Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 22 September 2015
Sec. Neuropharmacology

Sensorimotor gating impairments induced by MK-801 treatment may be reduced by tolerance effect and by familiarization in monkeys

\r\nPatricia G. SalettiPatricia G. Saletti1Rafael S. MaiorRafael S. Maior1Etsuro HoriEtsuro Hori2Hisao NishijoHisao Nishijo2Carlos Tomaz,*Carlos Tomaz1,3*
  • 1Primate Center and Laboratory of Neurosciences and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biology, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
  • 2System Emotional Science, Graduate School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan
  • 3Neurosciences Research Group, Universidade CEUMA, São Luís, Brazil

Dizocilpine (MK-801) is a non-competitive NMDA antagonist that induces schizophreniclike effects. It is therefore widely used in experimental models of schizophrenia including prepulse inhibition (PPI) impairments in rodents. Nevertheless, MK-801 has never been tested in monkeys on a PPI paradigm. In order to evaluate MK-801 effects on monkeys’ PPI, we tested eight capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) using three different doses of MK-801 (0.01; 0.02; 0.03 mg/kg). Results show PPI impairment in acute administration of the highest dose (0.03 mg/kg). PPI impairment induced by MK-801 was reversed by re-exposure to the PPI test throughout treatment trials, in contrast with rodent studies. These results indicate that tolerance effect and familiarization with PPI test may reduce the sensorimotor gating deficits induced by MK-801 in monkeys, suggesting a drug-training interaction.

Introduction

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) impairment is characteristic of schizophrenic patients (Braff et al., 2001a; Ludewig et al., 2003), and also usually observed in rodents after MK-801 injection (Long et al., 2006; Arai et al., 2008; Ishii et al., 2010; Gururajan et al., 2011; Gomes et al., 2014; Khella et al., 2014; Park et al., 2014). PPI test is characterized as a slight stimulus (prepulse) presented at a specific interval before the startling stimulus (pulse), leading to a reduction of the startle response. In this sense, PPI allows the evaluation of sensorimotor gating mechanisms through behavioral responses (Graham, 1975; Braff and Geyer, 1990; Geyer et al., 2001). Deficits in PPI reflect sensorimotor gating abnormalities that are prominent in schizophrenic patients (Braff et al., 2001a,b; Mackeprang et al., 2002; Kumari et al., 2003; Ludewig et al., 2003; Preuss et al., 2011). Since PPI test is a simple and direct way to measure a deficit in information processing, it is largely used in animal models, especially in rodents (e.g., Long et al., 2006; Arai et al., 2008; Ishii et al., 2010; Gururajan et al., 2011; Gomes et al., 2014; Khella et al., 2014; Park et al., 2014).

Hypofunction of glutamatergic receptors has been regarded as one of the explanations for the psychopathology of schizophrenia. As others NMDA antagonists—phencyclidine (PCP) and ketamine—MK-801 is extremely common in schizophrenia model studies. This is due to the fact that these drugs trigger schizophrenic-like effects as seen in several studies with rodents. It has been demonstrated that MK-801 induces recognition memory impairments, hyperactivity and hyperlocomotion (Bradford et al., 2010; Park et al., 2014; Basurto et al., 2015), learning, memory and spatial memory impairments (Hikichi et al., 2013; Karamihalev et al., 2014), social recognition deficits (Yoshimi et al., 2015), as well as impairments in cognitive set-shifting (Svoboda et al., 2015), and prepulse inhibition disruption (PPI) in rodents (Long et al., 2006; Arai et al., 2008; Ishii et al., 2010; Gururajan et al., 2011; Gomes et al., 2014; Khella et al., 2014; Park et al., 2014).

Studies with non-human primates have demonstrated that MK-801 is effective at inducing schizophrenic-like effects. Ogura and Aigner (1993), using rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) demonstrated that MK-801 impairs visual recognition memory. In the same way, Buffalo et al. (1994) performed an Operant Test Battery, also in rhesus using MK-801. They showed MK-801 effectively disrupted animals accuracy in learning, motivation, short-term memory, and color discrimination tasks. Working memory is also impaired by acute and chronic MK-801 treatment in rhesus (Tsukada et al., 2005). Likewise, in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus jacchus), MK-801 impaired acquisition of shape discrimination and visuospatial conditional tasks using the Wisconsin General Test Apparatus (Harder et al., 1998; Harder and Ridley, 2000). A more recent study with rhesus monkeys evidenced that a single administration of MK-801 at doses of 0.02 and 0.04 mg/kg was able to impair spatial working memory (Wang et al., 2012).

To our knowledge, the effects of MK-801 on PPI have not yet been investigated in primates. Since there are significant morphological and neuropharmacological differences between rodents and primates (e.g., Maior et al., 2011), we employed a whole-body PPI protocol (Saletti et al., 2014) in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) to evaluate MK-801 effects on sensorimotor gating.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the Institute of Biology, University of Brasilia (UnBDOC no 131791/2013). Furthermore, the procedures were conducted according to guidelines of the Brazilian Society of Animal Experimentation and followed the Principles of Laboratory Animal Care (NIH publication no. 85-23, revised 1996).

Experiments were performed in Primate Center at University of Brasilia, Brazil. Animals were allocated into pairs or triads in home cages (3 × 3 × 1.8 m) with natural substrate, rope swings and nest boxes. In their home cages, they were given access to food twice a day, once early in the morning and once at the end of the day; water was offered ad libitum by automatic drinking tap nozzle. The subjects were always under natural conditions of lightness and temperature. No animal was submitted to any kind of suffering. Beyond that, in order to minimize the stress of life in captivity, environmental enrichment for the animals is provided in the Primate Center. It is important to emphasize that no subject has been euthanized after this study.

Subjects

Eight capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) were employed in this study, 6 females and 2 males, weighting between 2.5 and 5 kg. Seven animals had been used in PPI test previously (Saletti et al., 2014), and only one female was naïve. Nonetheless, no animal was previously exposed to the experimental drugs. All experiments were conducted between 8 and 12 am, 5 days a week. No food or water deprivation has been enforced, except during trials.

Startle Measurement

Prepulse inhibition tests were performed inside a primate chamber (60 × 30 × 30 cm), built in transparent acrylic material of 15 mm thick, placed above a wooden box (45 × 40 × 40 cm). Animals were placed inside the chamber with its head out through an adjustable neck hole. Two speakers (Foster Model FT96H Frequency band; 4 KHz∼30 KHz) were attached to a head box (30 × 30 × 25 cm) on the top of the chamber. The speakers were kept each at a distance of 10 cm of the monkey’s head and were connected to a sound generator (O’Hara & Co., Ltd., Tokyo). On the bottom of the chamber, an accelerometer was (Inntechno Japan Co.ltd., Model: BDK3) connected to an amplifier (O’Hara & Co., Ltd.) which captured animal’s whole-body movement. The whole system was connected to a recording software (Animal Startle—PCI 6024E, developed by O’Hara & Co., Ltd.), interfaced with Windows XP operational system (for more details see, Saletti et al., 2014).

Prepulse inhibition tests were conducted in an acoustic isolated room next to the subjects’ home cage. Inside the test room, a permanent white noise was generated (65 dB) and a video camera (Model Clone #1004124) was used to monitor the animal during tests for animals’ safety.

Drug

MK-801 (0; 0,01; 0,02; 0,03 mg/kg—Sigma-Aldrich, Brazil) was dissolved in Tween 80 (Sigma-Aldrich, Brazil) and 0.9% saline (1:19) administered intramuscularly (i.m.) in a volume of 1 ml/kg. All doses were based in studies with non-human primates (Buffalo et al., 1994; Harder et al., 1998).

Procedures

Each session test consisted in 10 equal and consecutive blocks of 3 pseudorandomized stimuli each (pulse-alone, 115 dB, 40 ms duration; prepulse-alone, 80 dB, 20 ms duration and pulse-prepulse, 120 ms interval). Startle response was recorded as the maximum peak amplitude over 600 ms after each presentation.

Prepulse inhibition sessions were carried out with a 2-week interval. A week in which the subject received either vehicle (VEH) or any dose of MK-801 was considered a test-week. Therefore, the washout period for MK-801 was, at least 2 weeks. Subjects were divided into two groups: G1 received vehicle (VEH) at the first test-week and G2 received VEH at the fourth test-week. In the remaining test-weeks (G1: second–fourth test-weeks; G2: first–third test-weeks), they received three different doses of MK-801 (0.01, 0.02, 0.03 mg/kg i.m.) randomly assigned once a test-week. Each group comprised four subjects. PPI test was performed 20 min after MK-801 administration.

Statistical Analysis

All statistic tests were conduct in IBM SPSS Statistics Version 20.

Shapiro Wilk Test was performed to verify the normality of the data. Since the data did not show normal distribution, Friedman’s Test was performed to examine the effects of MK-801 on startle amplitude and percentage of PPI.

Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test was performed to analyze independent data of percentage of PPI in temporal effect of MK-801, and Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test to examine dependent data in temporal effect of the drug.

To normalize the data, we calculated the percentage of inhibition of the startle response for each subject by the following formula: 100 × (pulse-alone – prepulse-pulse)/pulse-alone as done in previous PPI studies (Winslow et al., 2002, 2007; Saletti et al., 2014).

Results with p ≤ 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All data are represented as the mean of startle amplitude or percent of PPI ± standard error of mean ( ± SEM).

Results

Figure 1 indicates the percentage of inhibition for each randomly assigned administration (VEH and three doses of MK-801), regardless of experimental group. Friedman’s Test indicated a decrease in percentage of inhibition in comparison with VEH only for the higher dose of MK-801 (0.03 mg/kg; X2 = 9.800; p = 0.002; Figure 1, n = 8).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. MK-801 disrupted percent of PPI only in higher dose (0.03 mg/kg) compared to VEH. Bars indicate percent of inhibition of startle response (mean ± SEM, n = 8) for each administrated dose. *Indicates significant difference between 0.03 mg/kg and VEH (p = 0.002) in Friedman’s test.

In Figure 2, we show the mean startle response amplitude when pulse-alone and prepulse+pulse are considered separately, again regardless of experimental group. In pulse-alone situation, startle response differs only from VEH at the dose of 0.03 mg/kg (X2 = 5.000; p = 0.025, n = 8). In prepulse+pulse stimuli, no dose was statistically different from VEH (X2 = 7.738; p = 0.052, n = 8).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. MK-801 decreased startle response amplitude after administration of the higher dose (0.03 mg/kg) compared to VEH. Bars indicate startle response amplitude (mean ± SEM, n = 8) when presented pulse-alone trials (black bars) and prepulse+pulse trials (gray bars). *Indicates significant difference in pulse-alone trials between 0.03 mg/kg and VEH (p = 0.025) and 0.01 mg/kg (p = 0.014) in Friedman’s test.

In our test procedure, we used a repeated treatment design in which all subjects underwent the same treatments. As explained above, we divided the animals in two groups (G1 and G2) and compared the PPI response between these two groups at the first and fourth test-weeks. This procedure was conducted in order to evaluate a possible temporal effect of MK-801 administration. Figure 3 shows the temporal effect of MK-801 administration. A deficit in the percent of PPI can be seen in animals that received MK-801 at the first test-week (MK-801/G2, n = 4), regardless of the dose that each monkey received, in comparison to animals that received VEH at the first test-week (VEH/G1, n = 4) (Z = –3.830; p < 0.001). Additionally, there was an increase in PPI after repeated injections of MK-801 and repeated PPI tests (Z = –4.359; p < 0.001), as observed in PPI of MK-801/G2 and MK-801/G1. A statistical difference was also observed between MK-801 at first test-week (MK-801/G2) and VEH at fourth test-week (VEH/G2) (Z = –2.151; p = 0.032). There was no statistical difference between either VEH administrations regarding percentage of inhibition (Z = –0.414; p = 0.684). Also, no difference was found between VEH at fourth test-week (VEH/G2) and MK-801 at fourth test-week (MK-801/G1) (Z = –0.799; p = 0.429), nor between VEH at first test-week (VEH/G1) and MK-801 at fourth test-week (MK-801/G1) (Z = –1.116; p = 0.265; Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Habituation effect of MK-801 over repeated PPI sessions. (A) Circles indicate percent of PPI of G1 group (n = 4), and triangles, of G2 group (n = 4). White symbols indicate response after VEH administration and black symbols indicate response after MK-801 administration, regardless of dose (mean ± SEM). *Indicates significant difference of MK-801/G2 compared to all of other. (MK-801/G2 vs. VEH/G1, p < 0.001; MK-801/G2 vs. MK-801/G1, p < 0.001; MK-801/G2 vs. VEH/G2, p = 0.032). (B) At the first test-week, startle amplitude on pulse trials did not differ between groups, unlike on prepulse+pulse trials. Bars indicate startle response amplitude. Black bars demonstrate startle response on pulse-alone trials, and gray bars demonstrate startle response on prepulse-pulse trials. *Indicates significant difference on pulse-alone trials between VEH/G2 and all other (p < 0.036). +Indicates significant difference on prepulse-pulse trials between MK-801/G1 and all other (p < 0.032). #Indicates statistical difference on prepulse+pulse trials between VEH/G1 and MK-801/G2 (p = 0.042).

Figure 3B shows the mean startle response amplitude when pulse-alone and prepulse+pulse are considered separately. In pulse-alone trials, startle response of VEH at fourth test-week (VEH/G2) was different from all other pulse-alone (vs. VEH/G1: Z = –2.223, p = 0.026; vs. MK-801/G2: Z = –2.850, p = 0.004; vs. MK-801/G1: Z = –2.098, p = 0.036). In prepulse+pulse trials, MK-801 at fourth test-week was statistically different from all others (vs. VEH/G1: Z = –2.139, p = 0.032; vs. VEH/G2: Z = –3.662, p < 0.001; vs. MK-801/G2: Z = –2.560, p = 0.010). Moreover, VEH at first test-week (VEH/G1) differs from MK-801 at first test-week (MK-801/G2) (Z = –2.035, p = 0.042).

Discussion

The present study shows the effects of MK-801 on capuchin’s PPI response. Pretreatment with MK-801 has induced a decrease in PPI, as well as in startle amplitude at the highest dose (0.03 mg/kg). Other MK-801 studies in primates using different paradigms also indicate that most behavioral changes are observed at the dose of 0.03 mg/kg or higher (Ogura and Aigner, 1993; Buffalo et al., 1994; Harder et al., 1998). It is possible that higher doses might be more effective at disrupting PPI. Despite that, higher doses of MK-801 also induce a strong ataxia effect and parkinsonism, which could have disrupted subjects’ performance on PPI task (Crossman et al., 1989; Rupniak et al., 1992). Nevertheless, we observed, but not quantified, changes in monkey’s motor behavior typical of ataxia especially at 0.02 and 0.03 mg/kg. Motor disturbances, as ataxia, were previously reported in these doses in other monkey species (Boyce et al., 1991; Rupniak et al., 1992).

Prepulse inhibition was reduced after acute MK-801 administration, in agreement with previous studies with rodents (e.g., Feifel and Priebe, 1999; Long et al., 2006; Gururajan et al., 2011; Khella et al., 2014). This effect, however, is not observed after fourth test-weeks of testing, even with 2-week test interval, and monkeys’ PPI response becomes similar to the first trial. As seen in Figure 3B, there was no difference in startle response between both groups in the first test-week. On the other hand, prepulse+pulse startle responses were significantly different between groups in the first test-week, indicating a clear PPI disruption. Although some studies with PPI paradigm showed that MK-801 effects on rodents could be intensified after repeated sessions (Schulz et al., 2001; Gomes et al., 2014), our results indicate a reversal of PPI disruption in monkeys. Similar results were observed in rodents after successive injections of D2-like dopamine receptor agonists, such as amphetamine, cocaine, apomorphine, and quinpirole (Druhan et al., 1998; Byrnes and Hammer, 2000; Feifel et al., 2002; Culm and Hammer, 2004; Culm et al., 2004; Li et al., 2011), which was interpreted as a tolerance effect to these drugs. In the same way, tolerance was observed after repeated administration of MK-801 in rats’ motor activity (Dall’Olio et al., 1992). In addition, the repeated treatment with MK-801 decreased D2 receptor density (Dall’Olio et al., 1992). Since D2 agonist could lead to PPI disruption (Ralph and Caine, 2005), MK-801 tolerance in our study might be attributed to a decrease in D2 receptor density. However, effects of D2 agonists on PPI are species-dependent (Ralph and Caine, 2005).

Familiarization with test environment might also prevent MK-801-induced impairment on PPI. Several studies with learning and memory in rodents evidenced that NMDA antagonists lose their effects after the animal’s familiarization with the test environment (Shapiro and O’Connor, 1992; Uekita and Okaichi, 2005; Chan and McNally, 2009). Following this trend, Ennaceur et al. (2011) showed that MK-801 increased anxiety level in mice and this anxiogenic effect decreased after repeated exposures to the experimental apparatus. Other NMDA antagonists also induce similar effects when the animal is familiarized to the environment (Saucier et al., 1996; Roesler et al., 1998; Sanders and Fanselow, 2003). NMDA antagonists’ block hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and this effect can be altered by pretraining or by familiarization to the test environment due to latent learning (Otnaess et al., 1999; Ennaceur et al., 2011). In addition, it is known that hippocampus is a structure related to PPI modulation (Swerdlow et al., 2001; Wolf et al., 2006; Kohl et al., 2013).

Although PPI reflects automatic preattentive process (Graham, 1975), higher level of cognitive process might be involved in PPI response. There is a positive correlation between increase PPI and superior strategy formation abilities in humans, which could be related to a more efficient early information process (Bitsios et al., 2006). Furthermore, several studies have demonstrated that directing attention to the prepulse signal increases PPI (see Li et al., 2009, for review). In our experimental design, subjects served as their own control, which leads to repeated trials. In this sense, familiarization with test conditions might explain changes in the effect of MK-801. It is also possible that PPI test familiarization may have increased attention to prepulse signaling, thus enhancing PPI response.

As reported by Li et al. (2011), PPI test consists of repeated presentations of mild stressors startle stimuli, and the constant repetition of this test may provoke alterations of drug effects on PPI response. Indeed, since MK-801 half-life is approximately 2 h (Hatfield et al., 1992), and our PPI sessions were always at least 2 weeks apart, the observed PPI reversal cannot be ascribed to the MK-801 effect alone. Therefore, the unexpected reversal of PPI disruption observed in the present study is suggestive of a drug-training interaction. Similar experiments with rats and mice found instead an increased MK-801 effect over repeated sessions (Schulz et al., 2001; Gomes et al., 2014). In that sense, our results underscore the neuropharmacological differences between rodents and primates (Maior et al., 2011) and the importance of non-human primates for basic research and preclinical testing.

Conclusion

The present study demonstrated the effectiveness of MK-801 on PPI disruption in capuchin monkeys. In addition, our results suggest a drug-training interaction effect on the PPI response after repeated administration of MK-801. The habituation process observed in monkeys indicates that MK-801 adverse effects, such as sensorimotor gating impairments, may be reduced by MK-801 tolerance effect and after familiarization to PPI test. Therefore, future PPI tests with MK-801 should employ separate treatment groups to avoid habituation effects induced by extensive repeated sessions.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by FAP/DF grant to CT (No 193.000.033/2012) and JSPS Asian Core Program. We thank G. V. da Silva, A. Araujo, R. S. Oliveira, and C. D. Azevedo for excellent animal care and A.P.S. RM for the construction of the experimental apparatus. PS was recipient of a doctoral fellowship from CNPq.

References

Arai, S., Takuma, K., Mizoguchi, H., Ibi, D., Nagai, T., Takahashi, K., et al. (2008). Involvement of pallidotegmental neurons in methamphetamine- and MK-801-induced impairment of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in mice: reversal by GABAB receptor agonist baclofen. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 3164–3175. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basurto, E., Flores, O. G., and Hoffman, K. L. (2015). Glycinamide prevents MK-801-induced hyperactivity and deficits in object recognition memory in an animal model of positive and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 166, 349–350. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bitsios, P., Giakoumaki, S. G., Theou, K., and Frangou, S. (2006). Increased prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response is associated with better strategy formation and execution times in healthy males. Neuropsychologia 44, 2494–2499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyce, S., Rupniak, N. M., Steventon, M. J., Cook, G., and Iversen, S. D. (1991). Psychomotor activity and cognitive disruption attributable to NMDA, but not sigma, interactions in primates. Behav. Brain Res. 42, 115–121. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80002-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradford, A. M., Savage, K. M., Jones, D. N. C., and Kalinichev, M. (2010). Validation and pharmacological characterisation of MK-801-induced locomotor hyperactivity in BALB/C mice as an assay for detection of novel antipsychotics. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 212, 155–170. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1938-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braff, D. L., and Geyer, M. A. (1990). Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47, 181–188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810140081011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., Light, G. A., Sprock, J., Perry, W., Cadenhead, K. S., et al. (2001a). Impact of prepulse characteristics on the detection of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 49, 171–178. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00139-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2001b). Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 156, 234–258. doi: 10.1007/s002130100810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buffalo, E. A., Gillam, M. P., Allen, R. R., and Paule, M. G. (1994). Acute behavioral effects of MK-801 in rhesus monkeys: assessment using an operant test battery. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 48, 935–940. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90203-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrnes, J. J., and Hammer, R. P. (2000). The disruptive effect of cocaine on prepulse inhibition is prevented by repeated administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 22, 551–554. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00151-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, W. Y. M., and McNally, G. P. (2009). Conditioned stimulus familiarity determines effects of MK-801 on fear extinction. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 303–314. doi: 10.1037/a0014988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossman, A. R., Peggs, D., Boyce, S., Luquin, M. R., and Sambrook, M. A. (1989). Effect of the NMDA antagonist MK-801 on MPTP-induced parkinsonism in the monkey. Neuropharmacology 28, 1271–1273. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(89)90221-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Culm, K. E., and Hammer, R. P. (2004). Recovery of Sensorimotor gating without G protein adaptation after repeated D 2 -like dopamine receptor agonist treatment in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 308, 487–494. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.057158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Culm, K. E., Lugo-Escobar, N., Hope, B. T., and Hammer, R. P. (2004). Repeated quinpirole treatment increases cAMP-dependent protein kinase activity and CREB phosphorylation in nucleus accumbens and reverses quinpirole-induced sensorimotor gating deficits in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1823–1830. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dall’Olio, R., Gandolfi, O., and Montanaro, N. (1992). Effect of chronic treatment with dizocilpine (MK-801) on the behavioral response to dopamine receptor agonists in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 107, 591–594. doi: 10.1007/BF02245275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Druhan, J. P., Geyer, M. A., and Valentino, R. J. (1998). Lack of sensitization to the effects of d-amphetamine and apomorphine on sensorimotor gating in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 135, 296–304. doi: 10.1007/s002130050513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennaceur, A., Michalikova, S., Van Rensburg, R., and Chazot, P. L. (2011). MK-801 increases the baseline level of anxiety in mice introduced to a spatial memory task without prior habituation. Neuropharmacology 61, 981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feifel, D., and Priebe, K. (1999). The effects of subchronic haloperidol on intact and dizocilpine- disrupted sensorimotor gating. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 146, 175–179. doi: 10.1007/s002130051103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feifel, D., Priebe, K., Johnstone-Miller, E., and Morgan, C. J. (2002). Sensorimotor gating effects produced by repeated dopamine agonists in a paradigm favoring environmental conditioning. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 162, 138–146. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1026-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geyer, M. A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Braff, D. L., and Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology 156, 117–154. doi: 10.1007/s002130100811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, F. V., Issy, A. C., Ferreira, F. R., Viveros, M.-P., Del Bel, E. A., and Guimaraes, F. S. (2014). Cannabidiol attenuates sensorimotor gating disruption and molecular changes induced by chronic antagonism of NMDA receptors in Mice. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 1–10. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyu041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, F. K. (1975). The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. Psychophysiology 12, 238–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gururajan, A., Taylor, D. A., and Malone, D. T. (2011). Effect of cannabidiol in a MK-801-rodent model of aspects of Schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 299–308. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harder, J. A., Aboobaker, A. A., Hodgetts, T. C., and Ridley, R. M. (1998). Learning impairments induced by glutamate blockade using dizocilpine (MK-801) in monkeys. Br. J. Pharmacol. 125, 1013–1018. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0702178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harder, J. A., and Ridley, R. M. (2000). The 5-HT1A antagonist, WAY 100635, alleviates cognitive impairments induced by dizocilpine (MK-801 ) in monkeys. Neuropharmacology 39, 547–552. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(99)00179-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatfield, R. H., Gill, R., and Brazell, C. (1992). The dose-response relationship and therapeutic window for dizocilpine (MK-801) in a rat focal ischaemia model. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 216, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(92)90201-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hikichi, H., Kaku, A., Karasawa, J., and Chaki, S. (2013). Stimulation of metabotropic glutamate (mGlu) 2 receptor and blockade of mGlu1 receptor improve social memory impairment elicited by MK-801 in rats. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 122, 10–16. doi: 10.1254/jphs.13036FP

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishii, D., Matsuzawa, D., Kanahara, N., Matsuda, S., Sutoh, C., Ohtsuka, H., et al. (2010). Effects of aripiprazole on MK-801-induced prepulse inhibition deficits and mitogen-activated protein kinase signal transduction pathway. Neurosci. Lett. 471, 53–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karamihalev, S., Prickaerts, J., and van Goethem, N. P. (2014). Donepezil and the alpha-7 agonist PHA 568487, but not risperidone, ameliorate spatial memory deficits in a subchronic MK-801 mouse model of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 272, 248–251. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khella, R., Short, J. L., and Malone, D. T. (2014). CB2 receptor agonism reverses MK-801-induced disruptions of prepulse inhibition in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 231, 3071–3087. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3481-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, S., Heekeren, K., Klosterkötter, J., and Kuhn, J. (2013). Prepulse inhibition in psychiatric disorders–apart from schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 445–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumari, V., Gray, J. A., Geyer, M. A., Ffytche, D., Soni, W., Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., et al. (2003). Neural correlates of tactile prepulse inhibition: a functional MRI study in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 122, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(02)00123-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Du, Y., Li, N., Wu, X., and Wu, Y. (2009). Top-down modulation of prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in humans and rats. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 1157–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., He, W., and Chen, J. (2011). Time course of prepulse inhibition disruption induced by dopamine agonists and NMDA antagonists: effects of drug administration regimen. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 99, 509–518. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, L. E., Malone, D. T., and Taylor, D. A. (2006). Cannabidiol reverses MK-801-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 795–803. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludewig, K., Geyer, M. A., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2003). Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01925-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackeprang, T., Kristiansen, K. T., and Glenthoj, B. Y. (2002). Effects of antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition of the startle response in drug-naïve schizophrenic patients. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 863–873. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01409-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maior, R. S., Barros, M., and Tomaz, C. (2011). “Contributions of non-human primates to the understanding of cocaine addiction,” in Psychiatric Disorders—Trends and Developments, ed. T. Uehara, (Rijeka: InTech), 339–366. doi: 10.5772/26644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogura, H., and Aigner, T. G. (1993). MK-801 impairs recognition memory in rhesus monkeys: comparison with cholinergic drugs. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 266, 60–64.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Otnaess, M. K., Brun, V. H., Moser, M. B., and Moser, E. I. (1999). Pretraining prevents spatial learning impairment after saturation of hippocampal long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 19, RC49.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Park, S. J., Lee, Y., Oh, H. K., Lee, H. E., Lee, Y., Ko, S. Y., et al. (2014). Oleanolic acid attenuates MK-801-induced schizophrenia-like behaviors in mice. Neuropharmacology 86, 49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, U. W., Zimmermann, J., Watzke, S., Langosch, J., Siafarikas, N., Wong, J. W. M., et al. (2011). Short-term prospective comparison of prepulse inhibition between schizophrenic patients and healthy controls. Pharmacopsychiatry 44, 102–108. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1271687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ralph, R. J., and Caine, S. B. (2005). Dopamine D1 and D2 agonist effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotion: comparison of Sprague-Dawley rats to Swiss-Webster, 129X1/SvJ, C57BL/6J, and DBA/2J mice. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 312, 733–741. doi: 10.1124/jpet.104.074468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roesler, R., Vianna, M., Sant’Anna, M. K., Kuyven, C. R., Kruel, A. V., Quevedo, J., et al. (1998). Intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5 impairs retention of an inhibitory avoidance task: protection from impairment by pretraining or preexposure to the task apparatus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 69, 87–91. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rupniak, N. M. J., Boyce, S., Steventon, M. J., Iversen, S. D., and Marsden, C. D. (1992). Dystonia induced by combined treatment with L-dopa and MK-801 in parkinsonian monkeys. Ann. Neurol. 32, 103–105. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saletti, P. G., Maior, R. S., Hori, E., de Almeida, R. M., Nishijo, H., and Tomaz, C. (2014). Whole-body prepulse inhibition protocol to test sensorimotor gating mechanisms in monkeys. PLoS ONE 9:e105551. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, M. J., and Fanselow, M. S. (2003). Pre-training prevents context fear conditioning deficits produced by hippocampal NMDA receptor blockade. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 80, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/S1074-7427(03)00040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saucier, D., Hargreaves, E. L., Boon, F., Vanderwolf, C. H., and Cain, D. P. (1996). Detailed behavioral analysis of water maze acquisition under systemic NMDA or muscarinic antagonism: nonspatial pretraining eliminates spatial learning deficits. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 103–116. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.1.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, B., Fendt, M., Pedersen, V., and Koch, M. (2001). Sensitization of prepulse inhibition deficits by repeated administration of dizocilpine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 156, 177–181. doi: 10.1007/s002130100776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, M. L., and O’Connor, C. (1992). N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist MK-801 and spatial memory representation: working memory is impaired in an unfamiliar environment but not in a familiar environment. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 604–612. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.4.604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svoboda, J., Stankova, A., Entlerova, M., and Stuchlik, A. (2015). Acute administration of MK-801 in an animal model of psychosis in rats interferes with cognitively demanding forms of behavioral flexibility on a rotating arena. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:75. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swerdlow, N. R., Hanlon, F. M., Henning, L., Kim, Y. K., Gaudet, I., and Halim, N. D. (2001). Regulation of sensorimotor gating in rats by hippocampal NMDA: anatomical localization. Brain Res. 898, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02143-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsukada, H., Nishiyama, S., Fukumoto, D., Sato, K., Kakiuchi, T., and Domino, E. F. (2005). Chronic NMDA antagonism impairs working memory, decreases extracellular dopamine, and increases D1 receptor binding in prefrontal cortex of conscious monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1861–1869. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uekita, T., and Okaichi, H. (2005). NMDA antagonist MK-801 does not interfere with the use of spatial representation in a familiar environment. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 548–556. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Chen, Y., Carlson, S., Li, L., Hu, X., and Ma, Y. (2012). Interactive effects of morphine and scopolamine, MK-801, propranolol on spatial working memory in rhesus monkeys. Neurosci. Lett. 523, 119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winslow, J. T., Noble, P. L., and Davis, M. (2007). Modulation of fear-potentiated startle and vocalizations in juvenile rhesus monkeys by morphine, diazepam, and buspirone. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 389–395. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winslow, J. T., Parr, L. A., and Davis, M. (2002). Acoustic Startle, prepulse inhibition and fear-potentiated startle measured in rhesus monkeys. Soc. Biol. Psychiatry. 51, 859–866. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01345-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, R., Dobrowolny, H., Matzke, K., Paelchen, K., Bogerts, B., and Schwegler, H. (2006). Prepulse inhibition is different in two inbred mouse strains (CPB-K and BALB/cJ) with different hippocampal NMDA receptor densities. Behav. Brain Res. 166, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshimi, N., Futamura, T., and Hashimoto, K. (2015). Improvement of dizocilpine-induced social recognition deficits in mice by brexpiprazole, a novel serotonin—dopamine activity modulator. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 356–364. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PPI, dizocilpine (MK-801), schizophrenia, NMDA receptor, habituation

Citation: Saletti PG, Maior RS, Hori E, Nishijo H and Tomaz C (2015) Sensorimotor gating impairments induced by MK-801 treatment may be reduced by tolerance effect and by familiarization in monkeys. Front. Pharmacol. 6:204. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2015.00204

Received: 21 July 2015; Accepted: 04 September 2015;
Published: 22 September 2015.

Edited by:

Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, USA

Reviewed by:

Karthik Bodhinathan, Sanford Burnham Prebys Medical Discovery Institute, USA
Christian P. Müller, Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany

Copyright © 2015 Saletti, Maior, Hori, Nishijo and Tomaz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carlos Tomaz, Primate Center and Laboratory of Neurosciences and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biology, University of Brasilia, Campus Darcy Ribeiro, Brasilia 70910-900, Brazil, ctomaz@unb.br

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.