Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 05 July 2022
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Smart Mobile Data Collection in the Context of Neuroscience, Volume II View all 6 articles

Daily Contributors of Tinnitus Loudness and Distress: An Ecological Momentary Assessment Study

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Mathematics, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 3Center for Neuroprosthetics, Institute of Bioengineering, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Radiology and Medical Informatics, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Psychology, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 6Institute of Clinical Epidemiology and Biometry, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 7Department of Otolaryngology, University Hospital Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany

Background: Tinnitus is a heterogeneous condition which may be associated with moderate to severe disability, but the reasons why only a subset of individuals is burdened by the condition are not fully clear. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) allows a better understanding of tinnitus by capturing the fluctuations of tinnitus symptoms, such as distress and loudness, and psychological processes, such as emotional arousal, overall stress, mood, and concentration and how these variables interact over time. Whether any of those variables have an influence over the next day, that is, whether any of these variables are auto- or cross-correlated, is still unanswered.

Objectives: Assess whether behavioral and symptom-related data from tinnitus users from the TrackYourTinnitus (TYT) mobile app have an impact on tinnitus loudness and distress on subsequent days.

Methods: Anonymized data was collected from 278 users of the iOS or Android TYT apps between 2014 and 2020. Tinnitus-related distress, tinnitus loudness, concentration level, mood, emotional arousal, and overall stress level were assessed using either a slider or the Wong-Baker Pain FACES scale via a daily survey. Three modeling strategies were used to investigate whether tinnitus loudness and distress are affected by previous days symptoms or psychological processes: auto- and cross correlations, regressions with elastic net regularization, and subgrouping within group iterative multiple model estimation (S-GIMME).

Results: No autocorrelation or cross-correlation was observed at the group level between the variables assessed. However, application of the regression models with elastic net regularization identified individualized predictors of tinnitus loudness and distress for most participants, with the models including contemporaneous and lagged information from the previous day. S-GIMME corroborated these findings by identifying individualized predictors of tinnitus loudness and distress from the previous day.

Discussion: We showed that tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress are affected by the contemporaneous and lagged dynamics of behavioral and emotional processes measured through EMA. These effects were seen at the group, and individual levels. The relevance EMA and the implications of the insights derived from it for tinnitus care are discussed, especially considering current trends toward the individualization of tinnitus care.

Introduction

Tinnitus is a condition in which phantom sounds are perceived without a corresponding external stimulus. Those sounds usually take the form of ringing, hissing, or buzzing, but other less common types of perception have also been reported (Baguley et al., 2013; Langguth et al., 2013). The underlying causes of tinnitus are not fully clear, but auditory pathway deafferentation is commonly recognized as key factor in the etiology of tinnitus (Shore et al., 2016). Although tinnitus is generally a benign condition, its bothersome manifestation, which is estimated to affect 1% of the population (Biswas and Hall, 2020), can be both debilitating (Cima, 2018) and costly (Maes et al., 2013). Tinnitus can be subdivided into two categories: acute and chronic (de Ridder et al., 2021). The former category describes the rather common phenomenon of phantom sounds being perceived for several seconds or minutes after insult to the auditory system (e.g., listening to loud music); whereas the latter category refers to uninterrupted perception of the phantom sound for at least 3 months. In its chronic presentation, tinnitus rarely resolves entirely (Tunkel et al., 2014; Simões et al., 2021).

Available treatments are not effective at suppressing the chronic phantom perception for most sufferers. As a result, most treatment strategies seek to reduce tinnitus-related distress (Tunkel et al., 2014). There is a growing consensus that tinnitus is a heterogeneous condition and that this characteristic may significantly impact both how the condition is experienced by patients and the efficacy of treatments (Simoes et al., 2019; Kleinjung and Langguth, 2020). Factors demonstrated to affect treatment outcomes include sociodemographics, personality, and tinnitus characteristics, though many others are currently being investigated (Genitsaridi et al., 2020). For example, the heritability of tinnitus differs depending on its laterality (i.e., whether sounds are perceived in one or both ears) and the patient's gender (Maas et al., 2017). Furthermore, evidence suggests that personality traits may explain the response to online cognitive behavior therapy treatment (Kleinstäuber et al., 2018), but not acoustic stimulation (Hafner et al., 2020). It is unclear which factors are related to tinnitus-related disability, as the psychoacoustic properties of tinnitus (e.g., laterality, loudness, pitch, type of perceived sound) do not fully explain the tinnitus distress (Cederroth et al., 2019; Kleinjung and Langguth, 2020). Therefore, it is of great clinical interest to understand which factors are associated with distress, especially at the individual level.

The advent of minimally intrusive longitudinal sampling methods has allowed researchers and clinicians to develop predictive models at the individual level, while at the same time maximizing the ecological validity of assessments (Wright and Woods, 2020). Not surprisingly, there is growing interest in using ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in the fields of psychology and psychopathology in general (Wright and Woods, 2020), as it provides clinicians and researchers a tool to investigate the interplay between emotions, symptoms, and behaviors, while considering contextual associations (e.g., how mental phenomena co-occur and interact over time) in naturalistic settings (Shiffman et al., 2008). In the context of tinnitus, Probst et al. (2016), modeled patterns of daily fluctuation of tinnitus characteristics and identified a mediatorial role of tinnitus loudness on stress, while Pryss et al. used EMA to establish that patients often have recollection bias regarding tinnitus fluctuations throughout the day (Pryss et al., 2019). However, most of the studies in the tinnitus field using EMA have focused on group-level analysis. A recurring clinical topic is how uniquely tinnitus is experienced by patients, with different triggers leading to higher tinnitus distress and/or loudness in certain patients, but not others. Identification of factors that could lead to personalized interventions is of great clinical utility.

Therefore, the objective of this study was to apply EMA 206 methods to identify factors (see Table 2) that could be associated with tinnitus loudness and distress at both the individual and group levels. Furthermore, data collected used EMA sampling were used to identify whether such factors influence loudness and/or distress on a subsequent day. To achieve these objectives, we first investigated whether states such as tinnitus loudness, distress, and mood impact autocorrelates or cross-correlates throughout subsequent days at the group level. Second, we modeled data at the individual level using linear regressions with elastic net regularization for each unique time series with tinnitus distress and tinnitus loudness as dependent variables. Third, we used subgrouping within group iterative multiple model estimation (S-GIMME) to obtain unique models for each participant on contemporaneous and lagged effects between the variables collected with EMA.

Methods

Data Preparation

The data analyzed in this study were collected between 2014 and 2020 from the “TrackYourTinnitus” (TYT) app, which is freely available on both Android and iOS mobile devices (available as TrackYourTinnitus). After registration, participants complete an 8-question survey with questions related to their current perception of tinnitus and mood. Users are free to use the app for an indeterminate time and without frequency restrictions. An overview of the variables included in this study is available in Table 2. Two variables were excluded from this study: “do you perceive your tinnitus right now?” and “do you feel irritable right now?”, as those questions were binary and continuous variables were necessary to obtain auto- and cross-correlations. The remaining variables were rated either with a slider (questions 2, 3, 6, 7) or using a variation of the Wong-Baker pain FACES scale [WBS, questions 4–5, (Garra et al., 2013)]. Variables measured with a slider ranged from 0 to 100, and variables measured with the WBS could be answered with a 9-point scale, with each figure representing different levels of discomfort. Both types of variables were treated as continuous (Rhemtulla et al., 2012). Informed consent was obtained from users to have their data anonymously used for scientific purposes. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Regensburg (Study approval number 15-101-0204).

During registration, users were asked to fill-in two questionnaires: the Mini-Tinnitus Questionnaire (Hiller and Goebel, 2004) and the Tinnitus Sample Case History Questionnaire, TSCHQ (Langguth et al., 2007). In addition, users responded to a question about their worst tinnitus-related symptom. Mini-TQ is commonly used in clinical trials and ambulatory assessment as a screening tool for tinnitus-related distress. The questionnaire possesses good psychometric properties (correlation > 0.9 with the original 52-item Tinnitus Questionnaire, test-retest reliability of 0.89, and Cronbach's alpha of 0.9) and consists of 12 questions. The second questionnaire is part of an international effort to standardize data collection and reporting in tinnitus research and is also a standard screening tool. The TSCHQ consists of 34 questions related to tinnitus characteristics (e.g., the type of perceived sounds, duration of tinnitus, subjective loudness), life history (e.g., whether family members also suffer from tinnitus), and common comorbidities (e.g., headaches, insomnia, hearing aids). Both questionnaires were used for the description of the sample.

Regarding the usage of app, users could set push notifications to on or off and were allowed to report their status at any time and as often as they wanted. Only time series datasets with at least 10 days of consecutive sampling were included in the analysis as a compromise between having enough data to model the data and not excluding too many data-points due to its length, as longer streaks of sequential observations become increasingly rarer (See Supplementary Figure 1). If the same user had two sequences of observations that lasted at least 10 days, those two sequences were analyzed independently. The results reported in this article were obtained by selecting the first observation of each day. Missing values from a given sequence were imputed using the “aregImpute” function from the Hmisc package with default settings in R.

Statistical Analysis

Different statistical techniques were used to describe the relation between the variables collected with EMA, both at the group and individual levels. Autocorrelations and cross-correlations were used to obtain statistical associations at the group level, whereas linear regressions with elastic net regularization and unified structural equation modeling were used to obtain individualized models. These methods are described in the following.

Auto- and Cross-Correlation

Autocorrelation can be described as the correlation of a variable with itself at different time lags (Beal and Weiss, 2003). Likewise, cross-correlations measure the correlation between two variables (e.g., tinnitus loudness and distress) at different timepoints. This way, we could estimate whether any of the six variables shown in Table 2 are related with themselves (e.g., autocorrelation) or with one another (e.g., cross-correlation) at subsequent days at the group level.

Elastic Net Regularization

Elastic net regularization is an increasingly popular method intended to account for datasets with large numbers of predictors, especially when those may be correlated. Elastic net shrinks the coefficients of correlated predictors and performs feature selection by setting some of them to 0 (Zou and Hastie, 2005). This property of the model was used to build individualized models for each sequence of observations to predict both tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress.

All variables in the regression were modeled as being linearly related to the outcome measures. A 10-fold cross-validation was computed with the default settings using the cv.glmnet function from the glmnet package. The final model was selected based on the lambda one standard error from the minimum for parsimonious results (Zou and Hastie, 2005).

Unified Structural Equation Modeling

Unified Structural Equation Modeling (uSEM) combines structural equation modeling and vector autoregression and can be used to extract autoregressive and cross-lagged effects from time series. As a result, uSEM has been used widely in psychological and medical science to estimate contemporaneous and lagged effects neuropsychological phenomena such as brain activity recorded from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioral/emotional fluctuations recorded by EMA. Additionally, uSEM allows for estimates to be calculated at the individual, subgroup, and group levels. The validity and reliability of this method in obtaining individualized estimates from intensive longitudinal data sampling have been previously discussed (Wright and Woods, 2020) and explored both with simulated and empirical data (Lane et al., 2019). For this analysis, uSEM was estimated using the gimmeSEM function with the subgrouping option from the gimme package (Gates et al., 2016).

All analyses were performed in R (version 4.0.1, R Core Team, 2018). Mathematical descriptions of auto- and cross-correlations, as well as elastic net regularization and uSEM are presented in the Supplementary Materials. Auto- and cross-correlations were conducted with an internal script adapted by JB from the functions “acf” and “ccf” available in R. The adapted functions calculate weighted average auto- and cross correlations and therefore were applied to multiple sequences of observations.

Results

Table 1 summarizes the demographics of the samples used for both analyses. Of the original dataset, 57% of the data were excluded from the analysis as the data were not obtained from that sequence for at least 10 uninterrupted days (see Supplementary Figure 1). Thus, the sample of study 1 consisted of 488 unique sequences from 278 users. Following the guidelines of the authors of the S-GIMME package, only sequences with at least 60 days of uninterrupted usage were included in the second analysis (Lane et al., 2019). Thus, the sample consisted of 32 sequences from 32 unique users. Table 2 shows how the EMA questions were formulated (translated into English from German), with their abbreviations. Two questions, namely questions 1 and 8, were excluded from the analysis as they were dichotomous and continuous variables were necessary for computing auto- and cross-correlations.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics collected via the TSCHQ during registration in the APP.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. TYT questions included in the study.

Figure 1 illustrates how uniquely tinnitus is uniquely experienced by four arbitrarily selected TYT users. The left column of the figure shows the mean values and their dispersion with density plots; the middle column shows the fluctuation of those variables through time with time series plots; the right column depicts the contemporaneous relation between variables with correlational heat maps. Overall, these four examples highlight how symptoms may be burdensome, how they fluctuate over time, and how they interact with each other.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Variability between four TYT users, each represented in one row (A–D) who completed around 85 days of continuous EMA. Density plots (left), time series (middle) and correlation heat maps (right) highlight how emotional arousal (AR), concentration (CO), tinnitus loudness (LO), mood (MO), stress (ST), and tinnitus distress (TD) interact with each other.

Auto and/or Cross-Correlations Between Tinnitus Loudness, Distress, and Variables Related to Mood

First, we investigated whether the six variables were autocorrelated (see Figure 2). None of the lagged variables was outside the 95% confidence interval (red dashed lines), suggesting that there was no autocorrelation. Autocorrelations in lag 0 were always 1, since the nominators and denominators were identical in those cases (see Supplementary Equation 1). Next, we investigated whether there was cross-correlation between the variables (see Figure 3). Like the previous results, no correlation at lags > 0 was observed. However, corroborating previous findings, we observed contemporaneous correlations (i.e., at lag 0) between loudness & tinnitus distress, tinnitus distress & mood, tinnitus loudness & stress, tinnitus distress & stress, mood & emotional arousal, mood & stress, and arousal & stress.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Autocorrelation of the six variables included in the analysis. Each lag represents a different day of usage. Red dashed lines represent the 95% confidence interval. LO, Loudness; TD, Tinnitus Distress; MO, Mood; AR, Arousal; ST, Stress; CO, Concentration.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Cross-correlation of the potential combinations of the six variables included in the analysis. Each lag represents a different day of usage. Red dashed lines represent the 95% confidence interval. LO, Loudness; TD, Tinnitus Distress; MO, Mood; AR, Arousal; ST, Stress; CO, Concentration.

Individualized Models With Elastic Net Regularization

Next, we investigated whether elastic net regressions could be used for individualized inference about tinnitus loudness (see Figures 4A–C) and tinnitus distress (see Figures 4D–F). For this analysis, contemporaneous variables and lagged variables from the previous days (acronyms ending with “1” in Figures 4A,C,D,F) were used as independent variables in regression setups. For 27% and 31% of the sample, no predictors of loudness and tinnitus distress were found (see Figures 4A,D, respectively). For the remaining sample, the R2 for each time series varied considerably (see Figures 4B,E). Figures 4C,F summarize these findings with box plots. Although certain variables were almost only positively associated with the outcome measures (e.g., stress), other variables presented both positive and negative valence throughout the sample (e.g., concentration and emotional arousal).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Coefficients of penalized regression models with TD (A–C) and LO (D–F) as dependent variables. Sequences in which the dependent variable had no variability were excluded from this analysis. The coefficients and R2 of the individualized models on the Y-axis of (A,B) and (D,E) are aligned. Left column: standardized coefficients uniquely estimated for each sequence. Middle column: the amount of variance explained, measured with R2, for each unique sequence. Right column: The adjusted coefficients of (A,D), and R2, of (B,E) are presented with box and dot plots.

Based on the model coefficients, positive associations were observed between tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress among 314 users (65%) at the contemporaneous level and 17 (3.52%) users at the lagged level. stress was positively associated with tinnitus distress for 133 users (27.5%) and mood was negatively associated with tinnitus distress in 105 (21.7) cases. The associations between tinnitus distress and the remaining variables, positive or negative, were present only among a fraction of users (0.6–11.4%). Apart from the association between loudness and tinnitus distress, few associations with tinnitus loudness (0.2–11%) were observed. Stress was positively associated with tinnitus loudness for 53 (11%) of the users, and mood was negatively associated with 50 users (10.4%).

Idiographic Modeling With S-GIMME

Only time series with at least 60 sequential observations were included in this analysis, yielding a sample size of 32 unique users. The models converged in all cases and a good fit was observed: [average: chi-square (44, N = 32) = 65.56, p = 0.12, comparative fit index = 0.98, root-mean square error of approximation = 0.06, non-normed fit index = 0.96, standardized root mean residual = 0.06]. Individual model fits are available as Supplemental Material.

Figure 5 shows the paths for the four individuals from Figure 1. The variable “Day”, that is, the position in the time series was encoded as an exogenous variable, meaning that the variable could predict any other variables, but not the other way around. Both contemporaneous (solid) and lagged (dashed) paths were obtained in all cases, including the four highlighted cases in Figure 5. Although some paths were shared across subjects, e.g., the effect of tinnitus distress on loudness, other dynamics were idiographic (e.g., whereas stress had a positive contemporaneous effect on tinnitus distress for user shown in Figure 5A, the relationship was inverted for the user shown in Figure 5D, and no relationship was seen for the other two users).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Model estimated by S-GIMME by the same four (A–D) individuals from Figure 1. Solid lines represent a contemporaneous effect (lag = 0), and dashed lines represent lagged effects (lag = 1). The circles represent the variables included in the study. Arrows indicate the direction of the relationship. Red arrows indicate positive regression paths and blue lines indicate negative regression paths.

Description of Subgroups Identified by S-GIMME

S-GIMME identified two subgroups based on the dynamic similarities between variables collected through EMA. Group 1 consisted of 19 individuals, while group 2 consisted of 13 individuals. No differences were observed between the subgroups in terms of mean EMA scores, sociodemographics, or tinnitus characteristics (see Table 3). The paths obtained from S-GIMME for each subgroup are shown in Figure 6. Interestingly, most of the lagged relations were observed in subgroup 1 (i.e., dashed lines), whereas subgroup 2 consisted mainly of contemporaneous (i.e., solid lines), suggesting that S-GIMME could distinguish users whose tinnitus is mostly modulated from contemporaneous effects from users whose tinnitus is modulated by dynamics from the previous day. The green paths shown in Figure 6 highlight effects specific to all members of a subgroup. Of interest, most of the effects specific for subgroup 2 originated from either tinnitus distress or loudness. These findings suggest that S-GIMME could not only distinguish users with contemporaneous or lagged dynamics, but also users whose tinnitus distress and tinnitus loudness were associated with another, and with emotional arousal, stress, and mood (see Figure 6, right).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Description of the 32 users included in the analysis with S-GIMME.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Two subgroups were identified by S-GIMME (left: N = 19, right: N = 13). The black lines represent the paths present at the group level (e.g., paths shared by both subgroups), the green lines represent the paths present at the subgroup level, and the gray lines represent the paths at the individual level. Solid lines represent contemporaneous effects, and dashed lines represent lagged effects. Arrow heads indicate the direction of the relationship.

Discussion

In this study, we identified individuals whose tinnitus loudness and distress are affected by behavioral and emotional processes of the same and of the previous day. We started by showing that at the group level, no evidence of autocorrelation or cross-correlation could be observed between the six EMA variables. However, modeling data at the individual level revealed that tinnitus loudness and distress were auto- and cross-lagged from one day to another for several individuals (see Figures 35). Lastly, we used S-GIMME to explore the unique interplay of these variables, effectively modeling how heterogeneously tinnitus manifests itself over time.

Our results provide a template for how to model tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress at the individual level (see Figures 35). The interaction between these variables constitutes a complex mosaic of how tinnitus is experienced; Although the uniqueness of experiencing tinnitus has been widely acknowledged (Tunkel et al., 2014; Elgoyhen et al., 2015; Cederroth et al., 2019), empirical studies that demonstrate this complex relationship were lacking. Furthermore, using S-GIMME, we were able to incorporate the effect of time into the models, a critical component of experiencing tinnitus (Probst et al., 2017) often neglected in empirical studies due to the challenges of conducting multiple samplings in the traditional ambulatory setting.

Corroborating previous findings, we showed a positive association between stress, tinnitus loudness, and tinnitus distress (Scott et al., 2009; Probst et al., 2016). Interestingly, we also observed that emotional arousal, mood and concentration had ambivalent associations with both loudness and distress (see Figures 4C,F). Such associations could explain why tinnitus is uniquely experienced, and future research could further investigate factors associated with tinnitus distress and loudness, such as behavior, emotional and cognitive dynamics samples from EMA. Tinnitus is known to have potential negative consequences on cognition (Andersson and McKenna, 2009; Mohamad et al., 2016; Neff et al., 2021), but this is the first time a positive association between concentration and tinnitus distress/loudness has been shown. Future studies should further investigate this seemingly paradoxical relationship, especially considering that concentration problems are one of the core domains that tinnitus patients would like to have as outcome measures from clinical interventions (Hall et al., 2018, 2019). For example, this positive association could be related to compensatory mechanisms.

Other core domains include the ability to ignore the phantom perception, its intrusiveness, the sense of control over one's body, quality of sleep, and negative thoughts and beliefs. The first three domains could be investigated by adapting questions from the Tinnitus Functional Index, a widely used, validated questionnaire that captures different dimensions of tinnitus distress (Meikle et al., 2012). Negative thoughts could be investigated by adapting the positive and negative affect schedule (Watson et al., 1988), similar to other studies in the field of psychopathology using EMA (Wright and Woods, 2020; Heller et al., 2021). Sleep problems remain one of the main complaints of clinical tinnitus patients (Tunkel et al., 2014; Asnis et al., 2018; Lu et al., 2020; Inagaki et al., 2021; Richter et al., 2021), but no study to date investigated its effect using an EMA design, despite the evidence of its relevance on other chronic, disabling conditions (Short et al., 2017).

Our work suggests that only a subset of patients experience lagged effects between the variables sampled through EMA (see Figures 46). This finding could be leveraged to deliver personalized interventions through mobile apps, especially among users where tinnitus distress and loudness and auto or cross-correlated. For example, the results suggest that patients with tinnitus may benefit from just-in-time adaptive intervention (JITAI) (Nahum-Shani et al., 2016; Wang and Miller, 2019). JITAI uses mobile sensing to deliver customized interventions based on unique fluctuations recorded by the EMA. Such a system has been used in several fields, including physical health, addiction, and mental care research, but not in tinnitus (Wang and Miller, 2019). For example, customized interventions could be delivered to patients whose tinnitus loudness and/or distress are auto- or cross-correlated over days: Once the algorithm anticipates a potential spike in tinnitus loudness or stress on the next day, an intervention such as psychoeducation tips for coping with tinnitus through push notifications (Unnikrishnan et al., 2020), sound therapy (Tyler et al., 2018; Kutyba et al., 2019, 2021), online delivered cognitive behavior techniques (Weise et al., 2016; Kleinstäuber et al., 2018; Andersson et al., 2019), or meditation techniques (Fitzgerald et al., 2021) could be activated. Future studies could evaluate whether these results are replicated with different sampling frequencies. For example, an EMA every 8 h has been shown to be well tolerated in clinical settings (Shiffman et al., 2008) and would considerably reduce the duration of the study, and potentially increase the adherence to using the APP (Schleicher et al., 2020).

Methodological limitations should be considered when interpreting these results. For example, missing values continue to constitute a significant challenge for researchers, including those using EMA. We implemented a popular and robust method for data imputation; however, empirical evidence that this is the optimal method for imputing data from EMA is not available. A recent study investigated the causes of discontinuation of TYT use (Schleicher et al., 2020), but no clear predictors of adherence to app use were found. Furthermore, results suspected of bias cannot be easily discarded, as only a fraction of users used the app for more than 10 consecutive days (Supplemental Material). Therefore, it is unlikely that our sample is representative of the whole population, and caution should be exercised when interpreting the results and its generalizability. The possibility of low stability from the coefficients obtained from elastic net should also be considered, especially when covariates are highly correlated (Shen et al., 2016). Although providing daily information about tinnitus could increase user distress by repeatedly directing their attention to their tinnitus, previous work has suggested that using the TYT app does not harm users (Schlee et al., 2016). However, randomized control trials investigating the potential side-effects of recording intensive longitudinal data are still missing.

In summary, we show that tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress are affected by both contemporaneous and lagged behavioral and emotional processes measured through EMA. Additionally, we showed that S-GIMME can distinguish users whose tinnitus distress and loudness are modulated by contemporaneous effects from those where those dynamics are modulated by both contemporaneous and lagged effects. Distinguishing these two subgroups could be therapeutical value, especially when aligned with just-in-time adaptive interventions to mitigate or prevent future peaks of tinnitus distress or increased loudness.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available upon request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Regensburg (Study approval number 15-101-0204). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JS, WS, JB, and RP designed the study. JS, WS, and JB analyzed the data. JS, WS, PN, BL, and SM interpreted the results and conclusions. JS wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme, Grant Agreement Number 848261.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.883665/full#supplementary-material

References

Andersson, G., and McKenna, L. (2009). The role of cognition in tinnitus. Acta Otolaryngol. 126(suppl. 556), 39–43. doi: 10.1080/03655230600895226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Titov, N., Dear, B. F., Rozental, A., and Carlbring, P. (2019). Internet-delivered psychological treatments: from innovation to implementation. World Psychiatry 18, 20–28. doi: 10.1002/wps.20610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asnis, G. M., Majeed, K., Henderson, M. A., Sylvester, C., Thomas, M., and Garza, R. D. L. (2018). An examination of the relationship between insomnia and tinnitus: a review and recommendations. Clin. Med. Insights Psychiatry 9, 1179557318781078. doi: 10.1177/1179557318781078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baguley, D., McFerran, D., and Hall, D. (2013). Tinnitus. Lancet 382, 1600–1607. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60142-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beal, D. J., and Weiss, H. M. (2003). Methods of ecological momentary assessment in organizational research. Organ. Res. Methods 6, 440–464. doi: 10.1177/1094428103257361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswas, R., and Hall, D. A. (2020). The behavioral neuroscience of tinnitus. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 51, 3–28. doi: 10.1007/7854_2020_154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cederroth, C. R., Gallus, S., Hall, D. A., Kleinjung, T., Langguth, B., Maruotti, A., et al. (2019). Editorial: towards an understanding of tinnitus heterogeneity. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11, 53. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cima, R. F. F. (2018). Bothersome tinnitus. HNO 66, 369–374. doi: 10.1007/s00106-018-0502-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Ridder, D., Schlee, W., Vanneste, S., Londero, A., Weisz, N., Kleinjung, T., et al. (2021). Tinnitus and tinnitus disorder: theoretical and operational definitions (an international multidisciplinary proposal). Prog. Brain Res. 260, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2020.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elgoyhen, A. B., Langguth, B., de Ridder, D., and Vanneste, S. (2015). Tinnitus: perspectives from human neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 632–642. doi: 10.1038/nrn4003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, B. P., Stocking, C., Ralli, M., and Sheppard, A. (2021). At-home meditation for tinnitus management. Hear. Bal. Commun. 19, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/21695717.2020.1870825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garra, G., Singer, A. J., Domingo, A., and Thode, H. C. (2013). The Wong-Baker pain FACES scale measures pain, not fear. Pediatr. Emerg. Care 29, 17–20. doi: 10.1097/PEC.0b013e31827b2299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, K. M., Lane, S. T., Varangis, E., Giovanello, K., and Guiskewicz, K. (2016). Unsupervised classification during time-series model building. Multivariate Behav. Res. 52, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2016.1256187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genitsaridi, E., Hoare, D. J., Kypraios, T., and Hall, D. A. (2020). A review and a framework of variables for defining and characterizing tinnitus subphenotypes. Brain Sci. 10, 938. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hafner, A., Schoisswohl, S., Simoes, J., Schlee, W., Schecklmann, M., Langguth, B., et al. (2020). Impact of personality on acoustic tinnitus suppression and emotional reaction to stimuli sounds. Prog. Brain Res. 260, 187–203. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2020.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Hibbert, A., Smith, H., Haider, H. F., Londero, A., Mazurek, B., et al. (2019). One size does not fit all: developing common standards for outcomes in early-phase clinical trials of sound-, psychology-, and pharmacology-based interventions for chronic subjective tinnitus in adults. Trends Hear. 23, 2331216518824827. doi: 10.1177/2331216518824827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Smith, H., Hibbert, A., Colley, V., Haider, H. F., Horobin, A., et al. (2018). The COMiT'ID study: developing core outcome domains sets for clinical trials of sound-, psychology-, and pharmacology-based interventions for chronic subjective tinnitus in adults. Trends Hear. 22, 2331216518814384. doi: 10.1177/2331216518814384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heller, A. S., Stamatis, C. A., Puccetti, N. A., and Timpano, K. R. (2021). The distribution of daily affect distinguishes internalizing and externalizing spectra and subfactors. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 130, 319–332. doi: 10.1037/abn0000670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiller, W., and Goebel, G. (2004). Rapid assessment of tinnitus-related psychological distress using the Mini-TQ. Int. J. Audiol. 43, 600–604. doi: 10.1080/14992020400050077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inagaki, Y., Suzuki, N., Oishi, N., Goto, F., Shinden, S., and Ogawa, K. (2021). Personality and sleep evaluation of patients with tinnitus in Japan. Psychiatr. Q. 92, 249–257. doi: 10.1007/s11126-020-09794-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinjung, T., and Langguth, B. (2020). Avenue for future tinnitus treatments. Otolaryngol. Clin. North Am. 53, 667–683. doi: 10.1016/j.otc.2020.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinstäuber, M., Weise, C., Andersson, G., and Probst, T. (2018). Personality traits predict and moderate the outcome of Internet-based cognitive behavioural therapy for chronic tinnitus. Int. J. Audiol. 57, 1–7. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2018.1432902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutyba, J., Jedrzejczak, W. W., Gos, E., Bieńkowska, K., Raj-Koziak, D., and Skarżyński, P. H. (2021). Self-help interventions chosen by subjects with chronic tinnitus—a retrospective study of clinical patients. Int. J. Audiol. 1–6. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2021.1964040. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutyba, J., Jedrzejczak, W. W., Raj-Koziak, D., Gos, E., and Skarzynski, P. H. (2019). Tinnitus sound therapy with a mobile application: case study. J. Hear. Sci. 9, 51–56. doi: 10.17430/1003717

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, S. T., Gates, K. M., Pike, H. K., Beltz, A. M., and Wright, A. G. C. (2019). Uncovering general, shared, and unique temporal patterns in ambulatory assessment data. Psychol. Methods 24, 54–69. doi: 10.1037/met0000192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langguth, B., Goodey, R., Azevedo, A., Bjorne, A., Cacace, A., Crocetti, A., et al. (2007). Consensus for tinnitus patient assessment and treatment outcome measurement: tinnitus research initiative meeting, Regensburg, July 2006. Prog. Brain Res. 166, 525–536. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66050-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langguth, B., Kreuzer, P. M., Kleinjung, T., and de Ridder, D. (2013). Tinnitus: causes and clinical management. Lancet Neurol. 12, 920–930. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70160-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, T., Li, S., Ma, Y., Lai, D., Zhong, J., Li, G., et al. (2020). Positive correlation between tinnitus severity and poor sleep quality prior to tinnitus onset: a retrospective study. Psychiatr. Q. 91, 379–388. doi: 10.1007/s11126-019-09708-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maas, I. L., Brüggemann, P., Requena, T., Bulla, J., Edvall, N. K., Hjelmborg, J. v.B., et al. (2017). Genetic susceptibility to bilateral tinnitus in a Swedish twin cohort. Genet. Med. 19, 1007–1012. doi: 10.1038/gim.2017.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, I. H. L., Cima, R. F. F., Vlaeyen, J. W., Anteunis, L. J. C., and Joore, M. A. (2013). Tinnitus. Ear Hear. 34, 508–514. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31827d113a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meikle, M. B., Henry, J. A., Griest, S. E., Stewart, B. J., Abrams, H. B., McArdle, R., et al. (2012). The tinnitus functional index. Ear Hear. 33, 153–176. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31822f67c0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohamad, N., Hoare, D. J., and Hall, D. A. (2016). The consequences of tinnitus and tinnitus severity on cognition: a review of the behavioural evidence. Hear. Res. 332, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahum-Shani, I., Smith, S. N., Spring, B. J., Collins, L. M., Witkiewitz, K., Tewari, A., et al. (2016). Just-in-time adaptive interventions (JITAIs) in mobile health: key components and design principles for ongoing health behavior support. Ann. Behav. Med. 52, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9830-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neff, P., Simões, J., Psatha, S., Nyamaa, A., Boecking, B., Rausch, L., et al. (2021). The impact of tinnitus distress on cognition. Sci. Rep. 11, 2243. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81728-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Probst, T., Pryss, R., Langguth, B., and Schlee, W. (2016). Emotional states as mediators between tinnitus loudness and tinnitus distress in daily life: results from the “TrackYourTinnitus” application. Sci. Rep. 6, 20382. doi: 10.1038/srep20382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Probst, T., Pryss, R. C., Langguth, B., Rauschecker, J. P., Schobel, J., Reichert, M., et al. (2017). Does tinnitus depend on time-of-day? An ecological momentary assessment study with the “trackyourtinnitus” application. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9, 253. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pryss, R., Probst, T., Schlee, W., Schobel, J., Langguth, B., Neff, P., et al. (2019). Prospective crowdsensing versus retrospective ratings of tinnitus variability and tinnitus–stress associations based on the TrackYourTinnitus mobile platform. Int. J. Data Sci. Anal. 8, 327–338. doi: 10.1007/s41060-018-0111-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., and Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychol. Methods 17, 354–373. doi: 10.1037/a0029315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, K., Zimni, M., Tomova, I., Retzer, L., Höfig, J., Kellner, S., et al. (2021). Insomnia associated with tinnitus and gender differences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 3209. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18063209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlee, W., Pryss, R. C., Probst, T., Schobel, J., Bachmeier, A., Reichert, M., et al. (2016). Measuring the moment-to-moment variability of tinnitus: the trackyourtinnitus smart phone app. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 294. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schleicher, M., Unnikrishnan, V., Neff, P., Simoes, J., Probst, T., Pryss, R., et al. (2020). Understanding adherence to the recording of ecological momentary assessments in the example of tinnitus monitoring. Sci. Rep. 10, 22459. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79527-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, B., Lindberg, P., Melin, L., and Lyttkens, L. (2009). Predictors of tinnitus discomfort, adaptation and subjective loudness. Br. J. Audiol. 24, 51–62. doi: 10.3109/03005369009077842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., Han, B., and Braverman, E. (2016). Stability of the elastic net estimator. J. Complex. 32, 20–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jco.2015.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., and Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4, 1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shore, S. E., Roberts, L. E., and Langguth, B. (2016). Maladaptive plasticity in tinnitus — triggers, mechanisms and treatment. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 12, 150–160. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Short, N. A., Allan, N. P., and Schmidt, N. B. (2017). Sleep disturbance as a predictor of affective functioning and symptom severity among individuals with PTSD: an ecological momentary assessment study. Behav. Res. Ther. 97, 146–153. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simoes, J., Neff, P., Schoisswohl, S., Bulla, J., Schecklmann, M., Harrison, S., et al. (2019). Toward personalized tinnitus treatment: an exploratory study based on internet crowdsensing. Front. Public Health 7, 157. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2019.00157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simões, J. P., Neff, P. K. A., Langguth, B., Schlee, W., and Schecklmann, M. (2021). The progression of chronic tinnitus over the years. Sci. Rep. 11, 4162. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83068-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunkel, D. E., Bauer, C. A., Sun, G. H., Rosenfeld, R. M., Chandrasekhar, S. S., Cunningham, E. R., et al. (2014). Clinical practice guideline. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 151(2_suppl.), S1–S40. doi: 10.1177/0194599814547475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, R. S., Owen, R. L., Bridges, J., Gander, P. E., Perreau, A., and Mancini, P. C. (2018). Tinnitus suppression in cochlear implant patients using a sound therapy app. Am. J. Audiol. 27, 1–8. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJA-17-0105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unnikrishnan, V., Schleicher, M., Shah, Y., Jamaludeen, N., Pryss, R., Schobel, J., et al. (2020). The effect of non-personalised tips on the continued use of self-monitoring mHealth applications. Brain Sci. 10, 924. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., and Miller, L. C. (2019). Just-in-the-moment adaptive interventions (JITAI): a meta-analytical review. Health Commun. 35, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2019.1652388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS Scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weise, C., Kleinstäuber, M., and Andersson, G. (2016). Internet-delivered cognitive-behavior therapy for tinnitus. Psychosom. Med. 78, 501–510. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. G. C., and Woods, W. C. (2020). Personalized models of psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 16, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-102419-125032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, H., and Hastie, T. (2005). Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 67, 301–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tinnitus, subtyping, ecological momentary assessment, distress, mood, mental health, loudness

Citation: Simoes J, Bulla J, Neff P, Pryss R, Marcrum SC, Langguth B and Schlee W (2022) Daily Contributors of Tinnitus Loudness and Distress: An Ecological Momentary Assessment Study. Front. Neurosci. 16:883665. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.883665

Received: 25 February 2022; Accepted: 02 June 2022;
Published: 05 July 2022.

Edited by:

Julian Keil, University of Kiel, Germany

Reviewed by:

Takwa Gabr, Kafrelsheikh University, Egypt
Don J. McFerran, Colchester Hospital University NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Simoes, Bulla, Neff, Pryss, Marcrum, Langguth and Schlee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jorge Simoes, jorge.simoes@ukr.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.