Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 28 April 2022
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Effort-Based Decision-Making and Cognitive Fatigue View all 21 articles

Training Willpower: Reducing Costs and Valuing Effort

  • 1Research Centre on Cognition and Learning, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, University of Poitiers, Poitiers, France
  • 2School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD, Australia

The integrative model of effortful control presented in a previous article aimed to specify the neurophysiological bases of mental effort. This model assumes that effort reflects three different inter-related aspects of the same adaptive function. First, a mechanism anchored in the salience network that makes decisions about the effort that should be engaged in the current task in view of costs and benefits associated with the achievement of the task goal. Second, a top-down control signal generated by the mechanism of effort that modulates neuronal activity in brain regions involved in the current task to filter pertinent information. Third, a feeling that emerges in awareness during effortful tasks and reflects the costs associated with goal-directed behavior. The aim of the present article is to complete this model by proposing that the capacity to exert effortful control can be improved through training programs. Two main questions relative to this possible strengthening of willpower are addressed in this paper. The first question concerns the existence of empirical evidence that supports gains in effortful control capacity through training. We conducted a review of 63 meta-analyses that shows training programs are effective in improving performance in effortful tasks tapping executive functions and/or self-control with a small to large effect size. Moreover, physical and mindfulness exercises could be two promising training methods that would deserve to be included in training programs aiming to strengthen willpower. The second question concerns the neural mechanisms that could explain these gains in effortful control capacity. Two plausible brain mechanisms are proposed: (1) a decrease in effort costs combined with a greater efficiency of brain regions involved in the task and (2) an increase in the value of effort through operant conditioning in the context of high effort and high reward. The first mechanism supports the hypothesis of a strengthening of the capacity to exert effortful control whereas the second mechanism supports the hypothesis of an increase in the motivation to exert this control. In the last part of the article, we made several recommendations to improve the effectiveness of interventional studies aiming to train this adaptive function.

Keep the faculty of effort alive in you by a little gratuitous exercise every day.

James (1918, p. 127)

Introduction

In daily life, our behavior mainly depends on routinized, automatic and unconscious processes (Ouellette and Wood, 1998; Kahneman, 2011). However, in some cases, effortful control is required to perform a more demanding task, such as maintaining concentration on complex problem solving (e.g., academic tasks), sustaining attention on infrequent cues (e.g., vigilance tasks), repressing immediate impulses to secure delayed benefits or avoid expected costs (e.g., self-control situations), or exercising at an uncomfortable intensity (e.g., sport and rehabilitation situations). Effortful control is deliberate, costly and exerted over brain areas involved in the achievement of a task goal (André et al., 2019; Müller and Apps, 2019). Effortful control is the product of the activity of the mechanism of effort anchored in the salience network (for more details, see André et al., 2019), which includes the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula (Seeley et al., 2007). The metaphor of the steering wheel (Bargh, 1997; Baumeister and Sommer, 1997) is relevant and illustrates the importance of effortful control in behavior: Even if a car is driven straight-ahead 95% of the time (thus no need for steering), a car without a steering wheel is not 95% as good as a car with one.

People who have a high capacity to exert effortful control are more likely to perform better in work, school and sport situations that require controlled attention or self-control. On the contrary, people who have a low dispositional capacity to exert effortful control, such as individuals with addictions, obsessive-compulsive disorder or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, generally present difficulties to regulate intrusive thoughts and emotions and to delay rewards (Pinto et al., 2014; Lugo-Candelas et al., 2017; Eichholz et al., 2020; Lim et al., 2020). In fact, developmental studies have shown that effortful control capacity in childhood predicts academic achievement, physical health, substance dependence, personal finances, antisocial behaviors and criminal offending outcomes later in life (Tarter et al., 2003; Moffitt et al., 2011; Liew, 2012; Fergusson et al., 2013; Daly et al., 2015; Holmes, 2018; for a review see Robson et al., 2020). Strengthening the capacity to exert effortful control through training could be a good way to improve quality of life and well-being of individuals, particularly those who have a low capacity. The term ‘capacity’ refers here to the ability or skill to exert effortful control rather than the maximum amount of resources devoted to effortful control. The aim of the present article is to show that the capacity to exert effortful control is trainable and to propose two plausible neurophysiological mechanisms supporting these durable changes in capacity.

The strength model of self-control (Baumeister et al., 2007, 2018) proposes that self-control could be strengthened through training. Taking the metaphor of the muscle, this model assumes that regular exertions of self-control can improve willpower strength and stamina, just as exercise training can strengthen muscles. The mechanisms underlying these gains in self-control would be an improvement in the self-regulatory general core capacity, i.e., increasing available self-control resources (Oaten and Cheng, 2006). Another important prediction of this model is that the improvements in the general capacity induced by the training program can be extended to other spheres of self-regulation unrelated to what had been practiced (Baumeister et al., 2006). In support of this model, two recent meta-analyses showed that self-control training is effective at strengthening the ability to self-regulate (Friese et al., 2017; Beames et al., 2018).

The strength model of self-control makes a last important prediction: the capacity to exert effortful control can be temporarily weakened after the performance of a first effortful task. This phenomenon called ‘ego depletion effect’ was recently challenged regarding its actual existence (Carter et al., 2015; Vadillo, 2019), and replicability (Xu et al., 2014; Hagger et al., 2016; Lurquin et al., 2016; Osgood, 2017; Alós-Ferrer et al., 2019; Vohs et al., 2021). This debate, which certain researchers considered as closed, is beyond the scope of this paper. But, what does it mean for the present theory if the ego depletion effect is so small that it is practically impossible to study? Strengthening willpower through training should increase the ability to compensate for a temporary weakening of the capacity to exert effortful control (i.e., an ego depletion effect). Consequently, any reader could think that it would be useless to study the possible strengthening of willpower through training if the ego depletion effect does not exist or is negligible.

Three arguments justify the pertinence of studying the improvement of the capacity to exert effortful control through training in spite of this questioning about ego depletion. First, denying a possible transient weakening of the capacity to exert effortful control after a long and intense use of this capacity is ignoring all the literature on cognitive, mental and central fatigue. Cognitive fatigue is generally evidenced in vigilance tasks by a decrement of performance with time-on-task (Mackworth, 1964; Boksem et al., 2005; Ackerman, 2011). In other respect, sport sciences are interested in the impact of mental or central fatigue induced by long and highly demanding cognitive tasks on sport performance. Two systematic reviews conducted on this topic showed a consistent effect of cognitive fatigue on endurance performance (Van Cutsem et al., 2017; Pageaux and Lepers, 2018). Moreover, a recent study showed that performance decreased with time-on-task during a classical depleting task; i.e., the ‘e’ letter task (Arber et al., 2017).

Second, willpower is the capacity to exert effortful control in spite of high costs (Baumeister and Tierney, 2011). As we will see further, different categories of costs are involved in effort-based decision making (i.e., decision about the amount of effortful control dedicated to the achievement of the task goal). Ego depletion and cognitive fatigue belong to the same category of costs: a temporary weakening of the capacity to exert effortful control that requires a compensatory investment in effortful control to maintain performance (André et al., 2019). Other categories of costs can modulate the effort-based decision making, such as the pain associated with the achievement of the task goal (e.g., muscle pain while carrying out a resistance exercise) or the risk of repeated failures (e.g., ego threat or threat to the physical integrity associated with task failures). In this perspective, strengthening willpower allows to cope with a large variety of stressful situations, including fatiguing tasks, painful tasks and risky tasks. For instance, a long-distance runner (e.g., ultra-marathon) has to cope with cognitive fatigue, muscular fatigue, and muscular pain; i.e., the athlete has to maintain an effortful control in spite of these costs if he/she wants to succeed. Consequently, even if the cognitive fatigue associated to the task is negligible, a successful coping with the other constraints of the task justify to train willpower.

Third, the transient weakening and the durable strengthening of the capacity to exert effortful control rely on two distinct neurobiological mechanisms that can be studied separately. As suggested by several authors, the temporary weakening of the capacity to exert effortful control relies on a short-term synaptic mechanism induced by an accumulation of adenosine in prefrontal brain regions involved in the ongoing task (Martin et al., 2018; André et al., 2019). By contrast, as we will see further, the durable strengthening of the capacity to exert effortful control relies on long-term synaptic mechanisms modifying the efficacy of glutamatergic synapses involved in the circuitry connecting the anterior cingulate cortex with brain structures computing costs and benefits (see the section “Neural Bases of Gains in Effortful Control Capacity through Training”). These two phenomena relying on two distinct neurobiological mechanisms, the existence or non-existence of the former does not in any way affect the existence or non-existence of the later, and reciprocally.

The concept of ‘resources’ applied to self-control and ego depletion has also been criticized and some authors, such as Michael Inzlicht, preferred to develop a non-resource-based account of the short- and long-term dynamic characteristics of self-control (Inzlicht et al., 2014b). Evidence for this model has not been forthcoming, and indeed the central prediction — that ego depletion manipulations reduce motivation to exert self-control on the dependent measure — has failed repeatedly (see Baumeister and Vohs, 2016).

Concerning the trainability of the capacity to exert effortful control, the alternative theory proposed by Inzlicht et al. (2014a,b) emphasizes that the motivation to exert effortful control can be increased using motivational techniques, such as implementation intentions and motivational interviewing (for a review, Berkman, 2016). In contrast to this model, we make a clear distinction between the capacity to exert effortful control and the motivation to exert effortful control. People can have the ability without being motivated (and vice versa). For example, some studies clearly showed that individuals are sometimes able to engage in effortful control (i.e., a capacity) but decide not to engage (i.e., a motivation) (e.g., Treadway et al., 2009). Therefore, the decision is not made toward the desired rewards but in order to escape the cost of the effort. As mentioned above, capacity refers to the ability to mobilize brain resources dedicated to effortful control, whereas motivation refers to the motive to mobilize these resources. Generally, training programs aim to develop capacities, and motivational techniques help researchers and practitioners increase the motivation and volition of individuals to engage in these effortful interventions and training programs. Michie et al. (2013) identified up to 93 theory-based behavior change techniques (BCTs) aiming to improve adherence to interventions. The use of these techniques is a prerequisite for the success of an intervention, but they are not the heart of the intervention and do not fully explain the improvement in trained capacity. Generally, the tasks and exercises repeatedly practiced by the participants constitute the true active element leading to a change in the capacity to be improved.

The integrative model of effortful control published by the authors in 2019 (André et al., 2019) proposed a theoretical framework based on recent findings in the field of neuroscience that define clearly what is effort and effortful control and which neuronal network underpins the capacity to exert effortful control. It particularly invokes the following contributions from neuroscience: the theory of attentional effort regarding the role of the cholinergic pathway in the generation and maintenance of the effort signal (Sarter et al., 2006), the theory of the dissociation between the salience network and the executive control network (Seeley et al., 2007; Seeley, 2019), the theory of the dynamic network connectivity regarding the short-term neuroplastic mechanisms that can explain a reduction in prefrontal activity following an exposure to stress or fatigue (Arnsten, 2009; Arnsten et al., 2010, 2012), and the theory of the expected value of control concerning the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in effort-based decision making (Shenhav et al., 2013, 2017).

The main proposal of this model is that effort designates three functional parts: (1) a mechanism anchored in the salience network (i.e., the mechanism of effort), which specializes in perceiving and responding to homeostatic and allostatic demands (Seeley, 2019), (2) a control signal (i.e., the effort signal) that is the main product of the mechanism of effort, that oscillates in the theta band (Onton et al., 2005; Sauseng et al., 2007; Kao et al., 2013), and whose the function is to filter information in the brain regions receiving this control signal, (3) a perception that emerges in awareness during effortful tasks (i.e., the perception of effort), which is a secondary product of the mechanism of effort and reflects the costs associated with the goal-directed behavior. Exerting effortful control, i.e., generating the control signal, is the main function of the mechanism of effort.

The strength model of self-control and the integrative model of effortful control share two important predictions: (1) the capacity to exert effortful control can be temporarily weakened when it is overloaded and used during a long period; (2) the capacity to exert effortful control can be durably improved through extensive and adapted training. However, our integrative model of effortful control differs from the strength model of self-control in three important points: (1) the mechanism underpinning the transient decrease in effortful control capacity (i.e., ego depletion effect or cognitive fatigue effect) is not viewed as the depletion of a resource, but as the weakening of the capacity of a neural system to generate a control signal because of a short-term synaptic mechanism induced by an accumulation of adenosine in prefrontal brain regions involved in the ongoing task, (2) predictions are made at the behavioral and neurophysiological levels and not only at the behavioral level (e.g., durable increase of performance accompanied by a durable increase in between-network connectivity with training), (3) the general core capacity that can be temporarily weakened through intensive use and durably strengthened through training is anchored in the salience network and not in the executive control network that underpins inhibitory control.

The present article focuses on the mechanisms leading to improvements in the capacity to exert effortful control. Motivational techniques are viewed here as moderators that facilitate the engagement of effortful control in training tasks throughout the entire duration of the intervention. The modulatory influence of these moderators on mechanisms leading to an increase in the engagement of effortful control is beyond the scope of this paper.

More specifically, the present paper aims to describe the hypothetical neurophysiological mechanisms that could underpin improvements in the capacity to exert effortful control. Arguing that training increases the amount of available resources (i.e., the capacity of a tank) is not a sufficient level of explanation to improve the methodology, efficacy, and effectiveness of effortful control interventions. This paper tries to answer the two following questions: Is there clear evidence for improvements in effortful control capacity with training? And if so, which durable changes in brain functioning explain these increments in effort capacity?

The following sections provide answers to these questions. In the first section “Definitions,” we present several interrelated concepts that are the object of the training. In the second section “Improvements in Effortful Control with Practice: An Umbrella Review of Meta-Analytic Reviews,” we summarize the main results of several meta-analytic reviews examining the effects of training on the capacity to exert effortful control. We discuss the significance and the size of this effect as a function of several moderators, such as the duration of the intervention and the type of exercises used to train the capacity to exert effortful control. We also address the issue concerning the generalizability/transferability of gains in effortful control capacity. Then, in the third section “Neural Bases of Gains in Effortful Control Capacity through Training,” we describe two brain mechanisms that could explain these training effects. Finally, in the last section “Challenging the Trainability of Effortful Control Capacity,” we formulate a series of recommendations to examine these training effects in the future.

Definitions

As mentioned in the previous section, a gain in capacity in effortful control can be very beneficial for an individual to increase his/her likelihood of success in personal achievement. In this section, we present the main concepts that constitute the target of the training interventions.

Two broad categories of training programs that are able to improve effortful control capacity have been identified (Beames et al., 2018). The first category of training programs aims to improve executive functions, whereas the second category aims to strengthen self-control, willpower or self-regulation. The following paragraphs will help the reader to disentangle the links between all these closely related concepts and then to understand more clearly how they fit together.

The concept of executive functions (EFs) comes from cognitive psychology and designates high-level cognitive functions anchored in the executive control network, which is a frontoparietal network bilaterally involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the posterior parietal cortex (Seeley et al., 2007). Executive control must be distinguished from effortful control that is exerted by another large-scale network: the salience network (Seeley et al., 2007; Seeley, 2019). These two networks are both activated as soon as someone is engaged in a cognitive or physical task (i.e., they are task-positive networks) but ensure different functions. The level of activation of these two networks depends, among other things, on the cognitive load of the ongoing task (Paus et al., 1998). Executive control allows individuals to mentally shift through ideas, to reason before acting, to cope with novel and unexpected challenges, to resist temptations and to stay focused on a specific goal (Diamond, 2013), whereas effortful control helps targeted brain regions involved in the ongoing task to keep the focus on relevant task features (André et al., 2019). The salience network and the executive control network are bidirectionally interconnected. Effortful control enhances executive functioning whereas executive functions send cost signals to the salience network that generates effortful control according to a cost/benefit decision-making.

Miyake et al. (2000) identified three main separable EFs that share commonalities: (a) shifting between tasks or mental sets, (b) updating and monitoring of working memory representations, and (c) inhibition of dominant or prepotent responses. The first component of EFs is also called ‘cognitive flexibility’ and corresponds to the ability to shift from one mental set to another mental set, from one set of guidelines for action to a different set (e.g., shifting from a status of an offensive player to a status of defender in basket-ball as soon as the ball is caught by the opponents). The second component is the ability to maintain, refresh and manipulate relevant information in working memory (e.g., performing the mental rotation of the representation of an object). The third component, also called ‘inhibitory control,’ ‘intentional inhibition,’ or ‘controlled inhibition,’ is the ability to repress or stop prepotent impulses, unwanted and intrusive thoughts, embarrassing emotions, or automatic motor responses.

More recently, Zelazo and Carlson (2012) introduced a new taxonomy of EFs, taking into account the context in which participants exert executive control. These authors proposed distinguishing cool EFs solicited and assessed in emotionally neutral contexts, such as laboratory settings, and hot EFs involved in motivationally and emotionally significant high-stakes situations, such as multiplayer online role-playing games or real social situations in daily life. As discussed later, these two categories of EFs are used in different types of interventions aiming to develop effortful control capacity.

Inhibitory control presents many similarities with the concept of self-control used in social psychology when the latter is more restrictively designated as the ability to follow rules or inhibit immediate desires so as to delay gratification (e.g., Muraven and Baumeister, 2000, p. 247), as well as to interrupt undesired behavioral tendencies and refrain from acting on them (e.g., Tangney et al., 2004, p. 274). However, the concept of self-control has a larger meaning when it is used interchangeably with the concept of self-regulation (Baumeister and Vohs, 2016, p. 70). Based on this larger meaning, it refers to the ability to voluntarily regulate attention, emotion, and behavior in the service of more highly valued goals and represents the deliberate, conscious, effortful subset of self-regulation (Baumeister et al., 2007, p. 351).

Willpower is a folk term referring to mental energy that is expended in difficult acts of self-control, such as resisting temptation and delaying gratification (Baumeister and Tierney, 2011). It is often used in connection with making current sacrifices for the sake of long-term benefits and goals. In the same way, effortful control invokes executive functions and helps to inhibit behavioral impulses so as to regulate emotions and behaviors, thereby enabling people to adjust to situations in flexible, adaptive fashion (André et al., 2019). The common theme is that the Self exerts effort to regulate its own responses to produce preferred outcomes. Philosophers have identified a set of virtues or skills associated with a strong willpower, such as persistence, endurance, perseverance, resoluteness and patience (Roberts, 1984; Steutel, 1999; Szutta, 2020). All these virtues help an individual to remain focused on his/her intended goals and to facilitate their achievement. In the framework of the integrative model of effortful control, we assume that willpower is the capacity to exert effortful control in difficult situations, such as sustaining attention in boring vigilance task or maintaining a high intensity of exercise in spite of fatigue and pain.

One important commonality between EFs and self-control, in both its more restrictive and larger meaning, is that all these high-level cognitive functions require effortful control. Based on the framework of the integrative model of effortful control (André et al., 2019), we assume here, that the self-regulatory general core capacity, which can be temporarily weakened through intensive use and durably strengthened through training, corresponds to the effortful control capacity ensured by the salience network. In addition, we assume that the good functioning of the executive control network, which underpins EFs, depends directly on the effortful control exerted by the salience network.

Effortful control is not conceived here as a depletable resource but as a control signal that can be weakened and/or deteriorated under the effect of fatigue (for more information about the mechanisms underpinning the possible weakening of this control signal, see André et al., 2019). In the same way, effortful control capacity can be conceived as the function of the mechanism of effort to generate this control signal, which can be directly assessed by measuring spectral power of theta-wave activity above prefrontal areas (e.g., Cavanagh and Franck, 2014; Fairclough and Ewing, 2017). Higher the density of prefrontal theta-wave activity is, higher the engagement in effortful control. Exerting effortful control means that the organism needs to mobilize energy, and the activation of the sympathetic system is closely linked to the exertion of effortful control (Critchley, 2005). In that way, indexes of sympathetic activity, such as pupil size and pre-ejection period, are used as indirect measures of effortful control (Richter et al., 2008; van der Wel and van Steenbergen, 2018).

Based on the above, we can make a series of hypotheses: (1) the capacity to maintain a high level of effortful control over time in spite of fatigue or pain (i.e., a high level of concentration or effort engagement) can be strengthened through training programs involving effortful activities; (2) training programs targeting self-control or EFs stimulate effortful control and can strengthen this general capacity; (3) training programs more specifically targeting EFs lead to several synergistic effects: a strengthening of the effortful control capacity through durable changes within the salience network, a strengthening of the EFs through durable changes within the executive control network and a strengthening of the connectivity between these two networks.

Finally, the notion of transfer is central in the cognitive training literature and related to the generalizability of the gain obtained through extensive practice. Transfer distance refers to the similarities between the trained tasks and the tasks used to demonstrate a gain in performance at the end of the intervention (i.e., the principal outcome). Two types of transfer can be distinguished: (a) ‘near-transfer’ effects when trained tasks and postintervention untrained tasks are similar, (b) ‘far-transfer’ effects when trained tasks and postintervention tasks are dissimilar. The ultimate goal of interventions targeting effortful control capacity is to promote far-transfer effects because the gain in this general capacity should ideally be transferable to a broad range of everyday functional activities.

Improvements in Effortful Control With Practice: An Umbrella Review of Meta-Analytic Reviews

In this section, we summarize the main results of meta-analyses focusing on the long-term effects of different types of training methods stimulating effortful control. As mentioned earlier, Beames et al. (2018) distinguished two main categories of training methods: methods focusing on improving executive functions and methods focusing on strengthening self-control. Each following subsection addresses three important issues: the effectiveness of the training method to increase performance in effortful tasks that engage EFs or self-control, the stability of these gains once training stops and the generalizability/transferability of these gains. The method used to select, extract information and evaluate for risk of bias in these meta-analyses is detailed in the Supplementary Material.

Interventions Targeting Executive Functions

A very large number of studies have examined the effectiveness of miscellaneous training methods on EFs. Four main categories of training methods can be distinguished: process-based cognitive training, physical training, video-game training, and mindfulness training. Process-based cognitive training aims to directly increase the efficiency of specific cognitive processes, such as core EFs, through extensive repeated practice of affectively neutral computerized and/or manual cognitive tasks tapping the targeted cognitive process. Physical training aims to improve higher cognitive functions, such as EFs and episodic memory, through the regular practice of aerobic, resistance and/or coordinative exercises. Video game training stimulates miscellaneous cognitive functions, such as hot EFs, through video games, exergames or serious games that generally involve motivationally salient contexts or simulated social contexts generating heightened emotion. Mindfulness training is the regular practice of exercises maintaining attention to the current situation while concurrently acknowledging any thoughts or feelings that arise in consciousness (Bishop et al., 2004).

Process-Based Cognitive Training Interventions

Table 1 summarizes the results of sixteen meta-analyses published from 2011 to 2021, which focused on the effect of process-based cognitive training on EFs (near-transfer effects) and other far-transfer outcomes. Strategy-based training methods were not taken into consideration because they focus more heavily on compensatory rather than restorative methods, bypassing deficient cognitive processes and teaching alternative approaches to achieving goals (Mowszowski et al., 2016). For instance, strategy-based training programs aiming to compensate for memory deficits typically include internal techniques (e.g., categorizing or visualizing information to be remembered, encoding through multiple sensory channels) and external techniques (e.g., using environmental cues, calendars or memory notebooks).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Meta-analyses reporting effect sizes of process-based cognitive training on executive functions and other far-transfer outcomes.

The methods used to calculate the effect sizes varied greatly across meta-analyses. The most commonly used methods were Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988) and Hedge’s g (Hedges, 1981), but alternative methods to calculate standardized mean difference (SMD) have also been used (e.g., Morris, 2008).

The 16 meta-analyses included in Table 1 principally targeted three populations: children, adolescents and older adults. Ten out of 16 meta-analyses showed a significant and small to moderate effect of process-based cognitive training on near-transfer outcomes (i.e., performance in tasks different from trained tasks but tapping the same cool EFs). By contrast, only four meta-analyses reported a significant effect of process-based cognitive training on far-transfer outcomes (Rapport et al., 2013; Nguyen et al., 2019; Basak et al., 2020; Scionti et al., 2020). However, several categories of far-transfer outcomes must be distinguished. Performance in tasks tapping untrained EFs belongs to the first category of far-transfer outcomes, for instance, the effect of a working-memory training program using n-back tasks on inhibitory control assessed with a Stroop task. Performance in academic or everyday functioning tasks belongs to the second category of far-transfer outcomes (e.g., literacy tasks, calculation tasks). Performance in emotional and social self-regulation tasks (i.e., hot executive functions) belongs to the third category of far-transfer outcomes. Finally, blinded or unblinded subjective ratings of problem behaviors (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, quick-temperedness and disruptiveness) by a relative, a teacher or a caregiver belong to the fourth category of far-transfer outcomes.

Rapport et al. (2013) showed that programs designed to train working memory, EFs, and attention in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) lead to significant, small magnitude improvements in the first category of outcomes, but non-significant changes for the second and fourth categories of outcomes (i.e., academic achievement measures and blinded behavior ratings, respectively). In the same way, the meta-analysis conducted by Scionti et al. (2020) in preschool children showed that process-based cognitive training programs lead to significant far-transfer benefits in the first category, but not to outcomes belonging to the three other categories. The meta-analysis of Nguyen et al. (2019) focused on far-transfer effects in the first category only and confirmed that these gains can be observed in older adults. Finally, the meta-analysis conducted by Basak et al. (2020) in older adults showed overall significant net gains of process-based cognitive training versus the control conditions on everyday functional outcomes, but these gains were obtained through training programs targeting processing speed.

To sum-up, process-based cognitive training successfully improve EFs with a small to moderate effect size on near-transfer outcomes. However, they generally fail to induce far-transfer outcomes, such as performance in everyday tasks involving EFs or self-control. This last result suggests that process-based cognitive training methods induce gains in cognition that are not sufficiently generalizable and transferable to train willpower.

Physical Training Interventions

Table 2 summarizes the results of 28 meta-analyses published during the period 2003–2021, which reported the effect sizes of chronic exercise on EFs. These meta-analyses targeted children and adolescents (7 meta-analyses), young and middle-aged adults (7 meta-analyses), and older adults (14 meta-analyses). Seven meta-analyses focused on symptomatic populations (AD, ADHD, chronic brain disorders, and MCI). A large majority of meta-analyses (26 out of 28) showed a significant effect of exercise training on EFs. Among the four meta-analyses with the highest quality score (Karr et al., 2014; Alvarez-Bueno et al., 2017; Biazus-Sehn et al., 2020; Ludyga et al., 2020; M = 13.75/16; SD = 0.5), three clearly showed a significant effect of exercise training on EFs. None of these meta-analyses examined the effect of exercise interventions on other secondary effortful control domains.

Two meta-analyses focusing on the effect of interventional studies combining physical and process-based cognitive training on EFs were selected for the present systematic review (Zhu et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2020; see Supplementary Table 3.1). Both of them showed a significant but small effect of these combined interventions on EFs.

Video Game Training Interventions

Three meta-analyses examining the effect of video game training on EFs (Stanmore et al., 2017; Mura et al., 2018; Mansor et al., 2020) have been selected for the present systematic review. The meta-analysis of Stanmore et al. (2017) reported the results of 17 studies conducted in adults ranging from 17 to 85 years of age. These authors observed a significant effect of exergames on global EFs (g = 0.256, 13 studies), cognitive flexibility (g = 0.348, 8 studies), and inhibitory control (g = 0.90, 5 studies), but a non-significant effect on working memory (4 studies) and problem solving (3 studies). The meta-analysis of Mura et al. (2018) reported the results of 13 intervention studies in persons suffering from neurological disabilities (multiple sclerosis, poststroke hemiparesis, Parkinson’s disease, dementia, dyslexia, and Down syndrome). They showed a significant and positive effect of exergames on EFs (SMD = 0.53, eight studies) but not on attention (seven studies). The meta-analysis conducted by Mansor et al. (2020) included 27 intervention studies and examined the effect of video game training on EFs in older adults. Video game training had no significant effects on attention (8 studies), reasoning (10 studies), cognitive flexibility (15 studies), and inhibitory control (15 studies). By contrast, video game training led to a significant and moderate effect on working memory updating (g = 0.37, 19 studies). The duration of video game interventions ranged from 2 to 24 weeks, with an average of 9.4 weeks for the three meta-analyses. The three meta-analyses did not report any other far-transfer outcomes. Supplementary Table 3.2 describes the main characteristics of these three meta-analyses.

Mindfulness Training Interventions

Finally, eight meta-analyses including randomized controlled studies reporting mean effect sizes of mindfulness training interventions on EFs have been selected in the present systematic review (Chan et al., 2019; Dunning et al., 2019; Cásedas et al., 2020; Poissant et al., 2020; Im et al., 2021; Millett et al., 2021; Verhaeghen, 2021; Yakobi et al., 2021). The characteristics of these meta-analyses are detailed on Supplementary Table 3.3. Two meta-analyses focused on specific populations: the meta-analysis of Chan et al. (2019) on older adults and the meta-analysis of Dunning et al. (2019) on children and adolescents. All the six other meta-analyses mainly concerned young and middle-aged adults. Seven out of these eight meta-analyses showed a significant and small to moderate effect of mindfulness training on EFs (Dunning et al., 2019; Cásedas et al., 2020; Poissant et al., 2020; Im et al., 2021; Millett et al., 2021; Verhaeghen, 2021; Yakobi et al., 2021). The eight meta-analyses shared 31.6% of duplicates. Mindfulness-based programs reported in these meta-analyses were in average shorter than exercise training programs listed in Table 2 (6.6 weeks vs. 23.5 weeks, respectively), but as exercise training programs they provide additional benefits on mental health and well-being (e.g., reduction of anxiety, depression and reactivity to stress).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Meta-analyses reporting an effect of chronic exercise on executive functions.

Interventions Targeting Self-Control

A few interventions have explored the beneficial effects of self-control training on self-control capacity. Self-control interventions do not focus specifically on inhibitory control but generally use a large variety of training tasks involving one or several spheres of self-control described by Hagger et al. (2010), such as volition and social processing. Four meta-analyses examined the effects of self-control training in young adults (Hagger et al., 2010; Inzlicht and Berkman, 2015; Friese et al., 2017; Beames et al., 2018). These meta-analyses have included 33 intervention studies, 11 of which are unpublished. Two other meta-analyses focused on children and adolescents (Piquero et al., 2016; Pandey et al., 2018). Together, they included 41 intervention studies, seven of which were in common and 16% were unpublished studies. All these meta-analyses showed a significant effect of training on self-control capacity. The mean effect size ranged from small (d = 0.17, Inzlicht and Berkman, 2015) to large (d = 1.07, Hagger et al., 2010) in young adults and was moderate for children and adolescents (d = 0.32, Piquero et al., 2016; d = 0.42, Pandey et al., 2018). Interestingly, Friese et al. (2017) showed that training effects were significantly larger when the task showing the training effect was preceded by a depleting effortful task (g = 0.60) rather than when it was not (g = 0.21). This last result suggests that benefits from self-control training are more pronounced for the capacity to maintain effortful control over time (i.e., stamina or resistance to cognitive fatigue) rather than the capacity to exert strong effortful control during a short period of time (i.e., strength of effortful control).

In young adults, the interventions included a large variety of training tasks, such as using a non-dominant hand, maintaining good posture, avoiding sweets, performing inhibitory control tasks (e.g., Stroop task) or practicing physical exercises. In preschool and kindergarten children, half of the interventions used a curriculum-based approach implemented in classrooms including circle-time games, storytelling, book reading, and self-talk. In preadolescents and adolescents, the training strategies mainly included activities such as role-playing, cognitive modeling, psychoeducational group therapeutic lessons, physical exercises, and mindfulness and/or yoga exercises. Nevertheless, the amount of effortful control required by this large diversity of activities is rarely assessed.

Regarding the transferability of gains in self-control, intervention studies with children and adolescents showed a main positive effect on far-transfer outcomes, such as academic achievement, mental health, social skills, frequency of school suspensions, and educational attainment, but a weaker effect on substance abuse when comparing the treatment group with the control group. In young adults, the effect of self-control training on far-transfer outcomes was not conclusive. The two most recent meta-analyses showed contradictory results. The meta-analysis of Beames et al. (2018) found that the effect sizes for health and well-being outcomes were small-to-medium and significantly different from zero whereas the meta-analysis of Friese et al. (2017) failed to show significant effects for the same outcomes.

What Did We Learn From These Meta-Analytic Reviews?

In the present umbrella review of meta-analytic reviews, we analyzed the results from 63 meta-analyses interested in the effect of miscellaneous interventions aiming to durably improve EFs and self-control efficiency. A large majority of these meta-analyses (i.e., 79.37%, 50/63) showed that training programs are effective in improving performance in tasks tapping EFs and/or self-control with a small to large effect size. The transferability of these gains is more nuanced. Process-based and video game interventions failed to show far-transfer effects on academic or everyday functioning tasks. By contrast, self-control interventions seem more effective in producing far-transfer gains in other domains of self-control than trained domain. Intervention studies based on physical training listed in Table 2 and those based on mindfulness exercises rarely assess secondary outcomes, such as performance in academic or everyday functioning tasks. Consequently, it is difficult to assess the generalizability of these two types of interventions in the different domains of self-control. However, training effortful control through physical exercises or mindfulness exercises and observing gains in EFs could be considered far-transfer effects.

The interventions listed in the 63 meta-analyses mainly focused on children, adolescents and older adults, with the exception of mindfulness-based interventions. These three populations share a common characteristic: their EFs undergo drastic and quick changes in efficiency. Indeed, EFs are still developing in children and adolescents (De Luca et al., 2003; Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006) and declining in older adults (Spreng et al., 2017). Consequently, these populations situated at the two extremes of the lifespan are likely more sensitive to the effects of moderators, such as training and chronic stress, which improve or impair these high-level cognitive functions, respectively. For that reason, researchers should focus on these three populations when examining the effects of training on EFs and effortful control, because they would increase the likelihood to observe a significant effect.

For the same reason, it would be very interesting to examine the sensitivity to training for different symptomatic populations suffering from a recurrent mental fatigue (e.g., fragile older adults, multiple sclerosis patients, traumatic brain injured people or cancer patients treated with chemotherapy) or having a low dispositional capacity to exert effortful control (e.g., individuals with addictions, depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder). Few intervention studies targeting effortful control have been conducted in these populations.

If gains in EFs and self-control through training programs are based on durable changes taking place within large-scale networks, we can hypothesize that the stability of improvement in EFs and/or self-control over time could be an important index of training success. Consequently, intervention studies assessing near- and far-transfer effects in several follow-up assessments after program cessation are very good arguments for real durable changes.

Process-based cognitive interventions reported follow-up measurements in only 26.4% of the studies, whereas self-control, physical exercise, video game and mindfulness interventions rarely reported this type of information. The duration between the postintervention and the follow-up varied greatly among the studies reporting a follow-up: from 3 weeks to 10 years. When reported, effects on follow-up outcomes were significant with small to moderate size (Rapport et al., 2013; Nguyen et al., 2019; Takacs and Kassai, 2019), or non-significant (Lampit et al., 2019). However, several confounding factors, such as regular effortful activities practiced by participants in continuation of the training program or completely independent of the training program (e.g., playing chess outside of engaging with an aerobic exercise program), can moderate the outcomes associated with self-control and EF efficiency that are measured at follow-up, and these must be more rigorously controlled for in the future.

The quality of the 63 selected meta-analyses (see section S5 in Supplementary Material) is globally low. According to the AMSTAR2 risk of bias assessment scale (Shea et al., 2017), 54 meta-analyses (85.71%) are of critically low quality (M = 10.48/16; SD = 2.17), i.e., present more than one critical weakness. The three more frequent critical flaws are: (a) not providing a list of excluded studies with reasons of exclusion (87.30%), (b) not pre-registering the review methods prior to the conduct of the review (71.43%) and (c) not accounting for risk of bias in individual studies when discussing the results of the review (65.08%). Future meta-analyses on this topic will have to address these issues. However, a majority of the selected meta-analyses used a satisfactory technique for assessing the risk of bias in individual studies (83.13%), provided a satisfactory explanation for the heterogeneity observed in the results of the review (79.37%) and carried out an adequate investigation of publication bias with a discussion of its likely impact on the results of the review (77.78%).

This section clearly shows that all the above-mentioned training methods allow improving EFs and strengthening self-control. The generalizability of these gains seems more evident and robust in self-control training interventions. Which mechanisms can explain these gains and their transferability? The aim of the next section is to propose plausible and rational neurobiological mechanisms to explain the effects of effortful control training. A recent meta-analysis on the topic mentions that the mechanisms underlying these effects are poorly understood (Friese et al., 2017).

Neural Bases of Gains in Effortful Control Capacity Through Training

The aim of this section is to clarify the neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning the improvements in effortful control capacity through training programs. We assume that the improvements in the capacity to exert effortful control results in learning processes based on long-term synaptic plasticity, which take place in specific regions of the central nervous system involved in the engagement of effortful control. The description of these mechanisms requires the use of a theoretical framework proposing several neuronal networks as possible targets of these durable changes in activity and/or connectivity with training. We will use the integrative model of effortful control proposed by André et al. (2019) as a model of reference.

According to this model, effortful control is a top-down oscillatory control signal generated by a large functional neuronal network called the salience network (Seeley et al., 2007; Seeley, 2019). Converging empirical evidence from neuroscience suggests that different brain structures involved in the salience network, such as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, integrate costs and benefits associated with the achievement of the ongoing task to make decisions about the amount of effortful control dedicated to this task (e.g., Kennerley et al., 2006; Shenhav et al., 2013, 2017; Klein-Flügge et al., 2016).

On the one hand, benefits are the immediate or delayed positive consequences associated with the achievement of the task goal. They include all types of rewards (e.g., food, money, pleasure, social rank). On the other hand, costs are associated with the detrimental consequences an individual has to cope with while attempting to achieve an intended goal, such as expending limited resources or feeling pain. They depend on task constraints (i.e., the higher the constraints are, the higher the costs are) and participant characteristics (i.e., the lower the capacity to exert effortful control is, the higher the cost of effort). They include different categories of costs that are detailed hereafter and summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The different categories of costs that influence effort-based decision making and determine the amount of effortful control dedicated to a task.

André et al. (2019) distinguished metabolic or energetic costs (e.g., muscular and brain glucose necessary to reach the task goal) and computational costs (e.g., number of effort-dedicated processing units devoted to the task). However, three other main categories of cost computed by different cortical areas have been described in neuroscience (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of cortical areas involved in effort costs computation. The supplementary motor area is involved in computation of motor costs, the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in executive control costs, and the anterior insula in risk- and pain-related costs. These three regions are interconnected to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) through glutamatergic (Glu) pathways. The red arrows represent the cost signals sent by these cortical areas to the ACC that integrates costs and benefits signals and makes decisions on how much effort deploying to achieve the task goal. The black arrows represent the control signal sent by the ACC to the brain areas computing the cost signals to enhance their capacity of processing.

The first and certainly most studied category includes costs related to the physical activity necessary to achieve the task goal. These motor costs encompass energetic costs associated with energy expenses made by the muscles (i.e., intensity of muscle contraction) as well as computational costs associated with the complexity of the movement (e.g., number of motor units involved, complexity of the coordination timing between these motor units). Several fMRI and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies conducted in humans have suggested that the supplementary motor area (SMA) is involved in the coding of these motor costs (Pessiglione et al., 2007; Kurniawan et al., 2010; Burke et al., 2013; Zénon et al., 2015; Bonnelle et al., 2016).

The second category of costs is related to the degree of engagement of brain regions subserving EFs, such as working memory updating, inhibitory control and planning (Duncan and Owen, 2000; McGuire and Botvinick, 2010; Baumgartner et al., 2011). These executive control costs encompass energetic costs (i.e., brain glucose expended by each processing unit involved in executive control) and computational costs (i.e., number of processing units allocated to task performance relative to the limited number of available processing units). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which lies in the middle frontal gyrus, is an important hub in the executive control network (Menon, 2011) and its activity is associated with the executive control costs. For instance, several fMRI and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) studies have shown that activation in the left DLPFC scales linearly with working memory load (Barch et al., 1997; Braver et al., 1997; Jansma et al., 2000; Veltman et al., 2003; Fishburn et al., 2014; McKendrick and Harwood, 2019), indicating load-dependent recruitment of the DLPFC. In addition, transcranial direct current stimulation of the left DLPFC, which facilitates neural activity within this cortical area, reduces the cost of performing a cognitive task on gait and postural control (Zhou et al., 2014). Finally, a more recent study showed that executive control costs are anticipated by the DLPFC (Vassena et al., 2019). Other cortical areas, such as the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), which is located in the inferior frontal gyrus and ensures inhibitory control (Aron, 2007; Berkman et al., 2009; Aron et al., 2014a,b), could also participate to the computation in executive control costs.

The third and last category of costs includes both risk- and pain-related costs. Three types of risk-related costs have been identified: (1) the risk of not obtaining a reward, (2) the risk of losing an already obtained reward, and (3) the risk of experiencing negative consequences while obtaining a reward. A large number of studies have shown that the anterior insula computes these three types of risk (Burke and Tobler, 2011; Burke et al., 2013; for a meta-analysis, Mohr et al., 2010). This brain region is also involved in the subjective value of pain in effort-based decision-making (Talmi et al., 2009).

In this cost-benefit effort-based decision-making framework, two main mechanisms can explain a durable improvement in the capacity to exert effortful control with training: (1) a durable decrease in the effort costs; and (2) a durable increase in the value of the benefit resulting from goal-directed activities that requires effortful control (i.e., effort valuing). In the next subsections, we more precisely describe the two mechanisms that may underpin gains in effortful control capacity through exercise as well as mindfulness and self-control training.

Durable Reductions in Effort Costs Through Physical Training

According to the first mechanism, regularly practicing effortful exercises would lead to a progressive reduction in effort costs: that is, practice increases efficiency, and makes better performance possible with the same or less effort. Motor costs, executive control costs and pain-related costs are likely to decrease with physical training.

Reductions in effort costs are frequently observed in kinesiology and sport sciences with regard to physical effort. It is easy to understand this common phenomenon: individuals who take part in a physical training program that includes effortful exercises generally improve cardiorespiratory fitness as well as muscular strength, and they become increasingly efficient at practicing these exercises (Lin et al., 2015; Montero and Lundby, 2015; Lee and Stone, 2020). Consequently, the same exercise (i.e., same duration and same absolute intensity) requires more effort and energy at the beginning of the training program than at its end. Perceived exertion decreases with training (e.g., Farhat et al., 2015). In this way, sedentary or physically unfit people who start regular exercises progressively develop a higher tolerance for exercise and effort (e.g., Gomes-Neto et al., 2016). Symmetrically, people with a high cardiorespiratory fitness perceive a given absolute intensity of exercise as less effortful than people with a low cardiorespiratory fitness do (Eston and Brodie, 1986; Pfeiffer et al., 2002).

In addition, if the gain in effortful control acquired through physical training is transferable to the cognitive domain, it can be inferred that this gain in efficiency should be observed in the activation of brain areas involved in tasks tapping EFs. More precisely, a decrease of activation in brain areas involved in the salience and/or executive control networks should be observed after the end of the physical training program compared to before its beginning.

A set of six studies explored the effects of chronic exercise on gains in executive control and their brain activation correlates. The researchers used a flanker task (Colcombe et al., 2004; Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011; Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2013; Krafft et al., 2014), an antisaccade task (Davis et al., 2011; Krafft et al., 2014), or an n-back task (Nishiguchi et al., 2015) during fMRI scans before and after the exercise program. Half of the studies involved children, and the other half involved older adults. The duration of exercise programs varied from 13 weeks to 12 months. Four studies showed a positive effect of chronic exercise on behavioral performance, but two studies failed to find such an effect (Krafft et al., 2014; Nishiguchi et al., 2015). In contrast, all six studies showed a decrease in brain activity during the cognitive task at the end of the training program compared to the beginning, suggesting a higher efficiency in brain areas belonging to the salience network or the executive control network. These areas included the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011; Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2013; Nishiguchi et al., 2015), anterior cingulate cortex (Colcombe et al., 2004; Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011; Krafft et al., 2014), posterior parietal cortex (Davis et al., 2011; Krafft et al., 2014), and right superior temporal gyrus (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011). These results suggested that physical training reduces the executive control costs associated with the performance of a cognitive task tapping EFs. In functional brain imagery, a decrease in BOLD response or blood flow in a specific brain region involved in the performance of the task and associated with a stable or better level of performance with repetition of the same task is generally interpreted as an increase in efficiency of the neuronal networks thanks to practice. In the present case, it would be a decrease in the need for top-down control and then a decrease in energetic cost associated with a lower top-down control. To our knowledge, no study examined the effect of chronic exercises on motor costs, i.e., BOLD fMRI variations, while performing a physical exercise before and after a physical training program, certainly because of the higher risk of head movement artifacts in the MRI scanner.

Durable Reductions in Effort Costs Through Extensive Practice of Motor and Cognitive Skills

A decrease in computational cost, also known as attentional cost, can be observed with learning through a process of automatization. When people repeatedly perform a motor skill or a cognitive task, they progressively reduce the computational cost of the activity. From this perspective, the acquisition of automaticity can be viewed as the gradual withdrawal of effortful control. A large number of studies using the dual-task protocol have supported the fact that throughout the process of motor skill acquisition, the involvement of effortful control (i.e., attentional control) decreases across training sessions or blocks of trials (e.g., Brown and Carr, 1989; Wulf et al., 2001; Goh et al., 2014). The tenet of these studies is that the lower the attentional cost of performing the primary task (i.e., the motor skill) while simultaneously carrying out the secondary task (i.e., a cognitive task tapping executive control) is, the higher the automaticity of the motor skill.

This reduction in computational cost can be explained within the framework of the integrative model of effortful control. As mentioned earlier, this model assumes that effort is a mechanism anchored in a large functional neuronal network called the salience network (Seeley, 2019). The ‘mechanism of effort’ includes a limited number of interconnected processing units that integrates information regarding the costs and benefits associated with the achievement of the task goal and generates the effort signal, which is a top-down control signal optimizing the information processing of miscellaneous brain regions involved in the task. These effort-dedicated processing units are assumed to be anchored in the cortical minicolumns belonging to several cortical areas in the salience network, such as the anterior cingulate cortex, frontal operculum and anterior insula.

A high engagement of effortful control in the initial phase of learning followed by a progressive decrease in the need for effortful control in later phases of learning should be observed at the level of effort-dedicated processing units. Two hypothetical complementary mechanisms can explain this reduction in effortful control with learning: (1) the recruitment of a lower number of effort-dedicated processing units to perform the task and/or (2) a higher efficiency of these processing units at exerting effortful control (i.e., strengthened connectivity within each processing unit). These two mechanisms should lead to a lower activation of brain regions belonging to the salience network, and other top-down control brain regions involved in the task, such as the executive control network, by the end of the acquisition phase. Overall, fMRI studies examining patterns of activation in brain areas during cognitive tasks support quite well the hypothesis of a decrease in energetic and/or computational costs following several weeks of a training program that could include cognitive tasks or motor skills.

A set of six intervention studies confirmed that process-based cognitive training and motor skill learning led to a decrease in activation in brain areas belonging to the salience and executive control networks. The authors of these studies asked their participants to practice the following tasks: a self-initiated, self-paced, memorized sequential finger motor task while performing a letter-counting task (Wu et al., 2004); a visual serial reaction time task while performing a tone-counting task (Poldrack et al., 2005); an emotion regulation task (Berkman et al., 2014); a stop-signal task involving motor response inhibition (Beauchamp et al., 2016); and an n-back task (Heinzel et al., 2016; Miró-Padilla et al., 2019). The training volume ranged from 60 min (Berkman et al., 2014; Beauchamp et al., 2016) to 540 min (Heinzel et al., 2016), and participants were mainly young adults except for one study that preferentially included older adults (Heinzel et al., 2016). The results of these six studies confirmed a decrease in BOLD activity in brain regions in the salience network (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex) but also in numerous other regions in the executive control network, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex confirming a decrease in executive control cost with training.

Increase in Connectivity: A Biomarker of Efficiency

An increase in the efficiency of effort-based processing units reflecting task automatization should also be evidenced by an increase in connectivity within the salience network and/or between the salience network and other large-scale networks, such as the executive control network: the higher the between-network connectivity is, the lower the effort cost. As mentioned earlier, these changes in connectivity are generally observed by using resting-state fMRI coupled with a seed-based functional connectivity analysis.

We found five studies using this method that focused on the link between gains in automaticity or performance through process-based cognitive training and an increase in connectivity within and between top-down control networks. First, Mohr et al. (2015) showed that a higher connectivity between the salience network and the dorsal attention network correlated with practice-related efficiency gains. These authors also observed that short-term task automatization was accompanied by decreased activation in the executive control network, indicating a release of high-level cognitive control, and a segregation of the default mode network from task-related networks. Second, Chapman et al. (2015) conducted a 12-week gist reasoning training and observed that functional connectivity increased monotonically within the default mode and executive control networks, from pre-training to the end of training and from pretraining to midtraining, respectively, in the process-based cognitive training group relative to the control group. Third, Cao et al. (2016) examined training-related changes in functional connectivity within and between the default mode, executive control and salience networks 1 year after the training ended. In their experiment, healthy older adults were randomly included in a 3-month multidomain process-based cognitive training group or in a wait-list control group. The authors observed increased functional connectivity within the executive control network after training compared with the baseline. Fourth, Thompson et al. (2016) examined functional connectivity within and between the executive control and dorsal attention networks in young adults during task performance before and after 20 days of training on either a dual n-back working memory task or a demanding visuospatial attention task involving multiple object tracking. Learning selectively occurred in the n-back training group, who displayed marked gains on the trained task and not in the visuospatial attention training group. This n-back training induced significant increases in functional connectivity within and between the two networks. Fifth, Sánchez-Pérez et al. (2019) showed that a computer-based program aiming to train schoolchildren in cognitive tasks that mainly tap working memory leads to improvements in cognitive and academic skills compared with an active control group. They also found stronger relationships between inhibitory control scores and functional connectivity within the executive control network in trained children than in children from the control group.

In light of all the results presented in the two preceding sections, we can conclude that the hypothesis of a decrease in effort costs with training is plausible and supported by behavioral as well as activation and resting-state functional brain imaging data.

Durable Increases in Effort Valuing With Training

According to the second mechanism, prolonged experience in exerting effortful control would increase the value of a goal that required effort to be reached (e.g., Inzlicht et al., 2018): the higher the level of practice in effortful tasks is, the higher the expected benefit from any activity that requires effortful control. This hypothesis was initially formulated by Eisenberger in the framework of learned industriousness theory (Eisenberger et al., 1976; Eisenberger, 1992). This theory is based on the operant conditioning process (Skinner, 1938), a type of associative learning process through which the strength of a behavior is modified by a reinforcer. In operant conditioning, reinforcement occurs only after the organism intentionally executes a specified behavioral act. For instance, a child may learn to perform a chore without complaints to receive praise. From this perspective, animals and humans learn to engage in effortful tasks to maximize rewards. The learned industriousness theory views effort as a secondary reinforcer. If an organism learns that effortful tasks are consistently associated with greater rewards, the feeling of effort experienced during a task increases the expectation of a large reward once the task is performed.

Robert Eisenberger and his team conducted a series of intervention studies in animals and humans from the seventies to the nineties to demonstrate the soundness of this theory. The first experiment included a training program staggered over several days and was conducted with children (Eisenberger et al., 1985). In this experiment, 46 children were separated into three groups. Participants in the first group were paid for high effort in tasks involving object counting, picture memory, and shape matching, whereas participants in the second group were paid the same amount of money for a low-effort version of the same tasks. Participants in the third group did not undergo effort training. The training program for the first two groups included three training sessions given on consecutive days. Before and after the training program, all the participants made repeated choices between the tedious tasks of copying non-sense words for a large monetary reward versus waiting the equivalent duration for a small monetary reward. Before the intervention, the three groups did not differ in the number of times they chose to work for the larger reward. By contrast, after the intervention, the high-effort group chose the high-effort/high-reward alternative more frequently than did either the low-effort group or the control group, whereas the latter two groups did not significantly differ. Eisenberger and Adornetto (1986) replicated these results in a very similar experiment that manipulated the delay to the reward in addition to the effort required to obtain the reward. The results of these two studies clearly showed that repeatedly rewarding high levels of effort increases a person’s generalized choice of high-effort large rewards over low-effort small rewards and may contribute to individual differences in the willingness to postpone gratification in pursuit of long-term goals.

In a third experiment, Eisenberger et al. (1989) replicated these results in animals and trained two groups of rats to run down a runway for food pellets in a low-effort or high-effort condition for 18 days. In the low-effort condition, the rats received one pellet for one trip during the entire training period, whereas in the high-effort condition, they received one pellet for one trip at the beginning of training and one pellet for five trips at the end of training. Two groups of rats were added as control groups and received the same number and temporal distribution of pellet presentations as in the two experimental groups but without the instrumental requirement (i.e., completion of a given number of round trips). At the end of the training program, the four groups of rats performed 12 choice test sessions the same day. They were tested by giving repeated choices of exerting low force on one lever for a small reward versus exerting high force on the alternative lever for a large reward. The results clearly showed that rats in the high-effort training group chose the high-effort, large-reward goal box more frequently than the three other groups. These results demonstrated that training animals in a rewarding high-effort task during several sessions increased the likelihood that these animals chose to exert a higher level of effortful control associated with a higher reward in a subsequent transfer task. A more recent study (Laurence et al., 2015) replicated these results in rats with a similar protocol but with a longer training program. For a period of 7 weeks, exercise rats were individually placed in a rodent running ball for five sessions per week (20 min/session). To our knowledge, this series of experiments initiated by Eisenberger constitutes the first elements of proof that repeatedly associating high effort with high reward during a training phase can transfer to other tasks and drive the trained individuals to choose more effortful tasks to increase the likelihood of gaining more benefits.

Where do the long-term synaptic changes underpinning the association between high effort and high reward take place in the brain? A series of experiments mainly conducted in rodents identified a set of four interconnected key structures allowing animals to overcome effort costs to obtain greater benefits. Figure 2 illustrates the connections between these four structures: the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), nucleus accumbens (NAC), basolateral amygdala (BLA), and ventral tegmental area (VTA).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic illustration of the key structures and neurotransmitter pathways involved in effort-based decision-making in rodents and more particularly those that allow animals to overcome effort costs to obtain higher rewards. Pathway A connects the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens (NAC). Pathway B connects the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to the NAC. Pathway C connects the basolateral amygdala (BLA) to the ACC. Destruction of dopamine terminals in the NAC (Cousins and Salamone, 1994), lesions of the ACC (Walton et al., 2002) and bilateral inactivation of the BLA (Floresco and Ghods-Sharifi, 2007) impair effort-based decision-making and reduce the preference of animals to exert more effort to obtain a larger reward. These three structures clearly participate to a bias of behavior toward response options leading to larger rewards that come at larger costs but their respective contribution differ. In situations where an animal must choose between response options associated with differential magnitudes of reward, BLA neurons would encode the expected magnitude of reward that each choice may provide. This reward-related information would be relayed to the ACC via glutamatergic (Glu) projections. The ACC would bias behavior in a particular direction by integrating these reward-related signals with other information about response costs associated with each action. Then, the ACC would send the result of the decision-making to the NAC for an implementation of the appropriate behavioral output. Dopaminergic (DA) input from the ventral tegmental area to the NAC would be essential to energize appropriately the chosen instrumental activity in order to obtain the expected reward.

John Salamone from the University of Connecticut and his collaborators took a first step in the comprehension of effort-based decision-making. In rodents, effort-based decision-making is typically assessed using tasks that offer animals a choice between a relatively preferred reinforcer (i.e., reward) that can only be obtained by a high exertion of effort versus a lower effort/lower value option (for reviews, see Assadi et al., 2009; Salamone et al., 2018). In the first experiment, working for a preferred food (i.e., high carbohydrate pellets) by lever pressing was the high-effort/high reward option, whereas simply approaching and consuming a less rewarding food (i.e., ordinary lab chow) was the low-effort/low-reward option (Cousins and Salamone, 1994). Rats typically pressed at high rates to obtain the preferred food and ate little of the lab chow; i.e., they preferentially chose the high-effort/high-reward option. However, dopamine depletions produced by injections of the neurotoxic agent 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) in the NAC produced a dramatic decrease in lever pressing and an increase in chow consumption (Cousins and Salamone, 1994).

These results have been replicated in a different experimental setup (Salamone et al., 1994; Cousins et al., 1996; Denk et al., 2005). Rats were trained on a T-maze task with one arm containing a large reinforcer (four pellets) associated with a large vertical barrier (44 cm) and the other arm containing a small reinforcer (two pellets) associated with unobstructed access. Similar to previous experiments, in standard conditions, animals prioritized the high-effort/high-reward option, and this effect was reversed when 6-OHDA was injected into the NAC or when rats received injections of 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol, a dopamine antagonist. In other words, disruption of the dopaminergic pathway by drug treatment led rats to prefer the low effort/low reward option. These results showed that across a wide variety of tasks, administration of low doses of DA antagonists and NAC DA depletions have a detrimental effect on effort-based decision-making, producing a low-effort bias that shifts animals away from the high-effort option and toward the low-effort choice. Other authors obtained similar results with similar experimental setups and different dopamine receptor antagonists, such as flupenthixol (Floresco et al., 2008). A similar paradigm in which subjects choose between two options with different benefits and costs and a manipulation of dopamine availability has not yet been tested in primates or humans (Assadi et al., 2009).

Mark Walton from the University of Oxford and his collaborators used the same paradigm but targeted the ACC (Walton et al., 2002, 2003, 2009; Rudebeck et al., 2006, experiment 2). As Salamone and his team showed, all animals preferred to select the high-cost/high-reward option in the standard T-maze task. In these experiments, rats had to choose between a high effortful action (i.e., climbing a 30-cm barrier) to obtain a large quantity of reward (high-cost/high-reward) or a lower effortful action (i.e., climbing a 10-cm barrier) to obtain a smaller reward (low-cost/low-reward). However, after excitotoxic lesions of the ACC, rats selected the low-cost/low-reward response on nearly every trial. In contrast, both control animals and rats with prelimbic and infralimbic lesions continued to choose to climb the larger barrier for the larger reward. These results indicated that the ACC is an important region within the medial frontal cortex when evaluating how much effort to expend for a specific reward.

Stan Floresco from the University of British Columbia took a third step in the comprehension of brain mechanisms supporting effort-based decision-making (Floresco and Ghods-Sharifi, 2007). In their first experiment, they used exactly the same T-maze task as Walton and coworkers but focused on the role of the BLA in the effort-based decision-making process. They replicated the results in standard conditions and observed that bilateral inactivation of the BLA via infusion of the local anesthetic bupivacaine hydrochloride impaired decision-making by reducing the preference for the high-effort/high-reward arm.

From the above, we hypothesize that in animals and humans, the generalized bias toward high effort/large rewards resulting from effortful control training is inscribed within the circuitry described in Figure 2, and more specifically, in glutamatergic synapses connecting the BLA, ACC, NAC and VTA. To our knowledge, only one recent study conducted in humans with fMRI (Bernacer et al., 2019) showed that functional connectivity between the amygdala and ACC was strengthened after a 3-month fitness program (20–30 min sessions of walking and running on a treadmill, 2–3 days a week for 3 months).

The two preceding sections show that our field needs more theory-driven studies using animals as well as activation and resting-state fMRI in humans to determine precisely where when and how these durable changes in neural activity and connectivity occur. Some methodological suggestions in this direction will be made in the following section.

Challenging the Trainability of Effortful Control Capacity

The preceding sections provide arguments for a possible strengthening of effortful control capacity through the practice of effortful tasks. Then, two plausible mechanisms have been proposed to explain these gains in effortful control capacity. The aim of this last section is to address several theoretical and methodological issues to improve the effectiveness of training programs and comprehension of the mechanisms that underpin these gains in effortful control capacity.

The first issue concerns the choice of an appropriate protocol to prove and generalize a causal relationship between the regular practice of effortful tasks and durable improvements in effortful control capacity. The best way to eliminate bias that comes from confounders and demonstrate causality is to conduct randomized controlled trials (RCTs). In RCTs, study participants are randomly assigned to either receive the treatment or be in a control group (placebo). In the present case, the treatment group receives the training program aiming to improve effortful control capacity.

Proposing a control intervention that is as similar as possible to the treatment intervention with the exception that the level of effortful control differs across group activities is certainly the most difficult methodological issue to address in the context of an RCT protocol using human activities. An appropriate strategy could be to include two control groups: an active control group practicing activities requiring little effort (e.g., relaxation exercises, passive stretching exercises, massage and hydromassage sessions, watching emotionally neutral but interesting documentaries) and a passive control group that does not change its life habits during the period of the intervention. Fifteen out the 63 meta-analyses included in the present systematic review considered the type of control group as a moderator of the effect size of the intervention. Five out of these 15 meta-analyses showed that the effect size was significantly larger for studies that used a passive control group rather than an active one (Karr et al., 2014; Beames et al., 2018; Northey et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2019; Ren et al., 2021).

Regarding the treatment intervention, we recommend the use of effortful exercises (e.g., a combination of aerobic and resistance exercises) that stimulate brain plasticity (Fernandes et al., 2017; Walsh and Tschakovsky, 2018), in combination with cognitive tasks tapping EFs or mindfulness exercises. Physical exercises and cognitive tasks can be performed sequentially or simultaneously (team games or situational problem-solving tasks). The same is true for physical exercises and mindfulness exercises (e.g., yoga).

The second issue concerns the content of the treatment intervention program to generate transferable gains in effortful control capacity. In this perspective, the training program must be tailored, progressive and varied to optimize the likelihood of success in obtaining the desired effect. Tailoring the program means individualizing task difficulty and exercise intensity (e.g., difficulty expressed in percentage of individual’s maximal capacity). The respect for this first principle ensures that there will be no large imbalances in perception of task-related constraints across participants, thereby resulting in quite similar levels of engagement. The second principle concerns the progressive increase in task difficulty and exercise intensity throughout the training program. This second principle allows the maintenance of a high level of participant engagement throughout the program. At last, it is important to vary training exercises to improve the generalizability/transferability of gains in the capacity to exert effortful control (Eisenberger et al., 1982) and reduce boredom.

The third issue concerns the choice of the outcomes that will assess the gain in effortful control capacity. These outcomes can be assessed at three levels of observation (i.e., subjective, behavioral, and physiological) and at different times of the intervention study (e.g., before and after the program). Behavioral indexes, such as the level of performance in a specific task, are valuable data that provide information about the level of engagement of the participant in the task and his/her skill level in this task. Experimenters need to choose tasks sensitive to practice effects with no risk of ceiling effects. The subjective measurements, such as effort required to perform the task and perceived fatigue at the end of the task, contribute to and facilitate the interpretation of results. Physiological indexes of effort engagement (i.e., effortful control), such as pupil size, pre-ejection period (PEP) and prefrontal theta power density, may contribute to the picture by adding objective measurements of effort costs and top-down control to cope with the task goals. All these indexes (subjective, behavioral, and physiological) are complementary and make their own contribution to understanding variations in outcomes as a function of the intervention. A large majority of RCTs selected in the reviewed meta-analyses did not use physiological indexes of effortful control.

In addition to the outcomes described previously, we recommend assessing at least three categories of transfer outcomes: (1) near-transfer outcomes such as performance in tasks tapping EFs and self-control (e.g., use of the sequential task protocol before and after the intervention); (2) far-transfer outcomes related to performance in everyday functioning tasks, such as academic performance; and (3) far-transfer outcomes concerning general self-regulation abilities, such as snacking, speeding, and periods of inattention. It could also be appropriate to have several follow-up assessments (or retention tests), e.g., 1 month, 3 months, and 6 months after the end of the training program, to show stability of the gains in effortful control capacity. Few interventional studies include follow-up measurements.

The fourth and last issue concerns the choice of an appropriate method that allows a better understanding of the durable changes in connectivity occurring within and between several large-scale neuronal networks involved in effortful tasks, such as the salience network, the executive control network, the default network and the mesolimbic network. In the future, resting-state and activation functional MRI techniques in conjunction with graph theory could be used before and after the training program to disentangle the role of these brain networks in the improvement to the capacity to exert effortful control. Only few interventional studies used functional MRI to assess network connectivity.

We are aware that the type of RCT described above is time and money consuming, but it is the best guarantee to demonstrate that this type of intervention is a plausible and possible way to train the effortful control capacity and explain which mechanisms underpin these durable gains. In addition, the gains provided by the identification of the determinants of the effectiveness of willpower training programs overcome the costs of the research leading to such scientific advances. As mentioned in the introduction, these gains in willpower can increase the likelihood of success, well-being and productivity of each individual in society.

Conclusion

The first question we addressed in this paper concerns the existence of empirical evidence that supports possible gains in effortful control capacity through training. In the second section “Improvements in Effortful Control with Practice: An Umbrella Review of Meta-Analytic Reviews,” we provided clear evidence that executive control and effortful control can be improved through interventions using physical, cognitive or mindfulness exercises. However, we showed that the generalizability of these gains depends directly on the type of training interventions. In other words, people can definitely be trained to improve their executive functioning and self-control, but results have been inconsistent and variable as to how widely the improvements generalize to tasks different from those used in the training. Self-control training programs seem more effective than process-based training programs in inducing generalizability. Moreover, physical and mindfulness exercises seem to be two promising training methods that deserve to be included in self-control training programs. The higher effectiveness of self-control training programs in leading to generalizable gains most likely rests on the fact that these training programs include a greater variety of effortful tasks than process-based training programs.

The second question concerns the durable changes in brain structure and brain functioning that explain these increments in the capacity to exert effortful control. We pointed out two plausible brain mechanisms that can explain these gains in top-down control: (1) a decrease in effort costs combined with a greater efficiency of brain regions involved in the task and (2) a change in the value of effort through operant conditioning in the context of high effort and high reward. Our article shows that these two mechanisms have received clear empirical support from functional brain imaging studies in humans and neurophysiological studies in animals. The first mechanism is rather in favor of the hypothesis of the strengthening of the capacity to exert effortful control (i.e., more effortful control with less energy). By contrast, the second mechanism rather supports the motivational hypothesis: a durable predisposition to engage in effortful activities (i.e., an amplification of the benefit signal). Both mechanisms are certainly synergistic in contributing to how training improves effortful control. In addition, Bavelier and Green (2019) presented very interesting arguments suggesting that these two systems (i.e., the attentional/effortful control system and the reward system) foster learning and brain plasticity.

Based on the present literature review, what are the most pressing questions that would need further data collection on this topic in the near future? First, we need more resting-state electroencephalographic (EEG) and brain imaging studies examining the durable changes in connectivity, within and between large-scale neuronal networks, induced by training programs aiming to improve the capacity to exert effortful control. Three between-network connectivity hypotheses could be tested: (1) an increase of connectivity with training between the salience network and the executive control network supporting the strengthening hypothesis, (2) a decrease of connectivity with training between the executive control network and the default-mode network also supporting the strengthening hypothesis, and (3) an increase in connectivity between the mesolimbic reward network and the salience network supporting the motivational hypothesis. Second, we need to define more precisely the characteristics of the theory-based training programs that are more effective to strengthen the general capacity to exert effortful control, more particularly the most effective training exercises and the minimum volume of training needed to obtain significant gains according to the target population. Third, we need to know which theory-based behavioral change techniques are most effective at maintaining an effortful training program in the long-term.

Finally, if training programs are effective in strengthening effortful control capacity, citizens should be encouraged to practice and maintain engagement with such programs over the long term to continue developing these gains throughout their lives. In this way, health policies could promote the maintenance of a virtuous circle between healthy behaviors, including “willpower training” and the capacity to exert effortful control (for a description of this virtuous circle, see Audiffren and André, 2019). Based on this virtuous circle, training improves the capacity to exert effortful control and then a higher capacity to exert effortful control facilitates the maintenance of training and healthy behaviors.

Author Contributions

MA, NA, and RB participated to the conceptualization of the theoretical ideas, writing of the article, and manuscript revision. MA and NA conducted the literature reviews. MA elaborated the tables and figures. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Regional Council of New-Aquitaine, France (AAPR2020F-2020-8595710).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Phillip Tomporowski for his comments regarding the first version of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.699817/full#supplementary-material

References

Ackerman, P. L. (2011). Cognitive Fatigue: Multidisciplinary Perspectives On Current Research And Future Applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, doi: 10.1037/12343-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alós-Ferrer, C., Ritschel, A., García-Segarra, J., and Achtziger, A. (2019). Habituation does not rescue depletion: two tests of the ego-depletion effect. J. Econ. Psychol. 75:102122. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2018.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez-Bueno, C., Pesce, C., Cavero-Redondo, I., Sanchez-Lopez, M., Martinez-Hortelano, J. A., and Martınez-Vizcaino, V. (2017). The effect of physical activity interventions on children’s cognition and metacognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 729–738. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

André, N., Audiffren, M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2019). An integrative model of effortful control. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13:79. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arber, M. M., Ireland, M. J., Feger, R., Marrington, J., Tehan, J., and Tehan, G. (2017). Ego depletion in real-time: an examination of the sequential-task paradigm. Front. Psychol. 8:1672. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 410–422. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. T., Paspalas, C. D., Gamo, N. J., Yang, Y., and Wang, M. (2010). Dynamic network connectivity: a new form of neuroplasticity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 365–375. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F. T., Wang, M. J., and Paspalas, C. D. (2012). Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron 76, 223–239. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist 13, 1–15. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014a). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014b). Right inferior frontal cortex: addressing the rebuttals. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:905. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assadi, S. M., Yücel, M., and Pantelis, C. (2009). Dopamine modulates neural networks involved in effort-based decision-making. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 383–393. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Audiffren, M., and André, N. (2019). The exercise-cognition relationship: a virtuous circle. J. Sport Health Sci. 8, 339–347. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Forman, S. D., Noll, D. C., and Cohen, J. D. (1997). Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 35, 1373–1380. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00072-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A. (1997). “The automaticity of everyday life,” in The Automaticity Of Everyday Life: Advances In Social Cognition, Vol. 10, ed. R. S. Wyer Jr. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 1–61. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1997.10403402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barha, C. K., Davis, J. C., Falck, R. S., Nagamatsu, L. S., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2017). Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 46:71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basak, C., Qin, S., and O’Connell, M. A. (2020). Differential effects of cognitive training modules in healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment: a comprehensive meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychol. Aging 35, 220–249. doi: 10.1037/pag0000442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Sommer, K. L. (1997). “Consciousness, free choice, and automaticity,” in The Automaticity Of Everyday Life: Advances In Social Cognition, Vol. 10, ed. R. S. Wyer Jr. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 75–81.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering The Greatest Human Strength. New York, NY: Penguin press.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Vohs, K. D. (2016). “Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: assessment, controversies, update,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 54, eds J. M. Olson and M. P. Zanna (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier Academic Press), 67–127. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M., DeWall, C. N., and Oaten, M. (2006). Self-regulation and personality: How interventions increase regulatory success, and how depletion moderates the effects of traits on behavior. J. Pers. 74, 1773–1801. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00428.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., and Vohs, K. D. (2018). The strength model of self-regulation: Conclusions from the second decade of willpower research. Perspect. Psychol. Science 13, 141–145. doi: 10.1177/1745691617716946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgartner, T., Knoch, D., Hotz, P., Eisenegger, C., and Fehr, E. (2011). Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1468–1476. doi: 10.1038/nn.2933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bavelier, D., and Green, C. S. (2019). Enhancing attentional control: lessons from action video games. Neuron 104, 147–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beames, J. R., Schofield, T. P., and Denson, T. F. (2018). “A meta-analysis of improving self-control with practice,” in The Routledge International Handbook Of Self-Control In Health And Well-Being, eds D. de Ridder, M. Adriaanse, and K. Fujita (London: Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group), 405–417. doi: 10.4324/9781315648576-32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchamp, K. G., Kahn, L. E., and Berkman, E. T. (2016). Does inhibitory control training transfer? Behavioral and neural effects on an untrained emotion regulation task. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 1374–1382. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkman, E. T. (2016). “Self-regulation training,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, Third Edition: Research, Theory, And Applications, eds K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 440–457.

Google Scholar

Berkman, E. T., Burklund, L., and Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Inhibitory spillover: Intentional motor inhibition produces incidental limbic inhibition via right inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage 47, 705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkman, E. T., Kahn, L. E., and Merchant, J. S. (2014). Training-induced changes in inhibitory control network activity. J. Neurosci. 34, 149–157. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3564-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernacer, J., Martinez-Valbuena, I., Martinez, M., Pujol, N., Luis, E. O., Ramirez-Castillo, D., et al. (2019). An amygdala-cingulate network underpins changes in effort-based decision making after a fitness program. NeuroImage 203:116181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biazus-Sehn, L. F., Schuch, F. B., Firth, J., and Stigger, F. S. (2020). Effects of physical exercise on cognitive function of older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 89:104048. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2020.104048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. (N. Y.) 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J., and Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnelle, V., Manohar, S., Behrens, T., and Husain, M. (2016). Individual differences in premotor brain systems underlie behavioral apathy. Cereb. Cortex 26, 807–819. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., and Noll, D. C. (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. NeuroImage 5, 49–62. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. L., and Carr, T. H. (1989). Automaticity in skill acquisition: mechanisms for reducing interference in concurrent performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 15, 686–700. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.15.4.686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, C. J., and Tobler, P. N. (2011). Reward skewness coding in the insula independent of probability and loss. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 2415–2422. doi: 10.1152/jn.00471.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, C. J., Brünger, C., Kahnt, T., Park, S. Q., and Tobler, P. N. (2013). Neural integration of risk and effort costs by the frontal pole: only upon request. J. Neurosci. 33, 1706–1713. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3662-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Z., Jiang, W., Yin, J., Chen, Z., Wang, J., and Wang, X. (2020). Effects of Tai Chi Chuan on cognitive function in older adults with cognitive impairment: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Evid. Based Complement Alternat. Med. 2020:6683302. doi: 10.1155/2020/6683302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, W., Cao, X., Hou, C., Li, T., Cheng, Y., Jiang, L., et al. (2016). Effects of cognitive training on resting-state functional connectivity of default mode, salience, and central executive networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:70. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., and McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144, 796–815. doi: 10.1037/xge0000083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cásedas, L., Pirruccio, V., Vadillo, M. A., and Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Does mindfulness meditation training enhance executive control? A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in adults. Mindfulness 11, 411–424. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01279-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Franck, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K., Voss, M. W., Knecht, A. M., Pontifex, M. B., Castelli, D. M., et al. (2013). The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:72. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. S. Y., Deng, K., Wu, J., and Yan, J. H. (2019). Effects of meditation and mind–body exercises on older adults’ cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Gerontologist 59, e782–e790. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnz022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, S. B., Aslan, S., Spence, J. S., Hart, J. J. Jr., Bartz, E. K., Didehbani, N., et al. (2015). Neural mechanisms of brain plasticity with complex cognitive training in healthy seniors. Cereb. Cortex 25, 396–405. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. T., Etnier, J. L., Chan, K. H., Chiu, P. K., Hung, T. M., and Chang, Y. K. (2020). Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 50, 1451–1467. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01292-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis For The Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 3316–3321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400266101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., Dittmann, R. W., et al. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cousins, M. S., and Salamone, J. D. (1994). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions in rats affect relative response allocation in a novel cost/benefit procedure. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 49, 85–91. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90460-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cousins, M. S., Atherton, A., Turner, L., and Salamone, J. D. (1996). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions alter relative response allocation in a T-maze cost/benefit task. Behav. Brain Res. 74, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00151-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 154–166. doi: 10.1002/cne.20749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daly, M., Delaney, L., Egan, M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2015). Childhood self-control and unemployment throughout the life span: evidence from two British cohort studies. Psychol. Sci. 26, 709–723. doi: 10.1177/0956797615569001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dauwan, M., Begemann, M. J. H., Slot, M. I. E., Lee, E. H. M., Scheltens, P., and Sommer, I. E. C. (2021). Physical exercise improves quality of life, depressive symptoms, and cognition across chronic brain disorders: a transdiagnostic systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Neurol. 268, 1222–1246. doi: 10.1007/s00415-019-09493-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 30, 91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Luca, C. R., Wood, S. J., Anderson, V., Buchanan, J.-A., Proffitt, T. M., Mahony, K., et al. (2003). Normative data from the CANTAB. I: development of executive function over the lifespan. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 242–254. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.2.242.13639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denk, F., Walton, M. E., Jennings, K. A., Sharp, T., Rushworth, M. F. S., and Bannerman, D. M. (2005). Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort. Psychopharmacology 179, 587–596. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2059-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., and Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunning, D. L., Griffiths, K., Kuyken, W., Crane, C., Foulkes, L., Parker, J., et al. (2019). The effects of mindfulness-based interventions on cognition and mental health in children and adolescents – a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 244–258. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichholz, A., Schwartz, C., Meule, A., Heese, J., Neumüller, J., and Voderholzer, U. (2020). Self-compassion and emotion regulation difficulties in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 27, 630–639. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R. (1992). Learned industriousness. Psychol. Rev. 99, 248–267. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R., and Adornetto, M. (1986). Generalized self-control of delay and effort. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1020–1031. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.5.1020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R., Masterson, F. A., and McDermitt, M. (1982). Effects of task variety on generalized effort. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 499–505. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R., Mitchell, M., and Masterson, F. A. (1985). Effort training increases generalized self-control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1294–1301. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.5.1294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R., Park, C. D., and Frank, M. (1976). Learned industriousness and social reinforcement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 33, 227–232. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.33.2.227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, R., Weier, F., Masterson, F. A., and Theis, L. Y. (1989). Fixed-ratio schedules increase generalized self-control: preference for large rewards despite high effort or punishment. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 15, 383–392. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.15.4.383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eston, R. G., and Brodie, D. A. (1986). Responses to arm and leg ergometry. Br. J. Sports Med. 20, 4–6. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.20.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairclough, S. H., and Ewing, K. (2017). The effect of task demand incentive onneurophysiological and cardiovascular markers of effort. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 119, 58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falck, R. S., Davis, J. C., Best, J. R., Crockett, R. A., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2019). Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 79, 119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farhat, F., Masmoudi, K., Hsairi, I., Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Mchirgui, R., Triki, C., et al. (2015). The effects of 8 weeks of motor skill training on cardiorespiratory fitness and endurance performance in children with developmental coordination disorder. Appl. Physiol Nutr. Metab. 40, 1269–1278. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2015-0154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Febbraro, G. A. R., and Clum, G. A. (1998). Meta-analytic investigation of the effectiveness of self-regulatory components in the treatment of adult problem behavior. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 18, 143–161. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00008-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., and Horwood, L. J. (2013). Childhood self-control and adult outcomes: results from a 30-year longitudinal study. J. Am. Acad. Child. Psy. 52, 709–717. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, J., Arida, R. M., and Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2017). Physical exercise as an epigenetic modulator of brain plasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 443–456. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishburn, F. A., Norr, M. E., Medvedev, A. V., and Vaidya, C. J. (2014). Sensitivity of fNIRS to cognitive state and load. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:76. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B., and Ghods-Sharifi, S. (2007). Amygdala-prefrontal cortical circuitry regulates effort-based decision making. Cereb. Cortex 17, 251–260. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B., Tse, M. T. L., and Ghods-Sharifi, S. (2008). Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 1966–1979. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., and Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1077–1099. doi: 10.1177/1745691617697076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, H.-T., Gordon, J., Sullivan, K. J., and Winstein, C. J. (2014). Evaluation of attentional demands during motor learning: validity of a dual-task probe paradigm. J. Mot. Behav. 46, 95–105. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2013.868337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes-Neto, M., Bernardone Saquetto, M., Magalhaes Silva, C., Oliveira Carvalho, V., Ribeiro, N., and Sena Conceiçao, C. (2016). Effects of respiratory muscle training on respiratory function, respiratory muscle strength, and exercise tolerance in patients poststroke: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 97, 1994–2001. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2016.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, W., Zang, M., Klich, S., Kawczynski, A., Smoter, M., and Wang, B. (2020). Effect of combined physical and cognitive interventions on executive functions in older adults: a meta-analysis of outcomes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:6166. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17176166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., et al. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 546–573. doi: 10.1177/1745691616652873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’ estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Stat. 6, 107–128. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.78.051505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinzel, S., Lorenz, R. C., Pelz, P., Heinz, A., Walter, H., Kathmann, N., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of training and transfer effects in working memory in older adults. NeuroImage 134, 236–249. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindin, S. B., and Zelinski, E. M. (2012). Extended practice and aerobic exercise interventions benefit untrained cognitive outcomes in older adults: a meta-analysis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 136–141. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03761.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, C. J. (2018). Today’s decisions, tomorrow’s outcomes: does self-control explain the educational smoking gradient? Soc. Sci. Res. 70, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2017.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Zhao, X., Li, B., Cai, Y., Zhang, S., Wan, Q., et al. (2021). Comparative efficacy of various exercise interventions on cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. J. Sport Health Sci. 11, 212–223. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2021.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Im, S., Stavas, J., Lee, J., Mir, Z., Hazlett-Stevens, H., and Caplovitz, G. (2021). Does mindfulness-based intervention improve cognitive function? A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 84:101972. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., and Berkman, E. (2015). Six questions for the resource model of control (and some answers). Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 9-10, 511–524. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., and Teper, R. (2014a). Exploring the mechanisms of self-control improvement. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 302–307. doi: 10.1177/0963721414534256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Macrae, C. N. (2014b). Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., and Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: effort is both costly and valued. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, W. M., Davis, N., Sands, S. A., Whittington, R. A., and Sun, L. S. (2016). Physical activity and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. J. Neurosurg. Anesthesiol. 28, 373–380. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0000000000000349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1918). The Principles In Psychology, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.

Google Scholar

Jansma, J. M., Ramsey, N. F., Coppola, R., and Kahn, R. S. (2000). Specific versus nonspecific brain activity in a parametric n-back task. NeuroImage 12, 688–697. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Google Scholar

Kao, S. C., Huang, C. J., and Hung, T. M. (2013). Frontal midline theta is a specific indicator of optimal attentional engagement during skilled putting performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 35, 470–478. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.5.470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karch, D., Albers, L., Renner, G., Lichtenauer, N., and von Kries, R. (2013). The efficacy of cognitive training programs in children and adolescents—a meta-analysis. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 110, 643–652. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2013.0643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., and Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2014). An empirical comparison of the therapeutic benefits of physical exercise and cognitive training on the executive functions of older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled trials. Neuropsychology 28, 829–845. doi: 10.1037/neuro0000101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennerley, S. W., Walton, M. E., Behrens, T. E., Buckley, M. J., and Rushworth, M. F. (2006). Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 940–947. doi: 10.1038/nn1724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein-Flügge, M. C., Kennerley, S. W., Friston, K., and Bestmann, S. (2016). Neural signatures of value comparison in human cingulate cortex during decisions requiring an effort-reward trade-off. J. Neurosci. 36, 10002–10015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0292-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krafft, C. E., Schwarz, N. F., Chi, L., Weinberger, A. L., Schaeffer, D. J., Pierce, J. E., et al. (2014). An 8-month randomized controlled exercise trial alters brain activation during cognitive tasks in overweight children. Obesity 22, 232–242. doi: 10.1002/oby.20518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurniawan, I. T., Seymour, B., Talmi, D., Yoshida, W., Chater, N., and Dolan, R. J. (2010). Choosing to make an effort: the role of striatum in signaling physical effort of a chosen action. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 313–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00027.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., and Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 661–726. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lampit, A., Hallock, H., and Valenzuela, M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 11:e1001756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lampit, A., Heine, J., Finke, C., Barnett, M. H., Valenzuela, M., Wolf, A., et al. (2019). Computerized cognitive training in multiple sclerosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 33, 695–706. doi: 10.1177/1545968319860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landrigan, J.-F., Bell, T., Crowe, M., Clay, O. J., and Mirman, D. (2020). Lifting cognition: a meta-analysis of effects of resistance exercise on cognition. Psychol. Res. 84, 1167–1183. doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01145-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurence, N. C., Labuschagne, L. G., Lura, B. G., and Hillman, K. L. (2015). Regular exercise enhances task-based industriousness in laboratory rats. PLoS One 10:e0129831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, B. J., Gasson, N., Bucks, R. S., Troeung, L., and Loftus, A. M. (2017). Cognitive training and noninvasive brain stimulation for cognition in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 31, 597–608. doi: 10.1177/1545968317712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., and Stone, A. J. (2020). Combined aerobic and resistance training for cardiorespiratory fitness, muscle strength, and walking capacity after stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 29:104498. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2019.104498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyland, A., Rowse, G., and Emerson, L.-M. (2019). Experimental effects of mindfulness inductions on self-regulation: systematic review and meta-analysis. Emotion 19, 108–122. doi: 10.1037/emo0000425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive functions, and education: Bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 105–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, M. S. M., Cheung, F. Y. L., Kho, J. M., and Tang, C. S.-K. (2020). Childhood adversity and behavioural addictions: the mediating role of emotion dysregulation and depression in an adult community sample. Addict. Res. Theory 28, 116–123. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2019.1594203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, X., Zhang, X., Guo, J., Roberts, C. K., McKenzie, S., Wu, W.-C., et al. (2015). Effects of exercise training on cardiorespiratory fitness and biomarkers of cardiometabolic health: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 4:e002014. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.115.002014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S., Yu, Q., Li, Z., Cunha, P. M., Zhang, Y., Kong, Z., et al. (2020). Effects of acute and chronic exercises on executive function in children and adolescents: a systemic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 11:554915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Pühse, U., Looser, V. N., and Kamijo, K. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 603–612. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0851-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lugo-Candelas, C., Flegenheimer, C., McDermott, J. M., and Harvey, E. (2017). Emotional understanding, reactivity, and regulation in young children with ADHD symptoms. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 1297–1310. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0244-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lurquin, J. H., Michaelson, L. E., Barker, J. E., Gustavson, D. E., Von Bastian, C. C., Carruth, N. P., et al. (2016). No evidence of the ego-depletion effect across task characteristics and individual differences: a pre-registered study. PLoS One 11:e0147770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackworth, J. F. (1964). Performance decrement in vigilance, threshold, and high-speed perceptual motor tasks. Canad. J. Psychol. 18, 209–223. doi: 10.1037/h0083302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansor, N. S., Chow, C. M., and Halaki, M. (2020). Cognitive effects of video games in older adults and their moderators: a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression. Aging Ment. Health 24, 841–856. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1574710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, K., Meeusen, R., Thompson, K. G., Keegan, R., and Rattray, B. (2018). Mental fatigue impairs endurance performance: a physiological explanation. Sports Med. 48, 2041–2051. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0946-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, J. T., and Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 7922–7926. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910662107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKendrick, R., and Harwood, A. (2019). Cognitive workload and workload transitions elicit curvilinear hemodynamics during spatial working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:405. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, S., Richardson, M., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., et al. (2013). The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann. Behav. Med. 46, 81–95. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millett, G., D’Amico, D., Amestoy, M. E., Gryspeerdt, C., and Fiocco, A. J. (2021). Do group-based mindfulness meditation programs enhance executive functioning? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence. Conscious. Cogn. 95:103195. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miró-Padilla, A., Bueichekú, E., Ventura-Campos, N., Flores-Compañ, M.-J., Parcet, M. A., and Ávila, C. (2019). Long-term brain effects of N-back training: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. 13, 1115–1127. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9925-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, H., Wolfensteller, U., Betzel, R. F., Misic, B., Sporns, O., Richiardi, J., et al. (2015). Integration and segregation of large-scale brain networks during short-term task automatization. Nat. Commun. 7:13217. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, P. N. C., Biele, G., and Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Neural processing of risk. J. Neurosci. 30, 6613–6619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero, D., and Lundby, C. (2015). The effect of exercise training on the energetic cost of cycling. Sports Med. 45, 1603–1618. doi: 10.1007/s40279-015-0380-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organ. Res. Methods 11, 364–386. doi: 10.1177/1094428106291059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mowszowski, L., Lampit, A., Walton, C. C., and Naismith, S. L. (2016). Strategy-based cognitive training for improving executive functions in older adults: a systematic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 26, 252–270. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9329-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, T., and Apps, M. A. J. (2019). Motivational fatigue: a neurocognitive framework for the impact of effortful exertion on subsequent motivation. Neuropsychologia 123, 141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mura, G., Carta, M. G., Sancassiani, F., Machado, S., and Prosperini, L. (2018). Active exergames to improve cognitive functioning in neurological disabilities: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 54, 450–462. doi: 10.23736/S1973-9087.17.04680-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muraven, M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol. Bull. 126, 247–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L., Murphy, K., and Andrews, G. (2019). Immediate and long-term efficacy of executive functions cognitive training in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 145, 698–733. doi: 10.1037/bul0000196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L., Murphy, K., and Andrews, G. (2021). A game a day keeps cognitive decline away? A systematic review and meta-analysis of commercially-available brain training programs in healthy and cognitively impaired older adults. Neuropsychol. Rev. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09515-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishiguchi, S., Yamada, M., Tanigawa, T., Sekiyama, K., Kawagoe, T., Suzuki, M., et al. (2015). A 12-week physical and cognitive exercise program can improve cognitive function and neural efficiency in community-dwelling older adults: a randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 63, 1355–1363. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., and Rattray, B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 154–160. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., and Cheng, K. (2006). Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 717–733. doi: 10.1348/135910706X96481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. NeuroImage 27, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osgood, J. M. (2017). Effect of ego-depletion typing task on Stroop does not extend to diverse online sample. J. Artic. Support Null Hypothesis 13, 83–90.

Google Scholar

Ouellette, J. A., and Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol. Bull. 124, 54–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pageaux, B., and Lepers, R. (2018). The effects of mental fatigue on sport-related performance. Prog. Brain Res. 240, 291–315. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.00

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandey, A., Hale, D., Das, S., Goddings, A.-L., Blakemore, S.-J., and Viner, R. M. (2018). Effectiveness of universal self-regulation–based Interventions in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 172, 566–575. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.0232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauli Pott, U., Mann, C., and Becker, K. (2020). Do cognitive interventions for preschoolers improve executive functions and reduce ADHD and externalizing symptoms? A meta analysis of randomized controlled trials. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 30, 1503–1521. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01627-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Koski, L., Caramanos, Z., and Westbury, C. (1998). Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies. Neuroreport 9, R37–R47. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199806220-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H., Dolan, R. J., et al. (2007). How the brain translates money into force: a neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science 316, 904–906. doi: 10.1126/science.1140459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeiffer, K. A., Pivarnik, J. M., Womack, C. J., Reeves, M. J., and Malina, R. M. (2002). Reliability and validity of the Borg and OMNI rating of perceived exertion scales in adolescent girls. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 34, 2057–2061. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000039302.54267.BF

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, A., Steinglass, J. E., Greene, A. L., Weber, E. U., and Blair Simpson, H. (2014). Capacity to delay reward differentiates obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 653–659. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Farrington, D. P., Diamond, B., and Reingle Gonzalez, J. M. (2016). A meta-analysis update on the effectiveness of early self-control improvement programs to improve self-control and reduce delinquency. J. Exp. Criminol. 12, 249–264. doi: 10.1007/s11292-016-9257-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poissant, H., Moreno, A., Potvin, S., and Mendrek, A. (2020). A meta-analysis of mindfulness-based interventions in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact on ADHD symptoms, depression, and executive functioning. Mindfulness 11, 2669–2681. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01458-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Asarnow, R. F., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. J. Neurosci. 25, 5356–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3880-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., and Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 1237–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, R., Trout, A. L., and Schartz, M. (2005). Self-regulation interventions for children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Except. Child. 71, 361–377.

Google Scholar

Ren, F. F., Chen, F. T., Zhou, W. S., Cho, Y. M., Ho, T. J., Hung, T. M., et al. (2021). Effects of Chinese mind-body exercises on executive function in middle-aged and older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:656141. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.656141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, M., Friedrich, A., and Gendolla, G. H. (2008). Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology 45, 869–875. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00688.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. C. (1984). Will power and the virtues. Philos. Rev. 93, 227–247.

Google Scholar

Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., and Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 146, 324–354. doi: 10.1037/bul0000227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudebeck, P. H., Walton, M. E., Smyth, A. N., Bannerman, D. M., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2006). Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1161–1168. doi: 10.1038/nn1756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Ferrigno, S., Yang, J.-H., Rotolo, R. A., and Presby, R. E. (2018). The psychopharmacology of effort-related decision making: dopamine, adenosine, and insights into the neurochemistry of motivation. Pharmacol. Rev. 70, 747–762. doi: 10.1124/pr.117.015107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Cousins, M. S., and Bucher, S. (1994). Anhedonia or anergia? Effects of haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in a T-maze cost/benefit procedure. Behav. Brain Res. 65, 221–229. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90108-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Pérez, N., Inuggi, A., Castillo, A., Campoy, G., García-Santos, J. M., González-Salinas, C., et al. (2019). Computer-based cognitive training Improves brain functional connectivity in the attentional networks: a study with primary school-aged children. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:247. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, L. M. J., Hortobagyi, T., la Bastide-van Gemert, S., van der Zee, E. A., and van Heuvelen, M. J. G. (2019). Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 14:e0210036. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarter, M., Gehring, W. J., and Kozak, R. (2006). More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res. Rev. 551, 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Hoppe, J., Klimesch, W., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F. C. (2007). Dissociation of sustained attention from central executive functions: local activity and interregional connectivity in the theta range. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 587–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05286.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scionti, N., Cavallero, M., Zogmaister, C., and Marzocchi, G. M. (2020). Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:2812. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W. (2019). The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J. Neurosci. 39, 9878–9882. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-17.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shea, B. J., Reeves, B. C., Wells, G., Thuku, M., Hamel, C., Moran, J., et al. (2017). AMSTAR 2: a critical appraisal tool for systematic reviews that include randomised or non-randomised studies of healthcare interventions, or both. BMJ 358:j4008. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j4008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79, 217–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Lieder, F., Kool, W., Griffiths, T. L., Cohen, J. D., et al. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 99–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, D. S., Mauser, J., Nuno, M., and Sherzai, D. (2017). The efficacy of cognitive intervention in Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI): a meta-analysis of outcomes on neuropsychological measures. Neuropsychol. Rev. 27, 440–484. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9363-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behaviour Of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis. New York, NY: Appleton-Century.

Google Scholar

Smith, P. J., Blumenthal, J. A., Hoffman, B. M., Cooper, H., Strauman, T. A., Welsh-Bohmer, K., et al. (2010). Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom. Med. 72, 239–252. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., and Laine, M. (2017). Working memory training revisited: a multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 1077–1096. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, N., Shoemaker, L., and Turner, G. R. (2017). “Executive functions and neurocognitive aging,” in Executive Functions in Health and Disease, ed. E. Goldberg (New York, NY: Academic Press), 169–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanmore, E., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., de Bruin, E. D., and Firth, J. (2017). The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steutel, J. (1999). “The virtues of will-power: self-control and deliberation,” in Virtue Ethics And Moral Education, eds D. Carr and J. Steutel (London: Routledge), 129–142.

Google Scholar

Szutta, N. (2020). The virtues of will-power – from a philosophical and psychological perspective. Ethical Theory Moral Pract. 23, 325–339. doi: 10.1007/s10677-020-10068-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., and Kassai, R. (2019). The efficacy of different interventions to foster children’s executive function skills: a series of meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 145, 653–697. doi: 10.1037/bul0000195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talmi, D., Dayan, P., Kiebel, S. J., Frith, C. D., and Dolan, R. J. (2009). How humans integrate the prospects of pain and reward during choice. J. Neurosci. 29, 14617–14626. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2026-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., and Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72, 271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarter, R. E., Kirisci, L., Mezzich, A., Cornelius, J. R., Pajer, K., Vanyukov, M., et al. (2003). Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2016). Intensive working memory training produces functional changes in large-scale frontoparietal networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 575–588. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00916

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Schwartzman, A. N., Lambert, W. E., and Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS One 4:e6598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vadillo, M. A. (2019). Ego depletion may disappear by 2020. Soc. Psychol. 50, 282–291. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Bailey, S., Meeusen, R., and Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 47, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Wel, P., and van Steenbergen, H. (2018). Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2005–2015. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1432-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassena, E., Gerrits, R., Demanet, J., Verguts, T., and Siugzdaite, R. (2019). Anticipation of a mentally effortful task recruits dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: an fNIRS validation study. Neuropsychologia 123, 106–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veltman, D. J., Rombouts, S. A. R., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 18, 247–256. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00049-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P. (2021). Mindfulness as attention training: meta-analyses on the links between attention performance and mindfulness interventions, long-term meditation practice, and trait mindfulness. Mindfulness 12, 564–581. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01532-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2011). Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:26. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohs, K. D., Schmeichel, B. J., Lohmann, S., Gronau, Q. F., Finley, A. J., Ainsworth, S. E., et al. (2021). A multisite preregistered paradigmatic test of the ego-depletion effect. Psychol. Sci. 32, 1566–1581. doi: 10.1177/0956797621989733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, J. J., and Tschakovsky, M. E. (2018). Exercise and circulating BDNF: Mechanisms of release and implications for the design of exercise interventions. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 43, 1095–1104. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2018-0192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walters, G. D. (2000). Behavioral self-control training for problem drinkers: a meta-analysis of randomized control studies. Behav. Ther. 31, 135–149. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80008-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, M. E., Bannerman, D. M., Alterescu, K., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2003). Functional specialization within medial frontal cortex of the anterior cingulate for evaluating effort-related decisions. J. Neurosci. 23, 6475–6479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-16-06475.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, M. E., Bannerman, D. M., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2002). The role of rat medial frontal cortex in effort-based decision making. J. Neurosci. 22, 10996–11003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10996.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, M. E., Groves, J., Jennings, K. A., Croxson, P. L., Sharp, T., Rushworth, M. F. S., et al. (2009). Comparing the role of the anterior cingulate cortex and 6-hydroxydopamine nucleus accumbens lesions on operant effort-based decision making. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1678–1691. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06726.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S. L., Loh, V., Lampit, A., Bateman, J. E., and Birney, D. P. (2018). Meta-analysis of the effects of computerized cognitive training on executive functions: a cross-disciplinary taxonomy for classifying outcome cognitive factors. Neuropsychol. Rev. 28, 232–250. doi: 10.1007/s11065-018-9374-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsch, L., Alliott, O., Kelly, P., Fawkner, S., Booth, J., and Niven, A. (2021). The effect of physical activity interventions on executive functions in children with ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 20:100379. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2020.100379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C., Yi, Q., Zheng, X., Cui, S., Chen, B., Lu, L., et al. (2019). Effects of mind-body exercises on cognitive function in older adults: a meta-analysis. JAGS 67, 749–758. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, T., Kansaku, K., and Hallett, M. (2004). How self-initiated memorized movements become automatic: a functional MRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1690–1698. doi: 10.1152/jn.01052.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., McNevin, N., and Shea, C. H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54A, 1143–1154. doi: 10.1080/02724980143000118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Ye, M., Wang, L., and Zheng, G. (2021). Effects of physical exercise on executive function in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Physical exercise for executive function. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 114:103810. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2020.103810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Demos, K. E., Leahey, T. M., Hart, C. N., Trautvetter, J., Coward, P., et al. (2014). Failure to replicate depletion of self-control. PLoS One 9:e109950. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, Y., Yang, Y., and Huang, T. (2019). Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 1397–1404. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yakobi, O., Smilek, D., and Danckert, J. (2021). The effects of mindfulness meditation on attention, executive control and working memory in healthy adults: a meta analysis of randomized controlled trials. Cognitive Ther. Res. 45, 543–560. doi: 10.1007/s10608-020-10177-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., and Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child. Dev. Perspect. 6, 354–360.

Google Scholar

Zénon, A., Sidibé, M., and Olivier, E. (2015). Disrupting the supplementary motor area makes physical effort appear less effortful. J. Neurosci. 35, 8737–8744. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Huntley, J., Bhome, R., Holmes, B., Cahill, J., Gould, R. L., et al. (2019). Effect of computerised cognitive training on cognitive outcomes in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open 9:e027062. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Li, C., Zou, L., Liu, X., and Song, W. (2018). The effects of mind-body exercise on cognitive performance in elderly: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ Res. Public Health 15, 2791. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15122791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Hao, Y., Wang, Y., Jor’dan, A., Pascual-Leone, A., Zhang, J., et al. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation reduces the cost of performing a cognitive task on gait and postural control. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 1343–1348. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, L., Li, L., Wang, L., Jin, X., and Zhang, H. (2020). Physical activity for executive function and activities of daily living in AD Patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 11:560461. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Yin, S., Lang, M., He, R., and Li, J. (2016). The more the better? A meta-analysis on effects of combined cognitive and physical intervention on cognition in healthy older adults. Ageing Res. Rev. 31, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, L., Loprinzi, P. D., Yeung, A. S., Zeng, N., and Huang, T. (2019). The beneficial effects of mind-body exercises for people with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 100, 1556–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2019.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive training, effort, executive functions, transfer, exercise training, effortful control, self-control, mindfulness training

Citation: Audiffren M, André N and Baumeister RF (2022) Training Willpower: Reducing Costs and Valuing Effort. Front. Neurosci. 16:699817. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.699817

Received: 24 April 2021; Accepted: 21 March 2022;
Published: 28 April 2022.

Edited by:

Carlos Alos-Ferrer, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Masataka Watanabe, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Japan
Evan C. Carter, United States Army Research Laboratory, United States

Copyright © 2022 Audiffren, André and Baumeister. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michel Audiffren, michel.audiffren@univ-poitiers.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.