Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 17 January 2020
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Imaging Methods Editor’s Pick 2021 View all 31 articles

Differential tDCS and tACS Effects on Working Memory-Related Neural Activity and Resting-State Connectivity

  • 1Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Institute of Biomedical Research August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Magnetic Resonance Image Core Facility, Institute of Biomedical Research August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Neuroradiology Section, Radiology Service, Centre de Diagnòstic per la Imatge, Barcelona, Spain
  • 5Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors, Dortmund, Germany
  • 6Hinda and Arthur Marcus Institute for Aging Research, Hebrew SeniorLife, Boston, MA, United States
  • 7Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 8Guttmann Brain Health Institute, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació Guttmann, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Bellaterra, Spain
  • 9Department of Neurology, University Medical Hospital Bergmannsheil, Bochum, Germany

Transcranial direct and alternating current stimulation (tDCS and tACS, respectively) entail capability to modulate human brain dynamics and cognition. However, the comparability of these approaches at the level of large-scale functional networks has not been thoroughly investigated. In this study, 44 subjects were randomly assigned to receive sham (N = 15), tDCS (N = 15), or tACS (N = 14). The first electrode (anode in tDCS) was positioned over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the target area, and the second electrode (cathode in tDCS) was placed over the right supraorbital region. tDCS was delivered with a constant current of 2 mA. tACS was fixed to 2 mA peak-to-peak with 6 Hz frequency. Stimulation was applied concurrently with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) acquisitions, both at rest and during the performance of a verbal working memory (WM) task. After stimulation, subjects repeated the fMRI WM task. Our results indicated that at rest, tDCS increased functional connectivity particularly within the default-mode network (DMN), while tACS decreased it. When comparing both fMRI WM tasks, it was observed that tDCS displayed decreased brain activity post-stimulation as compared to online. Conversely, tACS effects were driven by neural increases online as compared to post-stimulation. Interestingly, both effects primarily occurred within DMN-related areas. Regarding the differences in each fMRI WM task, during the online fMRI WM task, tACS engaged distributed neural resources which did not overlap with the WM-dependent activity pattern, but with some posterior DMN regions. In contrast, during the post-stimulation fMRI WM task, tDCS strengthened prefrontal DMN deactivations, being these activity reductions associated with faster responses. Furthermore, it was observed that tDCS neural responses presented certain consistency across distinct fMRI modalities, while tACS did not. In sum, tDCS and tACS modulate fMRI-derived network dynamics differently. However, both effects seem to focus on DMN regions and the WM network-DMN shift, which are highly affected in aging and disease. Thus, albeit exploratory and needing further replication with larger samples, our results might provide a refined understanding of how the DMN functioning can be externally modulated through commonly used non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, which may be of eventual clinical relevance.

Introduction

Working memory (WM) provides temporary storage and manipulation of information required for a variety of complex cognitive tasks (Baddeley, 1992, 2010). WM capacity plays a central role in daily life activities and is predictive for a wide-range of higher-level cognitive measures (Johnson et al., 2013; Unsworth et al., 2014). Impairments in WM entail functionally disabling symptoms in advanced age (Park et al., 2002; Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Anderson and Craik, 2017) and in several neuropsychiatric conditions (Lee and Park, 2005; Nakao et al., 2009).

The WM network (WMN) includes a fronto-parietal loop (Owen et al., 2005), where the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is of particular relevance (Curtis and D’Esposito, 2003; Barbey et al., 2013). This fronto-parietal circuit shows a negative correlation with the default-mode network (DMN; Fox et al., 2005; Buckner et al., 2008; Raichle, 2015). The DMN has been shown to be consistently activated during rest, while its nodes are inhibited during externally oriented tasks (Fox et al., 2005; Pfefferbaum et al., 2011). Thus, the brain may shift between two modes of information processing, one that puts the attentional focus on external stimuli and another one that relates to internally directed processing (Buckner et al., 2008). At the electrophysiological level, it has been shown that WM processes are mediated by synchronous firing of neural populations at distinct frequencies as well as via cross-frequency coupling (Sarnthein et al., 1998; Howard et al., 2003; Sauseng et al., 2005; Jensen and Colgin, 2007; Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014). A large body of literature indicates that the coupling of theta and gamma oscillations mediates communication within and between brain networks in general and during WM tasks in particular, possibly accounting for WM processing and capacity demands (for a review see Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Hanslmayr et al., 2019).

Despite the fact that neuroimaging and neurophysiological investigations have been providing relevant data on the anatomo-functional correlates of WMN and its anticorrelated systems (Nee et al., 2013; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014; Eriksson et al., 2015; Raichle, 2015), the cognitive benefits derived from interventional approaches aimed to improve WM functioning have been limited. Notwithstanding, new methodologies, such as transcranial electrical stimulation (tES), have recently shown potential to enhance WM performance by targeting its critical network hubs, such as the dlPFC (Fregni et al., 2005; Ohn et al., 2008; Zaehle et al., 2011; Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014; Meiron and Lavidor, 2014; Hoy et al., 2015; Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Dedoncker et al., 2016). Nevertheless, and despite this promising developments, data is still inconclusive, particularly in healthy populations (Tremblay et al., 2014; Hsu et al., 2015; Nilsson et al., 2015; Mancuso et al., 2016; Hill et al., 2017; Medina and Cason, 2017; Imburgio and Orr, 2018).

Among tES techniques, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) are the most commonly used (Polanía et al., 2018). tDCS delivers weak tonic currents to the scalp. During tDCS, neural membrane potentials are depolarized under the anode, leading to an increase in cortical excitability, while neural membrane hyperpolarization develops under the cathode, thereby diminishing cortical excitability (Purpura and McMurtry, 1965; Nitsche and Paulus, 2000; Nitsche et al., 2008). On the other hand, tACS applies a sinusoidal current to the scalp at specific frequencies, exerting an exogenous modulation of ongoing brain oscillations (Zaghi et al., 2010a; Ali et al., 2013; Antal and Paulus, 2013; Herrmann et al., 2013; Reato et al., 2013; Antal and Herrmann, 2016; Moisa et al., 2016). Beyond their immediate impact, both techniques display after-effects that can outlast the period of stimulation, probably due to their capability to induce neuroplasticity-like processes (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001; Liebetanz et al., 2002; Nitsche et al., 2003; Monte-Silva et al., 2013; Vossen et al., 2015; Kasten et al., 2016; Wischnewski et al., 2019).

In this context, only two studies have explored the differential impact of tDCS and tACS on WM performance (Hoy et al., 2015; Röhner et al., 2018), indicating a more relevant effect of tACS as compared to tDCS. However, the physiological underpinnings of those protocols over large-scale neural systems supporting the WM function remain understudied. Further, to our knowledge, a direct comparison of the effects of these different stimulation protocols at the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)-derived network level has not been so far investigated. Since aging and various neuropsychiatric disorders show alterations in WM circuits and may benefit from their modulation, a better insight on tES impact on the brain’s WMN and its linked neural systems, such as the DMN, would likely have clinical translational relevance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-four healthy young subjects [age mean ± standard deviation (SD), 25.25 ± 4.22 years; age range, 19–37 years; 20 females; years of education mean ± SD, 21.11 ± 3.40 years; 36 right-handed] naive to tES were recruited from the general population and provided informed consent to participate in this study, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964, last revision 2013). All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB 00003099). None of the participants reported a diagnosis of a neurological or psychiatric disorder. For all participants, MRI images were examined by a senior neuroradiologist for any clinically significant pathology (none found).

Experimental Design

The present study was conducted in a randomized between-subjects placebo-controlled design. Online effects of prefrontal tDCS and tACS on resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) were assessed. Furthermore, the online and post-stimulation impact of these intervention protocols on WM-related neural activity and performance was explored, using a similar experimental setting as described elsewhere (i.e., Meiron and Lavidor, 2014; Brauer et al., 2018). Participants were randomly assigned to receive sham stimulation (N = 15), anodal tDCS (N = 15) or theta tACS (N = 14; Figure 1A) over the left dlPFC (l-dlPFC; Figure 1B; see also see section “Transcranial Electrical Stimulation (tES) Parameters”). A simple randomization procedure was used (Altman and Bland, 1999; Kang et al., 2008).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study protocol. (A) Assignment of participants to one of the experimental groups. (B) Stimulation montage for all groups, with the first electrode (anode for tDCS) centered over the F3 (in red) and the second electrode (cathode for tDCS) placed over the FP2 (in blue) in a 10–10 system map. (C) Timeline of the procedures accomplished before, during and after the tES-MRI protocol. tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; tES, transcranial electrical stimulation; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; rs-fMRI, resting-state functional MRI; hr-3D, high-resolution three-dimensional.

First, a brief cognitive assessment was conducted to obtain an estimation of the intelligence quotient (IQ) of the participants, using the vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV). Subsequently and within the same experimental day, tES was applied inside an MRI scanner. Here, an rs-fMRI sequence (∼8 min) was acquired before subjects underwent two sequential task-based fMRI (tb-fMRI) acquisitions while performing two identical verbal n-back tasks (∼11 min each one; Sala-Llonch et al., 2012). The rs-fMRI and the first tb-fMRI datasets were acquired during stimulation (i.e., online fMRI n-back task) and the second tb-fMRI sequence after stimulation delivery was turned off (i.e., post-stimulation fMRI n-back task). A high-resolution three-dimensional (hr-3D) structural image (∼8 min) was acquired at the end of the MRI session for co-registration purposes. A questionnaire of tES-related adverse events was administered at the end of the experimental session [adapted from Brunoni et al., 2011; Figure 1C; see also Supplementary Material (SM)].

Transcranial Electrical Stimulation (tES) Parameters

Stimulation was delivered using a battery-driven MRI-compatible DC-Stimulator Plus (neuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany) and was transferred by two MRI-compatible conductive rubber electrodes (7 cm × 5 cm) positioned in a room adjacent to the MRI scanner. The same montage was used in all groups, as applied in similar recent comparative studies (Lang et al., 2019). According to the international 10–10 system of measurement, the first electrode (anode in tDCS) was positioned over the F3 (l-dlPFC) and the second electrode (cathode in tDCS) was placed over the FP2 (right supraorbital area). This is one of the standard montages frequently employed to stimulate the l-dlPFC (Fregni et al., 2005; Nitsche et al., 2008; Ohn et al., 2008), the target area. In all groups, the current was initially increased and finally decreased in a ramp-like fashion of 15 s. In the sham condition, the current delivery was terminated after 30 s of stimulation with no further blinding processes. In the real stimulation groups, the current was supplied during 20 min, which covered the rs-fMRI acquisition and the first tb-fMRI sequence. tDCS was delivered with a constant current of 2 mA. tACS was fixed to 2 mA peak-to-peak in a 6 Hz frequency. We selected 6 Hz as this frequency has been widely used in recent tACS WM investigations (i.e., Polanía et al., 2012; Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Violante et al., 2017; Brauer et al., 2018; Röhner et al., 2018; Lang et al., 2019). All stimulation parameters adhered to safety criteria guidelines (Zaghi et al., 2010b; Fertonani et al., 2015; Bikson et al., 2016; Woods et al., 2016; Matsumoto and Ugawa, 2017; see SM for more details).

N-Back Task

Subjects performed a verbal n-back task, a commonly used paradigm to investigate WM in fMRI (Owen et al., 2005). The n-back task had different levels of memory load (from 1 to 3 letters to be retained) and a basic level of target stimulus identification that were randomly presented during the two consecutive tasks achieved inside the MRI scan (Sala-Llonch et al., 2012; see SM for more information).

MRI Acquisition

All participants were scanned with a Siemens Magnetom Trio Tim Syngo 3 Tesla system using an 8-channel head coil at the Magnetic Resonance Image Core Facility (IDIBAPS) at the Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain. The imaging sequences were acquired with the following parameters. First, a rs-fMRI dataset (T2-weighted GE-EPI sequence; interleaved acquisition; repetition time [TR] = 2,700 ms; echo time [TE] = 30 ms; 40 slices per volume; slice thickness = 3.0 mm; interslice gap = 15%; voxel size = 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm; field of view [FOV] = 216 mm; 178 volumes) was acquired. Later, two identical fMRI n-back task datasets (T2weighted EPI scans; interleaved acquisition; TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 28 ms; 34 slices per volume; slice thickness = 3.5 mm; interslice gap = 15%; voxel size = 3.5 mm × 3.5 mm × 3.5 mm; FOV = 238 mm; 336 volumes) were acquired, one during stimulation and another one after stimulation cessation. Lastly, a hr-3D structural dataset (T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo [T1-weighted MPRAGE]; sagittal plane acquisition; TR = 2,300 ms; TE = 2.98 ms; inversion time [IT] = 900 ms; slice thickness = 1.0 mm; voxel size = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm; FOV = 256 mm; 240 slices) was acquired.

Image Analyses

The FMRIB Software Library (FSL; version 6.001) and the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI2) were used for preprocessing and analyzing neuroimaging data. Rs- and tb-fMRI preprocessing pipelines and head movement considerations are described in SM.

Functional Connectivity Analyses

Resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) analyses were performed using a seed-to-seed approach, following previous procedures in our group (i.e., Abellaneda-Pérez et al., 2019). Firstly, the concatenated fMRI dataset containing all rs-fMRI acquisitions from the entire sample was decomposed through independent component analysis (ICA) into 15 components using the Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC; version 3.15) algorithm, part of the FSL (Smith et al., 2004; Beckmann et al., 2005; Jenkinson et al., 2012). The components related to the archetypical resting-state network, namely the DMN (Fox et al., 2005; Buckner et al., 2008; Raichle, 2015), along with the WM-related systems, the left and right fronto-parietal networks (lFPN and rFPN, respectively), were selected in a similar manner as described in previous reports from our group (i.e., Sala-Llonch et al., 2012). Furthermore, the executive-control network (ECN) was additionally considered due to its prefrontal nodes and its known relevance in cognitive functions (Smith et al., 2009). Moreover, two components that do not include the l-dlPFC and are not related to cognitive processing were used as control networks, namely the sensorimotor and visual-medial networks (SMN and VMN, respectively), as has been done in previous work of our group (i.e., Peña-Gómez et al., 2012). All components were identified using spatial correlations against previously defined maps (Smith et al., 2009). Secondly, the main regions of interest (ROIs) were selected based on the peak voxels of each network (Figure 2 and Table 1). Next, spherical seeds with a 6-mm radius were placed over the identified regions, and ROI-specific time-series from the preprocessed and regressed data were extracted. Finally, to obtain an rs-FC measure for each seed-to-seed coupling in each subject, obtained ROI time-series were correlated with each other within every network using Pearson’s correlation coefficients.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Selected networks and their respective ROIs location. (A) Cognitive networks with their corresponding ROIs in red for the DMN, in light blue for the lFPN, in dark blue for the rFPN, and in green for the ECN. (B) Control networks with their corresponding ROIs in purple for the SMN and in orange for the VMN. DMN, default-mode network; lFPN, left fronto-parietal network; rFPN, right fronto-parietal network; ECN, executive-control network; SMN, sensorimotor network; VMN, visual-medial network. For ROI abbreviations see Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Selected networks and their respective ROIs with associated coordinates in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) system.

N-Back fMRI Data

Tb-fMRI data were analyzed with the FEAT-FSL software (Smith et al., 2004). At the first-level analysis, data were fit into a general linear model (GLM) containing the task time-series with a gamma convolution of the hemodynamic response function (Woolrich et al., 2001). In this GLM, four regressors and their first temporal derivatives were modeled: 0-back, 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back. We defined three contrasts of interest combining the distinct loads, as the difference of brain activity between 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back and the lowest load (0-back), namely: (1) lowest WM load: 1 > 0-back; (2) intermediate WM load: 2 > 0-back; and (3) highest WM load: 3 > 0-back. The results of the first-level analysis were further fit into higher-level or group-level statistics, performed using the FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME; Woolrich et al., 2004). We conducted GLM matrices modeling the different tES time-points (online and post-stimulation) and experimental groups (sham, tDCS, and tACS). Using this second-level GLM and the appropriate contrasts, we evaluated: (1) the group-mean activity maps of the three selected contrasts of interest in the first-level analysis (1 > 0-back, 2 > 0-back, and 3 > 0-back), in order to explore the WM-related neural patterns for each one in every tES time-point; (2) the interactions between tES time-point as a within-subject factor and group as a between-subject factor; (3) the patterns of time-related change within each group, as pairwise paired-samples t-tests; and (4) the group differences in each tES time-point separately, exploring neural changes in the following contrasts: tDCS vs. sham, tACS vs. sham and tDCS vs. tACS, as pairwise independent-samples t-tests. These analyses were performed voxel-wise and the statistical significance of the resulting maps was set at p ≤ 0.05 and z ≥ 2.3 (cluster-wise corrected).

Statistical Analyses

Non-imaging data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (IBM, Corp., Released 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Armonk, NY, United States: IBM, Corp.) and MATLAB (Version R2019a, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States). To evaluate differences between groups (sham, tDCS, and tACS) in seed-to-seed rs-FC connections, a one-factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted, and all post hoc pairwise comparisons were subjected to Bonferroni correction. This statistical procedure was also used when considering tES-related adverse events. To evaluate differences in cognitive performance, a univariate ANOVA was conducted with tES time-point (online and post-stimulation) as a within-subject factor and group (sham, tDCS, and tACS) as a between-subject factor. Following this ANOVA, if there were significant interactions, two pairwise analyses were conducted. First, paired-samples t-tests were conducted to assess differences in performance over time in each group. Second, independent-samples t-tests were conducted to compare performance between groups for each tES time-point separately. Further, to obtain summary statistics, we extracted the mean values of the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal from the fMRI clusters derived from the significant neuroimaging results. These data were used to plot the fMRI findings and to corroborate the obtained results. Moreover, these data were used to associate the fMRI BOLD signal with cognitive performance estimates and rs-FC data using Pearson’s correlations. All non-imaging statistical analyses were two-tailed and α was set at 0.05 (see SM for further details).

Results

Demographics and N-Back Task Performance

No differences in age, gender, years of education, laterality and premorbid intelligence were found between groups (all p values > 0.05; see Table 2). Behaviorally, we did not observe any significant difference in n-back task performance between tES time-points and experimental groups (Supplementary Figure S1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographics and neuropsychological data.

Effects of tDCS and tACS on rs-FC

Rs-FC was differentially modulated with respect to the specific tES protocol. An interaction between groups revealed different connectivity of distinct couplings within the DMN and the ECN. Comparing the real tES groups with sham we observed higher rs-FC in the tDCS group and lower connectivity in the tACS group. Contrasting across the two real tES groups, we observed higher rs-FC in the tDCS group as compared to the tACS group (Figure 3 and Table 3). No differences between groups were found for left and right FPNs. Control networks also remained unmodified.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Seed-to-seed statistically significant results within the DMN (in red) and the ECN (in green) for: (A) interaction between groups, (B) tDCS > sham, (C) tACS < sham, and (D) tDCS > tACS. tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation. For ROI abbreviations see Table 1.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Seed-to-seed connections with statistically significant interactions and subsequent significant pairwise post hoc analyses within the DMN and the ECN.

Effects of tDCS and tACS on WM-Related Neural Activity

An interaction between tES time-points and experimental groups was found in the precuneus (PCU) cortex and the posterior cingulate gyrus in the 2 > 0-back contrast (Supplementary Figure S2A). Further, another interaction was observed in the lateral occipital and the angular gyrus in the 3 > 0-back contrast (Supplementary Figure S2B). The group-mean activity maps obtained from each contrast (1 > 0-back, 2 > 0-back, and 3 > 0-back) in the sham group (considered as the ‘reference’ WM brain patterns) are additionally displayed in SM (Supplementary Figure S3). These maps show the characteristic fronto-parietal WM patterns and the expected task-deactivations, mainly placed within DMN-related areas, like the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the PCU.

Differential Effects Across tES Time-Points

The active stimulation groups showed differential changes in brain activity between both tES time-points. No time-related fMRI variances were found in sham participants. Specifically, neural modulations in the tDCS group were driven by clear decreases in brain activity post-stimulation as compared to online. These modulations were noticeable in view of the slight neural differences across time in the sham group within the observed significant areas. Anatomically, these changes were found in posterior midline structures during the lowest WM load and within medial frontal areas during the highest WM load (Figure 4A). Conversely, tACS effects appeared to be directed by manifest neural increases and robust lower deactivations online as compared to post-stimulation, in view of the minor brain activity changes in the sham group across time in the detected significant regions. At the anatomical level, increased neural activity was mainly evident during the intermediate WM load in the PCU, frontal and temporal regions, and the occipital lobe. Reduced deactivations were detected in the highest WM load in the medial frontal, posterior midline structures, and the left inferior parietal lobe (lIPL; Figure 4B). It is worth noticing that numerous of the detected fMRI clusters correspond to the main nodes of the DMN (i.e., those areas entailing typical task-deactivation processes).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison between the online and post-stimulation fMRI n-back tasks in each group. (A) Online vs. post-stimulation results in the tDCS group. (B) Online vs. post-stimulation results in the tACS group. Top: statistically significant fMRI activity maps on the standard MNI for each contrast of interest. Results are shown in red-yellow for higher activations (or lower deactivations) online as compared to post-stimulation. Down: plots of mean BOLD signal values at the fMRI clusters considering sham, tDCS and tACS where significant differences between both fMRI n-back tasks were found. Data are presented as mean with standard error of the mean (SEM). tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent.

Differential Online Effects

During the online fMRI n-back task performance, participants who received tACS exhibited larger brain activity compared to those who received sham in the three contrasts of interest. At the lowest and intermediate WM loads, more brain activity was found bilaterally in frontal regions, within the PCU, and in numerous widespread cortical (i.e., occipital) and subcortical (i.e., parahippocampal) regions. For the highest WM load, greater brain activity was only found bilaterally within the frontal pole (Figure 5A). Furthermore, the tACS group exhibited larger brain activity in frontal areas, the PCU, and within the lIPL, when compared to the tDCS group, in the intermediate and highest WM loads (Figure 5B). No significant differences were found when comparing tDCS with sham at this tES time-point.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Online fMRI n-back task results. (A) tACS vs. sham results are shown in red-yellow. (B) tDCS vs. tACS results are shown in green. Left: statistically significant fMRI activity maps on the standard MNI for each contrast of interest and group comparison. Right: plots of mean BOLD signal values at the fMRI clusters considering sham, tDCS and tACS where significant differences between groups were found. Data are presented as mean with SEM. tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent.

Differential Post-stimulation Effects

During the post-stimulation fMRI n-back task performance, differences in brain activity between tDCS and sham were detected. Specifically, in the intermediate WM load, the subcallosal cortex was found to be significantly more active in the tDCS than in the sham group. For the highest WM load, more fMRI signal was detected in a minor area of the right post-central gyrus in the tDCS group as compared to sham. Additionally, for the same WM load condition, the tDCS group showed significant lower brain activity within medial frontal structures when compared to sham (Figure 6A). When comparing tDCS vs. tACS, less brain activity was found in tDCS at the lowest WM load in distinct cortical (i.e., frontal and parietal) and subcortical (i.e., thalamic) areas. At the highest WM load, subjects who received tDCS showed lower activity as compared to tACS in right frontal areas only (Figure 6B). No significant differences were found when comparing tACS with sham at this tES time-point.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Post-stimulation fMRI n-back task results. (A) tDCS vs. sham results are shown in red-yellow for higher activations and blue-light blue for lower activations. (B) tDCS vs. tACS results are shown in green. Left and up: statistically significant fMRI activity maps on the standard MNI for each contrast of interest and group comparison. Right and below: plots of mean BOLD signal values at the fMRI clusters considering sham, tDCS and tACS where significant differences between groups were found. Data are presented as mean with SEM. tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent.

Associations Between WM-Related Neural Activity and Performance

At the highest WM load, those tDCS subjects with a greater reduction in brain activity post-stimulation compared to online within the fMRI cluster where tDCS showed less activity than sham post-stimulation (Figure 6A, blue-light blue fMRI cluster) showed faster reaction time (RT) post-stimulation as compared to online performance (r = 0.540, p = 0.038; Figure 7). Faster responses were not associated with lower accuracy (i.e., d′; p > 0.05). No significant associations were detected between brain activity and performance in the tACS group (all p values > 0.05).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Scatter plot showing the relationship at the highest WM load between tDCS-induced changes in BOLD signal post-stimulation when compared to online within the fMRI cluster where tDCS showed less activity than sham after stimulation (Figure 6A, blue-light blue fMRI cluster) and RT post-stimulation as compared to online. Data are presented with z scores. tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent; Diff, difference; RT, reaction time.

tES-Related Individual Variability

Significant associations between rs-FC and brain activity were detected in the tDCS group. More precisely, those subjects of the tDCS group who exhibited higher mPFC-lIPL connectivity within the DMN also showed less BOLD signal at the highest WM load in the mPFC post-stimulation (Figure 6A; blue-light blue fMRI cluster; r = −0.664, p = 0.007; Figure 8A). Similarly, those subjects in the tDCS group displaying greater lIPL-left cerebellum connectivity within the DMN also showed a higher reduction in brain activity within the PCU at the lowest WM load post-stimulation as compared to online (Figure 4A; red-yellow fMRI cluster; r = −0.643, p = 0.010; Figure 8B). No significant relationships were observed between rs-FC and brain activity in the tACS group (all p values > 0.05).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Scatter plots showing the relationships in the tDCS group between (A) mPFC-lIPL rs-FC and post-stimulation 3 > 0-back BOLD signal (in the tDCS < sham fMRI cluster) and (B) lIPL-lCer rs-FC and 1 > 0-back difference in BOLD signal (in the post-stimulation – online fMRI cluster). Data are presented with z scores. tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent; Diff, difference; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; lIPL, left inferior parietal lobule; rs-FC, resting-state functional connectivity; lCer, left cerebellum.

tES-Related Adverse Events

An interaction between experimental groups was found as regards tingling (H = 6.982, p = 0.030). Pairwise post hoc analyses revealed more tingling estimates in the tACS group as compared to sham (p = 0.025). Another interaction was found regarding phosphenes occurrence (χ2 = 11.360, p = 0.003). Pairwise comparisons revealed a higher phosphenes occurrence in the tACS group in comparison to the tDCS group (p = 0.002, Fisher’s exact test; see Supplementary Table S1).

Discussion

Our study employed fMRI connectivity and activity analyses to investigate, in an exploratory manner, the effects of two commonly used tES protocols over the WMN and its major anticorrelated circuit, the DMN. Albeit the usage of the same neurophysiological readout (i.e., fMRI) is a critical condition to be able to directly compare the effects of distinct stimulation modalities, this approach has seldom been used. To the best of our knowledge, there have only been two studies that compared the effects of tDCS and tACS on WM performance (Hoy et al., 2015; Röhner et al., 2018). However, these studies did not explore the distinct impact of those protocols on fMRI network dynamics. Our main results showed that using our tES montage: (1) prefrontal tDCS is capable of increasing rs-FC, mainly within the DMN, while prefrontal theta tACS appears to disrupt rs-fMRI systems. (2) Comparing both fMRI WM tasks, tDCS seem to exert its neural effects through a reduction on neural activity after stimulation, whilst tACS increase neural activity during stimulation, occurring both modulations mainly within DMN areas. (3) In the online fMRI WM task, tACS induced distributed neural activity which was not accommodated within the WMN, but overlapping certain posterior DMN areas. (4) In the post-stimulation fMRI WM task, tDCS strengthened expected medial prefrontal DMN deactivations, which correlated with faster responses. (5) Lastly, we observed that tDCS showed certain consistency on their neural effects across rs- and tb-fMRI, which was not the case for tACS.

Effects of tDCS and tACS on rs-FC

We observed higher connectivity within the DMN in the tDCS group. This is in line with a previous investigation reporting rs-FC increases in this system after prefrontal tDCS (Keeser et al., 2011). This connectivity increase has been proposed to reflect augmented resources and higher readiness to facilitate cognition (Keeser et al., 2011). On the contrary, tACS appeared to reduce DMN and ECN rs-FC. Although relevant data about the tACS effects on rs-fMRI dynamics have recently been reported using distinct tES montages (i.e., Cabral-Calderin et al., 2016; Vosskuhl et al., 2016; Bächinger et al., 2017; Weinrich et al., 2017), prefrontal theta tACS impact on resting-state connectivity remains largely uninvestigated. The observed reduction in rs-FC for the tACS group could be explained as a disruption of the endogenous theta rhythm of the stimulated area. The theta frequency band mediates long-range connections in the brain through phase synchrony and cross-frequency coupling (for a review see Lisman and Jensen, 2013). Therefore, it is plausible that tACS at 6 Hz introduced an exogenous rhythm that disrupted phase synchrony on the DMN and ECN and thus decreased rs-FC.

Effects of tDCS and tACS on WM-Related Neural Activity

Differential Effects Across tES Time-Points

In the present investigation, tDCS effects were primarily driven by reductions in neural activity after stimulation (Figure 4A). This is in line with the notion that some of the tDCS effects might take place after the cessation of stimulation rather than during stimulation, as observed in the motor cortex (Santarnecchi et al., 2014). On the contrary, tACS effects appeared to be particularly driven by changes during stimulation (Figure 4B). This is coherent with the outcome obtained using a similar protocol to ours, where a significant tACS effect on WM accuracy was only observed online, but not post-stimulation (Meiron and Lavidor, 2014). At the neuroanatomical level, the effects of both tES protocols on fMRI brain activity were mainly observed within relevant DMN areas, such as the mPFC and the PCU, the main hubs of this network (Buckner et al., 2008).

Differential Online Effects

In line with the abovementioned results, when exploring the neural modulations for each tES time-point, tACS effects were revealed to be different as compared to sham only in the online WM task (Figure 5A). Furthermore, neural activity changes were found in a cognitive load-dependent manner, were tACS engaged distributed neural resources in low load conditions, while at the highest demanding load, activity increases were only evident bifrontally. Notwithstanding, while the spatial distribution of the neuroimaging results did not completely overlap with the WM-dependent neural activity pattern, it plainly did meet some areas belonging to the DMN, such as the PCU. These tACS effects over the DMN were even more evident compared to tDCS (Figure 5B), where activity modulations in other DMN core areas also emerged, such as the lIPL. Altogether, prefrontal theta tACS during stimulation seemed to disrupt the characteristic WMN-DMN shift during externally oriented tasks (i.e., via a poorer DMN suppression, particularly within its posterior nodes). However, since no major significant behavioral differences were found between groups, this brain perturbation-like process (Paus, 2005) could have triggered compensatory overactivations in widespread regions regardless the WM-related areas, probably to allow a suitable response to the cognitive load required, and thus maintain cognition at an adequate level (i.e., such as the other experimental groups; see Supplementary Figure S1).

Differential Post-stimulation Effects

On the post-stimulation task, when compared to sham, we only detected brain activity changes in the tDCS group (Figure 6A). The most noteworthy outcome was a tDCS-induced activity reduction in a typically deactivated area corresponding to the anterior DMN node, namely the mPFC, in the highest WM load. It is worth noting that activity decreases are proportional to task difficulty (McKiernan et al., 2003). However, tDCS appeared to strengthen this characteristic medial prefrontal deactivation pattern. More interestingly, greater decreases in brain activity in this area were correlated with shorter response times (Figure 7). These results might be in line with the notion that tDCS increases neural efficiency on brain dynamics (Holland et al., 2011; Meinzer et al., 2012). In this case, neural efficiency may be operated via the inhibition of areas that need to be suppressed instead of an activity decrease in hyperactivated areas, as seen in normal and pathological aging (Meinzer et al., 2013, 2015). Thus, tDCS might be reinforcing the activation/deactivation WMN-DMN shift during the accomplishment of cognitive tasks, which is then associated with better performance.

Cognitive Performance

Despite the brain-behavioral associations previously mentioned (see section “Differential Post-stimulation Effects”), we were not able to detect any cognitive effects between tES time-points and experimental groups. In this sense, although some studies with the same tDCS montage have reported significant improvements on WM performance both online and post-stimulation (Fregni et al., 2005; Ohn et al., 2008), more recent investigations have revealed that a single session of prefrontal tDCS does not (or at best, modestly) induce cognitive improvements in WM performance in healthy subjects (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014; Nilsson et al., 2015; Mancuso et al., 2016; Hill et al., 2017; Medina and Cason, 2017; Imburgio and Orr, 2018). This may be due to ceiling effects in healthy populations (Hsu et al., 2015). Regarding the effects of our tACS montage on cognitive performance, certain improvements in fluid intelligence have been previously observed (Pahor and Jaušovec, 2014), but not on WM (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014). However, using slightly different tACS montages, either targeting prefrontal regions (Meiron and Lavidor, 2014; Alekseichuk et al., 2016) or the fronto-parietal circuit (Polanía et al., 2012; Violante et al., 2017), WM improvements have been reported. Yet, data in these regards is likewise inconsistent to date (Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018).

tES-Related Individual Variability

Notable inter- and intra-individual variability has been seen in response to distinct non-invasive brain stimulation protocols (i.e., Hamada et al., 2013; López-Alonso et al., 2014; Martin-Trias et al., 2018). More specifically, López-Alonso et al. (2014) observed that only 45% of subjects responded as expected when the motor cortex was targeted with anodal tDCS. However, whether individual responses to different stimulation techniques are consistent across distinct fMRI modalities has not yet been explored. Our study is the first to investigate this issue. Our results indicate that in the tDCS group, those subjects displaying higher rs-FC in specific couplings also exhibited greater tb-fMRI modulations (Figure 8). Therefore, subjects that received tDCS seem to display consistency on their neural effects across different fMRI modalities, which seems not to be the case for tACS.

Limitations

This investigation presents a number of limitations. First, although comparable to previous studies employing similar designs, the sample size was relatively modest. Second, an fMRI scan and a WM assessment at baseline (or using a cross-over design) could have permitted a more powerful analysis of the neural and cognitive effects of stimulation. Notwithstanding, to avoid practice effects, which previous WM studies have suffered (i.e., Röhner et al., 2018), a between-subjects design was selected. Further, in our study, methodological issues related to electrodes size or location might have underlie the absence of cognitive effects (for tDCS, see Imburgio and Orr, 2018; for tACS, see Mehta et al., 2015). Moreover, it is also worth noting that using individualized theta frequencies could have boosted cognitive performance in our experimental setting, although it does not guarantee any behavioral improvement by itself (Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014). Altogether, present data might provide a novel proof of concept of NIBS-fMRI effects for future research in the field. However, it is relevant to consider that our results should be interpreted in a cautious manner due to the stated limitations, which makes them exploratory and warranting further confirmation in larger studies.

Conclusion

In sum, in the present investigation we have shown that prefrontal tDCS and tACS appear to display different effects on rs- and tb-fMRI neural dynamics. However, both particularly would affect DMN functioning at rest as well as during WM performance. In fact, the DMN is known to be highly susceptible to alterations by means of transcranial stimulation (i.e., Antonenko et al., 2018). This is crucial given that this network supports basic cognitive processes (Buckner et al., 2008). In addition, it is negatively affected both by advancing age (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2016; Schultz et al., 2017; Staffaroni et al., 2018) and in distinct neuropsychiatric conditions (Beucke et al., 2014; Hu et al., 2017; Wise et al., 2017; Yan et al., 2019). Indeed, its capability to being deactivated during externally oriented tasks is a process strongly affected both in aging (Miller et al., 2008) and disease (Fryer et al., 2013). In this vein, present results, along with those of other investigations allowing a better understanding of how the distinct DMN mechanisms -and its relationships with other systems- can be extrinsically modulated might provide valuable knowledge for future applications in both basic research and clinical care.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Barcelona’s Bioethics Commission (IRB 00003099). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KA-P, LV-A, and DB-F designed the research and wrote the manuscript. KA-P and LV-A analyzed the data. KA-P, LV-A, RP-A, NB, M-FK, AP-L, MN, and DB-F edited the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO/FEDER; PSI2015-64227-R) and the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (MICIU/FEDER; RTI2018-095181-B-C21) to DB-F, which was also supported by an ICREA Academia 2019 grant award. KA-P was supported by a fellowship from the Spanish Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport (MECD; reference number, FPU14/02728). LV-A was supported by a fellowship associated with the MINECO/FEDER PSI2015-64227-R grant (reference number, BES-2016-077620). AP-L was supported by the Sidney R. Baer Jr. Foundation, the National Institutes of Health (NIH R01 MH100186, R01 NS073601, R01 HD069776, R21 MH099196, R21 NS082870, R21 NS085491, and R21 HD07616), Harvard Catalyst | The Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (NCRR and the NCATS NIH, UL1 RR025758), DARPA (via HR001117S0030), and the Football Players Health Study at Harvard University. The project that gave rise to these results also received the support of a fellowship from “la Caixa” Foundation (ID 100010434). The fellowship code is LCF/BQ/DI19/11730050. This research was furthermore supported by the Government of Catalonia (2017SGR748).

Conflict of Interest

AP-L serves on the scientific advisory boards for Starlab Neuroscience, Neuroelectrics, Axilum Robotics, Constant Therapy, NovaVision, Cognito, Magstim, Nexstim, and Neosync, and is listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging. MN serves on the scientific advisory board for Neuroelectrics.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are indebted to the Magnetic Resonance Imaging Core Facility of the IDIBAPS for the technical help.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2019.01440/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/
  2. ^ https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/

References

Abellaneda-Pérez, K., Vaqué-Alcázar, L., Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Jannati, A., Solana, E., Bargalló, N., et al. (2019). Age-related differences in default-mode network connectivity in response to intermittent theta-burst stimulation and its relationships with maintained cognition and brain integrity in healthy aging. Neuroimage 188, 794–806. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alekseichuk, I., Turi, Z., Amador de Lara, G., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2016). Spatial working memory in humans depends on theta and high gamma synchronization in the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Biol. 26, 1513–1521. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, M. M., Sellers, K. K., and Frohlich, F. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates large-scale cortical network activity by network resonance. J. Neurosci. 33, 11262–11275. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5867-12.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, D. G., and Bland, J. M. (1999). How to randomise. BMJ 319, 703–704. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7211.703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, N. D., and Craik, F. I. (2017). 50 years of cognitive aging theory. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 72, 1–6. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Transcranial alternating current stimulation and transcranial random noise stimulation: possible mechanisms. Neural Plast. 2016:3616807. doi: 10.1155/2016/3616807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:317. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Külzow, N., Sousa, A., Prehn, K., Grittner, U., and Flöel, A. (2018). Neuronal and behavioral effects of multi-day brain stimulation and memory training. Neurobiol. Aging 61, 245–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bächinger, M., Zerbi, V., Moisa, M., Polania, R., Liu, Q., Mantini, D., et al. (2017). Concurrent tACS-fMRI reveals causal influence of power synchronized neural activity on resting state fMRI connectivity. J. Neurosci. 37, 4766–4777. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1756-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science 255, 556–559. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20, 136–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M., and Grafman, J. (2013). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 49, 1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beucke, J. C., Sepulcre, J., Eldaief, M. C., Sebold, M., Kathmann, N., and Kaufmann, C. (2014). Default mode network subsystem alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 205, 376–382. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.137380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bikson, M., Grossman, P., Thomas, C., Zannou, A. L., Jiang, J., and Adnan, T. (2016). Safety of transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence based update 2016. Brain Stimul. 9, 641–661. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brauer, H., Kadish, N. E., Pedersen, A., Siniatchkin, M., and Moliadze, V. (2018). No modulatory effects when stimulating the right inferior frontal gyrus with continuous 6 Hz tACS and tRNS on response inhibition: a behavioral study. Neural Plast. 2018:3156796. doi: 10.1155/2018/3156796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., Amadera, J., Berbel, B., Volz, M. S., Rizzerio, B. G., and Fregni, F. (2011). A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 14, 1133–1145. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., and Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 86, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabral-Calderin, Y., Williams, K. A., Opitz, A., Dechent, P., and Wilke, M. (2016). Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates spontaneous low frequency fluctuations as measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 141, 88–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Shu, H., Chen, G., Ward, B. D., Antuono, P. G., Zhang, Z., et al. (2016). Staging alzheimer’s disease risk by sequencing brain function and structure, cerebrospinal fluid, and cognition biomarkers. J. Alzheimers Dis. 54, 983–993. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, C. E., and D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedoncker, J., Brunoni, A. R., Baeken, C., and Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples: influence of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul. 9, 501–517. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, J., Vogel, E. K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., and Nyberg, L. (2015). Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron 88, 33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fertonani, A., Ferrari, C., and Miniussi, C. (2015). What do you feel if I apply transcranial electric stimulation? Safety, sensations and secondary induced effects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 2181–2188. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., et al. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 23–30. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fryer, S. L., Woods, S. W., Kiehl, K. A., Calhoun, V. D., Pearlson, G. D., Roach, B. J., et al. (2013). Deficient suppression of default mode regions during working memory in individuals with early psychosis and at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 4:92. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamada, M., Murase, N., Hasan, A., Balaratnam, M., and Rothwell, J. C. (2013). The role of interneuron networks in driving human motor cortical plasticity. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1593–1605. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Axmacher, N., and Inman, C. S. (2019). Modulating human memory via entrainment of brain oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 42, 485–499. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., Rach, S., Neuling, T., and Strüber, D. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, A. T., Rogasch, N. C., Fitzgerald, P. B., and Hoy, K. E. (2017). Effects of prefrontal bipolar and high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation on cortical reactivity and working memory in healthy adults. Neuroimage 152, 142–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holland, R., Leff, A. P., Josephs, O., Galea, J. M., Desikan, M., Price, C. J., et al. (2011). Speech facilitation by left inferior frontal cortex stimulation. Curr. Biol. 21, 1403–1407. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, M. W., Rizzuto, D. S., Caplan, J. B., Madsen, J. R., Lisman, J., Aschenbrenner-Scheibe, R., et al. (2003). Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1369–1374. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N., Arnold, S., Windsor, K., John, J., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2015). The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls. Brain Cogn. 101, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, W., Ku, Y., Zanto, T. P., and Gazzaley, A. (2015). Effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on cognitive function in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 2348–2359. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, M. L., Zong, X. F., Mann, J. J., Zheng, J. J., Liao, Y. H., Li, Z. C., et al. (2017). A review of the functional and anatomical default mode network in schizophrenia. Neurosci. Bull. 33, 73–84. doi: 10.1007/s12264-016-0090-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imburgio, M. J., and Orr, J. M. (2018). Effects of prefrontal tDCS on executive function: methodological considerations revealed by meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 117, 156–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N., and Jaušovec, K. (2014). Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Biol. Psychol. 96, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Colgin, L. L. (2007). Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 267–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. K., McMahon, R. P., Robinson, B. M., Harvey, A. N., Hahn, B., Leonard, C. J., et al. (2013). The relationship between working memory capacity and broad measures of cognitive ability in healthy adults and people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 27, 220–229. doi: 10.1037/a0032060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, M., Ragan, B. G., and Park, J. H. (2008). Issues in outcomes research: an overview of randomization techniques for clinical trials. J. Athl. Train. 43, 215–221. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-43.2.215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasten, F. H., Dowsett, J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Sustained aftereffect of α-tACS lasts up to 70 min after stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:245. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keeser, D., Meindl, T., Bor, J., Palm, U., Pogarell, O., Mulert, C., et al. (2011). Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fMRI. J. Neurosci. 31, 15284–15293. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0542-11.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, S., Gan, L. S., Alrazi, T., and Monchi, O. (2019). Theta band high definition transcranial alternating current stimulation, but not transcranial direct current stimulation, improves associative memory performance. Sci. Rep. 9:8562. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44680-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., and Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 599–611. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebetanz, D., Nitsche, M. A., Tergau, F., and Paulus, W. (2002). Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain 125, 2238–2247. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Jensen, O. (2013). The theta-gamma neural code. Neuron 77, 1002–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Alonso, V., Cheeran, B., Río-Rodríguez, D., and Fernández-Del-Olmo, M. (2014). Inter- individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. 7, 372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancuso, L. E., Ilieva, I. P., Hamilton, R. H., and Farah, M. J. (2016). Does transcranial direct current stimulation improve healthy working memory? a meta-analytic review. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1063–1089. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Trias, P., Lanteaume, L., Solana, E., Cassé-Perrot, C., Fernández-Cabello, S., Babiloni, C., et al. (2018). Adaptability and reproducibility of a memory disruption rTMS protocol in the PharmaCog IMI European project. Sci. Rep. 8:9371. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27502-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, H., and Ugawa, Y. (2017). Adverse events of tDCS and tACS: a review. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 2, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2016.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., and Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 394–408. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J., and Cason, S. (2017). No evidential value in samples of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) studies of cognition and working memory in healthy populations. Cortex 94, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehta, A. R., Pogosyan, A., Brown, P., and Brittain, J. S. (2015). Montage matters: the influence of transcranial alternating current stimulation on human physiological tremor. Brain Stimul. 8, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinzer, M., Antonenko, D., Lindenberg, R., Hetzer, S., Ulm, L., Avirame, K., et al. (2012). Electrical brain stimulation improves cognitive performance by modulating functional connectivity and task-specific activation. J. Neurosci. 32, 1859–1866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4812-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinzer, M., Lindenberg, R., Antonenko, D., Flaisch, T., and Flöel, A. (2013). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation temporarily reverses age-associated cognitive decline and functional brain activity changes. J. Neurosci. 33, 12470–12478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5743-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinzer, M., Lindenberg, R., Phan, M. T., Ulm, L., Volk, C., and Flöel, A. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation in mild cognitive impairment: behavioral effects and neural mechanisms. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 1032–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.07.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiron, O., and Lavidor, M. (2014). Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S. L., Celone, K., DePeau, K., Diamond, E., Dickerson, B. C., Rentz, D., et al. (2008). Age-related memory impairment associated with loss of parietal deactivation but preservd hippocampal activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 2181–2186. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706818105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moisa, M., Polania, R., Grueschow, M., and Ruff, C. C. (2016). Brain network mechanisms underlying motor enhancement by transcranial entrainment of gamma oscillations. J. Neurosci. 36, 12053–12065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2044-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monte-Silva, K., Kuo, M. F., Hessenthaler, S., Fresnoza, S., Liebetanz, D., Paulus, W., et al. (2013). Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 6, 424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakao, T., Nakagawa, A., Nakatani, E., Nabeyama, M., Sanematsu, H., Yoshiura, T., et al. (2009). Working memory dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a neuropsychological and functional MRI study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 784–791. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Krawitz, A., et al. (2013). A Meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cereb. Cortex 23, 264–282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, J., Lebedev, A. V., and Lovden, M. (2015). No significant effect of prefrontal tDCS on working memory performance in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:230. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Fricke, K., Henschke, U., Schlitterlau, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2003). Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol. 553, 293–301. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527, 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57, 1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohn, S. H., Park, C. I., Yoo, W. K., Ko, M. H., Choi, K. P., Kim, G. M., et al. (2008). Time-dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working memory. Neuroreport 19, 43–47. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f2adfd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahor, A., and Jaušovec, N. (2014). The effects of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on fluid intelligence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 322–331. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahor, A., and Jaušovec, N. (2018). The effects of theta and gamma tACS on working memory and electrophysiology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:651. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., and Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 17, 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T. (2005). Inferring causality in brain images: a perturbation approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1109–1114. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peña-Gómez, C., Sala-Lonch, R., Junqué, C., Clemente, I., Vidal, D., Bargalló, N., et al. (2012). Modulation of large-scale brain networks by transcranial direct current stimulation evidenced by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Stimul. 5, 252–263. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfefferbaum, A., Chanraud, S., Pitel, A. L., Muller-Oehring, E., Shankaranarayanan, A., Alsop, D. C., et al. (2011). Cerebral blood flow in posterior cortical nodes of the default mode network decreases with task engagement but remains higher than in most brain regions. Cereb. Cortex 21, 233–244. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., Korman, C., Batsikadze, G., and Paulus, W. (2012). The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr. Biol. 22, 1314–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., and Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purpura, D. P., and McMurtry, J. G. (1965). Intracellular activities and evoked potential changes during polarization of motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 28, 166–185. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.1.166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reato, D., Rahman, A., Bikson, M., and Parra, L. C. (2013). Effects of weak transcranial alternating current stimulation on brain activity—a review of known mechanisms from animal studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:687. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röhner, F., Breitling, C., Rufener, K. S., Heinze, H. J., Hinrichs, H., Krauel, K., et al. (2018). Modulation of working memory using transcranial electrical stimulation: a direct comparison between TACS and TDCS. Front. Neurosci. 12:761. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F., and Uhlhaas, P. J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala-Llonch, R., Peña-Gómez, C., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Bargalló, N., Junqué, C., et al. (2012). Brain connectivity during resting state and subsequent working memory task predicts behavioural performance. Cortex 48, 1187–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Feurra, M., Barneschi, F., Acampa, M., Bianco, G., Cioncoloni, D., et al. (2014). Time course of corticospinal excitability and autonomic function interplay during and following monopolar tDCS. Front. Psychiatry 5:86. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarnthein, J., Petsche, H., Rappelsberger, P., Shaw, G. L., and von Stein, A. (1998). Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 7092–7096. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., and Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, A. P., Chhatwal, J. P., Hedden, T., Mormino, E. C., Hanseeuw, B. J., Sepulcre, J., et al. (2017). Phases of hyperconnectivity and hypoconnectivity in the default mode and salience networks track with amyloid and tau in clinically normal individuals. J. Neurosci. 37, 4323–4331. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3263-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staffaroni, A. M., Brown, J. A., Casaletto, K. B., Elahi, F. M., Deng, J., Neuhaus, J., et al. (2018). The longitudinal trajectory of default mode network connectivity in healthy older adults varies as a function of age and is associated with changes in episodic memory and processing speed. J. Neurosci. 38, 2809–2817. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3067-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, S., Lepage, J. F., Latulipe-Loiselle, A., Fregni, F., Pascual-Leone, A., and Théoret, H. (2014). The uncertain outcome of prefrontal tDCS. Brain Stimul. 7, 773–783. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., and Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cogn. Psychol. 71, 1–26. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Violante, I. R., Li, L. M., Carmichael, D. W., Lorenz, R., Leech, R., Hampshire, A., et al. (2017). Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. eLife 6:e22001. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossen, A., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2015). Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (a-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 8, 499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). BOLD signal effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) in the alpha range: a concurrent tACS–fMRI study. Neuroimage 140, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinrich, C. A., Brittain, J. S., Nowak, M., Salimi-Khorshidi, R., Brown, P., and Stagg, C. J. (2017). Modulation of long-range connectivity patterns via frequency-specific stimulation of human cortex. Curr. Biol. 27, 3061.e3–3068.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wischnewski, M., Engelhardt, M., Salehinejad, M. A., Schutter, D. J. L. G., Kuo, M. F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2019). NMDA receptor-mediated motor cortex plasticity after 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2924–2931. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, T., Marwood, L., Perkins, A. M., Herane-Vives, A., Joules, R., Lythgoe, D. J., et al. (2017). Instability of default mode network connectivity in major depression: a two-sample confirmation study. Transl. Psychiatry 7:e1105. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., et al. (2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage 21, 1732–1747. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Ripley, B. D., Brady, M., and Smith, S. M. (2001). Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. Neuroimage 14, 1370–1386. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, C. G., Chen, X., Li, L., Castellanos, F. X., Bai, T. J., Bo, Q. J., et al. (2019). Reduced default mode network functional connectivity in patients with recurrent major depressive disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 9078–9083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1900390116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaehle, T., Sandmann, P., Thorne, J. D., Jäncke, L., and Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaghi, S., Acar, M., Hultgren, B., Boggio, P. S., and Fregni, F. (2010a). Noninvasive brain stimulation with low-intensity electrical currents: putative mechanisms of action for direct and alternating current stimulation. Neuroscientist 16, 285–307. doi: 10.1177/1073858409336227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaghi, S., de Freitas Rezende, L., de Oliveira, L. M., El-Nazer, R., Menning, S., Tadini, L., et al. (2010b). Inhibition of motor cortex excitability with 15Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Neurosci. Lett. 479, 211–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (tb-fMRI), working memory (WM)

Citation: Abellaneda-Pérez K, Vaqué-Alcázar L, Perellón-Alfonso R, Bargalló N, Kuo M-F, Pascual-Leone A, Nitsche MA and Bartrés-Faz D (2020) Differential tDCS and tACS Effects on Working Memory-Related Neural Activity and Resting-State Connectivity. Front. Neurosci. 13:1440. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01440

Received: 13 September 2019; Accepted: 20 December 2019;
Published: 17 January 2020.

Edited by:

Oscar Arias-Carrión, Hospital General Dr. Manuel Gea Gonzalez, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Silmar Teixeira, Federal University of Piauí, Brazil
Daniel Keeser, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Abellaneda-Pérez, Vaqué-Alcázar, Perellón-Alfonso, Bargalló, Kuo, Pascual-Leone, Nitsche and Bartrés-Faz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David Bartrés-Faz, dbartres@ub.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.