Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 06 December 2023
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploring Goal-Directed Behavior Through Creativity: Perspectives from Psychology, Neuroscience, and Psychiatry View all 6 articles

Individual differences and creative ideation: neuromodulatory signatures of mindset and response inhibition

Radwa Khalil
&#x;Radwa Khalil1*Sergio Agnoli,&#x;Sergio Agnoli2,3Serena Mastria&#x;Serena Mastria4Angela KondinskaAngela Kondinska1Ahmed A. Karim,,&#x;Ahmed A. Karim1,5,6Ben Godde&#x;Ben Godde1
  • 1School of Business, Social and Decision Sciences, Constructor University, Bremen, Germany
  • 2Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
  • 3Marconi Institute for Creativity, Sasso Marconi, Italy
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Clinic Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 6Department of Health Psychology and Neurorehabilitation, SRH Mobile University, Riedlingen, Germany

This study addresses the modulatory role of individual mindset in explaining the relationship between response inhibition (RI) and divergent thinking (DT) using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Forty undergraduate students (22 male and 18 female), aged between 18 and 23 years (average age = 19 years, SD = 1.48), were recruited. Participants received either anodal tDCS of the right IFG coupled with cathodal tDCS of the left IFG (R + L−; N = 19) or the opposite coupling (R−L+; N = 21). We tested DT performance using the alternative uses task (AUT), measuring participants’ fluency, originality, and flexibility in the response production, as well as participants’ mindsets. Furthermore, we applied a go-no-go task to examine the role of RI before and after stimulating the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) using tDCS. The results showed that the mindset levels acted as moderators on stimulation conditions and enhanced RI on AUT fluency and flexibility but not originality. Intriguingly, growth mindsets have opposite moderating effects on the change in DT, resulting from the tDCS stimulation of the left and the right IFG, with reduced fluency but enhanced flexibility. Our findings imply that understanding neural modulatory signatures of ideational processes with tDCS strongly benefits from evaluating cognitive status and control functions.

Introduction

Creative ideation and response inhibition

Several candidates have been put forward to explain the information-processing mechanisms that underlie individual differences in creativity (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; Vartanian et al., 2007; Dorfman et al., 2008; Colom et al., 2010; Abraham, 2018). One of the potential explanatory mechanisms at the basis of individual differences in creativity is the disinhibition hypothesis (Hodin, 1954); highly creative individuals utilize cognitive disinhibition to access ordinarily hidden associations from conscious awareness (Schnier and Kris, 1953; Koestler, 1975; Eysenck, 1995; Carson, 2014). Studies suggest that high cognitive control supports the selection and actuation of more effective task strategies and suppresses interference from common and inappropriate response tendencies (Beaty and Silvia, 2012), which supports specific creative performances (Benedek and Fink, 2019). In terms of inhibiting salient but irrelevant responses, cognitive control appears to facilitate creative ideation. Inhibitory control (IC)1 is one of the core elements characterizing cognitive control in creative ideation (Benedek et al., 2012; Zabelina et al., 2019; Khalil et al., 2020, 2023a; Palmiero et al., 2022). It refers to suppressing increasing proactive interference of previous responses and resisting interference from potentially attention-capturing processes or contents (Harnishfeger, 1995; Diamond, 2013) to promote steady access to unrelated concepts and ideas, allowing for higher creativity.

There is a common agreement that IC involves cognitive and behavioral inhibition (Aron, 2007). Cognitive inhibition suppresses previously activated cognitive contents or processes, removes irrelevant acts or attention from consciousness, and resists attention-capturing processes or contents (Harnishfeger, 1995). Therefore, cognitive inhibition refers to inhibiting thoughts and memories as interference control (Diamond, 2013).

Behavioral inhibition includes (a) selective or focused attention, which refers to inhibition at the level of attention and hence attentional inhibition, and (b) response inhibition (RI), which refers to inhibition at the level of behavioral responses or motor actions and thus, to self-control and discipline (Diamond, 2013). At the neural level, it has been suggested that inhibitory processes fall within fronto-striatal neural circuits, including the basal ganglia and dorsolateral and ventrolateral parts of the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Aron, 2007; Munakata et al., 2012a,b; Bari and Robbins, 2013; Aron et al., 2014). People should immediately suppress spontaneous answers and explore unique thoughts to generate original ideas (Cassotti et al., 2016; Agnoli et al., 2020; Khalil et al., 2020). IC is, thus, a critical process for generating creative ideas. However, more research is required to fully comprehend the mechanistic relationship (Palmiero et al., 2022).

Recently, researchers using the transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) method highlighted particular brain dynamics sustaining IC that emerge to benefit creative thinking (Khalil et al., 2020; Li et al., 2022). Brain stimulation/neuromodulation techniques, such as tDCS, are gradually gaining recognition in the neuroscience of creativity (Lucchiari et al., 2018). These techniques facilitate the measurement of causality to understand the involvement of brain dynamics in the expression of creative behavior.

Hemispheric modulation of divergent thinking by tDCS

Research has mostly examined the functional lateralization of creativity, and the validity of the right brain hypothesis for the neuroscience of creativity is a virtual battleground for creativity researchers (Dietrich, 2004, 2019; Zaidel, 2013; Abraham, 2018). Indeed, the right brain hypothesis has been questioned (Abraham et al., 2012; Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2013; Beaty et al., 2014, 2017, 2018; Abraham, 2018), as creativity is based on information processing across several brain networks (Beaty et al., 2015, 2016, 2019; Beaty and Schacter, 2017). The temporal dynamics of divergent thinking (DT) were associated with brain activity (especially in the alpha frequency band) in both the right and left posterior cerebral regions when cerebral dynamics predicted the emergence of several indices of DT, such as originality and flexibility (Agnoli et al., 2020; Mastria et al., 2022).

The persistence of the creative right brain idea is remarkable, especially considering the earliest advocates of lateralization research have highlighted the significance of the specificity of each hemisphere in creativity (Bogen and Bogen, 1969; Miran and Miran, 1984; Hoppe, 1988). Stimulation techniques can be used to manipulate brain activity in the two hemispheres, and several researchers have suggested that tDCS may have the potential to boost creativity, even if other studies did not show any effect of tDCS on creative performance (Lucchiari et al., 2018; see Table 1 of Li et al., 2022).

Research suggests that bilateral stimulation of the PFC has a greater influence on creative cognition than unilateral stimulation (Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory, 2015; Khalil et al., 2020; PeÑa et al., 2021; Xiang et al., 2021). The study by Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory (2015) indeed supported the balance hypothesis, which states that DT requires a balance of frontal lobe function (right and left IFG). Results of this study revealed that left cathodal (i.e., inhibitory) and right anodal (i.e., excitatory) stimulation enhanced DT scores, while the reverse condition did not.

It has been observed that right anodal and left cathodal stimulation (R + L−) improved fluency in the IFG (Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory, 2015; Hertenstein et al., 2019; Khalil et al., 2020). IFG is the brain area involved in controlled retrieval from semantic memory (Binder et al., 2009), associative thinking (e.g., metaphor; Mashal et al., 2007), and creative writing (Shah et al., 2013). Other findings indicated that anodal stimulation over the dorsolateral PFC is more likely to contribute to originality relative to cathodal stimulation over the left ventrolateral PFC/inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Groborz and Necka, 2003; Beaty et al., 2016; Murray, 2017; Ghanavati et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2022). Stimulation site/protocol and target brain area variance may explain this inconsistency. Nevertheless, they suggested the complicated influence of both the left and right hemispheres on creative thinking.

The role of individual differences in terms of mindset

To understand the individual differences in creative performance, we suggest a differential approach taking into account both cerebral dynamics and behavioral-attitudinal dynamics. In the present study, we focused specifically on the differential role exerted by individual mindsets on the DT brain dynamics as stimulated through tDCS. Our mindsets influence indeed how we perceive the world and ourselves (Dweck, 2006, 2012; Bittner and Heidemeier, 2013; Gilead et al., 2014). In her seminal work, Dweck (2006) contrasted a growth mentality with a fixed mindset. In fixed mindsets, people believe they cannot enhance their intellectual abilities and feel helpless when assigned complex tasks. Therefore, they refrain from trying new ideas or problem-solving methods. In contrast, a growth mindset promotes self-esteem, openness, and cognitive flexibility. Thus, a growth mindset may encourage creativity, unlike a fixed attitude. Changing one’s perspective from fixed to growing can readily increase creativity. As a result, evaluating the role of mindsets in the context of individual differences in creative-divergent performance should be taken into account. Although mindset is central to most decision-making and perception research (Schwabe, 2004; Bhanji and Beer, 2012; Gilead et al., 2014; Hege et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018; Huijsmans et al., 2019; Muenks et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2020; Frank-Podlech et al., 2021; Hecht et al., 2021), it is still relatively ignored in the creativity literature.

Study hypotheses

The present work started from the results obtained in a previous study (Khalil et al., 2020), which revealed that IFG brain activity was altered by tDCS as an explanatory neural mechanism of the link between RI and DT. In the current work, we address a different research question using the data collected in the previous research, considering mindset as a prospective moderator. Adding to the previous research (Khalil et al., 2020), we implemented additional scoring methods for DT performance in a more comprehensive modality. DT indeed quantifies participants’ ideational indices, such as fluency (numbers of produced ideas), flexibility (numbers of semantic categories used to produce alternative ideas), and originality—a reliable measure of creative potential (Guilford, 1950; Runco and Acar, 2012; see also Agnoli et al., 2023).

In the research by Khalil et al. (2020), RI predicted DT performance by modulating left-to-right IFG activity efficiency; nevertheless, tDCS stimulation did not improve all creative ideation scores. For example, increased originality and flexibility, but not fluency, resulted from cathodal-tDCS (c-tDCS) stimulation targeting the left IFG coupled with anodal-tDCS (a-tDCS) over the right IFG (R + L−). Remarkably, RI had a moderation effect, as tDCS only enhanced originality and flexibility in the stimulation condition of R + L− when d’ (a measure extracted from the Go No Go Task-GNGT- measuring RI) was enhanced. This finding initiated a follow-up question of whether this enhanced d’ level (i.e., the moderation effect of RI) might reflect a specific state of mind that facilitates or attenuates the effects of tDCS on DT. The dependencies of the tDCS effect on the neuro-cognitive states of the participants have been suggested previously (Learmonth et al., 2015; Hsu et al., 2016). We expect that individual differences in mindset, as a cognitive state of mind, would help explain part of the variance of the moderation effect of d′ on the changes induced by tDCS on DT.

For this reason, we hypothesized that the participants’ mindset level could moderate the effect of tDCS and RI on the creative performance indices. Specifically, we expected that the change in creative performance induced by tDCS stimulation would not be influenced only by the RI level, as demonstrated in the previous study (Khalil et al., 2020), but also by participants’ mindsets. Whereas we expected to find the main and interactive effects of tDCS and RI on the change in DT, as an exploratory hypothesis - due to the lack of previous literature on the topic - we also expected an interaction effect of mindset with the previous variables on DT performance.

Methods

Participants

The sample included 40 undergraduate students (22 males and 18 females) from Constructor University (formerly Jacobs University) who were compensated with course credits or monetary rewards. Age ranged from 18 to 23 years (M = 19, SD = 1.48). The experimental protocol conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki and followed local ethical standards and German law. All participants underwent an eligibility screening for the tDCS, had no previous experience with this experiment’s materials and tDCS procedures, and gave written informed consent before starting the experiment. Participants with neurologic, medical, or psychiatric disorders were excluded from the study. All participants were right-handed based on the Edinburgh inventory assessments (Oldfield, 1971).

Mindset questionnaire

Based on Dweck (2006) growth mindset research, the Mindset Questionnaire was adapted from Diehl (2008) study (Appendix). This questionnaire was retrieved from http://www.classroom20.com/forum/topics/motivating-students-with.

This self-reported questionnaire contained 20 items assessing participants’ mindsets; a four-point Likert scale; participants had to choose “strongly agree,” “agree,” “disagree,” or “strongly disagree.” After evaluating the four Likert scale questionnaires, the participants’ scores ranged from 20 to 49 points.

Alternative uses task

The paper and pencil version of the Alternative Uses Task (AUT; Guilford et al., 1960) was used. Each participant was asked to write down alternative uses for two everyday objects (i.e., brick and paperclip) within 2 min (cf. Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory, 2015) after becoming familiar with an example of the uses for a newspaper. The words of the objects were all new in the pre-and post-tests and counterbalanced across participants.

Three measures of participants’ creative performance were derived from the AUT: fluency, flexibility, and originality (using external raters). Fluency was scored as the total number of valid responses generated by each participant. Flexibility was calculated by averaging the total number of conceptual categories utilized per object at the subject level according to pre-existing categories extracted ad hoc based on our data set (Reiter-Palmon et al., 2019), weighted for each participant’s total number of responses. Originality was calculated by involving two raters who independently judged the originality of each participant’s response (Silvia et al., 2008), showing good consistency and agreement with each other (Inter-rater reliability was operationalized via intraclass correlations (ICCs) across the two raters). ICCs ranged from 69 to 1.00 for AUT measures (fluency, flexibility, and originality). For each object, the responses were transcribed into a spreadsheet and alphabetically ordered within each object. The ID associated with each participant was then hidden. This procedure guaranteed that the serial position of the ideas did not bias the evaluation, as did the total number of the ideas in the set, the participant who generated the idea, and the preceding and successive ideas. The judges were asked to read all the responses before scoring them. Response originality was rated on a 1 (= not at all original) to 5 (= highly original) scale, according to one of the most accepted scoring methods (Silvia et al., 2008). Raters were instructed to consider and weigh three dimensions, i.e., uncommonness, remoteness, and cleverness (Forthmann et al., 2017). Specifically, uncommonness refers to the fact that an idea is not ordinarily encountered in participants’ responses; remoteness refers to the distance of an idea from what is commonly thought; and cleverness includes the concepts of imaginativeness, smartness, and funniness (Silvia et al., 2008; see also Forthmann et al., 2017). Only when ideas were evaluated highly on all three dimensions did raters score them as highly original (i.e., 5). In cases of enormous discrepancies between ratings, the judges reviewed their responses and assigned a score by consensus. The mean originality score for each idea was calculated from the ratings of the two raters. An average originality score per participant was obtained as the average originality for the two objects.

Go-NOGO task

The GNGT was implemented as described in the study by Swick et al. (2008). In Times New Roman size 140 font, lowercase black letters were presented on a white screen. The letter x represented the NoGo stimulus; in contrast, all the other alphabet letters referred to the Go stimuli. The participants were informed about pressing a lever once the Go stimulus was presented while inhibiting their response to the NoGo stimulus. The duration of stimuli was 200 ms, and interstimulus intervals were 1,500 ms. Two paradigms were used based on the proportion of Go/No-Go trials (50/50 and 90/10). The 50/50 paradigm included 70 Go/70 NoGo, while the 90/10 paradigm involved 126 Go/14 NoGo. The participants were familiarized with GNGT through a short practice set of 30 trials (50/50 Go/NoGo stimuli, randomly intermixed) and had a minute break between the 2 blocks.

Based on signal detection theory (Green and Swets, 1966), we used hit rate (HR) and false alarm (FA) rate to calculate the sensitivity index d-prime (d′) for accuracy, where increasing values of d′ refer to higher sensitivity to a given signal (i.e., GO stimuli). Using the linear log approach of Hautus (1995), HR and FA were adjusted to avoid values of 0 and 1, whereby 0.5 was added to HR and FA and 1 was added to Go and NoGo trials. The difference between the standardized (Z-transformed) HR and FA rate probability (d′ = zHR – zFA; Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999) is computed as d’.

Transcranial direct current stimulation

A battery-driven stimulator (Schneider Electronic, Gleichen, Germany) was used to apply tDCS over the left and right IFG, as shown in Figure 1. A constant current of 1 mA was applied via two saline-soaked sponge electrodes covering an area of 4 × 6 cm. tDCS was applied for 30 min with a 10 s ramp up and down (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000; Nitsche et al., 2003).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic illustration of the methodological procedure. The experiment is designed to determine the effect of tDCS (pre- and post-) on DT through measuring mindset and RI. Pre-tDCS measures involve the mindset questionnaire, AUT, and Go NoGO (GNGT). The participants were randomly assigned to two conditions. In one condition, a-tDCS was applied to the right IFG and c-tDCS to the left IFG. In the second condition, the other half of the participants received the opposite coupling. After the tDCS (post-tDCS), the performance in AUT and GNGT was measured.

An EEG cap was used to localize and mark the positions of F7 and F8 on the scalp, which were associated with the left and right IFG, respectively (Ozawa et al., 2014). We chose a bilateral bipolar-balanced montage to stimulate the left IFG (F7) and inhibit its right-hemispheric counterpart (F8) and vice versa (Neuling et al., 2012; Nasseri et al., 2015). This bilateral configuration resembles that used in previous studies on creativity (Chrysikou et al., 2013; Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory, 2015; Lucchiari et al., 2018; Khalil et al., 2020) and is particularly suitable to test the balance hypothesis (Mayseless and Shamay-Tsoory, 2015; Khalil et al., 2020, 2023b).

Procedure

Once participants had read the participant information sheet and signed the consent form, they were asked to complete a mindset self-report questionnaire. All participants took part in two tasks, namely, AUT and GNGT, before and after brain stimulation by tDCS. Participants were randomly divided into two stimulation groups (which we referred to as two conditions): one condition refers to anodal stimulation of right IFG coupled with cathodal stimulation of left IFG (i.e., R + L−), and the second condition refers to cathodal stimulation of right IFG coupled with anodal stimulation of left IFG (i.e., R−L+); Figure 1.

After the experiment, participants were orally examined to ensure they could not distinguish between the stimulation conditions. Potential tDCS side effects were evaluated with a questionnaire administered immediately at the end of the experimental session. All participants were verbally debriefed after the experimental session. The duration of the experiment was around 60 min, excluding 5 min for the introduction, the overview of the experiment, and signing the consent form.

Data analysis

To investigate the influence of the direction of tDCS stimulation (left vs. right IFG), the RI level, participants’ mindset, as well as their interactions, on the change in the creative-divergent performance, changes in originality, fluency, and flexibility were evaluated in three separate generalized linear mixed (GLM) models and treated as dependent variables using SPSS software, version 26. Robust error estimation was used to rule for the possible effect of outliers and violations of normality assumptions (Wu, 2009), controlling for the random effect of subjects.

Given that we were interested in the change in the creative performance between the two measurement sessions before and after the stimulation, an index of the change for each creative measure was derived by computing the difference (delta ∆) between the pre-stimulation performance and the post-stimulation performance in DT: positive values indicate an increase in creative performance after the stimulation, whereas negative values indicate a decrease in performance.

A series of GLM models (one per each creative performance index: ∆originality, ∆flexibility, and ∆fluency) were performed, entering tDCS CONDITION (two levels: R + L−, R−L+) as between-subjects fixed factor. The interactions (two and three-way) between tDCS CONDITION, RESPONSE INHIBITION (as measured through d’delta 90 as derived from the GNGT in the pre-stimulation session), and MINDSET (as measured through the scores obtained in the 4-point Likert scale) were added as fixed interaction factors to the models. Data and analysis code for this study are available upon request to the corresponding author.

Results

Originality

The first GLM model on the changes in originality did not show any significant main or interaction effects (all Fs < 1.748, ps > 0.191; see Supplementary Table).

Flexibility

The GLM model on flexibility first showed a main effect of the tDCS CONDITION, revealing that the stimulation of the left IFG was associated with an increase of ideational flexibility, F1,31 = 5.617, p = 0.024, b = 5.571, 95% CI = [0.777, 10.364]; Figure 2A.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Changes in creative performance (flexibility – panel A – and fluency – panel B) as a function of RI are measured in GNGT and the Mindset level. Note. Changes in flexibility and fluency are expressed in delta values: positive values indicate an increase in performance after the stimulation compared to the pre-stimulation condition; negative values indicate a decrease in performance after the stimulation.

Moreover, two-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION and MINDSET emerged (F2,31 = 3.945, p = 0.03), which showed that in the tDCS stimulation of the left IFG (R−L+ condition), an increase of mindset scores (i.e., from fixed to growth) was associated with a decrease of flexibility, b = −0.158, t2.31 = −2.780, p = 0.009, 95% CI = [−0.274, −0.042], whereas no effect of mindset emerged in the right (R + L−) IFG stimulation condition, b = 0.016, t2.31 = 0.404, p = 0.689, 95% CI = [−0.066, 0.099].

This model showed a two-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION and RESPONSE INHIBITION (F2,31 = 3.391, p = 0.03), which highlighted that with the tDCS stimulation of the left IFG, an increase of RI was associated with a decrease in flexibility, b = −1.390, t2.31 = −2.757, p = 0.010, 95% CI = [−2.418, −0.362], whereas no effect of RI level emerged in the tDCS stimulation of the right IFG, b = 0.180, t2.31 = 0.550, p = 0.586, 95% CI = [−0.486, 0.846]. Nevertheless, these two-way interactions were further explained by a three-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION, MINDSET, and RESPONSE INHIBITION, F2,31 = 4.052, p = 0.027. Specifically, whereas with the right IFG stimulation, no interaction between MINDSET and RESPONSE INHIBITION emerged (p = 0.607), in the left IFG stimulation condition, analyses revealed an interaction between MINDSET and RESPONSE INHIBITION, b = 0.042, t2.31 = 2.799, p = 0.009, 95% CI = [0.011, 0.073].

As shown in Figure 2A, a simple slopes analysis emphasized the moderator role of participants’ mindset on RI over its association with flexibility, b = 0.043, SE = 0.018, t = 2.301, p = 0.036; specifically, after the stimulation of the left IFG, an increase of RI was associated with an increase of flexibility only at high mindset scores (i.e., growth mindset), b = 0.477, SE = 0.205, t = 2.327, p = 0.034. In contrast, no association between RI and flexibility emerged at medium (i.e., neither fixed nor growth mindset), b = 0.112, SE = 0.167, t = 0.671, p = 0.512, and at low (i.e., growth mindset) mindset scores, b = −0.253, SE = 0.253, t = −1.0092, p = 0.332.

Fluency

The final GLM model on the changes in fluency showed a two-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION and MINDSET (F2,31 = 6.950, p < 0.01). In the tDCS stimulation of the right IFG (R + L− condition) an increase of mindset scores (i.e., from fixed to growth) was associated with an increase in fluency, b = 0.584, t2.31 = 3.700, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [0.262, 0.906], whereas no effect of mindset emerged in the left (R−L+) IFG stimulation condition, b = 0.136, t2.31 = 0.456, p = 0.650, 95% CI = [−0.486, 0.740].

This model showed a two-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION and RESPONSE INHIBITION (F2,31 = 4.934, p = 0.014), which indicated that with the tDCS stimulation of the right IFG, an increase of RI was associated with an increase in fluency, b = 3.501, t2.31 = 2.880, p = 0.007, 95% CI = [1.021, 5.980], whereas no effect emerged with the left IFG stimulation condition, b = 4.597, t2.31 = 1.255, p = 0.219, 95% CI = [−2.874, 12.068].

Finally, a three-way interaction between tDCS CONDITION, MINDSET, and RESPONSE INHIBITION emerged, F2,31 = 6.032, p < 0.01. With the left IFG stimulation, no interaction between MINDSET and RESPONSE INHIBITION emerged (p = 0.425), in contrast, in the right IFG stimulation condition, analyses revealed an interaction between MINDSET and RESPONSE INHIBITION, b = −0.124, t2.31 = −3.378, p = 0.002, 95% CI = [−0.199, −0.049].

As shown in Figure 2B, in the right IFG stimulation condition, a simple slopes analysis showed the moderator role of participants’ mindset on RI over its association with fluency, b = −0.124, t = −2.662, p = 0.017. Precisely, after the right IFG stimulation, an increasing score of RI was associated with a decrease in fluency at high participants’ mindset scores (i.e., growth mindset), b = 1.649, SE = 0.590, t = −2.792, p = 0.013, but not at medium (i.e., neither fixed nor growth mindset), b = −0.552, SE = 0.365, t = −1.511, p = 0.150, and at low (i.e., fixed mindset) mindset scores, b = −0.544, SE = 0.508, t = 1.072, p = 0.299.

Discussion

In this article, we examined the possibility of mindset being an explicatory factor for the interaction between RI and the neuromodulation induced by tDCS on DT. Interestingly, the mindset levels acted as moderators on stimulation conditions (i.e., the effectiveness of shifting activity from left to right IFG and vice versa using tDCS) and RI on the change of fluency and flexibility but not originality. The question arises: Why do mindsets moderate the change in fluency and flexibility but not the change in originality? One could argue that fluency and flexibility reflect quantitative measures and are thus conceptualized in the mindset frame as reflecting the mental operations related to ideation quantity. Such an interpretation is not feasible for originality (a qualitative aspect of creative ideation). It is also possible that this effect is an artifact of our method, which requires participants to produce alternative uses for everyday objects in a limited period. Originality requires time to emerge; consequently, only low levels of originality can be captured in the short duration of the experiments.

Intriguingly, growth mindsets had opposite moderator effects on the change of DT when high levels of RI were present in both stimulation conditions (which shows the effectiveness of shifting activity). These effects on DT were manifested in reduced fluency but enhanced flexibility. For the R + L− stimulation condition, a decrease in fluency was associated with high levels of d’ at high mindset scores (i.e., a growth mindset). On the contrary, high fluency scores were associated in this stimulation condition with a growth mindset when associated with low d’ levels. A different effect emerged in the R−L+ condition, especially on the flexibility index: an increase in flexibility was associated with high mindset scores (i.e., growth mindset) when associated with high d’ levels. For example, growth mindsets determined an enhanced level of d’, which is required for such processing and necessitates a temporary weakening of RI as a source of cognitive control. Therefore, the mindset modulated the effect of RI on the effectiveness of the activity shift between the left and right IFG on ideational fluency and flexibility.

These differential effects of the cortical activity shift on fluency and flexibility observed using the opposing stimulation condition could be explained using the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. This construct is one of the leading theoretical frameworks for clarifying differential bilateral frontal cortical activations (Dietrich, 2004). Given this hypothesis, we can argue that fluency and flexibility perform opposing flow experience functions, and accordingly, the activity shifts differ in these two indices for processing unconscious information and creative ideation.

What does it mean that activation of anodal right IFG is coupled with cathodal left IFG for fluency, while anodal left IFG is coupled with cathodal right IFG for flexibility? A possible explanation is that this is due to the right IFG’s association with language, which resonates with the nature of fluency (Marini et al., 2016; Gao et al., 2020). In contrast, the left IFG is associated with intention, self-regulation, and planning, functions involved in approach motivation and action-oriented processing (Petrides and Milner, 1982; Knight and Grabowecky, 1995; Tomarken and Keener, 1998; Kuhl, 2000), which aligns more with the mental operations involved in flexibility.

Regarding the empirical evidence on how mindset affects activity shift in IFG and creative ideation in the context of RI using tDCS, the jury is out, as the picture is mainly incomplete, particularly in creativity literature and using non-invasive brain stimulation techniques. These are still early times for investigating creative neurocognition through neuroscientific techniques. A more extensive and critical discussion about the utility and validity of these methods in delivering key insights about brain function will necessarily impact how we view the relevance of these findings for the field of neuroscience of creativity.

Concluding remarks, limitations and future directions

Our report shows that mindset, specifically growth mindsets, act as a cognitive moderator on the activity shift of left/right and right/left IFG through the different levels of RI. This moderating role enhances flexibility and fluency, even if associated with high and low levels of RI, respectively (cf. Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The summary graph illustrates how individual differences in creative ideation (fluency and flexibility) depend on cognitive control (i.e., RI) and cognitive status (i.e., mindset). The transient hypofrontality hypothesis (green left-right arrow) describes how stimulation can effectively shift cortical activity from left to right (R+L−) and vice versa (R−L+). The x-axis depicts mindset as a cognitive moderator on the activity shift of left/right and right/left IFG through the different levels of RI (y-axis). The y-axis refers to the increase in d’ of the RI level. The mindset axis refers to the increase in mindset scores from low (i.e., fixed mindset) to medium to high (i.e., growth mindset); the x-axis starts in the middle, directed by arrows, denoting an increase toward the left (flexibility) and right (fluency). The up and down arrows on the x-axis refer to the changes in fluency and flexibility when increasing mindset (i.e., growth mindset, x-axis) but low RI (y-axis). The up and down arrows on the y-axis refer to the changes in fluency and flexibility when increasing RI (i.e., d’, y-axis) but low mindset (i.e., fixed mindset, x-axis). The up and down arrows in the orthogonal boxes on the right and left sides refer to the changes in fluency and flexibility indices when high RI is associated with growth mindsets. The colors of the up and down arrows correspond to the colors of fluency (purple) and flexibility (blue).

Brain non-invasive methods such as tDCS and its application to creative ideation can benefit from considering individual cognitive status (i.e., mindset) tendencies to provide valuable insights into ideation neural processes, mainly when RI is evaluated. Our findings provide the first exploration cues toward a research agenda in which mindset, RI, and creative ideation are closely intertwined using tDCS. Hence, a better understanding of the link between effective activity shifts in the frontal areas (i.e., IFG) and creative ideation in the context of RI will allow us to move to the second step and address open questions related to mental operations in creativity literature.

To date, our report presents a novel perspective on the moderator role of mindset (as cognitive status, particularly the growth mindset) on DT. This idea of cognitive status is rooted in the psychoanalytic tradition, where Harris (1953) suggested that creativity results from the ability to easily fluctuate between “primary-process” and “secondary-process” cognition, which was cited in Schnier and Kris (1953). On the one hand, the primary process is free-associative, analogical, and concrete. Generally, it occurs in states of distractibility like fantasy, reverie, and dreaming but also occasionally during abnormal states, as in some forms of psychosis. On the other hand, the secondary process reflects the abstract and logical thought processes grounded in conscious reality.

It is therefore relevant to highlight that our interpretations should be read in light of their limitations, particularly given that investigations of mindset in the creativity literature are still in a largely exploratory state. One of the challenges of implementing mindset in creativity research is that there are several definitions of mindset across different relational contexts (social psychology vs. educational psychology vs. cognitive psychology; Herz et al., 2020).

Although our findings are merely exploratory, they offer a direction to refine current conceptualizations in both creative ideation’s cognitive and neural perspectives. We considered the influence of individual differences and highlighted their signatures, through mindset, on shaping creative ideation in the context of RI. For these reasons, the nature of the context in which creative idea generation is called for the mental operations beyond it cannot be disregarded.

To assemble the puzzle regarding understanding neural correlates and cortical activation of creative ideation, neuroscience research agendas must consider the variety of neuroscientific methodologies (Abraham, 2013, 2018; Khalil et al., 2019, 2023a; Khalil and Moustafa, 2022; Khalil and Demarin, 2023). Some researchers established neuroscientific methods and imaging analysis techniques have yet to find wide use in creativity research, notably combined methods (e.g., simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography; see Ullsperger, 2010). These fascinating techniques have been rarely implemented in the study of creativity. Moreover, the implementation of multivariate pattern analyses (Haxby et al., 2014), with a large sample of participants, and longitudinal approaches (Skup, 2010) could benefit creativity research, which is uncommon in the study of creativity (Schlegel et al., 2015). Estimating brain network structures using Bayesian approaches is another possibility. Durante and Dunson (2018) revealed that highly creative individuals often assemble inter-hemispheric connections. Consequently, the call for applying advances in neuroscientific methods could result in further progress in creativity research.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethical Committee at Constructor University Bremen. The studies were conducted following local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

RK, AK, AAK, and BG conceptualized and conducted the research. SA and SM analyzed the data. RK, SA, and SM drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2023.1238165/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^IC has generally been discussed as synonymous with inhibition, supporting a departure from functional fixedness by inhibiting typical or prepotent responses (Chrysikou, 2018, 2019; Palmiero et al., 2022).

References

Abraham, A. (2013). The promises and perils of the neuroscience of creativity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:246. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abraham, A. (2018). The neuroscience of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Abraham, A., Beudt, S., Ott, D. V. M., and Yves von Cramon, D. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: dissociations between frontal, parietal–temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Res. 1482, 55–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnoli, S., Mastria, S., Zanon, M., and Corazza, G. E. (2023). Dopamine supports idea originality: the role of spontaneous eye blink rate on divergent thinking. Psychol. Res. 87, 17–27. doi: 10.1007/s00426-022-01658-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., and Corazza, G. E. (2020). Predicting response originality through brain activity: an analysis of changes in EEG alpha power during the generation of alternative ideas. NeuroImage 207:116385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist 13, 214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aziz-Zadeh, L., Liew, S.-L., and Dandekar, F. (2013). Exploring the neural correlates of visual creativity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 475–480. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bari, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 108, 44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Barry Kaufman, S., and Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Sci. Rep. 5:10964. doi: 10.1038/srep10964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., et al. (2014). Creativity and the default network: a functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia 64, 92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Christensen, A. P., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). Creative constraints: brain activity and network dynamics underlying semantic interference during idea production. NeuroImage 148, 189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Benedek, M., Chen, Q., et al. (2018). Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 1087–1092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713532115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). “Creativity, self-generated thought, and the Brain's default network” in The creative self: Effect of beliefs, self-efficacy, mindset, and identity. (Eds). M. Karwowski and J. C. Kaufman (Netherlands: Elsevier), 171–183.

Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., and Schacter, D. L. (2019). Network neuroscience of creative cognition: mapping cognitive mechanisms and individual differences in the creative brain. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 27, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., and Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 319:9171. doi: 10.1037/a0029171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., and Fink, A. (2019). Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: the role of memory, attention, and cognitive control. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 27, 116–122. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 53, 480–485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhanji, J. P., and Beer, J. S. (2012). Taking a different perspective: Mindset influences neural regions that represent value and choice. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 782–793. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bittner, J., and Heidemeier, H. (2013). Competitive mindsets, creativity, and the role of regulatory focus. Think. Skills Creat. 9, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2013.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogen, J. E., and Bogen, G. M. (1969). The other side of the brain. 3. The corpus callosum and creativity. Bull. Los Angel. Neurol. Soc. 34, 191–220.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carson, S. (2014). “Cognitive disinhibition, creativity, and psychopathology” in The Wiley handbook of genius. (Ed). D. K. Simonton (US: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd), 198–221.

Google Scholar

Cassotti, M., Agogué, M., Camarda, A., Houdé, O., and Borst, G. (2016). Inhibitory control as a Core process of creative problem solving and idea generation from childhood to adulthood. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2016, 61–72. doi: 10.1002/cad.20153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrysikou, E. G. (2018). “The costs and benefits of cognitive control for creativity” in The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity. (Eds). R. E. Jung and O. Vartanian (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Chrysikou, E. G. (2019). Creativity in and out of (cognitive) control. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 27, 94–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). Non-invasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 81–89. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2013.768221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colom, R., Karama, S., Jung, R. E., and Haier, R. J. (2010). Human intelligence and brain networks. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 12, 489–501. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.4/rcolom

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, E. (2008). Motivating students with mindset coaching and how brains work (Dweck). Available at: https://www.classroom20.com/forum/topics/motivatingstudents

Google Scholar

Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 746–761. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietrich, A. (2019). Where in the brain is creativity: a brief account of a wild-goose chase. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 27, 36–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorfman, L., Martindale, C., Gassimova, V., and Vartanian, O. (2008). Creativity and speed of information processing: a double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personal. Individ. Differ. 44, 1382–1390. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durante, D., and Dunson, D. B. (2018). Bayesian inference and testing of group differences in brain networks. Bayesian Anal. 13, 29–58. doi: 10.1214/16-BA1030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. (2006). “Mindset: the new psychology of success” in Mindset: The new psychology of success. (New York: Random House Publishing Group).

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. (2012). Mindset: How you can fulfil your potential. London: Constable & Robinson.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Forthmann, B., Holling, H., Çelik, P., Storme, M., and Lubart, T. (2017). Typing speed as a confounding variable and the measurement of quality in divergent thinking. Creat. Res. J. 29, 257–269. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2017.1360059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank-Podlech, S., Watson, P., Verhoeven, A. A. C., Stegmaier, S., Preissl, H., and de Wit, S. (2021). Competing influences on healthy food choices: Mindsetting versus contextual food cues. Appetite. 166:105476. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Guo, X., Liu, C., Mo, Y., and Wang, J. (2020). Right inferior frontal gyrus: an integrative hub in tonal bilinguals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 2152–2159. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghanavati, E., Salehinejad, M. A., Nejati, V., and Nitsche, M. A. (2019). Differential role of prefrontal, temporal and parietal cortices in verbal and figural fluency: implications for the supramodal contribution of executive functions. Sci. Rep. 9:3700. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-40273-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilead, M., Liberman, N., and Maril, A. (2014). From mind to matter: neural correlates of abstract and concrete mindsets. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 638–645. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. G., and Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons Inc.

Google Scholar

Groborz, M., and Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control: explorations of generation and evaluation skills. Creat. Res. J. 15, 183–197. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2003.9651411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. Am. Psychol. 5, 444–454. doi: 10.1037/h0063487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P., Christensen, P. R., Merrifield, P. R., and Wilson, R. C. (1960). Alternate Uses (ALTUS) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. doi: 10.1037/t06443-000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). “The development of cognitive inhibition” in Interference and inhibition in cognition. (Eds). F. N. Dempster and C. J. Brainerd (Netherlands: Elsevier), 175–204.

Google Scholar

Harris, H. I. (1953). Review of Psychoanalytic explorations in art [Review of the book Psychoanalytic explorations in art, by E. Kris]. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 23, 658–659. doi: 10.1037/h0096625

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hautus, M. J. (1995). Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d′. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 27, 46–51. doi: 10.3758/BF03203619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J., Connolly, A. C., and Guntupalli, J. S. (2014). Decoding neural representational spaces using multivariate pattern analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 435–456. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hecht, C. A., Yeager, D. S., Dweck, C. S., and Murphy, M. C. (2021). Beliefs, affordances, and adolescent development: lessons from a decade of growth mindset interventions. Advan. Child Develop. Behav. 61, 169–197. doi: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2021.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hege, M. A., Veit, R., Krumsiek, J., Kullmann, S., Heni, M., Rogers, P. J., et al. (2018). Eating less or more – mindset induced changes in neural correlates of pre-meal planning. Appetite 125, 492–501. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertenstein, E., Waibel, E., Frase, L., Riemann, D., Feige, B., Nitsche, M. A., et al. (2019). Modulation of creativity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimul. 12, 1213–1221. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herz, N., Baror, S., and Bar, M. (2020). Overarching states of mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 184–199. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodin, J. P. (1954). The creative process: a symposium, ed. Brewster Ghiselin . College Art Journal, 13, 147–148. doi: 10.1080/15436322.1954.11465853

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoppe, K. D. (1988). Hemispheric specialization and creativity. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 11, 303–315. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(18)30482-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, T. Y., Juan, C. H., and Tseng, P. (2016). Individual differences and state-dependent responses in transcranial direct current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:643. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, F., Song, Y., Jiang, Y., Zhao, Q., and Luo, J. (2022). Where and how are original and valuable ideas generated? tDCS of the generation-related posterior temporal lobe and the executive control-related prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 32, 1004–1013. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huijsmans, I., Ma, I., Micheli, L., Civai, C., Stallen, M., and Sanfey, A. G. (2019). A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 11699–11704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818572116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., and Demarin, V. (2023). Creative therapy in health and disease: inner vision. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 1–19. doi: 10.1111/cns.14266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., Godde, B., and Karim, A. A. (2019). The link between creativity, cognition, and creative drives and underlying neural mechanisms. Front. Neural Circuits 13:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., Karim, A. A., and Godde, B. (2023b). Less might be more: 1 mA but not 1.5 mA of tDCS improves tactile orientation discrimination. IBRO Neurosci. Reports 15, 186–192. doi: 10.1016/j.ibneur.2023.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., Karim, A. A., Kondinska, A., and Godde, B. (2020). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of left and right inferior frontal gyrus on creative divergent thinking are moderated by changes in inhibition control. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 1691–1704. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02081-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., Lin, L., Karim, A. A., and Godde, B. (2023a). Response inhibition partly mediates the relationship between emotional states and creative divergent thinking. Creat. Res. J. 35, 596–613. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2023.2192605

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, R., and Moustafa, A. A. (2022). A neurocomputational model of creative processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 137:104656. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2022.104656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, R. T., and Grabowecky, M. (1995). “Escape from linear time: prefrontal cortex and conscious experience” in The cognitive neurosciences. (Ed). M. S. Gazzaniga (US: The MIT Press), 1357–1371.

Google Scholar

Koestler, Arthur . (1975). The act of creation. New York: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Kuhl, J. (2000). “A functional-design approach to motivation and self-regulation” in Handbook of self-regulation. (Eds). M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (Netherlands: Elsevier), 111–169.

Google Scholar

Learmonth, G., Thut, G., Benwell, C. S. Y., and Harvey, M. (2015). The implications of state-dependent tDCS effects in aging: Behavioural response is determined by baseline performance. Neuropsychologia 74, 108–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Beaty, R. E., Luchini, S., Dai, D. Y., Xiang, S., Qi, S., et al. (2022). Accelerating creativity: effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on the temporal dynamics of divergent thinking. Creat. Res. J. 35, 169–188. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2022.2068297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucchiari, C., Sala, P. M., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2018). Promoting creativity through transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). A critical review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marini, A., Galetto, V., Tatu, K., Duca, S., Geminiani, G., Sacco, K., et al. (2016). Recovering two languages with the right hemisphere. Brain Lang. 159, 35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2007). An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain Lang. 100, 115–126. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastria, S., Agnoli, S., Corazza, G. E., Grassi, M., and Franchin, L. (2022). What inspires us? An experimental analysis of the semantic meaning of irrelevant information in creative ideation. Think. Reason. 29, 698–725. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2132289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayseless, N., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2015). Enhancing verbal creativity: modulating creativity by altering the balance between right and left inferior frontal gyrus with tDCS. Neuroscience 291, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miran, M., and Miran, E. (1984). Cerebral asymmetries: neuropsychological measurement and theoretical issues. Biol. Psychol. 19, 295–304. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(84)90044-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muenks, K., Canning, E. A., LaCosse, J., Green, D. J., Zirkel, S., Garcia, J. A., et al. (2020). Does my professor think my ability can change? Students’ perceptions of their STEM professors’ mindset beliefs predict their psychological vulnerability, engagement, and performance in class. J. Exp. Psychol: General. 149, 2119–2144. doi: 10.1037/xge0000763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., and O’reilly, R. C. (2012a). A unified framework for inhibitory control executive control over inhibition. Trend Cognit. Sci. 15, 453–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011.A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., and Chatham, C. H. (2012b). Developing cognitive control: three key transitions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 71–77. doi: 10.1177/0963721412436807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L. L. (2017). Design fluency subsequent to onset of aphasia: a distinct pattern of executive function difficulties? Aphasiology 31, 793–818. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2016.1261248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasseri, P., Nitsche, M. A., and Ekhtiari, H. (2015). A framework for categorizing electrode montages in transcranial direct current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuling, T., Wagner, S., Wolters, C. H., Zaehle, T., and Herrmann, C. S. (2012). Finite-element model predicts current density distribution for clinical applications of tDCS and tACS. Front. Psychol. 3:83. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Nitsche, M. S., Klein, C. C., Tergau, F., Rothwell, J. C., and Paulus, W. (2003). Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 600–604. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00412-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527, 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozawa, S., Matsuda, G., and Hiraki, K. (2014). Negative emotion modulates prefrontal cortex activity during a working memory task: a NIRS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmiero, M., Fusi, G., Crepaldi, M., Borsa, V. M., and Rusconi, M. L. (2022). Divergent thinking and the core executive functions: a state-of-the-art review. Cogn. Process. 23, 341–366. doi: 10.1007/s10339-022-01091-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

PeÑa, J., Sampedro, A., GÓmez-Gastiasoro, A., Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N., Zubiaurre-Elorza, L., Aguiar, C., et al. (2021). The effect of changing the balance between right and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on different creativity tasks: a transcranial random noise stimulation study. J. Creat. Behav. 55, 899–915. doi: 10.1002/jocb.496

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 20, 249–262. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., Forthmann, B., and Barbot, B. (2019). Scoring divergent thinking tests: a review and systematic framework. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 13, 144–152. doi: 10.1037/aca0000227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an Indicator of creative potential. Creat. Res. J. 24, 66–75. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.652929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegel, A., Alexander, P., Fogelson, S., Li, X., Lu, Z., Kohler, P. J., et al. (2015). The artist emerges: visual art learning alters neural structure and function. NeuroImage 105, 440–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Rev. 84, 1–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnier, J., and Kris, E. (1953). Ernst Kris, psychoanalytic explorations in art. College Art J. 13, 55–57. doi: 10.1080/15436322.1953.11465833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, C. W. (2004). Keynote address: the calculus of disease-importance of an integrating mindset. Prev. Vet. Med. 62, 193–205.

Google Scholar

Shah, C., Erhard, K., Ortheil, H.-J., Kaza, E., Kessler, C., and Lotze, M. (2013). Neural correlates of creative writing: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1088–1101. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., et al. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2, 68–85. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skup, M. (2010). Longitudinal fMRI analysis: a review of methods. Stat. Interface 3, 235–252. doi: 10.4310/SII.2010.v3.n2.a10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swick, D., Ashley, V., and Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 9:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomarken, A. J., and Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: a self-regulatory perspective. Cognit. Emot. 12, 387–420. doi: 10.1080/026999398379655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullsperger, M. (2010). Simultaneous EEG and fMRI. Oxford University Press New York.

Google Scholar

Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., and Kwiatkowski, J. (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 1470–1480. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Dai, J., Li, J., Wang, X., Chen, T., Yang, X., et al. (2018). Neuroanatomical correlates of grit: Growth mindset mediates the association between gray matter structure and trait grit in late adolescence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1688–1699. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, L. (2009). Mixed effects models for complex data. US: CRC press.

Google Scholar

Xiang, S., Qi, S., Li, Y., Wang, L., Dai, D. Y., and Hu, W. (2021). Trait anxiety moderates the effects of tDCS over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) on creativity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 177:110804. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Wu, X., Huang, L., and Luo, S. (2020). Residential mobility mindset enhances temporal discounting in the loss framework. Physiol. Behav. 225:113107. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabelina, D. L., Friedman, N. P., and Andrews-Hanna, J. (2019). Unity and diversity of executive functions in creativity. Conscious. Cogn. 68, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaidel, D. W. (2013). Split-brain, the right hemisphere, and art: fact and fiction. Prog. Brain Res. 204, 3–17. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63287-6.00001-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: individual differences, mindset, tDCS, creative ideation, response inhibition, neuromodulation, divergent thinking

Citation: Khalil R, Agnoli S, Mastria S, Kondinska A, Karim AA and Godde B (2023) Individual differences and creative ideation: neuromodulatory signatures of mindset and response inhibition. Front. Neurosci. 17:1238165. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1238165

Received: 10 June 2023; Accepted: 10 November 2023;
Published: 06 December 2023.

Edited by:

Amadi Ogonda Ihunwo, University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa

Reviewed by:

Lucas Murrins Marques, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Chen-Yao Kao, National University of Tainan, Taiwan

Copyright © 2023 Khalil, Agnoli, Mastria, Kondinska, Karim and Godde. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Radwa Khalil, rkhalil@constructor.university

†ORCID: Radwa Khalil, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2632-8306

Sergio Agnoli, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3004-7988

Serena Mastria, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4987-8255

Ahmed Karim, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1763-1565

Ben Godde https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9101-7286

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.