Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Endocrinol., 19 November 2021
Sec. Pediatric Endocrinology
This article is part of the Research Topic Circadian Rhythm in Cellular Endocrinology View all 4 articles

Anxiety and Cognition in Cre- Collagen Type II Sirt1 K/O Male Mice

Biana Shtaif,Biana Shtaif1,2Shay Henry HornfeldShay Henry Hornfeld2Michal Yackobovitch-Gavan,Michal Yackobovitch-Gavan1,3Moshe Phillip,,Moshe Phillip1,2,3Galia Gat-Yablonski,,*Galia Gat-Yablonski1,2,3*
  • 1Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 2Laboratory for Molecular Endocrinology and Diabetes, Felsenstein Medical Research Center, Petach Tikva, Israel
  • 3The Jesse Z and Sara Lea Shafer Institute for Endocrinology and Diabetes, National Center for Childhood Diabetes, Schneider Children’s Medical Center of Israel, Petach Tikva, Israel

Introduction: Using transgenic collagen type II-specific Sirt1 knockout (CKO) mice we studied the role of Sirt1 in nutritional induced catch up growth (CUG) and we found that these mice have a less organized growth plate and reduced efficiency of CUG. In addition, we noted that they weigh more than control (CTL) mice. Studying the reason for the increased weigh, we found differences in activity and brain function.

Methods: Several tests for behavior and activity were used: open field; elevated plus maze, Morris water maze, and home cage running wheels. The level of Glu- osteocalcin, known to connect bone and brain function, was measured by Elisa; brain Sirt1 was analyzed by western blot.

Results: We found that CKO mice had increased anxiety, with less spatial memory, learning capabilities and reduced activity in their home cages. No significant differences were found between CKO and CTL mice in Glu- osteocalcin levels; nor in the level of brain SIRT1.

Discussion/Conclusion: Using transgenic collagen type II-specific Sirt1 knockout (CKO) mice we found a close connection between linear growth and brain function. Using a collagen type II derived system we affected a central regulatory mechanism leading to hypo activity, increased anxiety, and slower learning, without affecting circadian period. As children with idiopathic short stature are more likely to have lower IQ, with substantial deficits in working memory than healthy controls, the results of the current study suggest that SIRT1 may be the underlying factor connecting growth and brain function.

1 Introduction

In order to identify novel regulatory mechanisms in children’s growth, we have been focusing on the epiphyseal growth plate (EGP) and the important role of SIRT1 in regulating the nutrition-growth connection was unraveled (1, 2). The sirtuin (SIRT) family of proteins consists of proteins that act predominately as nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide-dependent (NAD+) – deacetylases and convey diverse functions in a variety of physiological settings.

SIRT1, the most conserved enzyme of this family and the most extensively studied, is a central component of numerous basic pathways. The expression and activity of SIRT1 are tightly regulated. SIRT1 is usually localized to the nucleus but may also translocate to the cytosol (3). It interacts and deacetylates histones and non-histone proteins such as p53, E2F1, NF-kB, and FOXO (4, 5) and through them affects cell proliferation, senescence, autophagy, stress responses, and apoptosis. SIRT1 has been shown to have a substantial role in longevity (6), response to food restriction (1, 7), and linear growth (1, 2).

We studied nutritional-induced catch-up growth (CUG) using a model consisting of food restriction followed by refeeding. SIRT1 was among the few proteins that was increased in EGPs of food-restricted rats (2). Additionally, we showed that miR-140 and miR-22 were reduced at the EGP by food restriction, and their mutual target, SIRT1, showed a subsequent increase (1). Next, we knocked out Sirt1 expression using a collagen-type II-specific Cre-Lox system (2), and investigated its role in nutrition-induced CUG.

Depletion of SIRT1 by using the collagen-II-specific Cre-Lox system led to increased height of the proliferative zone with less organized EGP. The CKO mice were less responsive to the nutritional manipulation, and their CUG was less efficient. They remained shorter than the CTL mice who corrected the food restriction-induced growth deficit during the re-feeding period. In addition, we identified a crucial role of SIRT1 in the response of the cortical and trabecular bone fractions to nutritional manipulation. Furthermore, there was a significant effect on animal weight, with the knockout mice being heavier than their control littermates. The difference in weight was more pronounced with age. Others showed that Sirt1+/− mice presented with early onset, moderate osteoarthritis relative to their wild type littermates (8). Therefore, we hypothesized that, as our model is cartilage specific, it may also affects articular cartilage and thus mobility. This led us to monitor the locomotor activity of the knockout mice in the open field, and thereafter to perform additional tests to examine effects on anxiety and spatial memory.

2 Materials and Methods

2.1 Animals

All the procedures and experiments were according to the Arrive guidelines (https://arriveguidelines.org) and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Tel Aviv University, that follows the NIH guide for the care and use of laboratory animals (protocol approval number 01-16-052) prior to onset of the study. As most mice homozygous for the Sirt1-null allele do not survive for more than 1 month, we used the Cre-loxP system to knockout cartilage-specific Sirt1. Previous studies showed that crossing this floxed allele to a whole-body Cre-expressing mouse recapitulated the Sirt1-null phenotype (9). Sirt1-CKO (referred to throughout the manuscript as CKO) mice were generated as previously described (2); Col II-Cre-recombinase removes exon 4 of the floxed Sirt1 gene (encoding a catalytic domain of the protein) specifically in chondrocytes (2, 9). All mice were genotyped at the age of three weeks from tail snips (2). Male mice were chosen to eliminate the confounding factor of sex on linear growth. CKO mice were obtained at the expected Mendelian ratio, survived normally, and mated successfully. Sirt1 flox/flox littermate mice without the Cre transgene were used as controls (CTL). Mice were maintained under pathogen-free conditions (temperature controlled to 25 ± 1°C, humidity 50 ± 2%, 12h light/dark cycle (apart from the circadian rhythm study); lights off at 18:00h) and allowed free access to food (2018SC; Teklad Rodent Diet, Envigo, Madison, WI, USA) and tap water. The CKO and CTL mice showed no differences in size and weight at birth and no significant postnatal growth retardation. Mice were designated “young” when age was 4-8 months and “old” when age was >8-11 months.

2.2 The Open Field Test

In the open field test (OFT) mice were placed in a 47×47×51 cm arena for 30 min (CKO, n=37; CTL, n=32; Table 1). The total distance travelled and the duration of time exploring the periphery and the center of the arena were video-tracked under normal light conditions for 30 min using the EthoVision XT 11.5 software (EthoVision 3.1 Noldus Information Technology B.V., Wageningen, The Netherlands). Data was used to measure locomotor activity and to explore features indicative of greater anxiety-like behavior and decreased exploratory activity (10).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Comparison between CKO mice and CTL mice in the Open Field test (OFT) according to age group (data is presented as mean ± SD).

2.3 The Elevated Plus Maze Test

The elevated plus maze (EPM) test consists of two open arms (25 × 5 cm, with 3-mm-high ledges) and two closed arms (25 × 5 cm, with 15-cm-high transparent walls) of the same size. The arms of the same type were arranged at opposite sides; all the arms and central squares were made of white plastic plates and elevated above the floor. Each mouse was placed in the central square of the maze (5 × 5 cm), facing one of the closed arms, and was recorded for 6 min (CKO, n=15; CTL, n=15; Table 2). Time spent in the closed or open arms or in the center (seconds), the proportion of time spent in open or closed arms (% time), the distance traveled (cm), and the number of total entries into the arms were calculated automatically by the EthoVision software. EPM is a behavioral test widely employed to assess anxiety-like behaviors in mice, relying on the aversion of rodents to open spaces (11).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Comparison between CKO and CTL animals in the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM) test according to age group (data is presented as mean±SD).

2.4 Morris Water Maze Test

Spatial learning and memory were measured using the Morris water maze test (MWM) in a circular water pool (diameter – 1.2 m) filled with water (22 ± 1◦C). The water maze was divided into four quadrants according to the ‘+’ shape; and a transparent platform was placed in the center of the 1st quadrant. The mice entered the water from the middle of the 1st, 2nd, 3rd and 4th quadrants, and the escape latency was recorded, namely the time from searching the platform to climbing the platform (CKO, n=15; CTL, n=15). If a platform was not found within 60 sec, the mouse was placed on the platform for 20 sec and then removed from the water maze, and the time was recorded as 60 sec. Each mouse was trained 4 times daily, and the mean time was calculated. The experiment was conducted for 6 consecutive days. A place navigation test was performed in the first 3 days, a space exploration test on the 4th day, and a reversal phase test on the 5-6th day. Space exploration was performed when the platform in the 1st quadrant was removed, and the mice entered the water from the middle of the 2nd quadrant. The traversing times, within 60 sec, across the target quadrant (i.e., the 1st quadrant) and the swimming time were recorded. During the reversal phase, the platform was moved to another quarter of the maze and the time was recorded to reach the platform. Data were recorded using the automated EthoVision tracking system. The test assesses the ability of the animals to locate the underwater hidden platform, using surrounding visual cues; and is thus used to explore spatial memory and learning.

2.5 Voluntary Wheel Running

Mice were allowed to run freely on the open surface of a slanted plastic saucer-shaped wheel [Low-Profile Wireless Running Wheel (Med Associates, Fairfax, USA) for mouse (15.25 × 10.25 × 3.3 cm)] placed inside the mouse cage, within routine daily rhythmicity patterns in a non-stressed laboratory environment (CKO, n=15; CTL, n=14). Rotations were electronically transmitted to a USB hub (DIG-804 USB interface hub), such that the frequency and rate of running could be captured. Mice were individually housed so that accurate recordings could be made for each animal. The hourly sum of wheel revolutions (wheel spins) was collected by SOF-860-wheel manager software (Med Associates, Georgia, USA). Data were collected 6 days, 24 h a day (12, 13). The pattern of running during day (light time; L) and night (dark time; D) was analyzed and used to measure voluntary activity.

2.6 Serum Analysis of Uncarboxylated Osteocalcin (Glu- Osteocalcin)

Uncarboxylated osteocalcin (GluOC) was measured, since previous work showed that it may connect bone and brain function, especially anxiety (14). Serum samples (CKO, n=26; CTL, n=22) were analyzed using an Elisa kit (Cat # MK129; Takara, Japan) according to the manufacturers’ instructions (detection limits=0.25ng/ml).

2.7 Western Blot Analysis of SIRT1

Brains of CKO and CTL mice were homogenized in a lysis buffer (200mM HEPES, 5mM EDTA, 150mM NaCl, 1% NP-40, 0.5% Na-deoxycholate), which was supplemented with a protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche, Basel, Switzerland) in a 1:12 ratio and phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Roche, Basel, Switzerland) in a 1:12 ratio and phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Roche, Basel, Switzerland) in a 1:10 ratio. Protein concentration was determined using the Pierce BCA Protein Assay Kit (Thermo Scientific, IL, USA), and 100μg proteins were analyzed per sample by sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS)-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (PAGE), followed by western immunoblotting. Proteins were transferred to nitrocellulose membranes (GE Healthcare, NJ, USA) by the wet blotting system (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA). Nitrocellulose membranes were then incubated in TBS-T (10mM Tris-HCl, 150mM NaCl, 0.1% Tween 20) with 5% skim milk solution for one hour to block nonspecific binding, and then incubated with the primary antibody (anti Sirt1- Millipore Cat #07-131) overnight at 4°C. Membranes were washed with TBS-T, incubated with a secondary fluorescent antibody (LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE, USA) decorated with IRDye® for one hour and washed again. Alpha-Tubulin (Cat#2144 Cell Signaling) served as the reference. Quantification was performed using the Odyssey (2.1.) application Software (LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE, USA)

2.8 Statistical Analysis

2.8.1 A mixed-model repeated-measures analysis was conducted to compare the effect of group (CTL vs. CKO) on weight during the study period. This analysis enabled using all the available data from the full sample of randomized animals, without imputing missing values. The model was specified with a between-group factor (CTL vs. CKO), a within-group factor of time, and group x time interaction. For this analysis, data are expressed as estimated marginal means and standard error.

2.8.2 For the between group comparison of the behavioral test results (OFT, EPM, MWM), independent sample T-tests were conducted.

2.8.3 For the MWM test analyses, a Cox regression analysis was used to evaluate the effects of the group (CKO vs. CTL) and the animal age on the time until test success for each of the experiment days.

2.8.4 For the running wheels test, area under the curve (AUC) was calculated for each period of time and compared between the groups using Independent Samples Mann Whitney U tests (due to skewed distribution of the AUC).

2.8.5 Data were generated and analyzed with SPSS software, version 25.0 (IBM). Differences were considered statistically significant at P<0.05.

3 Results

3.1. Effect on body weight: CKO mice were generally heavier (Supplementary Figure 1), with no significant differences in food consumption (average food consumption 3.68 ± 0.04 gr/day for the CTL mice and 3.56 ± 0.3 gr/day for the CKO mice; p=0.27). At the age of 21 days, no significant differences in weight were noted, however, there was a significant difference in the change over time between the CKO and CTL mice (β=0.03, SE=0.01, P=0.006), with a greater increase in weight gain in the CKO group [CTL group: y (weight) =14.92+0.16X (time); CKO group: y=14.08+0.19X] (2). We did not notice differences between the groups in behavior when they were in their home cages, either housed in small groups or in solitary cages (2).

3.2. Open field test (OFT) - using the OFT, no differences in gait were observed, however, there were significant differences in the number of steps (i.e. distance travelled). The results are presented in Table 1 and Figure 1 the first 10 minutes are presented in Table 1A and Figures 1A–C; results of the whole test (30 min.) are presented in Table 1B and Figures 1D–F. The distance covered by the CKO mice was substantially greater than that covered by the CTL mice, in both the first 10 minutes and when calculated for the whole duration of the test (in both P<0.001) (Table 1). While the effect of age was not significant in the first 10 minutes, the distance travelled during the whole period of the test (30 min.) decreased with age, in both CKO and CTL mice. Even though the distance was reduced with increased age of the CKO mice it was still significantly greater than in the CTL group (young CKO vs young CTL; Old CKO vs Old CTL, P<0.005 for both) (Table 1). CKO mice spent more time in the periphery and less in the center compared to CTL mice (p<0.05). To examine if greater anxiety might explain these differences, we performed a test that is more specific for anxiety assessment, the elevated plus maze test.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Results of the open field test (OFT). Graphs showing, for each of the groups (CKO and CTL), correlations between the animal age and (A, D) The total distance covered, (B, E) distance travelled in peripheral zone, (C, F) distance travelled in central zone. (A–C)- 10 minutes; (D–F) 30 minutes.

3.3. The elevated plus maze (EPM)- In the EPM test (Table 2 and Figure 2), the CKO mice covered a significantly greater distance than did the CTL mice (P<0.001); the difference was significant for both younger (P=0.014) and older mice (P=0.025) (Figure 2A). The time spent in the open arms was significantly less in young CKO than in young CTL (P=0.001). However, the time was similar in the older age group (P=0.815). This was evident by less time spent in the open arms among older than younger CTL mice (P=0.035), yet no such difference by age was observed among the CKO mice (Figure 2B). The number of entries into all the sections of the maze were greater for CKO than CTL mice (P ≤ 0.05). This indicates that the CKO mice were moving about quickly, although differences from the CTL were statistically significant only in entries by younger CKO mice into the closed arms and the center (P=0.001), and in entries of older CKO mice into the center (P=0.028). Taken together, these results support the greater anxiety of CKO mice that was demonstrated in the OFT.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Results of the elevated Plus Maze. Graphs showing, for each of the groups (CKO and CTL), correlations between the animal age and (A) total distance covered (cm) or (B) distance covered in the open arms (cm).

3.4. Morris water maze. We next compared spatial memory and learning abilities of CKO to CTL mice using the Morris water maze (Figure 3 and Table 3). The study was conducted for six consecutive days. Both groups showed improved learning with time, and on the third day, all the animals (from both groups) reached the platform rapidly, with no significant differences between the groups. In the first three days an improvement was noted for both CKO and CTL mice, however, CKO mice were slower in finding the platform on the first (P=0.076) and second (P=0.014) days. Comprehensive Cox regression analysis for the first 3 days (Table 3) supported these results. No differences were observed between the CKO and CTL mice in the space exploration and the reversal phase tests (days 4-6; data not shown).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Results of the Morris water maze (MWM). Cumulative escape latency in all 3 days comparing CKO and CTL mice. Data are presented as mean ± SD, *P < 0.05.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Multivariable Cox regression analysis for the MWM tests (time to platform) during the first three days.

3.5. Free running wheels. Wheel running in the home cages was used to assess voluntary activity in contrast to other experimental platforms that we used. After mice were allowed to accustom to the free running wheels, their running activity was recorded. Both the CKO and CTL groups used the wheels mostly during the active dark period (Figures 4A–C). The young mice showed significantly greater activity than the older mice in both groups (P<0.001), both in the night and the day (Figure 4C). However, contrasting with the results of the OFT and the EPM, young CKO mice showed significantly reduced activity compared to the CTL mice (P=0.001) (Figures 4A–D). No differences in circadian period were observed (CTL 24.4 hours ± 0.88, CKO 24.0 ± 1.48; P=0.5).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Results of the free running wheels. The average sum of hourly wheel revolutions (wheel spins) of (A) young and (B) old mice old during the night cycle. (C) Hourly sum of wheel revolution according to 6 hours interval during the day and night (*p < 0.05; **p = 0.001). (D) AUC analysis of the hourly sum of wheel spins of the active period (night) presented in A and B (*p < 0.05; †< 0.01).

3.6. Osteocalcin levels: Uncarboxylated osteocalcin (GluOC) was measured, since previous work showed that it may connect bone and brain function, especially anxiety (14). Levels of GluOC tended to be higher in the young CKO mice than in the young CTL mice, but the difference in magnitude was small (CKO 16.4 ± 1.3 ng/ml, CTL 15.0 ± 0.7 ng/ml; P=0.06). Levels were significantly reduced with age in both groups; differences between the CKO and CTL mice at age 3 months or older were not statistically significant.

3.7. Levels of SIRT1 in the brains of CKO and CTL mice were visualized using a western blot analysis, as several studies in mouse embryogenesis (9, 1517), showed transient expression of Col type II mRNAs in the brain. No gross difference in the total amount of SIRT1 protein was observed. Three bands with apparent molecular weight of 100-130 kDa were observed per lane, a pattern that is commonly seen in samples taken from cytosols or whole cells, compared to a single band of 110 kDa detected in nuclear extracts (18). In extracts derived from the CTL mice, the pattern was identical to that of a control mouse (C57Bl/6, which are the most widely used genetic background for genetically modified mice). In contrast, the relative distributions of the various bands differed substantially between the CKO and CTL mice; with the intermediate band more pronounced in extracts of CKO compared to CTL (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 Results of the western blot analysis of SIRT1. Significant differences in the pattern of SIRT1 in protein extracts derived from brains of C57BL mice (background control), CTL mice, CKO mice. The relative distribution of the different forms, in extracts derived from CTL mice is identical to that of other control mice (C57BL) and different from that of CKO mice.

4 Discussion

In this study, we found that Sirt1 knockout (CKO) mice have reduced activity, increased anxiety and decreased spatial learning and memory, compared to CTL mice. In a previous study (8), we found that the transgenic CKO mice had shorter bones with less organized EGP, had increased weight, reduced bone mineralization and were less responsive to the nutritional manipulation, with less efficient CUG.

While no gross effect was found in their gait, the CKO mice covered a significantly greater distance than the CTL mice in both the OF (19) and EPM tests (20, 21), and spent relatively more time in the sheltered areas of the arena or the maze, respectively. Increased levels of anxiety usually lead to less locomotion and decreased exploratory behavior with a preference to stay close to the walls of the field; while in this case the animals showed increased activity with decreased exploratory behavior. In contrast to the increased activity in the above- mentioned tests, the use of home cage running wheels showed reduced activity. This indicates that the increased locomotion activity of the CKO mice in the OF and the EPM tests was due to increased anxiety, and the increased body weight we noted at the initiation of this research can be attributed to a lower level of activity in the home cage. Slower learning revealed by the MWM, was evident during the first two days of the tests (especially on the second day). Data from the home cage running wheels and from circadian rhythm analysis indicated that there was no significant difference between the groups in the circadian period, this is in contrast to the brain-specific SIRT1 knockout mice (BSKO) (22, 23) that showed elongated circadian period (2426).

Compared to younger mice, older CTL tended to be less cautious in the OFT, exploring more willingly the center of the arena, consistent with previous reports of mice of a similar genetic background, C57BL/6J  (27, 28); no such effect was observed for old CKO mice. On EPM, maybe due to the shorter duration of the test, there was no significant effect of age on the total distance traveled in both groups. However, old CTL spent more time in the center compared to young CTL (P=0.044), concomitant with a reduction in the % of time spent in the open arms (p=0.035), and similarly to the OFT, no such effect was noted in the CKO mice.

Taken together, our results indicate that the CKO mice entailed disruption in a central control mechanism that affects activity, anxiety and cognition. Since we used a common system of Cre/Col II, expected to be specific for cartilage, the explanation for the effect on cognitive function and behavior was elusive. An extensive review of the literature suggests several possible explanations.

One is that the knockout of SIRT1 from EGP cartilage, which affected bone mineralization and maturation (2), affected the level of osteocalcin (OC) and its un-carboxylated form (GluOC); the latter was shown to serve as a link between bone and brain function, especially anxiety (14). OC is expressed and secreted by osteoblasts, hypertrophic chondrocytes, and adipocytes (29), and is the most abundant non-collagenous protein in bone. OC was found to be involved in multiple physiological processes such as energy metabolism, adipogenesis, neuronal development, muscle growth, and male fertility (30). Despite the important role of OC in bone mineralization and calcium ion homeostasis, OC −/− mice showed only moderately increased bone mass (31). Interestingly, however, these mice showed unexpectedly increased anxiety (31). OC was later found to regulate proper brain development and function, and OC−/− mice had smaller and less developed brains. The mature OC is post-translationally modified on three glutamate residues; a process that increases the affinity of OC for hydroxyapatite crystal; thus, most secreted OC is embedded in the bone matrix. The acidic environment generated during bone resorption promotes decarboxylation of OC to GluOC, decreasing its affinity and promoting its release into the circulation. GluOC crosses the blood brain barrier, directly preventing anxiety and depressive behavior while also strengthening spatial learning and memory. Therefore, if GluOC activity would explain the findings of the current study, we would have expected to see lower levels of GluOC in the serum of CKO mice. Serum analysis showed that the levels of GluOC were significantly reduced with age in both groups as also reported by others (32), however, we found a small increase in GluOC levels in CKO mice, which tended to be significant only in young animals. This may be associated with the reduced bone mineralization in CKO mice (2). In addition, our mice mated and reproduced normally, with no difference between the groups, also supporting the finding that GluOC levels were comparable between the groups (30). Our findings contrast with a publication that reported an inverse association between GluOC and body weight (33). We therefore conclude that GluOC does not explain the increased anxiety and lower cognitive function in the CKO mice.

Another plausible explanation is that the Cre-Col II system affected brain SIRT1, as effects similar to those we see in learning, memory and anxiety were previously reported in brain-specific SIRT1 knockout mice (BSKO) (22, 23), this is in spite of the fact that the driver that we used for Cre was Col type II and not nestin (34). Indeed, a recent review summarized the off-target effect of many skeletal Cre lines, showing that most Cre lines show some level of unintended activity in other tissues (35), thus the notion that Cre lines are specific to a particular cell or tissue has been misleading. While collagen type II is usually considered a hallmark for chondrogenic differentiation, several publications on mouse embryogenesis (9, 1517), as well as a study on the Cre Col II system (36) showed transient expression of Col type II mRNAs in a number of non-chondrogenic tissues such as notochord, sclerotome, pre-chondrogenic mesenchyme, heart, brain and eye. This transient expression (16), is suggested to have an important role in the proper development of all brain structures (17). The early expression of the collagen type II gene in the neuroepithelium at 9.5 days coincides with the period of extensive morphogenesis of the neural tube (15).

In BSKO it was shown that loss of function of SIRT1 impairs synaptic plasticity, memory formation and spatial learning, probably via a microRNA-mediated mechanism (22). SIRT1 has been suggested as a component of the molecular pathways that determine the fate of neuronal progenitor cells (37). Analysis of SIRT1 in the brain has shown that Sirt1 mRNA is highly expressed in metabolically relevant sites, including, the hypothalamic arcuate, and some areas in the hindbrain (38). In addition, it was suggested that SIRT1 regulates anxiety by the de-acetylation of a transcription factor regulating the monoamine oxidase A gene in the brain, which is involved in oxidative deamination of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin (39). Furthermore, in the same model, SIRT1 was shown to be involved in regulating the circadian rhythm (25).

There is a significant overlap between the expression of SIRT1 and sites of embryonic expression of collagen type II. However, western blot analysis of brain SIRT1 did not show the expected reduction in Sirt1 protein level, unlike the reduction shown in cartilage (2); in contrast, we found a significant difference in the pattern of SIRT1 forms. This may indicate changes in post-translational modifications, such as phosphorylation, as SIRT1 is known to be phosphorylated on numerous sites (40). Phosphorylation can increase its nuclear deacetylase activity (41, 42), and may also induce SIRT1 ubiquitination and proteasomal degradation (41).

It is important to note that there are several discrepancies between our findings and those of BSKO. For example, while it was reported that hypothalamic SIRT1 is crucially important for the central regulation of food intake (43), in our mice there was no difference in food consumption.

Furthermore, CKO mice showed hypo-activity when in their home cage, while brain POMC- SIRT1 k/o showed unaltered levels of activities (44). BSKO mice were reported to be dwarf (34), had reduced GH secretion, and reduced body weight and length at 10 weeks while in our study, CKO mice did not show gross growth inhibition; in contrast, their body weight was increased. In addition, BSKO mice were reported to have elongated circadian period, while the CKO and CTL show similar circadian period (26, 4547)

Overall, it is still not clear what are the underlying causes for the differential behavior of the CKO mice. Some of the behavioral presentation of the CKO mice are reminiscent of Attention Deficient Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD), in their reduced sensory regulation. Interestingly, a recent publication on children with ADHD showed significantly lower SIRT1 levels and significantly higher metalloproteinase-9 (MMP-9) levels in the serum (48). MMP-9, an endopeptidases involved in degradation of the extracellular matrix, is negatively regulated by SIRT1; MMP-9 and SIRT1 both act in the EGP and the brain and are important for brain development, synaptic plasticity, learning and memory (49). Their presence in the serum may suggest another link between EGP and brain.

Two thirds of the genetic syndromes with short stature as a feature listed in the genetics database, POSSUM (https://www.possum.net.au/) are associated with intellectual disability. Furthermore, more children with idiopathic short stature (ISS) were shown to have a lower IQ, lower fluid and quantitative reasoning, and substantial deficits in the visual-motor skills and working memory than healthy controls (50).

One possible explanation for lower test scores in intelligence and academic achievement tests in children with ISS might be an underlying condition that has caused both short stature and cognitive impairment, such as a genetic cause, malnutrition, improper psychosocial environment, the presence of general diseases and specific hormonal deficits. The influence of the environmental factors may be intertwined, making it difficult to ascertain the role of a specific factor in humans. Despite the significant progress in unraveling the complex mechanisms required for proper growth and development, the therapeutic options for both conditions are extremely limited. It has been proposed that pharmacological activation of SIRT1 with the SIRT1 activator, resveratrol, or with synthetic SIRT1-activating compounds (i.e.SRT2104) are used to treat neuropsychiatric disorders (51). Maybe these should be also considered for use in children with growth and development disorders, as we have previously shown that SIRT1 is required for efficient CUG (2). However, one should bear in mind that these studies were performed in a mice model and conclusions from this model to developmental process in children should be drawn with utmost caution.

Several publications show that SIRT1 plays an important role also in puberty, at least in females; over expression of SIRT1 in the hypothalamus was shown to cause delayed puberty in female mice (46, 47). Thus, in the current study, if the underlying cause for the behavioral changes had been due to reduction of SIRT1 in the brain, mice would have been expected to present with precocious puberty. No gross effect differences were noted in either male or females, although we did not check testosterone in males nor vaginal opening in the females, and this may be a limitation of the study (52). However, as most tests were performed on post pubertal mice, we do not consider it to be a significant confounding factor. Another limitation concerns the western blot, in which we have no explanation as to the different pattern of brain SIRT1 protein; it would have been best if we could have compared the brains to those of BSKO, however we could not obtain these mice.

The nature of the mechanism that enables the connection between growth and behavior is still elusive; we hope that in the future we will be able to decipher the underlying cause, and maybe find a common regulator that affects growth and cognition; this may open a new area for intervention and will enable development of novel therapeutic modalities.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Tel Aviv University, that follows the NIH guide for the care and us of laboratory animals (protocol approval number 01-16-052) prior to onset of the study.

Author Contributions

BS: conducted the study, analyzed the data and prepared the tables and figures. SHH helped performing and analyzing behavioral tests. MP: funding acquisition, MY-G performed the statistical analysis. GG-Y: conceived the idea, supervised the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Prof. Mona Dvir-Ginzberg from the Hebrew University and Prof. Elazar Zelzer from The Weizmann Institute of Science, for the transgenic animals; to Prof. Noga Kronfeld- Schor and Dr. Rotem Cohen from the Tel Aviv University for their help with the circadian rhythm analysis; to Dr. Michal Taler from Felsenstein Medical Research Center and Dr. Lior Bikovski from the Myers Neuro-Behavioral Core Facility of Tel Aviv University for helpful discussion and to Ms. Cindy Cohen for editing this manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fendo.2021.756909/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Follow-up of body weight to age 88 days shows that the CKO mice are generally heavier than the CTL mice (re-published with permission obtained from Bone).

Abbreviations

CKO, collagen type II-specific Sirt1 knockout; CTL, control; Sirt1, sirtuin 1; OC, osteocalcin.

References

1. Pando R, Even-Zohar N, Shtaif B, Edry L, Shomron N, Phillip M, et al. MicroRNAs in the Growth Plate Are Responsive to Nutritional Cues: Association Between miR-140 and SIRT1. J Nutr Biochem (2012) 23(11):1474–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2011.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Shtaif B, Bar-Maisels M, Gabet Y, Hiram-Bab S, Yackobovitch-Gavan M, Phillip M, et al. Cartilage -Specific Knockout of Sirt1 Significantly Reduces Bone Quality and Catch-Up Growth Efficiency. Bone (2020) 138:115468. doi: 10.1016/j.bone.2020.115468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Yanagisawa S, Baker JR, Vuppusetty C, Koga T, Colley T, Fenwick P, et al. The Dynamic Shuttling of SIRT1 Between Cytoplasm and Nuclei in Bronchial Epithelial Cells by Single and Repeated Cigarette Smoke Exposure. PloS One (2018) 13(3):e0193921. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sin TK, Yung BY, Siu PM. Modulation of SIRT1-Foxo1 Signaling Axis by Resveratrol: Implications in Skeletal Muscle Aging and Insulin Resistance. Cell Physiol Biochem (2015) 35(2):541–52. doi: 10.1159/000369718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Blander G, Bhimavarapu A, Mammone T, Maes D, Elliston K, Reich C, et al. SIRT1 Promotes Differentiation of Normal Human Keratinocytes. J Invest Dermatol (2009) 129(1):41–9. doi: 10.1038/jid.2008.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Herranz D, Serrano M. Impact of Sirt1 on Mammalian Aging. Aging (Albany NY) (2010) 2(6):315–6. doi: 10.18632/aging.100156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kanfi Y, Peshti V, Gozlan YM, Rathaus M, Gil R, Cohen HY. Regulation of SIRT1 Protein Levels by Nutrient Availability. FEBS Lett (2008) 582(16):2417–23. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2008.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gabay O, Oppenhiemer H, Meir H, Zaal K, Sanchez C, Dvir-Ginzberg M. Increased Apoptotic Chondrocytes in Articular Cartilage From Adult Heterozygous SirT1 Mice. Ann Rheum Dis (2012) 71(4):613–6. doi: 10.1136/ard.2011.200504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cheng HL, Mostoslavsky R, Saito S, Manis JP, Gu Y, Patel P, et al. Developmental Defects and P53 Hyperacetylation in Sir2 Homolog (SIRT1)-Deficient Mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2003) 100(19):10794–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934713100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. David DJ, Samuels BA, Rainer Q, Wang JW, Marsteller D, Mendez I, et al. Neurogenesis-Dependent and -Independent Effects of Fluoxetine in an Animal Model of Anxiety/Depression. Neuron (2009) 62(4):479–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Griebel G, Holmes A. 50 Years of Hurdles and Hope in Anxiolytic Drug Discovery. Nat Rev Drug Discovery (2013) 12(9):667–87. doi: 10.1038/nrd4075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Novak CM, Burghardt PR, Levine JA. The Use of a Running Wheel to Measure Activity in Rodents: Relationship to Energy Balance, General Activity, and Reward. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36(3):1001–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Sherwin CM. Voluntary Wheel Running: A Review and Novel Interpretation. Anim Behav (1998) 56(1):11–27. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Oury F, Khrimian L, Denny CA, Gardin A, Chamouni A, Goeden N, et al. Maternal and Offspring Pools of Osteocalcin Influence Brain Development and Functions. Cell (2013) 155(1):228–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cheah KS, Lau ET, Au PK, Tam PP. Expression of the Mouse Alpha 1(II) Collagen Gene Is Not Restricted to Cartilage During Development. Development (1991) 111(4):945–53. doi: 10.1242/dev.111.4.945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ng LJ, Tam PP, Cheah KS. Preferential Expression of Alternatively Spliced mRNAs Encoding Type II Procollagen With a Cysteine-Rich Amino-Propeptide in Differentiating Cartilage and Nonchondrogenic Tissues During Early Mouse Development. Dev Biol (1993) 159(2):403–17. doi: 10.1006/dbio.1993.1251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Leung AW, Wong SY, Chan D, Tam PP, Cheah KS. Loss of Procollagen IIA From the Anterior Mesendoderm Disrupts the Development of Mouse Embryonic Forebrain. Dev Dyn (2010) 239(9):2319–29. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.22366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Tulino R, Benjamin AC, Jolinon N, Smith DL, Chini EN, Carnemolla A, et al. SIRT1 Activity Is Linked to Its Brain Region-Specific Phosphorylation and Is Impaired in Huntington’s Disease Mice. PloS One (2016) 11(1):e0145425. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Stead JD, Clinton S, Neal C, Schneider J, Jama A, Miller S, et al. Selective Breeding for Divergence in Novelty-Seeking Traits: Heritability and Enrichment in Spontaneous Anxiety-Related Behaviors. Behav Genet (2006) 36(5):697–712. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9058-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of Open:Closed Arm Entries in an Elevated Plus-Maze as a Measure of Anxiety in the Rat. J Neurosci Methods (1985) 14(3):149–67. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lister RG. The Use of a Plus-Maze to Measure Anxiety in the Mouse. Psychopharmacol (Berl) (1987) 92(2):180–5. doi: 10.1007/BF00177912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gao J, Wang WY, Mao YW, Graff J, Guan JS, Pan L, et al. A Novel Pathway Regulates Memory and Plasticity via SIRT1 and miR-134. Nature (2010) 466(7310):1105–9. doi: 10.1038/nature09271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Michan S, Li Y, Chou MM, Parrella E, Ge H, Long JM, et al. SIRT1 Is Essential for Normal Cognitive Function and Synaptic Plasticity. J Neurosci (2010) 30(29):9695–707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0027-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Belden WJ, Dunlap JC. SIRT1 Is a Circadian Deacetylase for Core Clock Components. Cell (2008) 134(2):212–4. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Asher G, Gatfield D, Stratmann M, Reinke H, Dibner C, Kreppel F, et al. SIRT1 Regulates Circadian Clock Gene Expression Through PER2 Deacetylation. Cell (2008) 134(2):317–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Chang HC, Guarente L. SIRT1 Mediates Central Circadian Control in the SCN by a Mechanism That Decays With Aging. Cell (2013) 153(7):1448–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Shoji H, Takao K, Hattori S, Miyakawa T. Age-Related Changes in Behavior in C57BL/6J Mice From Young Adulthood to Middle Age. Mol Brain (2016) 9:11. doi: 10.1186/s13041-016-0191-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Tucker LB, McCabe JT. Behavior of Male and Female C57BL/6J Mice Is More Consistent With Repeated Trials in the Elevated Zero Maze Than in the Elevated Plus Maze. Front Behav Neurosci (2017) 11:13. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lee NK, Sowa H, Hinoi E, Ferron M, Ahn JD, Confavreux C, et al. Endocrine Regulation of Energy Metabolism by the Skeleton. Cell (2007) 130(3):456–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Karsenty G, Oury F. Regulation of Male Fertility by the Bone-Derived Hormone Osteocalcin. Mol Cell Endocrinol (2014) 382(1):521–6. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2013.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ducy P, Desbois C, Boyce B, Pinero G, Story B, Dunstan C, et al. Increased Bone Formation in Osteocalcin-Deficient Mice. Nature (1996) 382(6590):448–52. doi: 10.1038/382448a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Obri A, Khrimian L, Karsenty G, Oury F. Osteocalcin in the Brain: From Embryonic Development to Age-Related Decline in Cognition. Nat Rev Endocrinol (2018) 14(3):174–82. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2017.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Foresta C, Strapazzon G, De Toni L, Gianesello L, Calcagno A, Pilon C, et al. Evidence for Osteocalcin Production by Adipose Tissue and Its Role in Human Metabolism. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2010) 95(7):3502–6. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-2557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cohen DE, Supinski AM, Bonkowski MS, Donmez G, Guarente LP. Neuronal SIRT1 Regulates Endocrine and Behavioral Responses to Calorie Restriction. Genes Dev (2009) 23(24):2812–7. doi: 10.1101/gad.1839209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Couasnay G, Madel MB, Lim J, Lee B, Elefteriou F. Sites of Cre-Recombinase Activity in Mouse Lines Targeting Skeletal Cells. J Bone Miner Res (2021) 36(9):1661–79. doi: 10.1002/jbmr.4415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sakai K, Hiripi L, Glumoff V, Brandau O, Eerola R, Vuorio E, et al. Stage-And Tissue-Specific Expression of a Col2a1-Cre Fusion Gene in Transgenic Mice. Matrix Biol (2001) 19(8):761–7. doi: 10.1016/s0945-053x(00)00122-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Prozorovski T, Schulze-Topphoff U, Glumm R, Baumgart J, Schroter F, Ninnemann O, et al. Sirt1 Contributes Critically to the Redox-Dependent Fate of Neural Progenitors. Nat Cell Biol (2008) 10(4):385–94. doi: 10.1038/ncb1700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ramadori G, Lee CE, Bookout AL, Lee S, Williams KW, Anderson J, et al. Brain SIRT1: Anatomical Distribution and Regulation by Energy Availability. J Neurosci (2008) 28(40):9989–96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3257-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Libert S, Pointer K, Bell EL, Das A, Cohen DE, Asara JM, et al. SIRT1 Activates MAO-A in the Brain to Mediate Anxiety and Exploratory Drive. Cell (2011) 147(7):1459–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sasaki T, Maier B, Koclega KD, Chruszcz M, Gluba W, Stukenberg PT, et al. Phosphorylation Regulates SIRT1 Function. PloS One (2008) 3(12):e4020. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Nasrin N, Kaushik VK, Fortier E, Wall D, Pearson KJ, de Cabo R, et al. JNK1 Phosphorylates SIRT1 and Promotes Its Enzymatic Activity. PloS One (2009) 4(12):e8414. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zschoernig B, Mahlknecht U. Carboxy-Terminal Phosphorylation of SIRT1 by Protein Kinase CK2. Biochem Biophys Res Commun (2009) 381(3):372–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2009.02.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sasaki T, Kikuchi O, Shimpuku M, Susanti VY, Yokota-Hashimoto H, Taguchi R, et al. Hypothalamic SIRT1 Prevents Age-Associated Weight Gain by Improving Leptin Sensitivity in Mice. Diabetologia (2014) 57(4):819–31. doi: 10.1007/s00125-013-3140-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ramadori G, Fujikawa T, Fukuda M, Anderson J, Morgan DA, Mostoslavsky R, et al. SIRT1 Deacetylase in POMC Neurons Is Required for Homeostatic Defenses Against Diet-Induced Obesity. Cell Metab (2010) 12(1):78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2010.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bilu C, Einat H, Zimmet P, Vishnevskia-Dai V, Kronfeld-Schor N. Beneficial Effects of Daytime High-Intensity Light Exposure on Daily Rhythms, Metabolic State and Affect. Sci Rep (2020) 10(1):19782. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76636-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Aylwin CF, Lomniczi A. Sirtuin (SIRT)-1: At the Crossroads of Puberty and Metabolism. Curr Opin Endocr Metab Res (2020) 14:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.coemr.2020.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Vazquez MJ, Toro CA, Castellano JM, Ruiz-Pino F, Roa J, Beiroa D, et al. SIRT1 Mediates Obesity- and Nutrient-Dependent Perturbation of Pubertal Timing by Epigenetically Controlling Kiss1 Expression. Nat Commun (2018) 9(1):4194. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06459-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Uzun Cicek A, Mercan Isik C, Bakir S, Ulger D, Sari SA, Bakir D, et al. Evidence Supporting the Role of Telomerase, MMP-9, and SIRT1 in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). J Neural Transm (Vienna) (2020) 127(10):1409–18. doi: 10.1007/s00702-020-02231-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Reinhard SM, Razak K, Ethell IM. A Delicate Balance: Role of MMP-9 in Brain Development and Pathophysiology of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Front Cell Neurosci (2015) 9:280. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wheeler PG, Bresnahan K, Shephard BA, Lau J, Balk EM. Short Stature and Functional Impairment: A Systematic Review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med (2004) 158(3):236–43. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.3.236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Xie J, Zhang X, Zhang L. Negative Regulation of Inflammation by SIRT1. Pharmacol Res (2013) 67(1):60–7. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2012.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McCutcheon JE, Marinelli M. Age Matters. Eur J Neurosci (2009) 29(5):997–1014. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06648.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: SIRT1, anxiety, cognition, collagen type II, osteocalcin

Citation: Shtaif B, Hornfeld SH, Yackobovitch-Gavan M, Phillip M and Gat-Yablonski G (2021) Anxiety and Cognition in Cre- Collagen Type II Sirt1 K/O Male Mice. Front. Endocrinol. 12:756909. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.756909

Received: 11 August 2021; Accepted: 01 November 2021;
Published: 19 November 2021.

Edited by:

Mariacarolina Salerno, University of Naples Federico II, Italy

Reviewed by:

Nicola Improda, University of Naples Federico II, Italy
Anna Grandone, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Shtaif, Hornfeld, Yackobovitch-Gavan, Phillip and Gat-Yablonski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Galia Gat-Yablonski, galiagy@tauex.tau.ac.il

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.