Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 May 2021
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Empathy and Perspective Taking: Understanding the Mechanisms of Normal and Abnormal Experiences and Abilities View all 10 articles

Cognitive Empathy and Longitudinal Changes in Temporo-Parietal Junction Thickness in Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, The Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, OH, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States
  • 6School of Social Work, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

Objective: Deficits in cognitive empathy are well-documented in individuals with schizophrenia and are related to reduced community functioning. The temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is closely linked to cognitive empathy. We compared the relationship between baseline cognitive empathy and changes in TPJ thickness over 24 months between individuals with schizophrenia and healthy controls.

Methods: Individuals with schizophrenia (n = 29) and healthy controls (n = 26) completed a cognitive empathy task and underwent structural neuroimaging at baseline and approximately 24 months later. Symmetrized percent change scores were calculated for right and left TPJ, as well as whole-brain volume, and compared between groups. Task accuracy was examined as a predictor of percent change in TPJ thickness and whole-brain volume in each group.

Results: Individuals with schizophrenia demonstrated poorer accuracy on the cognitive empathy task (p < 0.001) and thinner TPJ cortex relative to controls at both time points (p = 0.01). In schizophrenia, greater task accuracy was uniquely related to less thinning of the TPJ over time (p = 0.02); task accuracy did not explain changes in left TPJ or whole-brain volume. Among controls, task accuracy did not explain changes in right or left TPJ, or whole-brain volume.

Conclusions: Our findings suggest that greater cognitive empathy may explain sustained integrity of the right TPJ in individuals with schizophrenia, suggesting a contributory substrate for the long-term maintenance of this process in psychosis. Cognitive empathy was not related to changes in whole-brain volume, demonstrating the unique role of the TPJ in cognitive empathy.

Introduction

Social cognition broadly refers to a psychological construct that describes how one thinks about themselves in relation to others and the processes involved in social interactions (1). One component of social cognition is empathy, which is the ability to be sensitive to and understand the mental state of others (2). More specifically, this component can be separated into two subsystems reflecting basic emotion-contagion and perspective-taking (3). The latter subsystem, termed cognitive empathy, requires the ability to infer or understand the perspective and emotions of others, and utilizes higher-order cognitive processes such as cognitive flexibility (4, 5). For example, if a friend has experienced a loss, one may be able understand their thoughts or emotions by imagining oneself in this situation, despite never having experienced loss personally.

Individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate difficulties in several aspects of social cognition, including misperception of social cues, poor mentalizing or perspective-taking ability, and less accurate emotion monitoring (6, 7). Subsequent to weaknesses in social cognitive abilities, many individuals with schizophrenia experience strained social relationships, which can contribute to impairments in day-to-day functioning (8). A meta-analysis by Fett et al. (9) determined that social cognition may be more highly related to aspects of functioning than general cognition. Further, deficits in cognitive empathy have been associated with lower functional capacity, social competence, and social attainment in schizophrenia after accounting for general cognition and psychopathology (1013). Additionally, interventions targeted toward improving aspects of social cognition may lead to improvements in social functioning (14). For example, an intervention designed to address multiple components of social cognition in individuals with psychosis led to significant improvement in social cognitive abilities (15, 16). While current research remains limited, there is some initial evidence that social skills training may improve real-world functioning (7). Given the link between cognitive empathy and functional abilities in schizophrenia, understanding the mechanisms underlying this fundamental ability is of great importance.

The brain networks underlying social cognitive processes are complex and may work in tandem during social interactions. Specifically, Van Overwalle and Baetens (17) describe a mirror system, which is a lower-level system that allows one to sense and recognize the goal of another's action and match it to a representation of our own, while the higher-order mentalizing system helps one to infer the thoughts and feelings of others and understand another's beliefs as separate from our own through use of “social intelligence.” According to the authors, the former system consists of the anterior intraparietal sulcus, premotor cortex, and superior temporal sulcus, while the latter includes the precuneus, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and temporo-parietal junction [TPJ; (17)]. Moreover, several studies indicate the TPJ is uniquely related to the higher order mentalizing system among healthy controls (1820). In contrast, individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate aberrant neural activation patterns in the TPJ while performing a cognitive empathy task (21) and other mentalizing tasks (6), with the most common finding being reduced activation, suggesting greater neural resources are needed for intact mentalizing. A review by Eddy (22) underscored the importance of the TPJ in the interface between processing sensory experiences and understanding one's internal mental or motivational state, and that this system may be compromised in neuropsychiatric populations, including schizophrenia.

In addition to abnormal functional activity, individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate reduced cortical thickness in regions associated with cognitive empathy, including the TPJ (2325). Notably, a prior study observed greater thickness of the TPJ was associated with stronger cognitive empathy in healthy controls, though this relationship was not observed in individuals with schizophrenia (26). However, few studies have examined longitudinal changes in neuroanatomical regions associated with social cognition, and how this may be related to cognitive empathy. This is of particular interest given the increased rates of cortical thinning in schizophrenia over time, and its relationship with functional and symptomatic outcomes (27). Interestingly, neuroanatomical substrates may be plastic over time, such that experience and behavior may alter neural circuits across psychiatric disorders (28). Thus, the present study examined whether accuracy on a cognitive empathy task explained significant variation in changes in TPJ thickness over 24 months in individuals with schizophrenia and healthy controls. We predicted that in individuals with schizophrenia, there would be greater thinning of the TPJ over time and that stronger baseline cognitive empathy abilities would be associated with less thinning of the TPJ in this group.

Methods

Participants

Individuals with schizophrenia (n = 29) and healthy controls (n = 26) were recruited as part of a larger observational study investigating social cognition in schizophrenia (12, 26); there were no interventions as part of the study. All participants were between the ages of 18–45. Participants were excluded if they: (1) met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-4th Edition [DSM-IV; (29)] criteria for substance abuse or dependence within the past 6 months; (2) had a severe medical condition; or (3) had sustained a significant head injury. Controls were additionally excluded if they had a lifetime history of a DSM-IV Axis I disorder or a first-degree relative with a psychosis spectrum disorder. The Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University approved all study procedures. All research participants provided written informed consent prior to study enrollment.

Study Measures

Demographic and Clinical Measures

Demographic and clinical characteristics were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; 26), which was administered by Master- and PhD-level research staff. Diagnosis was validated from a consensus between the SCID and a semi-structured interview with a study psychiatrist. Antipsychotic medication dosages were converted into chlorpromazine equivalents using a standardized method (30). Psychopathology was assessed in schizophrenia participants using the global ratings from the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (31) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (32). Participant demographics, as well as clinical characteristics of the schizophrenia group, are listed in Table 1. There were no group differences in age, sex, or race (all p-values > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study sample.

Cognitive Empathy Task

Cognitive empathy was assessed using the Emotional Perspective-Taking (EPT) task that was adapted into English (12) from the original version developed in Germany (33). In this task, participants were presented with scenes depicting social interactions between two Caucasian individuals (male or female), with one of the faces covered by a mask (Figure 1). After the scene was displayed for 4 s, two faces displaying different emotions were presented and participants were asked to choose which face best characterized the masked individual. Emotions included fear, anger, sadness, disgust, happiness, or neutrality. Participants responded using a response box, with left or right responses corresponding to the face on the left or right side of the screen. The task consisted of 60 trials, with 10 stimuli per emotion condition. The location of the responses was balanced across trials. Accuracy rates were calculated by dividing the total number of correct trials by the total number of trials completed.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schema of emotion perspective-taking task. Participants are asked to choose which emotional expression (shown on the right) fits with the masked face (shown on the left).

MRI Acquisition and Image Processing

MR scanning was performed at both baseline and follow-up visits (~24 months post-baseline) on a 3T TIM Trio system (Siemens Medical Systems) at Northwestern University's Center for Translational Imaging. Anatomical MRI was collected using a high-resolution 3D T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence optimized for gray-white contrast (echo time (TE) = 3.16 ms, repetition time (TR) = 2,400 ms, 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels, time = 8.09 min).

All MR images were processed using the FreeSurfer (FS) toolkit release 5.3.0 (34) and manually edited according to established guidelines (35). Embedded FS longitudinal algorithms that reduce intra-subject morphological variability inherent in image processing were used for reconstruction of the cortical surface (36). Based on prior literature support (see above), the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) was identified a priori as a key region involved in the regulation of cognitive empathy. Definition of this area was based on the supramarginal gyrus ROI derived from a default FS parcellation scheme (37) that was mapped across subjects using a non-linear surface registration procedure (38). Cortical thickness values (in mm) of the ROI from each subject in left and right hemispheres at both time points were calculated using embedded FS algorithms. Additionally, whole-brain volume was calculated in mm3 as the total volume of all brain voxels located above the cerebellar tentorium for each subject (i.e., “SupraTentorial” value computed by FS morphometry statistics). Whole-brain volume was calculated in lieu of global cortical thickness due to the regional differences that may affect global cortical thickness.

Data Analysis

Differences in age, years of education, and parental socioeconomic status [SES; (39)] were assessed using independent samples t-tests, and differences in race and gender were assessed using chi-squared tests. In order to examine differences in performance on the EPT task between groups, independent samples t-tests were used with accuracy percentage and reaction times as dependent variables. Prior to imaging analysis, a ~99% (3 SD) winsorization of cortical thickness values was conducted per group in order to reduce the influence of spurious outliers (40). Using this method, there were no outliers. We then assessed differences in TPJ thickness over time (i.e., baseline to 24 months) by conducting a Repeated-Measures Analysis of Variance (RM-ANOVA), using group as a between-subject factor, with time and hemisphere (left or right) as within-subject factors. Lastly, we conducted a linear regression model using cognitive empathy task accuracy as variable to explain percent change in TPJ thickness separately per group. For this model, the independent variable (TPJ thickness percent change) was calculated as a symmetrized percent change (spc) score that estimates rate of change with respect to average thickness across timepoints (41) using the following formula: [(thickness at time 2 – thickness at time 1)/(months between scans)]/0.5 * (thickness at time 2 + thickness at time 1). In order to determine if changed in TPJ thickness was uniquely related to cognitive empathy task accuracy, we also calculated a spc score for whole-brain volume using the same procedure and conducted a linear regression using cognitive empathy as a predictor for changes in whole-brain volume.

Results

Group Differences on Cognitive Empathy Task

Controls and individuals with schizophrenia differed in accuracy on the emotional perspective-taking task [t(53) = 4.75, p < 0.001], such that controls had a higher accuracy (mean = 0.85; SD = 0.07) than schizophrenia participants (mean = 0.73; SD = 0.11). Additionally, controls and schizophrenia participants differed in reaction time [t(53) = 2.33, p = 0.02], such that controls were faster in responding (mean = 1,415 ms; SD = 300 ms) than schizophrenia participants (mean = 1,643 ms; SD = 411 ms).

Group Differences in TPJ Thickness Over Time

A RM-ANOVA examining changes in TPJ thickness over time revealed an overall main effect of group on thickness, such that TPJ thickness in controls was greater than schizophrenia participants [F(1, 53) = 7.00, p = 0.01; Table 2]. There were no significant main effects for time (p = 0.45) or hemisphere (p = 0.06), and no significant interactions for group by time, group by hemisphere, or group by-time-by-hemisphere interaction effects (all p > 0.5).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means (standard deviations) of TPJ thickness (mm) in controls and schizophrenia participants at baseline and follow-up.

Baseline Cognitive Empathy Task Performance as a Predictor for Changes in TPJ Thickness

Among schizophrenia participants, task performance predicted right TPJ spc scores, such that lower accuracy predicted lower spc scores [i.e., greater TPJ thinning over time; F(1, 28) = 6.9, p = 0.01; β = 0.45] (Figure 2); cognitive empathy accuracy did not significantly predict left spc scores [F(1,28) = 0.57, p = 0.46; β = −0.14]. Among controls, accuracy did not significantly predict right [F(1, 25) = 0.46, p = 0.50; β = 0.14] or left spc scores [F(1, 25) = 0.68, p = 0.42; β = −0.17]. Lastly, cognitive empathy accuracy did not predict whole brain volume change in either schizophrenia participants [F(1, 25) = 0.66, p = 0.43; β = 0.15] or controls [F(1, 25)= 0.30, p = 0.59; β = 0.11].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Emotion perspective-taking performance predicts symmetrized percent change score in right TPJ in schizophrenia participants only.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between baseline cognitive empathy performance and longitudinal changes in a key brain region underlying cognitive empathy, the temporoparietal junction, in individuals with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Consistent with prior studies (12, 26), our findings suggest that individuals with schizophrenia had a lower accuracy on the cognitive empathy task than controls. Also, we observed that the TPJ was thinner at both time points in individuals with schizophrenia compared to controls, though neither group experienced a significant change in thickness over the 24-month interval. Among the individuals with schizophrenia, but not the controls, we observed that greater accuracy on the cognitive empathy task was related to less thinning of the right TPJ over time, and this relationship was not observed in the left TPJ nor with whole-brain volume.

Abnormal thinning of the cortex is a characteristic feature of schizophrenia (42), with implications for disrupted cognitive processes (43). Individuals with chronic schizophrenia may show particularly increased cortical thinning in prefrontal and temporal cortices (25), and greater rates of thinning may be related to negative symptoms in schizophrenia (44). Several studies have demonstrated specific abnormalities in the TPJ in schizophrenia (42, 45), while some ex vivo work in a smaller sample noted limited changes (46). In the present study, although there were significant differences in TPJ cortical thickness between individuals with schizophrenia and controls, there were no significant longitudinal changes. While sample size may have contributed to this lack of finding, it is not entirely surprising given longitudinal studies of cortical thickness in schizophrenia are mixed on progressive deterioration of the TPJ, with some demonstrating loss (47) and others not (27). Given the significant biological heterogeneity that exists in schizophrenia (48), it is likely our sample consisted primarily of individuals who demonstrated subtle or variable degrees of change in TPJ thickness over the 2-year period of our study.

The main finding that greater cognitive empathy accuracy at baseline predicts reduced cortical thinning over time in individuals with schizophrenia suggests a potential resilience mechanism as it pertains to the progressive features of the illness. Interestingly, this relationship did not appear at a global level, indicating this is not a general feature, but rather one that is specific to the right TPJ. Furthermore, a large meta-analysis concluded that the right TPJ is a central brain region that infers the goals of others, and that this region is not just engaged by the orientation to people or actions, but rather related to higher-order social cognitive process (17). This function differs from other neural regions involved in cognitive empathy such as the mPFC, which appears to be a substrate for the attribution of more stable traits about one's self or others (49). Additionally, functional neuroimaging studies in healthy controls have demonstrated distinct neural networks that implicate the ventral TPJ for cognitive based empathy network [i.e., theory of mind; (50)], further demonstrating the unique role of the TPJ in cognitive empathy. More broadly, there may be disruptions in connectivity between the right TPJ and other neural regions that support both social cognitive and general cognitive functions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, and insula in individuals with schizophrenia (51). Further, the right hemisphere is necessary for social communication, including understanding tone of voice or processing alternate meanings of statements; prior studies have demonstrated that individuals with schizophrenia have difficulty on assessments of these right hemisphere functions, therefore making it difficult to understand the intent of others (52). Importantly, findings of right hemisphere and rTPJ dysfunction during social cognitive tasks may be observed in siblings of individuals with schizophrenia, raising questions about the heritability of rTPJ functioning and social cognition (53, 54). These studies, in parallel with our findings, demonstrate not only the unique role of the right TPJ in cognitive empathy, but also suggest that compromised functioning of this region may lead to disrupted functioning of other networks that underlie social and general cognitive abilities.

Several reasons may account for stronger baseline cognitive empathy abilities predicting reduced cortical thinning in the right TPJ among individuals with schizophrenia. First, one hypothesis is that individuals with schizophrenia who regularly engage cognitive empathy skills may invoke biological mechanisms that protect against greater rates of cortical thinning. It has been hypothesized that increased cognitive stimulation may be related to brain reserve via plasticity, or the ability of the brain to change and adapt both structurally and functionally in response to different experiences (55). A prior study demonstrated a potential association between functional connectivity and structural integrity of the rTPJ, suggesting a relationship between structure and function of the TPJ during theory of mind processes (56). While the relationship between greater utilization of empathy skills and brain reserve has not been investigated at an experimental level, this study may have clinical implications for remediating reduced cognitive empathy abilities experienced by individuals with schizophrenia (57) and preservation of brain structure. Alternatively, our findings can be interpreted as reduced accuracy predicting greater rates of thinning in individuals in schizophrenia. This explanation somewhat parallels previous findings of the relationship between greater rates of thinning or volume loss and worse cognitive or symptom outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (27, 5860). However, this interpretation has been debated (61). Thus, it is also possible that thinning of the TPJ is already occurring in individuals with schizophrenia, as evidenced by overall group differences in cortical thickness, and that our behavioral marker of cognitive empathy is highlighting individuals with less distinct degradation. Further, preexisting TPJ thinning due to neurodevelopmental processes may make one more vulnerable to further thinning due to less use of this structure during social interactions. An alternative explanation is that our sample may include a distinct subgroup of individuals with schizophrenia with intact cortical thickness who also demonstrate preserved cognitive empathy abilities, while another subgroup of participants may demonstrate greater rate of cortical thinning. Although we were not powered to examine clusters of participants with varying levels of cortical thickness, leveraging a larger sample in future research may help clarify this finding. Nonetheless, our main finding that preserved baseline cognitive empathy predicted less thinning is in line with other studies in healthy individuals that demonstrate greater cortical integrity is related to empathy (26, 62, 63). Additionally, other studies have demonstrated that greater TPJ thinning in individuals with first-episode psychosis with persistent negative symptoms [often associated with reduced empathic abilities; (64, 65)] compared to controls (23). The lack of a relationship between right TPJ thinning and cognitive empathy performance in our healthy participants is not entirely unexpected given deriving relationships between brain structure and behavior in control groups requires larger samples to detect effects in variables with minimal variance (i.e., ceiling effects on cognitive empathy tasks and minor changes in cortical thickness in healthy individuals).

There are several limitations to this study that should be considered when interpreting these findings. First, cognitive empathy performance was measured at baseline, though not at the 24-month follow-up visit. Longitudinal assessment of cognitive empathy performance may be useful for determining if changes in TPJ thickness are related to changes in cognitive empathy, which would further establish the role of the TPJ in the cognitive empathy network and also raise implications for this region as an area of potential intervention (66). Second, changes in cortical thickness were measured over 2 years, which may not have been a sufficient enough time period to appreciate a greater degree of cortical thinning. Lastly, due to a small sample size and the requirement of imaging acquisition across 24 months, our findings may have limited generalizability. Beyond the identified limitations, our study had some notable strengths. First, to our knowledge, this is one of few studies that have measured brain structure longitudinally as it relates to cognitive empathy, in individuals with schizophrenia. Second, this study included a racially diverse sample in both the control and schizophrenia groups that may help enhance the generalizability of the findings. Lastly, the study used a well-validated measure of cognitive empathy (12, 33, 67).

Overall, our findings suggest that greater cognitive empathy may explain sustained integrity of the right TPJ in individuals with schizophrenia, suggesting a contributory substrate for the long-term maintenance of this process in psychosis. This study has implications for the progressive nature of brain structure changes in schizophrenia and how it may relate to behavior, namely cognitive empathy, and well as raises questions about a potential resilience mechanism. These findings also provide additional evidence for the unique role of the TPJ in cognitive empathy in schizophrenia.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found here: http://schizconnect.org; NMorphCH.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Northwestern University Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TK-R helped conceptualize the study, completed statistical analyses, and wrote the first draft. DC helped conceptualize the study, contributed to statistical analyses, and assisted with manuscript writing and editing. JP helped with study methods and contributed to manuscript editing. LW, VM, and JC contributed to the conceptualization of the study and assisted with manuscript editing. MS served as the principal investigator on this project, contributed to study conceptualization, oversaw all statistical analysis, and assisted with writing the first draft of the manuscript and subsequent manuscript editing. All authors approved the final manuscript and have made significant scientific contributions to this manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to JC (R01 MH056584), and an Institutional National Research Service Award to DC (T32 NS047987).

Conflict of Interest

JC was compensated for service on a Data Safety Monitoring Board for Sunovion Pharmaceuticals.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Penn DL, Corrigan PW, Bentall RP, Racenstein J, Newman L. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychol Bull. (1997) 121:114. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Smith A. Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. Psychol Rec. (2006) 56:3–21. doi: 10.1007/BF03395534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D. Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain. (2009) 132:617–27. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. (2004) 3:71–100. doi: 10.1177/1534582304267187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Walter H. Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: concepts, circuits, and genes. Emotion Rev. (2012) 4:9–17. doi: 10.1177/1754073911421379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:620–31. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Horan WP, Green MF. Treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: current status and future directions. Schizophr Res. (2019) 203:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pinkham AE. Social cognition in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. (2014) 75:14–9. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13065su1.04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fett AKJ, Viechtbauer W, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:573–588.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

10. Michaels TM, Horan WP, Ginger EJ, Martinovich Z, Pinkham AE, Smith MJ. Cognitive empathy contributes to poor social functioning in schizophrenia: evidence from a new self-report measure of cognitive and affective empathy. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 220:803–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.08.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Smith MJ, Horan WP, Karpouzian TM, Abram SV, Cobia DJ, Csernansky JG. Self-reported empathy deficits are uniquely associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2012) 137:196–202. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Smith MJ, Horan WP, Cobia DJ, Karpouzian TM, Fox JM, Reilly JL, et al. Performance-based empathy mediates the influence of working memory on social competence in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:824–34. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Karpouzian TM, Alden EC, Reilly JL, Smith MJ. High functioning individuals with schizophrenia have preserved social perception but not mentalizing abilities. Schizophr Res. (2016) 171:137–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nahum M, Lee H, Fisher M, Green MF, Hooker CI, Ventura J, et al. Online social cognition training in schizophrenia: a double-blind, randomized, controlled multi-site clinical trial. Schizophr Bull. (2020) 47:108–17. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Horan WP, Kern RS, Shokat-Fadai K, Sergi MJ, Wynn JK, Green MF. Social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia: an initial efficacy study of stabilized outpatients. Schizophr Res. (2009) 107:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Horan WP, Kern RS, Tripp C, Hellemann G, Wynn JK, Bell M, et al. Efficacy and specificity of social cognitive skills training for outpatients with psychotic disorders. J Psychiatr Res. (2011) 45:1113–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Van Overwalle F, Baetens K. Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage. (2009) 48:564–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mai X, Zhang W, Hu X, Zhen Z, Xu Z, Zhang J, et al. Using tDCS to explore the role of the right temporo-parietal junction in theory of mind and cognitive empathy. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:380. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Saxe R, Wexler A. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia. (2005) 43:1391–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Saxe R, Powell LJ. It's the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Sci. (2006) 17:692–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Smith MJ, Schroeder MP, Abram SV, Goldman MB, Parrish TB, Wang X, et al. Alterations in brain activation during cognitive empathy are related to social functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 41:211–22. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Eddy CM. The junction between self and other? Temporo-parietal dysfunction in neuropsychiatry. Neuropsychologia. (2016) 89:465–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bodnar M, Hovington CL, Buchy L, Malla AK, Joober R, Lepage M. Cortical thinning in temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) in non-affective first-episode of psychosis patients with persistent negative symptoms. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e101372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Nesvåg R, Lawyer G, Varnäs K, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Frigessi A, et al. Regional thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia: effects of diagnosis, age and antipsychotic medication. Schizophr Res. (2008) 98:16–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kuperberg GR, Broome MR, McGuire PK, David AS, Eddy M, Ozawa F, et al. Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2003) 60:878–88. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Massey SH, Stern D, Alden EC, Petersen JE, Cobia DJ, Wang L, et al. Cortical thickness of neural substrates supporting cognitive empathy in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2017) 179:119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. van Haren NEM, Schnack HG, Cahn W, Heuvel MP, van den Lepage C, et al. Changes in cortical thickness during the course of illness in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2011) 68:871–880. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kolb B, Gibb R, Robinson TE. Brain plasticity and behavior. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2003) 12:1–5. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, patient edition (SCID-I/P). New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute (2002).

30. Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Miller D, Ho BC. Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 67:255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa (1984).

32. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa (1984).

Google Scholar

33. Derntl B, Finkelmeyer A, Toygar TK, Hülsmann A, Schneider F, Falkenberg DI, et al. Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2009) 108:197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. (1999) 9:179–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ségonne F, Pacheco J, Fischl B. Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE T Med Imaging. (2007) 26:518–529.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. Reuter M, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage. (2010) 53:1181–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on mRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. (2006) 31:968–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. (1999) 8:272–84. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-019319998:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Barratt W. The Barratt Simplified Measure Of Social Status (BSMSS): Measuring SES. Terre Haute, IN: Indiana State University (2005).

40. Tukey JW. The future of data analysis. Ann Mathematical Stat. (1962) 33:1–67. doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177704711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. (2012) 61:1402–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. van Erp TGM, Walton E, Hibar DP, Schmaal L, Jiang W, Glahn DC, et al. Cortical brain abnormalities in 4474 individuals with schizophrenia and 5098 control subjects via the enhancing neuro imaging genetics through meta analysis (ENIGMA) consortium. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:6446654. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ehrlich S, Brauns S, Yendiki A, Ho B.-C., Calhoun V, et al. Associations of cortical thickness and cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:105011062. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Nenadic I, Yotter RA, Sauer H, Gaser C. Patterns of cortical thinning in different subgroups of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (2015) 206:479–83. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.148510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Narr KL, Bilder RM, Toga AW, Woods RP, Rex DE, Szeszko PR, et al. Mapping cortical thickness and gray matter concentration in first episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. (2005) 15:7087719. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Smiley JF, Konnova K, Bleiwas C. Cortical thickness, neuron density and size in the inferior parietal lobe in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2012) 136:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nesvåg R, Bergmann Ø, Rimol LM, Lange EH, Haukvik UK, Hartberg CB, et al. A 5-year follow-up study of brain cortical and subcortical abnormalities in a schizophrenia cohort. Schizophr Res. (2012) 142:209–16. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tamminga CA, Clementz BA, Pearlson G, Keshavan M, Gershon ES, Ivleva EI, et al. Biotyping in psychosis: using multiple computational approaches with one data set. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2021) 46:143–155.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Shamay-Tsoory SG. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist. (2011) 17:18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kanske P, Böckler A, Trautwein FM, Singer T. Dissecting the social brain: introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain–behavior relations for empathy and theory of mind. Neuroimage. (2015) 122:6–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Penner J, Osuch EA, Schaefer B, Theberge J, Neufeld RW, Menon RS, et al. Temporoparietal junction functional connectivity in early schizophrenia and major depressive disorder. Chronic Stress. (2018) 2:2470547018815232. doi: 10.1177/2470547018815232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Mitchell RL, Crow TJ. Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere? Brain. (2005) 128:963–78. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Goldschmidt MG, Villarreal MF, de Achával D, Drucaroff LJ, Costanzo EY, Castro MN, et al. Cluster b personality symptoms in persons at genetic risk for schizophrenia are associated with social competence and activation of the right temporo-parietal junction during emotion processing. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2014) 221:30–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Villarreal MF, Drucaroff LJ, Goldschmidt MG, de Achával D, Costanzo EY, Castro MN, et al. Pattern of brain activation during social cognitive tasks is related to social competence in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. (2014) 56:120–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Gelfo F, Mandolesi L, Serra L, Sorrentino G, Caltagirone C. The neuroprotective effects of experience on cognitive functions: evidence from animal studies on the neurobiological bases of brain reserve. Neuroscience. (2018) 370:218–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bitsch F, Berger P, Nagels A, Falkenberg I, Straube B. Mpaired right temporoparietal junction–hippocampus connectivity in schizophrenia and its relevance for generating representations of other minds. Schizophr Bull. (2019) 45:934–45. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Bonfils KA, Lysaker PH, Minor KS, Salyers MP. Empathy in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 249:293–303. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, Magnotta V, Pierson R, Ziebell S, Ho BC. Progressive brain change in schizophrenia: a prospective longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:672–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Watsky RE, Pollard KL, Greenstein D, Shora L, Dillard-Broadnax D, Gochman P, et al. Severity of cortical thinning correlates with schizophrenia spectrum symptoms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 55:130–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Walton E, Hibar DP, Van Erp TG, Potkin SG, Roiz-Santiañez R, Crespo-Facorro B, et al. Prefrontal cortical thinning links to negative symptoms in schizophrenia via the ENIGMA consortium. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:82–94. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Veijola J, Guo JY, Moilanen JS, Jääskeläinen E, Miettunen J, Kyllönen M, et al. Longitudinal changes in total brain volume in schizophrenia: relation to symptom severity, cognition and antipsychotic medication. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e101689. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Uribe C, Puig-Davi A, Abos A, Baggio HC, Junque C, Segura B. Neuroanatomical and functional correlates of cognitive and affective empathy in young healthy adults. Front Behav Neurosci. (2019) 13:85. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Sassa Y, Hashizume H, Sekiguchi A, Fukushima A, et al. Regional gray matter volume is associated with empathizing and systemizing in young adults. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e84782. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, Van Engeland. H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2007) 191:5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Shur S, Harari H, Levkovitz Y. Neurocognitive basis of impaired empathy in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. (2007) 21:431. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Donaldson PH, Rinehart NJ, Enticott PG. Noninvasive stimulation of the temporoparietal junction: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:547–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Pflum MJ, Gooding DC. Context matters: social cognition task performance in psychometric schizotypes. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 264:398–403. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, cognitive empathy, temporoparietal junction, neuroimaging, emotional perspective-taking

Citation: Karpouzian-Rogers T, Cobia D, Petersen J, Wang L, Mittal VA, Csernansky JG and Smith MJ (2021) Cognitive Empathy and Longitudinal Changes in Temporo-Parietal Junction Thickness in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 12:667656. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.667656

Received: 14 February 2021; Accepted: 15 April 2021;
Published: 14 May 2021.

Edited by:

Ana Seara-Cardoso, Universidade Do Minho, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Benjamin Straube, University of Marburg, Germany
Salvador Guinjoan, Laureate Institute for Brain Research, United States

Copyright © 2021 Karpouzian-Rogers, Cobia, Petersen, Wang, Mittal, Csernansky and Smith. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tatiana Karpouzian-Rogers, t-karpouzian@northwestern.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.