Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 05 August 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
Volume 7 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2022.937983

Musical extracurricular activities and adjustment among children from immigrant families: A 2-year quasi-experimental study

Elizabeth Olivier1† Véronique Dupéré2† Isabelle Archambault2 Marilou Meilleur2* Éliane Thouin2 Anne-Sophie Denault3
  • 1Département de Psychopédagogie et d’Andragogie, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Département des Fondements et Pratiques en Éducation, Université Laval, Quebec City, QC, Canada

This quasi-experimental study examines the impact on emotional/behavioral functioning (hyperactivity-inattention and internalizing symptoms) and school experiences (school engagement, positive experience in school) of La classe enchantée, a high-quality, non-selective extracurricular music program. Based on the program’s objectives and on the positive youth development model, it was anticipated that elementary school students participating in La classe enchantée would evolve more favorably in terms of these outcomes over the 2-year duration of the program spanning Grades 4 and 5, compared to non-participating schoolmates. The sample includes 72 children (25% in the intervention group, 47% girls, 93% first/second generation immigrants) from one low-income school, followed over 2 years starting in grade four (Mage = 9.30, SD = 0.49). Latent Growth Curve models show that, compared to classmates not in the program, children in La classe enchantée progressed more favorably over time on self-reported measures of internalizing symptoms and positive experiences in school. For teacher-rated hyperactivity-inattention and school engagement, the slopes indicated stability over time in both groups, although hyperactivity-inattention tended to improve among those with relatively high initial levels in the intervention group. These results suggest that engagement in quality musical extracurricular activities might boost emotional and school well-being and potentially reduce hyperactivity-inattention among a specific subgroup of children from immigrant families.

Introduction

Learning to play a music instrument is valuable in and of itself. Yet, besides its intrinsic value, it has long been argued that musical skill development among children might generate developmental benefits in other domains, through “transfer” processes (Hallam, 2015; Sala and Gobet, 2017; Swaminathan and Schellenberg, 2021). Learning music requires complex executive functions like attention and memory, as well as an understanding of mathematical concepts like fractions to grasp beats and musical notation. Thus, music instruction could support cognitive functioning and academic learning (Swaminathan and Schellenberg, 2021). This intuition has generated much empirical research, including experimental and quasi-experimental evaluations of music instruction programs. According to recent meta-analyses, it appears, however, that music education in school may not significantly improve cognitive outcomes like academic grades, working memory, or IQ scores in the general population of children (Sala and Gobet, 2017; Cooper, 2020), although the question is not settled and continues to be examined from new angles (e.g., Frischen et al., 2021).

The general focus on cognitive outcomes and neurological processes may have obscured some of the potential benefits of music instruction for children and youth. Learning music involves much more than learning to play an instrument, perhaps especially when learning occurs in an after-school, extracurricular setting involving group activities like orchestra and choral singing. According to the positive youth development perspective, learning music in such a context, through extracurricular activities (ECAs), offers opportunities to enjoy positive experiences and to develop personal strengths, like a sense of competence and connection to larger communities and institutions (notably to school), which in turn supports emotional and behavioral functioning (Lerner et al., 2015; Vandell et al., 2015). However, these psychosocial outcomes have received scant research attention in the specialized music literature. To illustrate, in a recent review of research linking music education and children and youth outcomes (Dumont et al., 2017), only 8 of the 46 reviewed studies measured impacts on social, emotional, or psychosocial functioning (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010; Pelham et al., 2011; Rickard et al., 2012, 2013; Schellenberg and Mankarious, 2012; Ritblatt et al., 2013; Degé et al., 2014; Schellenberg et al., 2015). These studies yielded mixed results, which is not surprising given wide differences in terms of developmental periods (toddlerhood to adolescence), types of music interventions (e.g., listening to music while doing other tasks, individual instrument instruction, choral singing), settings (in-school compulsory classes, extracurricular), duration (from a few sessions to a year), outcomes (e.g., depression, self-esteem, in-class behavior, attitudes toward school, pro-social skills), sample sizes (ranging between 52 and 195), and study design (correlational, quasi-experimental, experimental).

These studies were consistent in one respect, however: their lack of attention to children from diverse backgrounds in general, and from immigrant backgrounds in particular (for a similar argument, see Holochwost et al., 2017; Christensen, 2022). Yet, insights from qualitative studies suggest that music programs might fostering inclusion and a sense of belonging among children from immigrant families, whether first (born outside the host country) or second generation (born in the host country), who may particularly benefit from opportunities to communicate and express themselves non-verbally through music. This may be especially so for those whose mother tongue differs from the majority language (Marsh, 2012, 2017). Correlational findings from the literature on extracurricular activities in general (i.e., not focusing specifically on music) also suggest that children from immigrant families might particularly benefit from ECA involvement, although this potential is left significantly unfulfilled, as children from immigrant families participate comparatively less than their non-immigrant peers (Simpkins et al., 2013; Camacho and Fuligni, 2015; Lerner et al., 2017; Meier et al., 2018). Non-participation is further compounded for children from low-SES backgrounds or for those who are less well-adjusted in school in general (McCabe et al., 2020).

To address the noted gaps in the literature, the present study proposes a quasi-experimental evaluation of the impact of a high-quality, non-selective music ECA program offered over 2 years on the school experiences and emotional and behavioral outcomes of children attending a school almost exclusively serving first- and second-generation immigrant children. The evaluation focuses on outcomes similar to those used in previous studies, and includes indicators of positive functioning (i.e., positive school experiences, school engagement) and of difficulties or challenges (i.e., internalizing problems, hyperactivity-inattention) that might be promoted or improved through participation in the program.

School engagement is a construct generally understood to refer to students’ active investment and involvement in school, which usually includes behavioral, affective, and cognitive components (Archambault I. et al., 2019). In this study, only the behavioral component was considered, namely children’s engagement in classroom work. Concerning positive school experiences, they mostly refer to the children’s feelings of excitement and motivation about school. Internalizing problems and hyperactivity-inattention capture two major poles of student socio-emotional functioning and refer to difficulties that are relatively common among school-age children, including in non-clinical samples (Caspi et al., 2014; Achenbach et al., 2016). Internalizing problems refer mainly to symptoms of depression and anxiety, such as worry, sadness, and a negative mood (Garber and Rao, 2014; Vasey et al., 2014). Hyperactivity-inattention encompasses problematic behaviors such as impulsivity, restlessness, and difficulties concentrating on a task (Campbell et al., 2014).

These outcomes were assessed via either self-reports for emotional aspects usually best captured from children’s own points of view (i.e., internalizing problems, positive school experiences) or teacher-rated measures for behavioral aspects easily observed in the classroom (i.e., hyperactivity-inattention and behavioral engagement in academic work; De Los Reyes et al., 2015).

Music activities and psychosocial outcomes: Insights from a positive youth development lens

Besides pointing to the need to consider outcomes beyond cognitive functioning, the general literature on ECA underscores multiple factors shaping the potential benefits of music instruction (Eccles and Gootman, 2002; Vandell et al., 2015; Deutsch et al., 2017). In this literature, largely conducted within Lerner’s positive youth development perspective (Lerner et al., 2015), ECAs are defined as non-compulsory organized activities that occur regularly, take place outside of regular school hours, are adult led, involve peers, and focus on a single endeavor, like a sport or performing art. According to the positive youth development model, the psychosocial benefits of ECA, musical or otherwise, are not guaranteed, and are thought to depend on factors like the ECA’s quality, setting, and dosage (Simpkins, 2015). Providing optimal experiences along those lines might be particularly crucial for programs serving children and youth for whom access to excellent ECA programming is limited, such as those from immigrant backgrounds (Camacho and Fuligni, 2015; Park et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2017; Heath et al., 2018).

Extracurricular activities quality

Many authors emphasize that offering quality ECA capable of fostering positive development is complex and requires many ingredients (Eccles and Templeton, 2002; Hirsch et al., 2011; Larson et al., 2015; Roth and Brooks-Gunn, 2016; Fredricks et al., 2017). For ECA to be considered high-quality, it is crucial that participants experience healthy, supportive relationships with activity leaders and peers. In addition, quality ECAs offer individuals opportunities to develop their skills via intentional learning experiences and provide the group experiences supporting teamwork and the development of a shared sense of belonging. Quality ECAs also balance structure and autonomy. Adequately structured ECAs offer carefully planned, engaging, challenging activities scaffolded according to developmental needs, while also providing participants space to exert autonomy and make choices. Quality ECAs also offer youth occasion to build their leadership in the group and outside of it, through contributions to the broader community, for instance through public performances (Smith et al., 2010; Deutsch et al., 2017). This “real-life contribution in authentic settings” feature might be especially important in bringing children from immigrant families to positively engage in ECA (Heath et al., 2018), as these settings provide a unique opportunity for them to learn about the norms and expectations of their host society.

Extracurricular activities setting

Extracurricular activities are typically either community-based or school-based. Activities offered by community-based organizations can still be planned in close collaboration with schools (Hirsch et al., 2011). For instance, school staff can directly promote certain activities or invite community-organization staff to promote specific activities on the school premises. Schools can also provide venues and equipment for activities, whether or not they are directly in charge of managing them. Coordination with school staff appears particularly important for recruiting children and youth from immigrant backgrounds in ECA, as well as youth with learning, behavioral, or emotional difficulties, who also tend to participate less in ECA (Heath et al., 2018; McCabe et al., 2020). Such coordination may also facilitate the generalization of eventual positive impacts of ECA on outcomes observable in the classroom, like behavioral engagement in class.

Extracurricular activities dosage

Even ECA of the highest quality and offered in ideal settings may not yield benefits if children are insufficiently exposed. Dosage usually refers to both intensity and duration, which are often captured in the form of hours per week and years of continuous practice, respectively (Simpkins, 2015; Vandell et al., 2015). There are no clear guidelines regarding optimal dosage, but some findings suggest that significant impacts might require a minimum total exposure of around 45 h, and that programs lasting at least 2 years are associated with effect sizes many times larger than programs lasting a year or less (Reisner et al., 2004; Lauer et al., 2006; Roth et al., 2010; synthesized in Vandell et al., 2015). Similar dose-response patterns have been reported in studies specifically focused on music education (e.g., Holochwost et al., 2017; Knaus, 2021).

La classe enchantée: A high-quality music extracurricular activities program

La classe enchantée embodies many of the quality, setting, and dosage features recommended for ECA programs just discussed. The program results from a partnership between the youth community outreach branch of the Faculty of Music of the Université de Montréal (L’École des jeunes de la Faculté de musique de l’Université de Montréal) and the Montreal school board (Centre de service scolaire de Montréal). The explicit goal of the program is to improve access to high-quality classical music instruction by offering non-selective music ECA to children attending primary public schools located in neighborhoods with high concentrations of low-income immigrant families. To achieve this goal and reach children who do not typically participate in classical music ECA (e.g., children with less advantaged academic or psychosocial profiles; Vandell et al., 2015), recruitment efforts unfold in close collaboration with local schools and include special in-person exchanges to present the program and encourage children to participate regardless of their past music experiences, academic grades, or psychosocial profiles. The program is also offered at low (and sometimes no) cost, with fees adjusted according to families’ financial means. Also, the instruments are loaned for free to the participating children, who assume responsibility for taking good care of their instrument for the duration of the program.

In terms of dosage, the program offers weekly three-hour sessions offered during the weekend over multiple school years. The sessions include many “ingredients” of high-quality programming. They include instrument instruction in small subgroups as well as larger orchestral and choral activities, all involving students attending the same school. In a typical weekly session, students start with instrument instruction (e.g., harp instruction) in small groups of about five students playing the same instrument. Then, all subgroups (usually three) from the same grade (e.g., 4th grade students) participate in a session of auditory training together. Then, the weekly session ends with choral singing and orchestra practice, in which all the students involved in La classe enchantée participate, regardless of their grade level (typically, about 35 to 45 students; for more details, see Milette, 2021).

Even though the program activities are highly structured, participants also have opportunities to make significant choices. Notably, they can experiment with diverse instruments before choosing the one they want to learn. The program also provides meaningful opportunities to perform in public events at the participants’ school and in the larger community. To foster supportive, healthy relationships within the program, the activity leaders in charge of implementation (undergraduate music students) are carefully selected, trained, and supported by professional music educators from the community outreach branch of the Faculty of Music of the Université de Montréal. The program staff are also in regular contact with school personnel (e.g., teachers, psychosocial practitioners) and parents, to adapt their approach to students’ individual profiles and needs. A qualitative evaluation of the program confirmed that all the stakeholders involved, including the children and their families, experienced the program positively and considered that it fostered positive and supportive relationships both with peers and with the program staff (Authors, submitted).

Objectives and hypotheses

The goal of this study is to assess the efficacy of La classe enchantée in increasing positive adjustment in school and decreasing adjustment problems among children from immigrant backgrounds. More specifically, it aims to assess changes in two indicators of positive adjustment (positive school experiences and engagement) and two indicators of psychosocial adjustment problems (internalizing behaviors and hyperactivity-inattention) among children participating in La classe enchantée, while comparing these changes to those reported by classmates in the control group. To do so, this study followed students from one primary school across two consecutive school years (grades 4 and 5), including those who did (intervention group) and did not (control group) choose to participate in the program.

Based on the positive youth development model, we anticipated that students participating in La classe enchantée and their teachers would either report improved or stable school experiences, whereas those from the control group would report a slight decline, based on well-documented general declining trends for these outcomes over the primary school years (see Archambault I. et al., 2019). We also anticipated that children participating in La classe enchantée would experience a slight decrease in their internalizing behaviors and hyperactivity-inattention, whereas those levels would remain stable in the control group, as usually found in this age group (Campbell et al., 2014; Vasey et al., 2014). Besides levels of changes, we expected that the initial levels of these indicators should be similar across groups, since the general tendency for children with more advantaged academic and psychosocial profiles to participate more in ECA (Vandell et al., 2015) should be offset by the explicit commitment embedded in La classe enchantée to recruit atypical participants in the program.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants include 72 students from one school, followed from the beginning of their fourth grade to the end of their fifth grade. Among those, 18 (25.0%) participated in La classe enchantée, and 54 (75.0%) composed the control group. At the beginning of the study, students were between 8 and 10 years old (M = 9.30, SD = 0.49; 46.5% girls). Almost all (93%) were of immigrant background (47.9% first generation born outside Canada, 45.1% second generation born in Canada from parents born outside the country, and 7.0% of parents also born in Canada). A majority (74.6%) lived with both parents, whereas 25.4% lived in other family configurations (e.g., joint custody, mother only, father only, mother and her partner, father and his partner, other).

Procedures

The pilot version of La classe enchantée evaluated in the present study was implemented in one public primary school adjacent to the Université de Montréal’s campus. At the time of data collection, the school’s catchment area was considered low-income according to the official provincial Ministry of Education classification system (the school was ranked at the last decile position, indicating a concentration of low-income family in the school’s catchment at or above the 90th percentile; Ministry of Education, 2017).

Recruitment for the program took place in the fall of 2017, on the primary school premises. The program staff presented La classe enchantée to fourth-grade students and their parents while underscoring its non-selective nature and the possibility for students to participate regardless of past music experience or profiles in terms of grades, behavior, etc. Recruitment for the research project was launched a few weeks later, after obtaining independent approval from both the Université de Montréal’s IRB and the school board ethics committee (approval number: #CERAS-2017-18-073-D). All fourth-grade students (from three classrooms) were invited to participate in the research project, regardless of whether they participated in the program. To ensure renewed, ongoing consent, consent procedures were repeated a year later at the beginning of fifth grade. Consent to participate in the research project was obtained for all the students involved in La classe enchantée. In the control group, 18% of the families refused to participate in fourth grade. It turned out that some families initially refused to participate because they mistakenly thought that consent was required only for those involved in the program. Thus, some who had initially refused consented to participate the next year in fifth grade, and thus provided at least partial data (see section “Missing data”).

The musical activities started in January 2018 and continued until the end of that school year (June 2019), and then resumed the following school year (September 2019-June 2020). Data collection waves took place twice a year, and involved all participating students (i.e., La classe enchantée and Control group) and their homeroom teachers. Specifically, data collection waves took place in November 2017 (pretest T1), May 2018 (T2), December 2018 (T3), and June 2019 (T4).

Missing data

Missing data was due either to attrition (i.e., participants lost during the study, often because of residential mobility) or students or their teachers not answering a particular questionnaire (i.e., a student absent on the day of data collection or a teacher not returning a questionnaire to the research team). In terms of attrition, one participant from the control group was lost after T1, and 19 participants were lost after T2 (17 from the control group and 2 from La classe enchantée). Overall, missing data (due to attrition or not answering the questionnaire) was as follows: 19.4% at T1, 20.8% at T2, 29.2% at T3, and 29.2% at T4. Comparing students without and with missing data across time points revealed more missing data among those from the control group (χ2 = 10.667, df = 1, p = 0.001) and students not born in Canada (χ2 = 6.199, df = 1, p = 0.013). However, missing data was not related to gender, age, or family structure. Having missing data was associated with neither student-reported nor teacher-reported measures, except for a positive association found between having missing data and teacher-reported behavioral engagement at T1 (r = 0.381, p = 0.003) and T2 (r = 0.285, df = 1, p = 0.032). Missing data was handled using Mplus (Muthén and Muthén, 2017) Full Information Maximum Likelihood (Enders, 2010).

Measures

Sociodemographic characteristics

Students reported their gender (0 = girl; 1 = boy), their age in years (8 to 10 years old), their immigration status (0 = born in Canada [almost always from parents born outside the country]; 1 = born elsewhere), and their family structure (0 = living with both parents; 1 = else).

Student-rated internalizing behaviors

Students rated their internalizing behaviors on anxiety and depression items from the French adaptation (Tremblay et al., 1987) of the Preschool Behavior Questionnaire (Hoge et al., 1985). This questionnaire was initially developed for kindergarten students, but has been shown to be valid across the primary school years (Tremblay et al., 1987); as such, it has often been used with older school-age children, including during the later elementary school years (e.g., Olivier et al., 2020). Students responded to this 6-item scale (e.g., “You worry a lot” and “You are unhappy or sad”; αT1 = 0.694, αT2 = 0.825, αT3 = 0.859, αT4 = 0.895) on a response scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 3 (certainly true).

Student-rated positive experiences at school

Students rated four items from the “zest” subscale of the Positive Experiences at School Scale (Furlong et al., 2013). This scale captures positive attitudes toward school, like excitement, enthusiasm, and energy, through items like “I wake up in the morning excited to go to school,” “I get really excited about my school projects,” rated on a response scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 3 (very often). As a French validation of this scale has not yet been published, the research team conducted preliminary analyses to verify the quality of its psychometric properties. The translated version showed good internal consistency (e.g., αT1 = 0.762, αT2 = 0.829, αT3 = 0.823, αT4 = 0.815).

Teacher-reported hyperactivity-inattention

Teachers reported each of their students’ hyperactivity-inattention levels using one subscale from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 2001) adapted in French (Capron et al., 2007). They responded to this 9-item scale (e.g., “This student is restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long” and “This student is inattentive”; αT1 = 0.943, αT2 = 0.944, αT3 = 0.951, αT4 = 0.918) on a response scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 3 (certainly true).

Teacher-reported behavioral engagement

Teachers reported each of their students’ behavioral engagement levels in the classroom using a scale developed in French for the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (Pagani et al., 2010). Teachers responded to this 9-item scale (e.g., “This student puts a lot of effort in assignments” and “This student participates in class”; αT1 = 0.811, αT2 = 0.851, αT3 = 0.858, αT4 = 0.849) on a response scale ranging from 1 (never) to 3 (often).

Data analysis

Preliminary analyses included assessing whether students enlisted in La classe enchantée differed from the control group in terms of sociodemographic characteristics, as well as assessing correlations between all study variables. Using Mplus 8.4 (Muthén and Muthén, 2017), we then conducted Latent Growth Curve analysis (LGC, including intercept and linear slope) to assess changes in school experiences (positive school experiences and behavioral engagement) and behavior problems (internalizing behaviors and hyperactivity-inattention) across the two school years (T1 to T4) in both the La classe enchantée and control groups. LGC analysis is useful for examining group differences in program evaluations involving long-term follow-ups and multiple measures (e.g., Beauchaine et al., 2005; Liddle et al., 2009; Eddy et al., 2021).

To determine whether students from the two groups followed distinct developmental trajectories, we first assessed a model in which all parameters varied between groups and then progressively constrained them to equality across groups using the following sequence: (1) model freely estimated, (2) fixed intercept mean, (3) fixed intercept variance, (4) fixed slope mean, (5) fixed slope variance, and (6) fixed intercept-slope covariance. These models were compared using a chi-square difference test calculated using the Satorra-Bentler correction (Satorra and Bentler, 2010). When significant, this test indicates the presence of a difference on the constrained parameter between groups, as a result of which we retained the previous model (i.e., kept the parameter freely estimated between groups in the following models).

Model fit was assessed using the chi-square statistic (χ2), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) (Marsh et al., 2005). For the CFI and TLI, values above.90 and.95 respectively indicate an acceptable and excellent model fit. For the RMSEA, values smaller than.08 and.06 respectively suggest an acceptable and excellent fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1992; Schumacker and Lomax, 1996; Hu and Bentler, 1999).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Initial differences between groups

Comparing students who participated in La classe enchantée to those from the control group revealed no sociodemographic differences in proportions of boys and girls (χ2 = 3.641, df = 1, p = 0.056), of students born in Canada or elsewhere (χ2 = 0.019, df = 1, p = 0.982), of family structure (χ2 = 0.024, df = 1, p = 0.877), or of differences in mean age (t = 0.531, df = 1, p = 0.597). Similarly, according to descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviations) reported in Table 1, no differences between groups were found for self-reported internalizing problems or positive school experiences at T1. However, participating in La classe enchantée was associated with higher levels of teacher-reported hyperactivity-inattention and lower levels of behavioral engagement at T1. Thus, the few differences between the two groups were limited to teacher-reported measures, and indicated worse initial functioning in the intervention group as compared to the control group.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlations between study variables.

Other correlations

In Table 1, all significant associations were in the expected direction and indicated relative stability of the adjustment indicators across time points. Also, teachers’ perceptions of their students’ hyperactivity-inattention and behavioral engagement levels shared moderate to high correlations (r = −0.510 to −0.860), indicating that teachers tend to perceive the behavioral engagement of their students as closely tied to their hyperactivity-inattention levels.

Latent curve models

Model fit indices from the alternative Latent Curve Models are reported in Table 2 and detailed parameter estimates are reported in Supplementary Table 1 of the online Supplementary material. All models that were retained (those not in bold in Table 1) had an acceptable to an excellent level of fit.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results from the alternative latent growth curve models.

Looking first at the model with student-rated internalizing behaviors revealed that all parameters were equivalent between La classe enchantée and control groups, except for the means of the intercept and slope. As displayed in Figure 1, students in La classe enchantée had an initially (i.e., intercept) higher level of internalizing behaviors at pretest (M = 1.687, p < 0.001), which decreased (i.e., slope) over time (M = −0.094, p = 0.034), compared to the initially lower level found in students from the control group at pretest (M = 1.498, p < 0.001), which remained stable over time (M = 0.000, p = 0.986).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Latent curve model results per group.

Second, the model with student-rated positive school experiences revealed that all parameters were equivalent between groups, except for the slope means. As displayed in Figure 1, students from both groups reported the same initial level at pretest (M = 3.228, p < 0.000), which remained stable in the La classe enchantée group (M = −0.025, p = 0.641), but decreased in the control group (M = −0.192, p < 0.000).

Third, looking at the model with teacher-reported hyperactivity-inattention indicates that all parameters differed between groups, except for the slope means, which showed a stable level of hyperactivity-inattention in the two groups (M = −0.003, p = 0.867). The initial level of hyperactivity-inattention at pretest was higher in La classe enchantée group (M = 1.510, p < 0.000) compared to the control group (M = 1.300, p < 0.000). There was also greater variability (i.e., intercept and slope variance) in the La classe enchantée group than in the control group (see Supplementary Table 1 for detailed parameters). Finally, we found a negative association between intercept and slope in the La classe enchantée group (r = −0.099, p = 0.033), but not in the control group (r = −0.011, p = 0.387). In other words, students in La classe enchantée who had an initially higher level of hyperactivity-inattention tended to experience a greater decrease in these behaviors than those who had an initially lower level of such behaviors, whereas this association was not found in the control group. Results from this model should be interpreted with caution as the RMSEA indicated a poor level of fit, whereas the CFI and TLI suggested an acceptable level of fit.

Fourth, similarly to hyperactivity-inattention, the model with teacher-reported behavioral engagement indicates that all parameters differed between groups, except for the slope means, which showed a stable level of engagement in the two groups (M = 0.011, p = 0.532). The initial level of engagement at pretest was lower in the La classe enchantée group (M = 2.434, p < 0.000) compared to the control group (M = 2.668, p < 0.000). There was also greater variability (i.e., intercept and slope variance) in the La classe enchantée group than in the control group (see Supplementary Table 1 for detailed parameters). Finally, we found a positive association between the intercept and slope in the control group (r = 0.016, p = 0.045), but not in the La classe enchantée group (r = −0.033, p = 0.097). This indicates that, among students from the control group, having an initially higher level of engagement was associated with a greater increase in engagement over time than for those who had an initially lower level of engagement. This association was not found in students in the La classe enchantée group.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to evaluate, using a quasi-experimental design, the impact on various dimensions of psychosocial adjustment of La classe enchantée, a high-quality music ECA program offered to first- and second-generation immigrant children over two school years (fourth and fifth grade). Program impacts were examined from the point of view of children for internalizing symptoms and positive experiences in school, which are typically best captured via self-reported measures, and from their homeroom teacher’s perspective for externally visible behaviors, namely, hyperactivity-inattention and behavioral engagement in classroom work. The results were partially consistent with initial expectations of favorable program outcomes.

Initial similarities and differences between the intervention and control groups

The results first indicated that, contrary to trends reported both in the general ECA literature and in the more specialized literature focusing on music programs (Vandell et al., 2015; Alegrado and Winsler, 2020), children who participated in La classe enchantée were not more advantaged than their peers who did not volunteer to participate in the program. Sociodemographic variables were similar across groups, including for individual (gender, age) and family background (family structure, immigration status) characteristics. However, the groups differed in some aspects of initial adjustment (i.e., internalizing problems, hyperactivity-inattention, and behavioral engagement according to preliminary bivariate associations and/or estimated growth curve intercepts), but always in a direction pointing to better adjustment in the control group as compared to the intervention group.

These results suggest that with an intentional approach specifically designed to foster inclusivity, it is possible to support ECA enrollment among children who usually participate less (McCabe et al., 2020). Despite its appeal from an equity point of view, it should be kept in mind that the initial differences in favor of the control group complicate the interpretation of the results (discussed next), showing comparatively favorable evolution in the intervention group compared to the control group for some outcomes, as apparent differences could reflect differential regression-to-the mean processes rather than real program effects.

Apparent program effects

Even though caution is warranted in the context of a quasi-experimental study with initial differences between intervention and control groups, the results are generally consistent with the premise that high-quality music ECA might support positive adjustment among first- and second-generation immigrant children, at least for the two self-reported outcomes considered. For these outcomes – that is, positive school experiences and internalizing problems – children involved in La classe enchantée evolved more positively over time compared to their non-involved classmates. More specifically, students who participated in La classe enchantée reported, over the two-year observation window, decreasing levels of internalizing behaviors, whereas these behaviors remained stable for the control group. For positive school experiences, consistent with studies showing a slight decline in these positive experiences over time, students in the control group reported such a decline from fourth to fifth grade; however, this decline was not observed in students who participated in La classe enchantée.

The results thus suggest that the program helped children from immigrant families feel less distress and more positive emotions and excitement toward school. Given that these children are more likely to encounter challenges due to their family’s migration and resettlement trajectory (Motti-Stefanidi, 2019), the promotion of musical ECA appears an interesting, positive avenue to support their resilience. These results correspond to the positive youth development premises suggesting that high-quality ECAs in general support children’s well-being and positive attitudes toward larger institutions, most notably their school (Lerner et al., 2015), and that music ECA in particular can foster emotional regulation (Van Goethem and Sloboda, 2011; Archambault K. et al., 2019). They are also consistent with the idea that these benefits appear gradually, as exposure to high-quality ECA builds up over time (Simpkins, 2015; Vandell et al., 2015). Features of La classe enchantée corresponding to characteristics considered core for high-quality programming might have supported these positive outcomes, like the fact that the program involved schoolmates and promoted positive relationships with peers in the program, which continued to be experienced at school. Beyond relationships with peers and adults directly involved in the program, participation in the program also provided occasions to “shine” at school-wide concerts in front of the whole student body, teachers, and parents, as well as in other community venues. Qualitative interviews with participants suggested that these occasions contributed to the building of a general sense of excitement or “zest,” including toward school (Authors, submitted).

In contrast, results for teacher-reported outcomes showed no comparatively favorable trends on average over time in the intervention group as compared to the control group. In both groups, the average level of hyperactivity-inattention and behavioral engagement in classroom work remained stable over time, meaning that the initial discrepancy found between groups (in favor of the control group) lasted throughout the study. These results are consistent with Ciocanel et al.’s (2017) meta-analysis of positive youth development interventions, which found positive impacts for psychological adjustment (i.e., emotional distress, self-perceptions), but not for problem behavior. They are also consistent with the generally held consensus that hyperactivity and inattention problems (and associated difficulties in terms of behavioral engagement) result in substantial part from neurological functioning impairments, which are not likely to change through exposure to low-intensity ECA programs not specifically designed to impact such outcomes, as compared to targeted intensive interventions or medication (Thapar et al., 2013). It is also possible that gains made in an ECA taking place outside of regular school hours do not necessarily translate into the classroom context. Finally, measurement issues might also contribute to explain the different results for self-reported and teacher-reported outcomes. Participants in La classe enchantée might have consciously or unconsciously provided inflated scores, knowing that the research project’s goal was to evaluate the program. However, the fact that differences between groups tented to emerge slowly over time suggests against this interpretation.

Even though children in La classe enchantée did not improve on average in terms of teacher-rated hyperactivity-inattention, results showed that among this group only, the initial level of hyperactivity-inattention (i.e., intercept) was associated with evolution over time (i.e., slope), suggesting that the program might have yielded benefits among those who had initially higher hyperactivity-inattention levels. This result is consistent with other findings suggesting that participation in ECA might be especially beneficial for youth presenting an at-risk or less favorable initial profile (Simpkins, 2015; Vandell et al., 2015; Deutsch et al., 2017). However, given the small sample size, firm conclusions will require replication.

Limitations and future directions

The results should be interpreted with caution. As already stated, this study is based on a non-equivalent quasi-experimental group design, meaning that the results might overestimate or underestimate the real impacts of the program. The small sample size also yields low power to detect program impacts and to conduct subgroup analyses (e.g., by gender). In addition, the control group did not receive any alternative form of ECA; thus, the findings could reflect non-specific effects that other form of ECA programming might have achieved. The cognitive outcomes that have been the main focus of past research evaluating music education programs, such as memory, IQ, and executive functioning, were not part of this evaluation, which limits our capacity to compare the findings to those of previous studies. Finally, the high quality of the ECA experiences offered in La classe enchantée spanning 2 years is probably not representative of typical community-based offerings, thus limiting potential generalization. These limitations underscore the oft-repeated need for large randomized control trials to better understand ECA impacts and processes, and how they might depend on the characteristics of both participants and activities (Vandell et al., 2015; Roth and Brooks-Gunn, 2016).

Conclusion

This study adds to an emerging experimental and quasi-experimental research base emphasizing the potential of music ECA for supporting positive psychosocial outcomes among children and youth from diverse ethnic, migratory, and socioeconomic backgrounds (Holochwost et al., 2017, 2021). The findings suggest that targeted efforts specifically dedicated to that end can successfully encourage long-term ECA participation in diverse populations of children, including among subgroups with socioeconomic and behavioral profiles traditionally associated with non-participation. They also show that among first- and second-generation immigrants attending public low-income schools, sustained participation in quality music ECA has the potential to support some dimension of psychosocial adjustment. Although limited in several ways, these encouraging findings should provide support for the provision of high-quality ECA to improve access among underserved population, as well as the launch of larger, better control studies in the future.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical reasons related to confidentiality issues. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to VD, veronique.dupere@umontreal.ca.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Université de Montréal’s IRB (approval number: #CERAS-2017-18-073-D). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

EO performed the statistical analysis and drafted sections of the manuscript. VD conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, and drafted sections of the manuscript. IA participated in the design of the study and interpretation of the data, and helped to draft the manuscript. MM and ÉT coordinated the study, performed the measurements, and revised the manuscript. A-SD participated in the design of the study, the literature review, and the interpretation of the data and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Social Sciences and humanities Research Council of Canada [Grants 890-2017-0050 and 892-2017-2034].

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/feduc.2022.937983/full#supplementary-material

References

Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Rescorla, L. A., Turner, L. V., and Althoff, R. R. (2016). Internalizing/externalizing problems: review and recommendations for clinical and research applications. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 647–656. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alegrado, A., and Winsler, A. (2020). Predictors of taking elective music courses in middle school among low-SES, ethnically diverse students in Miami. J. Res. Music Educ. 68, 5–30. doi: 10.1177/0022429420908282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Goulet, M., Dupéré, V., and Gilbert-Blanchard, O. (2019). “Promoting student engagement from childhood to adolescence as a way to improve positive youth development and school completion,” in Handbook of Student Engagement Interventions: Working with Disengaged Students, eds J. A. Fredricks, A. L. Reschly, and S. L. Christenson (London: Elsevier), 13–29.

Google Scholar

Archambault, K., Vaugon, K., Deumié, V., Brault, M., Perez, R. M., Peyrin, J., et al. (2019). MAP: a personalized receptive music therapy intervention to improve the affective well-being of youths hospitalized in a mental health unit. J. Music Ther. 56, 381–402. doi: 10.1093/jmt/thz013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchaine, T. P., Webster-Stratton, C., and Reid, M. J. (2005). Mediators, moderators, and predictors of 1-year outcomes among children treated for early-onset conduct problems: a latent growth curve analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 371–388. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Soc. Methods Res. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camacho, D. E., and Fuligni, A. J. (2015). Extracurricular participation among adolescents from immigrant families. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 1251–1262. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0105-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, S. B., Halperin, J. M., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2014). “A developmental perspective on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),” in Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, eds M. Lewis and K. D. Rudolph (Boston, MA: Springer), 427–448.

Google Scholar

Capron, C., Thérond, C., and Duyme, M. (2007). Psychometric properties of the French version of the self-report and teacher strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 23, 79–88. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.2.79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., et al. (2014). The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 119–137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, K. M. (2022). A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Effects of After-School Programs on Academic, Social, Behavioral, Mental Health, and Identity Outcomes Among Youth with Marginalized Identities. Doctoral Dissertation, Boston. Available online at: https://scholarworks.umb.edu/doctoral_dissertations/676

Google Scholar

Ciocanel, O., Power, K., Eriksen, A., and Gillings, K. (2017). Effectiveness of positive youth development interventions: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 483–504. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0555-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, P. K. (2020). It’s all in your head: A meta-analysis on the effects of music training on cognitive measures in schoolchildren. Int. J. Music Educ. 38, 321–336. doi: 10.1177/0255761419881495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Los Reyes, A., Augenstein, T. M., Wang, M., Thomas, S. A., Drabick, D. A. G., Burgers, D. E., et al. (2015). The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychol. Bull. 141, 858–900. doi: 10.1037/a0038498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degé, F., Wehrum, S., Stark, R., and Schwarzer, G. (2014). Music lessons and academic self-concept in 12- to 14-year-old children. Music. Sci. 18, 203–215. doi: 10.1177/1029864914523283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, N. L., Blyth, D. A., Kelley, J., Tolan, P. H., and Lerner, R. M. (2017). “Let’s talk after-school: the promises and challenges of positive youth development for after-school research, policy, and practice,” in After-School Programs to Promote Positive Youth Development, ed. N. L. Deutsch (Berlin: Springer), 45–68.

Google Scholar

Dumont, E., Syurina, E. V., Feron, F. J. M., and van Hooren, S. (2017). Music interventions and child development: a critical review and further directions. Front. Psychol. 8:1694. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J., and Gootman, J. A. (2002). Community Programs to Promote Youth Development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Templeton, J. (2002). Extracurricular and other after-school activities for youth. Rev. Res. Educ. 26, 113–180. doi: 10.3102/0091732X026001113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eddy, L. D., Anastopoulos, A. D., Dvorsky, M. R., Silvia, P. J., Labban, J. D., and Langberg, J. M. (2021). An RCT of a CBT intervention for emerging adults with ADHD attending college: functional outcomes. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 50, 844–857. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2020.1867989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York, NY: Guildford.

Google Scholar

Fredricks, J. A., Naftzger, N., Smith, C., and Riley, A. (2017). “Measuring youth participation, program quality, and social and emotional skills in after-school programs,” in After-School Programs to Promote Positive Youth Development, ed. N. L. Deutsch (Cham: Springer), 23–43.

Google Scholar

Frischen, U., Schwarzer, G., and Degé, F. (2021). Music lessons enhance executive functions in 6-to 7-year-old children. Learn. Instruct. 74:101442. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2021.101442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furlong, M. J., You, S., Renshaw, T. L., O’Malley, M. D., and Rebelez, J. (2013). Preliminary development of the positive experiences at school scale for elementary school children. Child Ind. Res. 6, 753–775. doi: 10.1007/s12187-013-9193-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garber, J., and Rao, U. (2014). “Depression in children and adolescents,” in Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, 3rd Edn, eds M. Lewis and K. D. Rudolph (Boston, MA: Springer), 489–520.

Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1337–1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallam, S. (2015). The Power of Music: A Research Synthesis of the Impact of Actively Making Music on the Intellectual, Social and Personal Development of Children and Young People. London: International Music Education Research Centre (iMerc).

Google Scholar

Heath, R. D., Anderson, C., Turner, A. C., and Payne, C. M. (2018). Extracurricular activities and disadvantaged youth: a complicated—but promising—story. Urban Educ. 1–35. doi: 10.1177/0042085918805797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, B. J., Deutsch, N. L., and DuBois, D. L. (2011). After-School Centers and Youth Development: Case Studies of Success and Failure. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Hoge, R. D., Meginbir, L., Khan, Y., and Weatherall, D. (1985). A multitrait-multimethod analysis of the preschool behavior questionnaire. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 13, 119–127. doi: 10.1007/BF00918376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holochwost, S. J., Bose, J. H., Stuk, E., Brown, E. D., Anderson, K. E., and Wolf, D. P. (2021). Planting the seeds: orchestral music education as a context for fostering growth mindsets. Front. Psychol. 11:586749. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holochwost, S. J., Propper, C. B., Wolf, D. P., Willoughby, M. T., Fisher, K. R., Kolacz, J., et al. (2017). Music education, academic achievement, and executive functions. Psychol. Aesthe. Creat. Arts 11, 147–166. doi: 10.1037/aca0000112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knaus, M. C. (2021). A double machine learning approach to estimate the effects of musical practice on student’s skills. J. Royal Stat. Soc. Ser. A 184, 282–300. doi: 10.1111/rssa.12623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, R. W., Walker, K. C., Rusk, N., and Diaz, L. B. (2015). Understanding youth development from the practitioner’s point of view: a call for research on effective practice. Appl. Dev. Sci. 19, 74–86. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2014.972558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauer, P. A., Akiba, M., Wilkerson, S. B., Apthorp, H. S., Snow, D., and Martin-Glenn, M. L. (2006). Out-of-school-time programs: a meta-analysis of effects for at-risk students. Rev. Educ. Res. 76, 275–313. doi: 10.3102/00346543076002275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Bowers, E. P., and Geldhof, G. J. (2015). “Positive youth development and relational developmental systems,” in Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 7th Edn, eds W. F. Overton and P. C. Molenaar (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 607–651.

Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M., Wang, J., Hershberg, R. M., Buckingham, M. H., Harris, E. M., Tirrell, J. M., et al. (2017). “Positive youth development among minority youth: a relational developmental systems model,” in Handbook on Positive Development of Minority Children and Youth, eds N. J. Cabrera and B. Leyendecker (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 5–17.

Google Scholar

Liddle, H. A., Rowe, C. L., Dakof, G. A., Henderson, C. E., and Greenbaum, P. E. (2009). Multidimensional family therapy for young adolescent substance abuse: twelve-month outcomes of a randomized controlled trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 12–25. doi: 10.1037/a0014160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., and Grayson, D. (2005). “Goodness of fit in structural equation models,” in Contemporary Psychometrics, eds A. Maydeu-Olivares and J. J. McArdle (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 275–340.

Google Scholar

Marsh, K. (2012). “Music in the lives of refugee and newly arrived immigrant children in Sydney, Australia,” in Oxford Handbook of Children’s Musical Cultures, eds P. Campbell and T. Wiggins (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 492–509.

Google Scholar

Marsh, K. (2017). Creating bridges: music, play and well-being in the lives of refugee and immigrant children and young people. Music Educ. Res. 19, 60–73. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2016.1189525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, J., Dupéré, V., Dion, E., Thouin, É, Archambault, I., Dufour, S., et al. (2020). Why do extracurricular activities prevent dropout more effectively in some high schools than in others? A mixed-method examination of organizational dynamics. Appl. Dev. Sci. 24, 323–338. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2018.1484746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, A., Hartmann, B. S., and Larson, R. (2018). A quarter century of participation in school-based extracurricular activities: inequalities by race, class, gender and age? J. Youth Adolesc. 47, 1299–1316. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0838-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milette, M.-C. (2021). Évaluation de la mise en æuvre du programme d’activités parascolaires musicales La classe enchantée. Master’s thesis. Montreal, QC: Université de Montréal.

Google Scholar

Ministry of Education (2017). Deprivation Indices for Public Schools in Quebec. Quebec, QC: Quebec Government.

Google Scholar

Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2019). Resilience among immigrant youths: who adapts well, and why? Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 28, 510–517. doi: 10.1177/0963721419861412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus User’s Guide, 8th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Olivier, E., Morin, A. J. S., Langlois, J., Tardif-Grenier, K., and Archambault, I. (2020). Internalizing and externalizing behavior problems and student engagement in elementary and secondary school students. J. Youth Adolesc. 49, 2327–2346. doi: 10.1007/s10964-020-01295-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., Archambault, I., and Janosz, M. (2010). School readiness and later achievement: a French Canadian replication and extension. Dev. Psychol. 46, 984–994. doi: 10.1037/a0018881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, H., Lin, C.-H., Liu, C., and Tabb, K. M. (2015). The relationships between after-school programs, academic outcomes, and behavioral developmental outcomes of Latino children from immigrant families: findings from the 2005 National household education surveys program. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 53, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelham, W. E., Waschbusch, D. A., Hoza, B., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Sams, S. E., et al. (2011). Music and video as distractors for boys with ADHD in the classroom: comparison with controls, individual differences, and medication effects. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 1085–1098. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9529-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisner, E. R., White, R. N., Russell, C., and Birmingham, J. (2004). Building Quality, Scale, and Effectiveness in After-School Programs. Report prepared for the After-School Corporation. Washington, D.C: Policy Studies Associates.

Google Scholar

Rickard, N. S., Appelman, P., James, R., Murphy, F., Gill, A., and Bambrick, C. (2013). Orchestrating life skills: the effect of increased school-based music classes on children’s social competence and self-esteem. Int. J. Music Educ. 31, 292–309. doi: 10.1177/0255761411434824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rickard, N. S., Bambrick, C. J., and Gill, A. (2012). Absence of widespread psychosocial and cognitive effects of school-based music instruction in 10–13-year-old students. Int. J. Music Educ. 30, 57–78. doi: 10.1177/0255761411431399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritblatt, S., Longstreth, S., Hokoda, A., Cannon, B.-N., and Weston, J. (2013). Can music enhance school-readiness socioemotional skills? J. Res. Child. Educ. 27, 257–266. doi: 10.1080/02568543.2013.796333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, J. L., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2016). Evaluating youth development programs: progress and promise. Appl. Dev. Sci. 20, 188–202. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2015.1113879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, J. L., Malone, L. M., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010). Does the amount of participation in afterschool programs relate to developmental outcomes? A review of the literature. Am. J. Commu. Psychol. 45, 310–324. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9303-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala, G., and Gobet, F. (2017). When the music’s over. Does music skill transfer to children’s and young adolescents’ cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 20, 55–67. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika 75, 243–248. doi: 10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G., Corrigall, K. A., Dys, S. P., and Malti, T. (2015). Group music training and children’s prosocial skills. PLoS One 10:e0141449. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G., and Mankarious, M. (2012). Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood. Emotion 12, 887–891. doi: 10.1037/a0027971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (1996). A Beginner’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Simpkins, S. D. (2015). When and how does participating in an organized after-school activity matter? Appl. Dev. Sci. 19, 121–126. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2015.1056344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpkins, S. D., Delgado, M. Y., Price, C. D., Quach, A., and Starbuck, E. (2013). Socioeconomic status, ethnicity, culture, and immigration: examining the potential mechanisms underlying Mexican-origin adolescents’ organized activity participation. Dev. Psychol. 49, 706–721. doi: 10.1037/a0028399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C., Peck, S. C., Denault, A.-S., Blazevski, J., and Akiva, T. (2010). Quality at the point of service: profiles of practice in after-school settings. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 45, 358–369. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9315-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. P., Witherspoon, D. P., and Wayne Osgood, D. (2017). Positive youth development among diverse racial–ethnic children: quality afterschool contexts as developmental assets. Child Dev. 88, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaminathan, S., and Schellenberg, E. G. (2021). “Music training,” in Cognitive Training: An Overview of Features and Applications, eds T. Strobach and J. Karbach (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 307–318.

Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Cooper, M., Eyre, O., and Langley, K. (2013). Practitioner review: what have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 3–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, R. E., Desmarais-Gervais, L., Gagnon, C., and Charlebois, P. (1987). The preschool behavior questionnaire: stability of its factor structure between cultures, sexes, ages and socioecconomic classes. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 10, 467–484. doi: 10.1177/016502548701000406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Goethem, A., and Sloboda, J. (2011). The functions of music for affect regulation. Music. Sci. 15, 208–228. doi: 10.1177/1029864911401174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandell, D. L., Larson, R. W., Mahoney, J. L., and Watts, T. W. (2015). “Children’s organized activities,” in Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, ed. R. M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 305–344.

Google Scholar

Vasey, M., Bosmans, G., and Ollendick, T. (2014). “The developmental psychopathology of anxiety,” in Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, 3rd Edn, eds M. Lewis and K. Rudolph (Berlin: Springer), 543–560.

Google Scholar

Keywords: extracurricular activities, music, psychosocial outcomes, immigration, program evaluation

Citation: Olivier E, Dupéré V, Archambault I, Meilleur M, Thouin É and Denault A-S (2022) Musical extracurricular activities and adjustment among children from immigrant families: A 2-year quasi-experimental study. Front. Educ. 7:937983. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.937983

Received: 06 May 2022; Accepted: 21 July 2022;
Published: 05 August 2022.

Edited by:

Carolina Gonzálvez, University of Alicante, Spain

Reviewed by:

Francesca Giovanna Maria Gastaldi, University of Turin, Italy
Sabina Vidulin, Juraj Dobrila University of Pula, Croatia

Copyright © 2022 Olivier, Dupéré, Archambault, Meilleur, Thouin and Denault. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marilou Meilleur, marilou.meilleur@umontreal.ca

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download