweenness centrality: r = −0.016; p = 0.829; eccentricity: r = 0.112; p = 0.129), detailed in Table 4.

To further examine the regionally nodal characteristics of brain networks, the group difference in nodal eccentricity was tested in the MST topology. Eccentricity was significantly greater in children with ADHD than in TDC in the left intraparietal sulcus [ADHD: 7.328 ± 1.595; TDC: 6.783 ± 1.617, F(1,184) = 7.017, p = 0.009], bilateral ventral intraparietal [left—ADHD: 8.059 ± 1.457; TDC: 7.406 ± 1.584, F(1,184) = 11.305, p = 0.001; right—ADHD: 7.79 ± 1.545; TDC: 7.304 ± 1.365, F(1,184) = 9.206, p = 0.003], and left and right ventral occipital lobe [left—ADHD: 7.731 ± 1.655; TDC: 7.087 ± 1.755, F(1,184) = 6.143, p = 0.014; right: ADHD: 7.462 ± 1.736; TDC: 6.768 ± 1.637, F(1,184) = 8.809, p = 0.003], which also correlated with ADHD scores (left IPS: r = 0.165, p = 0.025; left vIPS: r = 0.205, p = 0.005; right vIPS: r = 0.154, p = 0.036; left vOC: r = 0.195, p = 0.008; right vOC: r = 0.172, p = 0.019), detailed in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Regions showing significant changes in each nodal eccentricity in ADHD.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate SW and MST properties of FPAN topology in children with and without ADHD. We found that, although brain functional networks exhibited economical SW topology in both groups, children with ADHD had greater MST diameter and lower leaf number, tree hierarchy, and kappa than TDC, and these variables were also associated with ADHD symptom score.

Since Watts and Strogatz proposed and quantitatively described SW networks (12), it has been applied in brain structural and functional networks in many studies using various imaging techniques including electroencephalography, magnetoencephalography, and functional MRI (13, 44). Wang and colleagues first explored SW topology in the whole-brain functional network in ADHD and found SW topology in TDC and children with ADHD, but children with ADHD had a tendency towards more regular networks. Consistent with previous studies (21, 45), we found that the FPAN had economical SW properties, which suggests that SW brain networks are robust in the face of disease (21). This supports the view that brain networks may have developed to maximize the efficiency of information processing. However, we did not find significant alterations in FPAN in children with ADHD. It may be that children with ADHD had no deficit in FPAN topology, or that any difference was too small to be captured by clustering coefficient and path length. Previous studies have used one of two approaches for normalizing clustering coefficient and path length. However, thresholding the functional connectivity matrix cannot provide a unique or consistent solution (27, 46). We used the second approach to normalize, whereby the observed network parameters were divided by the randomized networks parameters, but this approach may include too many low noise and may not be sensitive to developmental disease. MST is an unbiased approach that overcomes the normalization problem, can provide a unique and consistent solution, and can discard the low signal.

In the present study, MST analysis showed that diameter, leaf number, tree hierarchy, and kappa were altered in ADHD. Children with ADHD had greater diameter and lower leaf number, tree hierarchy, and kappa, and these variables significantly correlated with ADHD symptom score, indicating their clinical relevance. Decreased leaf number and increased diameter indicate a decreased global efficiency (47), suggesting that the FPAN had lower global efficiency in children with ADHD than in TDC. This is consistent with whole-brain deficits (21). Together with the significant negative correlation between ADHD symptom score and leaf number, and positive correlation between ADHD symptom score and diameter, this indicates the tendency to deviate from a more centralized organization (star-like topology) towards a more decentralized organization (line-like topology) in ADHD. The negative correlation between ADHD symptom score and tree hierarchy suggests that there is a sub-optimal balance between hub overload and functional integration in the network. Tree hierarchy can range from 0 to 1, and an optimal tree configuration is thought to correspond to a hierarchy value of around 0.5 (a compromise between a line-like and star-like topology). A star-like topology corresponds to hub overload, and a line-like topology corresponds to weak integration (32). The lower tree hierarchy in children with ADHD represents a more line-like topology, which is indicative of weak integration. This is consistent with the finding of a study based on whole-brain analysis that reported a decreased clustering coefficient, which corresponds to a local integration, in children with ADHD (48), in which they found a decreased clustering coefficient (corresponds to a local integration). Using MST analysis, we confirmed the decreased integration in children with ADHD. In addition, kappa, a measure that captures the broadness of the degree distribution, was lower in children with ADHD than in TDC and was negatively associated with ADHD symptom score. The lower kappa in children with ADHD reflects a reduced ease of synchronization, that is, decreased spread of information across the tree (41). As hypothesized, these findings indicate that FPAN topology is different in children with ADHD and TDC, and tends towards greater randomness and lower global efficiency and local integration in children with ADHD. The correlation between MST parameters and ADHD symptom score suggests that the abnormal MST topology may be useful in monitoring progression of the disease.

A low kappa value corresponds to a low number of highly connected nodes or “hubs.” The number of hubs is associated with the resilience of the network against attack. To further explore the damaged “hub” regions in the FPAN in children with ADHD, we further computed the eccentricity of each region. The eccentricity of a node is measured by the longest distance between that node and any other node. The closer a node is to the center of the tree, the lower its eccentricity. Low eccentricity indicates high global efficiency and centrality. We found greater eccentricity, reflecting lower global efficiency and centrality, in the left IPS, bilateral vIPS, and bilateral vOC. In general, these brain regions were concerned in ADHD studies. Previous studies found that the centrality in the IPS and vIPS regions of the FPAN was associated with alertness and the efficiency of the executive control system (8, 49). Consistent with the results of these studies, the decreased centrality in the IPS and vIPS in children with ADHD may be related to altered alerting and executive function in attention processing. These findings are in accordance with those of several structural and functional imaging studies that have found cortical atrophy and reduced activity in these regions in participants with ADHD (49). In addition, vOC also shows lower eccentricity in ADHD participants, which were compatible with previous studies that found decreased nodal efficiency and reduced volume in this region (21, 50).

In summary, this is the first study to reveal the topological properties of the FPAN in children with ADHD using resting-state functional MRI. We performed MST analysis of brain networks. This addresses the threshold and normalization problems encountered with conventional approaches, and was sensitive to changes in brain topography in children with ADHD. Clustering coefficient and path length were not successful in identifying deficits in the FPAN, whereas the MST parameters of leaf number, diameter, tree hierarchy, and kappa captured the tendency of ADHD brains to deviate from a more centralized organization (star-like topology) towards a more decentralized organization (line-like topology). This corresponds to a decreased global efficiency and weak integration. There were also differences in nodal eccentricity of the IPS, vIPS, and vOC in children with ADHD, reflecting a decreased efficiency and decentralized topology that was associated with deficits in alertness and executive function in attention processing. In addition, MST parameters were associated with clinical features of ADHD. These findings enhance our understanding of the underlying pathophysiology of ADHD and may facilitate evaluation and monitoring of clinical status in ADHD.

Despite the advantages of this study, some limitations should be noted. First, when using MST, we only used the “core” connections. This means that some information may have been lost. For example, clustering coefficient is a measure that cannot be examined in MST. To address this, we derived this information from SW topology. Previous studies found that, among children with ADHD, there are gender differences in brain structure (51); however, girls comprised only 35% of our participants. In addition, participants in the ADHD and TDC groups were not matched for gender or IQ. To address this limitation, we treated these variables as covariates; however, future studies should further explore differences in brain networks using participants matched for IQ and gender.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the IRBs of NYU and the NYU School of Medicine with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the IRBs of NYU and the NYU School of Medicine.

Author Contributions

YW analyzed the data and wrote the draft of the paper. CZ and FT amended and proofread the draft of the paper. CZ, FT, DW, MH, and MK participated in the discussion and offered some good ideas. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31662083).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the contribution of ADHD-200 consortium organizers for sharing the raw data.

References

1. Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron (2011) 72(4):665–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol (2011) 106(3):1125–65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2005) 360(1457):1001–13. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, et al. Consistent resting-state etworks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2006) 103(37):13848–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Thomason ME, Dassanayake MT, Shen S, Katkuri Y, Alexis M, Anderson AL, et al. Cross-hemispheric functional connectivity in the human fetal brain. Sci Transl Med (2013) 5(173):173ra124. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3004978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. van den Heuvel MP, Kersbergen KJ, de Reus MA, Keunen K, Kahn RS, Groenendaal F, et al. The neonatal connectome during preterm brain development. Cereb Cortex (2015) 25(9):3000–13. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102(27):9673–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Markett S, Reuter M, Montag C, Voigt G, Lachmann B, Rudorf S, et al. Assessing the function of the fronto-parietal attention network: insights from resting-state fMRI and the attentional network test. Hum Brain Mapp (2014) 35(4):1700–9. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T. Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex (2008) 18(11):2553–9. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Silk TJ, Vance A, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Cunnington R. Dysfunction in the fronto-parietal network in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav (2008) 2(2):123–31. doi: 10.1007/s11682-008-9021-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10(3):186–98. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’networks. Nature (1998) 393(6684):440. doi: 10.1038/30918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bassett DS, Bullmore ET. Small-world brain networks. Neuroscientist (2006) 12(6):512–23. doi: 10.1177/1073858406293182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stam CJ, van Straaten EC. The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin Neurophysiol (2012) 123(6):1067–87. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Douw L, van Dellen E, de Groot M, Heimans JJ, Klein M, Stam CJ, et al. Epilepsy is related to theta band brain connectivity and network topology in brain tumor patients. BMC Neurosci (2010) 11(1):103. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ponten SC, Bartolomei F, Stam CJ. Small-world networks and epilepsy: graph theoretical analysis of intracerebrally recorded mesial temporal lobe seizures. Clin Neurophysiol (2007) 118(4):918–27. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Rubinov M, Knock SA, Stam CJ, Micheloyannis S, Harris AWF, Williams LM, et al. Small-world properties of nonlinear brain activity in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30(2):403–16. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Faraone SV, Sergeant J, Gillberg C, Biederman J. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is it an American condition? World Psychiatry (2003) 2(2):104.

Google Scholar

19. Sanz-Arigita EJ, Schoonheim MM, Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Maris E, Barkhof F, et al. Loss of ‘small-world’ networks in Alzheimer’s disease: graph analysis of FMRI resting-state functional connectivity. PLoS One (2010) 5(11):e13788. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Stam CJ, de Haan W, Daffertshofer A, Jones BF, Manshanden I, van Cappellen van Walsum AM, et al. Graph theoretical analysis of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain (2009) 132(Pt 1):213–24. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wang L, Zhu C, He Y, Zang Y, Cao Q, Zhang H, et al. Altered small-world brain functional networks in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30(2):638–49. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2007) 104(49):19649–54. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Boersma M, Smit DJ, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJ, Delemarre-van de Waal HA, Stam CJ. Growing trees in child brains: graph theoretical analysis of electroencephalography-derived minimum spanning tree in 5- and 7-year-old children reflects brain maturation. Brain Connect (2013) 3(1):50–60. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Boersma M, Smit DJ, de Bie HM, Van Baal GC, Boomsma DI, de Geus EJ, et al. Network analysis of resting state EEG in the developing young brain: structure comes with maturation. Hum Brain Mapp (2011) 32(3):413–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smit DJ, de Geus EJ, Boersma M, Boomsma DI, Stam CJ. Life-span development of brain network integration assessed with phase lag index connectivity and minimum spanning tree graphs. Brain Connect (2016) 6(4):312–25. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Smit DJ, Boersma M, Schnack HG, Micheloyannis S, Boomsma DI, Hulshoff Pol HE, et al. The brain matures with stronger functional connectivity and decreased randomness of its network. PLoS One (2012) 7(5):e36896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Langer N, Pedroni A, Jancke L. The problem of thresholding in small-world network analysis. PLoS One (2013) 8(1):e53199. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Olde Dubbelink KT, Hillebrand A, Stoffers D, Deijen JB, Twisk JW, Stam CJ, et al. Disrupted brain network topology in Parkinson’s disease: a longitudinal magnetoencephalography study. Brain (2013) 137(1):197–207. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Çiftçi K. Minimum spanning tree reflects the alterations of the default mode network during Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Biomed Eng (2011) 39(5):1493–504. doi: 10.1007/s10439-011-0258-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ortega GJ, Sola RG, Pastor J. Complex network analysis of human ECoG data. Neurosci Lett (2008) 447(2–3):129–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.09.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stam CJ. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci (2014) 15(10):683–95. doi: 10.1038/nrn3801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bertolero MA, Yeo BT, D’Esposito M. The modular and integrative functional architecture of the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2015) 112(49):E6798–807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510619112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fair DA, Posner J, Nagel BJ, Bathula D, Dias TG, Mills KL, et al. Atypical default network connectivity in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 68(12):1084–91. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cocchi L, Bramati IE, Zalesky A, Furukawa E, Fontenelle LF, Moll J, et al. Altered functional brain connectivity in a non-clinical sample of young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Neurosci (2012) 32(49):17753–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3272-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yan CG, Wang XD, Zuo XN, Zang YF. DPABI: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics (2016) 14(3):339–51. doi: 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jinhui W, Xindi W, Mingrui X, Xuhong L, Alan E, Yong H. Corrigendum: GRETNA: a graph theoretical network analysis toolbox for imaging connectomics. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 9(386):386. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rubinov M, Kötter R, Hagmann P, Sporns O. Brain connectivity toolbox: a collection of complex network measurements and brain connectivity datasets. Neuroimage (2009) 47:S169. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)71822-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Humphries MD, Gurney K, Prescott TJ. The brainstem reticular formation is a small-world, not scale-free, network. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2006) 273(1585):503–11. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Maslov S, Sneppen K. Specificity and stability in topology of protein networks. Science (2002) 296(5569):910–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1065103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kruskal JB. On the shortest spanning subtree of a graph and the traveling salesman problem. Proc Am Math Soc (1956) 7(1):48–50. doi: 10.2307/2033241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Tewarie P, van Dellen E, Hillebrand A, Stam CJ. The minimum spanning tree: an unbiased method for brain network analysis. Neuroimage (2015) 104:177–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Boersma M, Kemner C, de Reus MA, Collin G, Snijders TM, Hofman D, et al. Disrupted functional brain networks in autistic toddlers. Brain Connect (2013) 3(1):41–9. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage (2002) 15(4):870–8. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum Brain Mapp (2007) 28(11):1178–93. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Achard S, Bullmore E. Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput Biol (2007) 3(2):e17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. van Wijk BC, Stam CJ, Daffertshofer A. Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One (2010) 5(10):e13701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Utianski RL, Caviness JN, van Straaten EC, Beach TG, Dugger BN, Shill HA, et al. Graph theory network function in Parkinson’s disease assessed with electroencephalography. Clin Neurophysiol (2016) 127(5):2228–36. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ghaderi AH, Nazari MA, Shahrokhi H, Darooneh AH. Functional brain connectivity differences between different ADHD presentations: impaired functional segregation in ADHD-combined presentation but not in ADHD-inattentive presentation. Basic Clin Neurosci (2017) 8(4):267. doi: 10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.4.267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Makris N, Biederman J, Valera EM, Bush G, Kaiser J, Kennedy DN, et al. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cereb Cortex (2007) 17(6):1364–75. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Durston S, Pol HEH, Schnack HG, Buitelaar JK, Steenhuis MP, Minderaa RB, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2004) 43(3):332–40. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200403000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wang Y, Xu Q, Li S, Li G, Zuo C, Liao S, et al. Gender differences in anomalous subcortical morphology for children with ADHD. Neurosci Lett (2018) 665:176–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, frontal–parietal attention network, small world, minimum spanning tree, resting connectivity

Citation: Wang Y, Tao F, Zuo C, Kanji M, Hu M and Wang D (2019) Disrupted Resting Frontal–Parietal Attention Network Topology Is Associated With a Clinical Measure in Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 10:300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00300

Received: 21 August 2018; Accepted: 16 April 2019;
Published: 10 May 2019.

Edited by:

Yuhui Du, Mind Research Network (MRN), United States

Reviewed by:

Rui Li, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Jiayu Chen, Mind Research Network (MRN), United States

Copyright © 2019 Wang, Tao, Zuo, Kanji, Hu and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fuxiang Tao, taofuxiang7@126.com
Daoyang Wang, daoyangwang@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.