Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 28 July 2017
Sec. Perception Science

Commentary: Interaction between facial expression and color

  • Università degli Studi “G. d'Annunzio” Chieti - Pescara, Pescara, Italy

A commentary on
Interaction between facial expression and color

by Nakajima, K., Minami, T., and Nakauchi, S. (2017). Sci. Rep. 7:41019. doi: 10.1038/srep41019

Face expression recognition can be considered, among others, one of the most complex and developed perceptual skills (Haxby et al., 2000). Although this skill seems to play an important role in social communication (Johnson et al., 1991), its performance has been shown to change during the life-span (Mather and Carstensen, 2003; Di Domenico et al., 2016) and seems to be influenced by different variables such as clinical conditions (Sato et al., 2002; Altamura et al., 2016), culture (Jack et al., 2012), and facial features (Horry et al., 2015; Zebrowitz et al., 2015).

In the study, by Nakajima et al. (2017), the authors implemented two experiments where they investigated if face expression perception could be influenced by facial color and vice versa. In both experiments the authors used a set of faces created by morphing two facial expressions: fear to anger and sadness to happiness. One of the three color conditions: reddish-colored, neutral-colored, and bluish-colored was assigned to each face on the assumption that fear and sadness could be attributed to a bluish face, while anger and happiness to a reddish face (Nakajima et al., 2015). In the first experiment participants were asked to identify, as quickly and accurately as possible the facial expression while ignoring its facial color. The results of experiment 1 showed that facial color modulated the facial expression discrimination and the reaction times (RT) in the discrimination task. Specifically, reddish-colored faces enhanced anger perception, while bluish-colored faces enhanced sadness perception In the second experiment, instead, subjects were asked to identify the facial color, while ignoring its facial expression. The results of experiment 2 showed that facial color perception was significantly shifted only for the sad expression, suggesting a limited effect of facial expression on facial color perception. The results of these two experiments showed that subjects recognized the bluish-colored faces as sadder compared to the reddish- and neutral-colored ones and the reddish-colored faces as angrier than the bluish- and neutral-colored faces. Furthermore, based on the RT analysis the authors determined that the RT for reddish-colored faces were shorter compared to the natural-colored faces. All together these results brought the authors to hypothesize a link between facial color and emotion perception, supporting the existing literature that link facial color to emotion perception.

One interpretation that can be added to the findings of Nakajima et al. (2015) is that color could have enhanced the emotional content of the face stimuli by improving the performance on the recognition task that subjects were asked to perform. Emotions have been shown to improve the performance on cognitive tasks in different domains, such as memory, learning, and perception (Fairfield et al., 2013, 2015a; Mammarella et al., 2016b; Palumbo et al., 2017a,b). A well-known effect is the positive bias (Kennedy et al., 2004). It has been shown, for example, that older adults tend to prefer positive contents by remembering and recognizing better positive stimuli compared to younger adults, who seem to perform better on negative contents (Mather and Carstensen, 2003; Fairfield et al., 2015b; Mammarella et al., 2016c). In a study of Di Domenico et al. (2015), for example, it has been shown that, in a dynamic task of face expression recognition, RT are influenced by the emotional content of the facial expression suggesting a preference for emotional expressions compared to neutral expressions. Furthermore, recent studies that have investigated the influence of color on stimuli, have shown that color may carry an affective connotation able to influence emotional reactions of the participants (e.g., O'Connor, 2011; Mammarella et al., 2016a). The relation between a specific color and its evoked valence has been largely investigated suggesting that colors such as red are associated with negative emotions, while green seems to evoke positive emotions contrary to blue that is often associated with fear and sadness (e.g., Elliot and Aarts, 2011; Nakajima et al., 2015; Mammarella et al., 2016a). Taken all together, these findings can lead to the hypothesis that in the study of Nakajima et al. the color of the face could have enhanced the emotional valence of the stimuli facilitating the recognition task. Not only the authors supported the findings that attribute certain color to a specific emotion, but they also showed a reduction in the RT for reddish-colored faces on anger perception compared to natural-colored faces. Considering the sample of participants recruited by the researchers (younger adults; mean age = 23.30), a reduction in the RT for the reddish-colored faces seems to be in line with previous researches suggesting a preference for negative emotions in younger adults (Mather and Carstensen, 2003; Kennedy et al., 2004). To support the latest hypothesis, further investigations that would involve recruiting participants of different ages and races, is required. In fact, one of the variables that has not been considered by Nakajima et al., is whether the color of the faces would influence participants of different ages, or races, differently in the facial expression recognition task. Furthermore, it should be noted that the authors did not report whether they controlled or balanced the emotional intensity of the faces used in the study. In fact, stimuli of different emotional intensity may introduce a potential bias in the results, especially in the fear-to-anger pairs. Another factor that should be taken into consideration is whether or not clinical conditions of the participants, such as mood disorders like depression, may affect the results. According to previous studies (e.g., Leppänen, 2006), it has been demonstrated that participants with depression tend to interpret neutral faces as negative faces.

The study of Nakajima et al. (2017) showed how color can influence the facial expression recognition. This research, together with future studies can lead to a better understanding of how high-level perception is modulated and how variables such as physical features, emotions, and colors interact with each other to produce the final perception.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Altamura, M., Padalino, F. A., Stella, E., Balzotti, A., Bellomo, A., Palumbo, R., et al. (2016). Facial emotion recognition in bipolar disorder and healthy aging. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 204, 188–193. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., Fairfield, B., and Mammarella, N. (2016). Fighting apathy in alzheimer's dementia: abrief emotional-based intervention. Psychiatry Res. 242, 331–335. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., Mammarella, N., and Fairfield, B. (2015). Aging and emotional expressions: is there a positivity bias during dynamic emotion recognition? Front. Psychol. 6:1130. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., and Aarts, H. (2011). Perception of the color red enhances the force and velocity of motor output. Emotion 11, 445–449. doi: 10.1037/a0022599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., and Di Domenico, A. (2013). Centenarians' “holy” memory: is being positive enough? J. Genet. Psychol. 174, 42–50. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2011.636399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., and Palumbo, R. (2015a). Running with emotion: when affective content hampers working memory performance. Int. J. Psychol. 50, 161–164. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., Palumbo, R., and Di Domenico, A. (2015b). Emotional meta-memories: a review. Brain Sci. 5, 509–520. doi: 10.3390/brainsci5040509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horry, R., Cheong, W., and Brewer, N. (2015). The other-race effect in perception and recognition: Insights from the complete composite task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 508–524. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., and Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 7241–7244. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200155109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychol. Sci. 15, 208–214. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leppänen, J. M. (2006). Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 34–39. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.46411.00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016a). When green is positive and red is negative: aging and the influence of color on emotional memories. Psychol. Aging 31, 914–926. doi: 10.1037/pag0000122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016b). Noradrenergic modulation of emotional memory in aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 27, 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016c). Self-generation and positivity effects following transcranial random noise stimulation in medial prefrontal cortex: a reality monitoring task in older adults. Cortex 91, 186–196. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol. Sci. 14, 409–415. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakajima, K., Minami, T., and Nakauchi, S. (2015). Effects of facial color on the subliminal processing of fearful faces. Neuroscience 310, 472–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakajima, K., Minami, T., and Nakauchi, S. (2017). Interaction between facial expression and color. Sci. Rep. 7:41019. doi: 10.1038/srep41019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, Z. (2011). Colour psychology and colour therapy: caveat emptor. Color Res. Appl. 36, 229–334. doi: 10.1002/col.20597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbo, R., D'Ascenzo, S., Quercia, A., and Tommasi, L. (2017a). Adaptation to complex pictures: exposure to emotional valence induces assimilative aftereffects. Front. Psychol. 8:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbo, R., Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., and Di Domenico, A. (2017b). Does make-up make you feel smarter? The “lipstick effect” extended to academic achievement. Cogent Psychol. 4:1327635. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1327635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Kubota, Y., Okada, T., Murai, T., Yoshikawa, S., and Sengoku, A. (2002). Seeing happy emotion in fearful and angry faces: qualitative analysis of the facial expression recognition in a bilateral amygdala damaged patient. Cortex 38, 727–742. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G. Jr., and Palumbo, R. (2015). Ailing voters advance attractive congressional candidates. Evol. Psychol. 13, 16–28. doi: 10.1177/147470491501300102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion, face recognition, emotion recognition, colors

Citation: Palumbo R and Di Domenico A (2017) Commentary: Interaction between facial expression and color. Front. Neurosci. 11:435. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00435

Received: 12 June 2017; Accepted: 14 July 2017;
Published: 28 July 2017.

Edited by:

Francesca Gasparini, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Xiaoyan Liao, Southern Medical University, China
Francisco Manuel Costela, Schepens Eye Research Institute, United States

Copyright © 2017 Palumbo and Di Domenico. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rocco Palumbo, roccopalumbo@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.