Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Pharmacol., 04 December 2018
Sec. Ethnopharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Natural Products-Based Drugs: Potential Therapeutics against Alzheimer's Disease and other Neurological Disorders View all 18 articles

Rhodiola rosea L. Improves Learning and Memory Function: Preclinical Evidence and Possible Mechanisms

\r\nGou-ping MaGou-ping Ma1Qun ZhengQun Zheng2Meng-bei XuMeng-bei Xu2Xiao-li ZhouXiao-li Zhou2Lin Lu*Lin Lu3*Zuo-xiao Li*Zuo-xiao Li4*Guo-Qing Zheng*Guo-Qing Zheng2*
  • 1Tongde Hospital of Zhejiang province, Hangzhou, China
  • 2Department of Neurology, The Second Affiliated Hospital and Yuying Children's Hospital of Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, China
  • 3School of Chinese Medicine, Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong, China
  • 4Department of Neurology, Affiliated Hospital of Southwest Medical University, Luzhou, China

Rhodiola rosea L. (R. rosea L.) is widely used to stimulate the nervous system, extenuate anxiety, enhance work performance, relieve fatigue, and prevent high altitude sickness. Previous studies reported that R. rosea L. improves learning and memory function in animal models. Here, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis for preclinical studies to assess the current evidence for R. rosea L. effect on learning and memory function. Ultimately, 36 studies involving 836 animals were identified by searching 6 databases from inception to May 2018. The primary outcome measures included the escape latency in Morris water maze (MWM) test on behalf of learning ability, the frequency and the length of time spent on the target quadrant in MWM test representing memory function, and the number of errors in step down test, dark avoidance test and Y maze test on behalf of memory function. The secondary outcome measures were mechanisms of R. rosea L. for learning and/or memory function. Compared with control, the pooled results of 28 studies showed significant effects of R. rosea L. for reducing the escape latency (P < 0.05); 23 studies for increasing the frequency and the length of time spent on the target quadrant (P < 0.05); and 6 studies for decreasing the number of errors (P < 0.01). The possible mechanisms of R. rosea L. are largely through antioxidant, cholinergic regulation, anti-apoptosis activities, anti-inflammatory, improving coronary blood flow, and cerebral metabolism. In conclusion, the findings suggested that R. rosea L. can improve learning and memory function.

Introduction

Lasting changes in behavior resulting from prior experience can be characterized as the result of learning, memory, and retrieval processes (Thompson, 1986). However, memory is vulnerable across the adult lifespan. A decrease in learning and memory functions is the most common complaint in normal aging process. In a large number of organic diseases, in which there is a physical change in the structure of an organ or part, such as amnesia, Alzheimer's disease (AD) and vascular dementia, the most prominent sign is memory impairment (Thompson, 1986). Currently, there is no valid treatment for cognition impairment in western medicine, although many potential agents exist through novel mechanisms (Parihar and Hemnani, 2004). Cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEIs) and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists are first-line pharmacotherapy for mild-to-moderate AD in clinical, with high non-response rate 50–75% (Johnson et al., 2004). Thus, it is urgent to seek new strategies to improve function of memory and cognition.

Rhodiola rosea L. (R. rosea L.), also known as Rhodiola, Roseroot, Arctic Root, and Golden Root, belongs to the plant family of Crassulaceae, subfamily of sedoideae and genus Rhodiola (Farhath et al., 2005). R. rosea L. and its ingredients replenish qi (vital energy), activate blood circulation, unblock blood vessels, enhance mental function, and smooth asthmatic conditions in traditional Chinese medicine (TCM) (Pharmacopoeia Committee of the People's Republic of China Ministry of health, 2005). Salidroside, p-tryosol, rosavin, pyridrde, rhodiosin, and rhodionin are the most unique active ingredients in the Rhodiola species, but vary in the amounts (Zhang et al., 2006). Of the Rhodiola species, R. rosea L. has been extensively studied on its phytochemical and toxicological properties (Kurkin and Zapesochnaya, 1985). Modern pharmacological studies indicate that its extracts can increase neurotransmitter level, central nervous system activity, and cardiovascular function. Current studies reported that R. rosea L. ingestion can improve cognitive function (Spasov et al., 2000), reduce mental fatigue (Shevtsov et al., 2003), promote free radical mitigation, and exists anti-oxidative (Zhang et al., 2007) and neuroprotective effect (Yu et al., 2008), increase endurance performance (De Bock et al., 2004), and treat symptoms of asthenia subsequent to intense physical and psychological stress (Lazarova et al., 1986). However, the current evidence of R. rosea L. for learning and memory function still lack systematic analysis. Thus, we conduct a preclinical systematic review of Rhodiola on learning and memory function to clarify its effectiveness and potential mechanisms on animal models.

Methods

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Stewart et al., 2015) and the Guidelines for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of animal studies (Sena et al., 2014) were abided.

Database and Literature Search Strategy

Six databases of PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science, Chinese National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI), Wanfangdatabase and VIP information database were electronically searched from the inception up to May 2018. Studies reporting the use of R. rosea L. and/or its bioactive ingredients for learning and memory function in animals were identified. Our literature search strategy was as following: 1. Rhodiola (s); 2. Rhodiola rosea (s); 3. Roseroot (s); 4. rhodioloside; 5. salidroside; 6. OR/1-5; 7. Memory; 8. Learning; 9. Cognitive function; 10. 6 AND (7 OR 8 OR 9); 11. Animals NOT humans; 12. 10 AND 11.

Study Selection

Two investigators independently screened the titles and/or abstracts based on the search strategy. Of the search results, we assessed the full-text articles for eligibility. Any uncertainty eligibility was resolved by discussion. Studies were eligible for our systematic review if they met: (1) Animal models were established for learning and memory injury; (2) Analyzed interventions were received R. rosea L. and/or its bioactive ingredients as monotherapy at any dose. Comparator interventions were isosteric non-functional liquid (normal saline) or no treatment; (3) the primary measured outcomes were indexes of learning and/or memory function tests, including Morris water maze (MWM), Y maze, step down test, dark avoidance test, active avoidance reaction and one step through test. The secondary outcome measures were mechanisms of R. rosea L. for learning and/or memory function. Pre-specified exclusion criteria were as follows: R. rosea L. was treated in conjunction with other compounds or R. rosea L.-based prescriptions, or without predetermined outcome index, or without in vivo model, or without control group, or duplicate publications. In the case of multiple publications from one study, we choose the articles with the largest sample or the earliest publication.

Data Extraction

The following details were extracted from each included study: (1) the first author's name, publication year; (2) individual data for each study, including animal species, number, sex, and weight; (3) type of animal model and anesthetic used in the model; (4) intervention characteristics, including timing for initial treatment, dosage and method of treatment, duration of treatment, and comparable treatment of control group;(5) main outcome measures on behavior tests and its corresponding p-value. For each comparison, we extracted data of mean value and standard deviation from each treatment and control group of every study. If the data for meta-analysis were missing or only expressed graphically, we tried to contact authors for further information or calculated by ourselves if available. Otherwise we only performed qualitative analysis. The data of highest dose was selected when the treatment group included various doses of the target drug. The result of the peak time point was included when the data were expressed at different times.

Quality Assessment

Two authors independently assessed the methodological quality of the included articles according to the Collaborative Approach to Meta-Analysis and Review of Animal Data from Experimental Studies (CAMARADES) 10-item checklist (Sena et al., 2007): (1) peer-reviewed publication; (2) statements of temperature control; (3) randomization to treatment or control group; (4) blinded induction of model; (5) blinded assessment of outcome; (6) use of anesthetic without significant intrinsic neuroprotective activity; (7) appropriate animal model; (8) sample size calculation; (9) compliance with animal welfare regulations; and (10) declaration of potential conflict of interests. Each study was given an aggregate quality score based on one-point awarding for each item. Discrepancies were resolved by discussion or consultation with corresponding author.

Statistical Analysis

Meta-analyses and sub-analyses were performed using RevMan 5.3 software. Outcome measures were all considered as continuous data and given an estimate of the combined overall effect sizes utilizing standard mean difference (SMD) with the random effects model. SMD with its 95% confidence interval (CI) was used to assess the strength of efficacy of R. rosea L. and/or its bioactive ingredients for learning and memory function. Publication bias was assessed with a funnel plot. To clarify the impact of factors potentially modifying the outcome measures, we also conducted sensitivity analyses and subgroup analyses according to the following variables: animal species, anesthetic used, type of animal model and the treatment time. The I2 statistic was used for assessment of heterogeneity among individual studies. Probability value P < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Study Inclusion

We identified 760 potentially relevant articles from the six databases. After removal of duplicates and irrelevant articles, 150 records remained. After going through the titles and abstracts, 55 were excluded because they were case reports, clinical trials or review articles. By reading the remaining full-text articles, 59 articles were excluded if: (1) not predetermined outcome index; (2) not published in peer-review journals; (3) compared with other medicine; (4) no in vivo model; (5) no control group; (6) conjunction with other compounds or R. rosea L.-based prescriptions. Finally, 36 eligible studies (You et al., 2000; Jiang et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003, 2017a,b; Xie, 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Deng, 2006; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2008, 2012, 2013; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Mao et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2011a,b, 2017; Sun et al., 2012; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Qi et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Vasileva et al., 2016; Ge et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017) involving 836 animals were identified (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Summary of the process for identifying candidate studies.

Characteristics of Included Studies

The basic characteristics of the included studies are summarized in Table 1. Thirty-six studies included were published between 2000 and 2017 and described comparisons based on three main outcome measures of learning and memory function. For animal species, 27 studies used rats including Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats (Wang et al., 2008; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b, 2017; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Qi et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017a,b; Wei, 2017) and Wistar rats (Jiang et al., 2001; Xie, 2003; Chen, 2008; Zhao et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2012; Vasileva et al., 2016; Ge et al., 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017) as animal models. Eight studies used mice including C57BL/6J (Mao et al., 2010), ICR (Deng, 2006; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012), BALB/C (Liu et al., 2003), Kunming mice (You et al., 2000; Wu et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2013; Ge et al., 2017). The remaining 1 study used mice without mentioning its species (Shi et al., 2006). Seventeen studies (You et al., 2000; Jiang et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003, 2017a; Xie, 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Deng, 2006; Shi et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2008; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Mao et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2017) induced cognitive impairment by Alzheimer's disease (AD) model, 8 studies (Chen, 2008; Liu, 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2013; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017b; Xiong and Gao, 2017) by vascular dementia (VD) model, 5 studies (Yang et al., 2011b; Qi et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Ge et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017) by hypobaric hypoxia model, 2 studies (Wang et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012) by sleep deprivation model, 2 studies (Zhao et al., 2010; Yang, 2017) by diabetes mellitus (DM) model, 1 study by status epileptics (SE) model (Yang et al., 2011b), 1 study (Wei, 2017) by posttraumatic stress disorder, and the remaining 1 study (Vasileva et al., 2016) by using scopolamine. For anesthesia chosen in experiments, 6 studies (Cao, 2009; Liu, 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2017b; Xiong and Gao, 2017) used chloral hydrate, 8 studies (Xie, 2003; Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Wei, 2017; Yang, 2017; Yang et al., 2017) used pentobarbital sodium, 1 study (Yan et al., 2015) used isoflurane,1 study (Wang et al., 2012) used ethyl ether, 3 studies (Liu et al., 2003; Deng, 2006; Zhao et al., 2010) needn't use it because of only neurobehavioral tests being conducted in rats/mice, and the remaining 17 studies did not report it. Thirty-four studies were conducted in China, 1 study (Vasileva et al., 2016) in Bulgaria, and the remaining one (Barhwal et al., 2015) in India. For outcome measures, 28 studies of comparisons reported learning data as escape latency in MWM (Jiang et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003, 2017a,b; Wu et al., 2004; Deng, 2006; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2008, 2012; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Zhao et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2011a,b, 2017; Sun et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Ge et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017), 23 studies of comparisons presented the frequency and/or the length of time spent on the target quadrant in MWM as the indicator of memory ability (Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2008; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Zhao et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2011a,b, 2017; Sun et al., 2012; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Ge et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a,b; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017), and 7 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2003; Deng, 2006; Liu, 2009; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2013; Vasileva et al., 2016) of comparisons reported memory outcome measure by the number of errors in step down test, dark avoidance test, the active avoidance test and/or Y maze. Additionally, 3 studies (Wu et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012) report the reaction time in Y maze. Glutathione (GSH) was reported in 5 studies (Wang et al., 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang et al., 2013); NADH/NADPH in 2 studies (Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015); superoxide dismutase (SOD) and/or malondialdehyde (MDA) in 14 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2017b; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017); NO and/or NOS in 3 studies (Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2013); acetylcholine (Ach) and/oracetylcholinesterase (AchE) in 7 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Xie, 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Zhang et al., 2013); caspase-3 in 3 studies (Qu et al., 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017b); tumor necrosis factor-α(TNF-α) in 1 study (Zou et al., 2009); nuclear factor κB (NF-κB) in 1 study (Zhang et al., 2013); Bcl-2 and/or Bax protein in the hippocampus in 5 studies (Cao, 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a; Wei, 2017).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of included 36 studies.

Study Quality

The score of study quality checklist items ranged from 1/10 to 6/10 in Table 2. Of which, 1 study (Vasileva et al., 2016) obtained 6 points, 10 studies obtained 5 points (Chen, 2008; Zou et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2012, 2013; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang et al., 2017), 9 studies (Liu et al., 2003, 2017b; Deng, 2006; Cao, 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Mao et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2010; Guo et al., 2017) obtained 4 points, 8 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Xie, 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Ji et al., 2009; Ge et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a; Wei, 2017; Yang, 2017) obtained 3 points, 7 studies (Shi et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Sun et al., 2012; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Qi et al., 2013) obtained 2 points, and the remaining one (You et al., 2000) obtained 1 point. Seven studies (Xie, 2003; Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Wei, 2017) are master's or doctoral thesis, and remaining studies were published in peer-reviewed journals or databases. Twenty-one studies (Liu et al., 2003, 2017a,b; Xie, 2003; Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Ji et al., 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2012, 2013; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Vasileva et al., 2016; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang et al., 2017) described control of the room temperature. Except six studies (You et al., 2000; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017a,b; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017), the remaining studies declared that they had random allocation to treatment and control groups. Twenty-one studies (Liu et al., 2003, 2017b; Xie, 2003; Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Zhao et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2012, 2013; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Yan et al., 2015; Vasileva et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2017; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017; Yang et al., 2017) used anesthetic without significant intrinsic vascular protection activity. Animal model with aged rats/ mice was used in 2 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Mao et al., 2010), with DM rats in 2 studies (Zhao et al., 2010; Yang, 2017). Thirteen studies (Wu et al., 2004; Chen, 2008; Zou et al., 2009; Mao et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2012, 2013; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Vasileva et al., 2016; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang et al., 2017) mentioned compliance with animal welfare regulations. Thirteen studies (Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Liu, 2009; Qu et al., 2009; Barhwal et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2015; Vasileva et al., 2016; Ge et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a,b; Xiong and Gao, 2017) contained statements on potential conflict of interests. There was neither study reporting that if the model establishment and outcome assessment were conducted in double-blind trial or not, nor calculating sample size in the animal experiment.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Risk of bias of the included studies.

Effectiveness

Twenty-eight studies reported the escape latency in MWM as the outcome measure of learning ability included in the analysis. We pooled the whole data to process and found a significant difference in favor of R. rosea L. treatment compared with control groups (P < 0.00001; SMD = −1.83, 95% CI [−2.03, −1.64]; Heterogeneity: χ2 = 174.39, df = 28 (P < 0.00001); I2 = 84%, Figure 2). Twenty-three studies reported the frequency and/or the length of time spent on the target quadrant as the indicator of memory ability. The pooled result showed that R. rosea L. significantly increased the frequency and the length of time spent on the target quadrant in MWM (P < 0.00001; SMD = 1.79, 95% CI [1.60, 1.98]; Heterogeneity: χ2 = 131.87, df = 32 (P < 0.00001); I2 = 76%, Figure 3). Seven studies reported memory outcome measure by the number of errors in step down test, dark avoidance test, the active avoidance test and Y maze. The pooled data showed that R. rosea L. resulted in a significant depression on the number of errors when comparing to that in control groups (P < 0.00001; SMD = −1.04, 95% CI [−1.35, −0.72]; Heterogeneity: χ2 = 6.93, df = 8 (P = 0.54); I2 = 0%, Figure 4).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing the escape latency in MWM compared with control group.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing the frequency and the length of time spent on the target quadrant in MWM compared with control group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. For decreasing the number of errors compared with control group.

Mechanisms of Rhodiola rosea for Learning and Memory Function

Compared with controls, meta-analysis of 5 studies (Wang et al., 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang et al., 2013) showed that R. rosea L. significantly increased the level of GSH (n = 50, SMD 1.67, 95% CI [1.20 to 2.14], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 2.09, df = 4 (P = 0.72); I2 = 0%), (Figure 5); 2 studies (Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015) for increasing the level of NADH and/or NADPH, (P < 0.05); meta-analysis of 12 studies (Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Zou et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2017b; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017) for increasing SOD level (n = 115, SMD 2.12, 95% CI [1.77 to 2.47], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 22.11, df = 11 (P = 0.02); I2 = 50%), (Figure 6); meta-analysis of 12 studies (Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2017b; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017) for reducing MDA level (n = 117, SMD−1.89, 95% CI [−2.22 to −1.56], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 18.08, df = 11 (P = 0.08); I2 = 39%), (Figure 7); 3 studies (Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2013) for enhancing the expression of NO and/or NOS (P < 0.05); meta-analysis of 2 studies (Jiang et al., 2001; Zhang et al., 2013) increasing the activity of Ach (n = 13, SMD 1.22, 95% CI [0.34 to 2.10], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 0.6, df = 1 (P = 0.44); I2 = 0%), (Figure 8A); meta-analysis of 5 studies (Wu et al., 2004; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Zhang et al., 2013) down-regulating the activity of AchE (n = 46, SMD −1.61, 95% CI [−2.11 to −1.12], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 6.86, df = 4 (P = 0.14); I2 = 42%), (Figure 8B); 3 studies (Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Qi et al., 2013) for reducing the amount of calcium in nerve cells, (P < 0.05); meta-analysis of 3 studies (Qu et al., 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017b) for down-regulating the expression of caspase-3 (n = 23, SMD −3.57, 95% CI [−4.62 to −2.52], P < 0.00001; heterogeneity: χ2 = 3.59, df = 2 (P = 0.17); I2 = 44%), (Figure 9); 5 studies (Cao, 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a; Wei, 2017) for increasing the expression of Bcl-2 and reducing the expression of Bax protein in the hippocampus,(P < 0.05); 1 study (Zou et al., 2009) for inhibiting the expression of TNF-α; 1 study (Zhang et al., 2013) for inhibiting the expression of nuclear factor κB (NF-κB).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for increasing glutathione compared with control group.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for increasing superoxide dismutase compared with control group.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing malondialdehyde compared with control group.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. (A) The forest plot: effects of textitRhodiola rosea L. for increasing acetylcholine; (B) The forest plot:effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing acetylcholinesterase compared with control group.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. The forest plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing caspase-3 compared with control group.

Subgroup Analysis and Sensitivity Analysis

To explore potential confounding factors which affected the outcome measures, we stratified analysis of the escape latency based on variables including animal species, animal model, the duration of treatment, and the quality of study. In the subgroup analysis of these factors, the effect size of rat species was larger than mice (SMD = −2.09 vs. SMD = −1.08, Figure 10A). Animal model showed great discrepancy in the overall effect of outcome measure, which the model of hypobaric hypoxia with scale of 16.4% weight accounted for smaller effect size than any other model (SMD = −1.18 vs. SMD pooled = −1.96, Figure 10B). The longer period of R. rosea L. treatment also showed greater effect size than the shorter treatment with 2 weeks or less (SMD = −1.92 vs. SMD = −1.83, Figure 10C). Notably, the lower quality studies did not exhibit larger effect size than the higher ones (SMD = −1.65 vs. SMD = −2.55, Figure 10D).

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Subgroup analyses of the escape latency. (A) The animal species on the effect size of the outcome measure; (B) the animal model on the effect size of the outcome measure; (C) the duration of the treatment on the effect size of the outcome measure; (D) the quality of studies on the effect size of the outcome measure. The magnitude of absolute value SMD reflected the effect size.

Sensitivity analyses showed that the results did not substantially alter after removing any one trial. However, when we only include studies using mice as animal models, meta-analysis of 5 studies (Liu et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Deng, 2006; Wang et al., 2013; Ge et al., 2017) showed a small difference in favor of R. rosea L. treatment compared with control groups with lower heterogeneity (n = 61, SMD = −1.08, 95%CI [−1.47, −0.68], P < 0.00001; Heterogeneity: χ2 = 6.22, df = 4 (P < 0.00001); I2 = 36%).

Discussion

Summary of Evidence

In this meta-analysis, we assessed R. rosea L. treatment on learning and memory function based on 36 eligible studies. The results revealed that R. rosea L. could evidently reduce the escape latency, improve the frequency and the length of time spent in MWM and decrease the number of errors in step down test, dark avoidance test, and Y maze when comparing with control groups in animal models.

Limitations

Some limitations should be considered while interpreting this study. First, the methodological quality of the included studies was considerably variable and inferior. Nearly all of the included studies had an overall assessment as “high risk of bias,” so we could not exclude that our results may be biased. Second, calculation of sample size and blindness of model establishment and outcome measurement are pivotal in quality control of research, yet no studies provided these critical information in this systematic review. Third, it's not worthy that almost all the included studies declared random allocation to treatment and control groups, while the detailed procedure was not supported at all. Additionally, gender difference was overlooked in the included study. Male/female mice models were used in the two studies (Wu et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2013) for cognitive experiments. Although the mechanism is unclear, a male advantage for working memory and a female advantage for visual memory and social cognition in rodent models were highlighted (Leger and Neill, 2016). Moreover, funnel plots (Figure 11) showed potential publication bias existed in this research field, suggesting studies with null effect are missing. Studies achieved statistically significant outcomes have been shown to be three times more likely to be published than that with null outcomes (Dickersin et al., 1987). Publication bias is due to multiple factor such as researchers and journal editors prefer positive results rather than negative or inconclusive results (Wolfgang, 2007). Thus, the effect of R. rosea L. on learning and memory function cannot be excluded from overall over estimation of effect sizes and efficacy, which may weaken the validity of conclusions.

FIGURE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 11. The funnel plot: effects of Rhodiola rosea L. for decreasing the escape latency in MWM.

Interpretation of the Results

Considerably high heterogeneity was present in this meta-analysis, the summary positive results should be interpreted with caution. Given that there are many potential sources of heterogeneity in the outcome, several means are taken into consideration for the finding of the causes. Firstly, random-effects models are used in our study. Heterogeneity is a key condition for the execution of meta-regression, but it can also cause confusion if confounding factors are not well-balanced. As small number of studies were included in this meta-analysis, we made the meta-regression with reservations and did subgroup analysis based on four potential confounding factors including animal species, animal model, the duration of treatment, and the quality of study. The results of subgroup analyses suggested that the first three factors were very likely to be the sources of heterogeneity in this research, while the poor quality of methodology still could not be exempted from the excuses for high heterogeneity. Sensitivity analyses have also been adopted to detect the effects of studies identified as being aberrant result, or being highly influential in the analysis (Haidich, 2010). While no studies identified as being aberrant result or being highly influential in the analysis from the results of sensitivity analyses in this review.

Implication for Further Studies

While mice models are increasing used for cognitive experiments involving learning and memory process that were originally designed for rat species, the stability of spatial cognitive representation in rats changes more slightly over time than in mice (Hok et al., 2016). In the subgroup analyses, rat species also showed greater effect size in depression of escape latency than that of mice. Thus, rat species were considered as suitable cognitive experiments involving learning and memory process. In addition, the impact of gender on cognitive function deserves attention. In the present study, male rats models and male/female mice models were used in the included studies of our review for working memory process, while no significant difference existed in the pooled result of meta-analysis in escape latency of MWM test after discarding two studies with male/female mice (Wu et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2013). However, a male advantage for working memory and a female advantage for visual memory and social cognition in rodent models were highlighted in recent systematic review (Leger and Neill, 2016). Thus, using a single sex animal model is considered more reasonable for study learning and memory function in future experiments.

Two dementia models of AD and vascular dementia (VaD) are most commonly approached for learning and memory research (Kalaria et al., 2008). However, there are several model methods for inducing these two dementia types and their differences of effectiveness and robustness are not investigated. For this systematic review, intra-peritoneal injection with scopolamine, combination with aluminum trichloride, D-galactose and scopolamine, intracerebroventricular injection with streptozotocin, and hippocampal injection with Aβ1−40 were the most approaches for AD models in the included studies. Different time scales of artery occlusion and different arteries selected for blood blocking were adopted for VaD models. In the subgroup analyses, six animal models including AD, VaD, hypoxia, sleep deprivation, epilepsy, and diabetes mellitus models were conducted for cognitive impairment, of which AD models accounted for 38.7% weight and VaD models accounted for 17.7% weight. These two most weight of models showed no significant difference in effect size on escape latency of MWM test, which can indirectly reflect the effectiveness and robustness of the two dementia models for cognitive impairment.

A lower-quality study trends toward better outcomes, leading to the global estimated effect overstated (García-Bonilla et al., 2012). In the present study, many domains had flaws in aspects of randomization, allocation concealment, and blinding and sample size calculation, which are the core standards of study design (Moher et al., 2015). Thus, we recommended that the experimental research of R. rosea L. for learning and memory function need be promoted by means of incorporating the ARRIVE guidelines (Kilkenny et al., 2012).

Long-term treatment for dementia progression with Gingko biloba showed great effect on prevention of cognitive decline (Dodge et al., 2008). In parallel, treatment with R. rosea L. more than 2 weeks showed greater effect size in the escape latency of MWM test than that of < 2 weeks' treatment in the subgroup analyses, suggesting that long-term treatment with R. rosea L. has a greater benefit for cognitive function. In view of the number of studies in subgroup analyses was relatively small and may lack of statistical power to detect smaller effect sizes. Therefore, we recommend that future studies involving this problem are conducted strictly complying with standards of research methodology and report their adequate information clearly.

Systemic review of animal studies plays a critical role in drug development and the clarification of physiological and pathological mechanisms of clinical research. In this systematic review, some included studies speculated on how R. rosea L. enhanced learning and memory function and the possible mechanisms are summarized as follows: (1) antioxidant through increasing the level of GSH (Wang et al., 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang et al., 2013), NADH/NADPH (Zhang et al., 2013; Barhwal et al., 2015), and enhancing SOD-induced antioxidant via attenuating chondriokinesis to reduce the release of MDA (Jiang et al., 2001; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011a,b; Zhang S. et al., 2012; Zhang X.X. et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2017b; Wei, 2017; Xiong and Gao, 2017; Yang, 2017); (2) improvement of the circulation by enhancing the expression of NO via up-regulating the expression of NOS (Deng, 2006; Chen, 2008; Wang et al., 2013); (3) cholinergic regulation through increasing the activity of Ach via down-regulating the activity of AchE (Jiang et al., 2001; Xie, 2003; Wu et al., 2004; Shi et al., 2006; Chen, 2008; Cao, 2009; Zhang et al., 2013); (4) inhibition of apoptosis through reducing the amount of calcium in nerve cells (Chen, 2008; Qu et al., 2009; Qi et al., 2013) and down-regulating the expression of caspase-3 (Qu et al., 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017b); (5) anti-inflammatory through inhibiting the expression of TNF-α (Zou et al., 2009) and NF-κB (Zhang et al., 2013); (6) increasing sirtuin 1 (SIRT1) activity through a cytochrome P4502E1 (CYP2E1)-regulated mechanism (Cao, 2009); (7) increasing the expression of Bcl-2 and reducing the expression of Bax protein in the hippocampus (Cao, 2009; Yan et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2017a; Wei, 2017) and improving the expression of PSD-95 and shank-1 protein in the hippocampus (Wang et al., 2008), alleviating apoptosis in the hippocampal CA1 area. The possible mechanisms of R. rosea L. for learning and/or memory function are through antioxidant, cholinergic regulation, anti-apoptosis activities, anti-inflammatory, improving coronary blood flow, and cerebral metabolism (Figure 12).

FIGURE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 12. A schematic representation of possible mechanisms of Rhodiola rosea L. for improving learning and memory function. Solid lines indicate established effects, whereas dashed lines represent putative mechanisms. *The expression of Bcl-2, Bax protein, PSD-95 and shank-1 protein in the hippocampus; **The activity of sirtuin 1.

Conclusion

We have provided a first-ever comprehensive preclinical systematic review of R. rosea L. for cognitive behavior in animal studies and our findings indicate that R. rosea L. improves learning and memory function in experimental models.

Author Contributions

GM, QZ, MX, XZ, ZL, LL, and GZ designed the study. GM and QZ collected the data. GM and MX performed all analyses. GM, QZ, ZL, LL, and GZ wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to writing of this manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by the grant of National Natural Science Foundation of China (81573750/81473491/81173395/H2902).

References

Barhwal, K., Das, S. K., Kumar, A., Hota, S. K., and Srivastava, R. B. (2015). Insulin receptor A and Sirtuin 1 synergistically improve learning and spatial memory following chronic salidroside treatment during hypoxia. J. Neurochem.. 135, 332–346. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, J. (2009). The Experimental Study of the Effect of Rhodiola Herb Against Activity of AchE, NOS and the Expression Bcl-2, Bax Proteinin the Hippocampus Tissue of Alzheimer's Disease in Rats., MDthesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CMFD&dbname=CMFD2009&filename=2009123437.nh&uid (Accessed March 5, 2016)

Chen, Y. Q. (2008). The Experimental Study of the Effete of Rhodiola Against Vaseular Dementia Rats on Activity of SOD, Content of MDA, Activity of AchE and Apoptosis of Neurons in Hippocampus Tissue., MDthesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CMFD&dbname=CMFD2009&filename=2008138185.nh&uid (Accessed July 5, 2016)

De Bock, K., Eijnde, B. O., Ramaekers, M., and Hespel, P. (2004). Acute Rhodiola rosea intake can improve endurance exercise performance. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab. 14, 298–307. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.14.3.298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, S. M. (2006). Study on the Quality Standard and Pharmacological Activity Process, Extraction of Total Tannins Inrhodiola. Ph.D. thesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CDFD&dbname=CDFD9908&filename=2006194899.nh&uid (Accessed July 5, 2016).

Dickersin, K., Chan, S., and Chalmers, T. C., (1987). Publication bias and clinical trials. Control. Clin. Trials. 8, 343–353. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(87)90155-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, H. H., Zitzelberger, T., Oken, B. S., Howieson, D., and Kaye, J. (2008). A randomized placebo-controlled trial of Ginkgo biloba for the prevention of cognitive decline. Neurology. 70, 1809–1817. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000303814.13509.db

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farhath, K., Amarinder, S. B., and Brahm, S. (2005). Rhodiola rosea: a versatile adaptogen. Compr. Rev. Food Sci. F. 4, 55–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1541-4337.2005.tb00073.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Bonilla, L., Campos, M., Giralt, D., Salat, D., Chacón, P., Hernández-Guillamon, M., et al. (2012). Evidence for the efficacy of statins inanimal stroke models: a meta-analysis. J. Neurochem. 122, 233–243.doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2012.07773.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, Y. L., Li, L. Q., Wang, H. Y., Zhang, J. B., Zhang, P. P., and Zhuang, Y. J. (2017). Effect of Herba Rhodiolae on learning and memory abilities and morphology of hippocampus in rats with intermittent hypoxia. Chinese J. Gerontol. 37, 4978–4979 doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-9202.2017.20.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, T. X., Liu, S., Zhang, L., Liu, D. X., Yang, Z. L., and Liu, S. Y. (2017). Effects of salidroside on learning and memory dysfunction in chronic hypoxic mice. Chin. J. Gerontol. 37, 6029–6031. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-9202.2017.24.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haidich, A. B. (2010). Meta-Analysis in Medical Research. Hippokratia.14, 29-−37. Available online at: http://xueshu.baidu.com/s?wd=paperuri%3A%2812aad278fcfead385616bbb203ebe932%29&filte, (Accessed March 5, 2016)

Hok, V., Poucet, B., Duvelle, É., Save, É., and Sargolini, F. (2016). Spatial cognition in mice and rats: similarities and differences in brain and behavior. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 7, 406–421. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ji, J. H., Dong, L. L., Cao, J., Yang, F. M., and Wang, J. (2009). Effects of Integripetal Rhodiola Herb on Ethology and Anti-0xydatiOn Potential of Brain Tissue of Alzheimer's Disease model Rats. Chin. J. Inf. Tradit. Chin. Med. 2, 38–40. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-5304.2018.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, W. H., Meng, X. T., Hao, L. M., Cui, L., Dong, Z. Y., and Wang, S. L. (2001). Study of anti-aging and anti-dementia effects of Rhodosin on aging rats and experimental dementia rats. J. N. Bethune Univ. Med. Sci. 2, 127–129. doi: 10.13481/j.1671-587x.2001.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, C. N., Roland, A., and Upton, N. (2004). New symptomatic strategies in Alzheimer's disease. Drug Discovery Discov. Today Therapeutic Ther. Strategies. 1, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ddstr.2004.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalaria, R. N., Maestre, G. E., Arizaga, R., Friedland, R. P., Galasko, D., and Hall, K. (2008). Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia in developing countries: prevalence, management, and risk factors. Lancet Neurol. 7, 812–826. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70169-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilkenny, C., Browne, W. J., Cuthill, I. C., Emerson, M., and Altman, D. G. (2012).Improving bioscience research reporting: the ARRIVE guidelines for reportinganimal research. Osteoarth. Cartil. 20, 256–260. doi: 10.1016/j.joca.2012.02.010

CrossRef Full Text

Kurkin, V. A., and Zapesochnaya, G. G. (1985). Chemical composition and pharmacological characteristics of Rhodiola rosea. J. Med. Plants 10, 1231–1245.

Google Scholar

Lazarova, M. B., Petkov, V. D., and Markovska, V. L. (1986). Effects of meclofenoxate and Extr. Rhodiolae roseae L. on electroconvulsive shock-impaired learning and memory in rats. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 8, 547–552.

Google Scholar

Leger, M., and Neill, J. C. (2016). A systematic review comparing sex differences in cognitive function in schizophrenia and in rodent models for schizophrenia, implications for improved therapeutic strategies. Neurosci. Biobehav. 68, 979–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, B. B. (2009). Effects of RSGS on Learning/Memory and the BLOOD Ach Content in Vascular Dementia Rats. MD thesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CMFD&dbname=CMFD2010&filename=2009206173.nh&uid (Accessed March 5, 2016)

Liu, N., Xu, M., Wang, H. L., Wang, W. T., Wang, Y. Y., and Wang, M.W. (2017b). The effects of Rhodosin on cognition in rats with vascular demetia. J. Hebei Med. Univ. 38, 621–624. doi: 10.3969/j.Issn.1007-3205.2017.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, N., Wang, J. H., Wang, H. L., Qiu, H. Q., Wang, Y. Y., and Wang, M. W. (2017a). The therapeutic effects of Rhodosin on cognition in AD rats. J. Hebei Med. Univ. 38, 133–137. doi: 10.3969/j.Issn.1007-3205.2017.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z. W., Wu, M. C., and Chen, P. (2003). Effects of Rhodiola henryi extract on learning memory and antihypoxia in mice. Acta Nutrimenta Sinica. 01, 101–104. doi: 10.13325/j.cnki.acta.nutr.sin

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mao, G. X., Deng, H. B., Yuan, L. G., Li, D. D., Li, Y. Y., and Wang, Z. (2010). Protective role of salidroside against aging in a mouse model induced by D-galactose. Biomed. Environ. Sci. 23, 161–166. doi: 10.1016/s0895-3988(10)60047-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Avey, M., Antes, G., and Altman, D. G. (2015). Erratum: the nationalinstitutes of health and guidance for reporting preclinical research. BMC Med.. 13, 1741–7015. doi: 10.1186/s12916-015-0321-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parihar, M. S., and Hemnani, T. (2004). Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis and therapeutic interventions. J. Clin. Neurosci. 11, 456–467. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2003.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pharmacopoeia Committee of the People's Republic of China Ministry of health (2005). Pharmacopoeia of People's Republic of China Part I. Beijing: Chemical Industry Press.

Qi, C. F., Dong, M. Y., Li, C. X., Zhang, J. F., Zhang, J. S., Li, F. X., et al. (2013). The mechanisms and effects of rhodiola on expression of parvalbumin in rat brains after hypobaric hypoxia injury. J. Qinghai Med. Coll. 3, 167–172. doi: 10.13452/j.cnki.jqmc.2013.03.001.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qu, Z. Q., Zhou, Y., Zeng, Y. S., Li, Y., and Chung, P. (2009). Pretreatment with Rhodiola rosea extract reduces cognitive impairment induced by intracerebroventricular streptozotocin in rats: implication of anti-oxidative and neuroprotective effects. Biomed. Environ. Sci. 22, 318–326. doi: 10.1016/S0895-3988(09)60062-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sena, E., van der Worp, H. B., Howells, D., and Macleod, M. (2007). How can we improve the pre-clinical development of drugs for stroke? Trends Neurosci. 30, 433–439. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sena, E. S., Currie, G. L., McCann, S. K., Macleod, M. R., and Howells, D. W. (2014). Systematic reviews and meta-analysis of preclinical studies: why perform them and how to appraise them critically. J. Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 34:737-−742. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shevtsov, V. A., Zholus, B. I., and Shervarly, V. I. (2003). A randomized trial of two different doses of a SHR-5 Rhodiola rosea extract versus placebo and control of capacity for mental work. Phytomedicine 10, 95–105. doi: 10.1006/phrs.1998.0410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. X., Yao, W. B., Wang, H., Guo, H., and Gao, X. D. (2006). Study on the synergistic effects of the extract of Rhodiola rosea L. and CoQ10 on improving the ability of learning and memory in mice. Chin. J. Bio. Pharm. 1, 15-−18. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-1678.2006.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spasov, A. A., Wikman, G. K., Mandrikov, V. B., Mironova, I. A., and Neumoin, V. V. (2000). A double-blind, placebo-controlled pilot study of the stimulating and adaptogenic effect of Rhodiola rosea SHR-5 extract on the fatigue of students caused by stress during an examination period with a repeated low-dose regimen. Phytomedicine. 7, 85–89. doi: 10.1016/S0944-7113(00)80078-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, L. A., Clarke, M., Rovers, M., Riley, R. D., Simmonds, M., Stewart, G., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analyses of individual participant data: the PRISMA-IPD Statement. JAMA 313, 1657–1665. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.3656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, H. Y., Dong, L. L., Cui, H. Y., and Cao, J. (2012). The Influence of Integripetal R hod iola Herb on Behavior and the Expression of Synapse—associated Protein in H ippocam pus of M od el Rats with Alzheimer's Disease. Chin. J. Integra. Med. Cardio-/Cerebrovasc Dis. 4, 455–456. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1672-1349.2012.04.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. F. (1986). The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science 233, 941–947. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(89)90010-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasileva, L. V., Getova, D. P., Doncheva, N. D., Marchev, A. S., and Georgiev, M. I. (2016). Beneficial effect of commercial Rhodiola extract in rats with scopolamine-induced memory impairment on active avoidance. J. Ethnopharmacol. 193, 586–591. doi: 10.1016/j.jep.2016.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Li, L., Cao, J., Ji, J. H., and Yang, F. M. (2008). Effects of integripetal rhodiola herb on spatial learning and memory abilities of AD-like rats as well as protein expression of CAT and GSH-Px in hippocamps of AD-like rats. J. Emerg. Tradit. Chin. Med. 12, 1725–1749. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1004-745X.2008.12.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. T., Zhou, H. R., Wu, D. M., Xu, H., Wang, B. X., Qiu, H. B., et al. (2012). Effect of riodiola extra on learning and memory and ultramicrostructure of cells in sleep deprivation rats. Progr. Anat. Sci. 4, 337–340. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-034186-6.50031-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Liu, Y. G., and Chen, Z. F. (2013). Effects of salidroside on learning and memory ability of vascular dementia in mice. Hebei. Med. J. 16, 2416–2418. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-7386.2013.16.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, J. X. (2017). Effects of Salidroside on Behavior and its Underlying Mechanisms in PTSD Rats. MD thesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CMFDanddbname=CMFD201801andfilename=1017237742.nhanduid (Accessed May 10, 2018)

Wolfgang, V. (2007). Publication bias in meta-analysis: prevention, assessment and adjustments. Psychometrika 72, 269–271. doi: 10.1007/s11336-006-1450-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y. Q., Yao, W. B., Gao, X. D., and Wang, H. (2004). Effects of the extracts of Rhodiola rosea L. on improving the ability of learning an memory in mice. J. Chin. Pharm. Univ. 1, 71–74. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1000-5048.2004.01.018

CrossRef Full Text

Xie, G. Q. (2003). Experimental Study on Rhodiosin and Melatonin for Senile Dementia Senile Dementia in Rats and its Possible Mechanism. MD thesis. Available online at: http://kns.cnki.net/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbcode=CMFDanddbname=CMFD9904andfilename=2003091197.nhanduid (Accessed March 5, 2016).

Xiong, Y., and Gao, L. L. (2017). Salidroside affects spatial memory,NF-κB and COX-2 expressions in vascular dementia rats. Chin. Arch. Tradi. Chin. Med. 35, 402–404. doi: 10.13193/j.issn.1673-7717.2017.02.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, Z. Q., Chen, J., Xing, G. X., Huang, J. G., Hou, X. H., and Zhang, Y. (2015). Salidroside prevents cognitive impairment induced by chronic cerebral hypoperfusion in rats. J. Int. Med. Res. 43, 402–411. doi: 10.1177/0300060514566648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, D. S., Gan, G. P., Le, H. H., Ji, G. J., Chen, S. L., and Hou, S. (2017). Effect of Salidroside on Learning and Memory Abilities and Expression of p75NTR Signal Transducers and Aβ in Hippocampus of AD Rat. Her. Med. 36, 141–144. doi: doi: 10.3837/j.Issn.1004-0781.2017.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Wei, M., Zhang, R., Li, H., Yan, Z. Q., and Liu, S.M. (2011b). Salidroside for epilepticus-induced cognitive deficits in rats and its possible m echanism. Progr. Modern Biomed. 15, 2868–2871. doi: 10.13241/j.cnki.pmb.2011.15.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J. H., Li, H., Xiu, B. H., Yan, Z. Q., and Liu, S. M. (2011a). Salidroside for Hypobaric Hypoxia—induced cognitive deficits in rats and its possible mechanism. Progr. Modern Biomed. 11, 2026–2029. doi: 10.13241/j.cnki.pmb.2011.11.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, N. (2017). Effect of the learning and memory ability and oxidative stress by rhodioside in diabetic model rats. ACTA Chin. Med. 32, 1426–1429. doi: 10.16368/j.issn.1674-8999.2017.08.376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

You, D. Q., Zou, R. F., Cheng, Q., and Cheng, K. M. (2000). Studies on the Effects of Rhodiola crenulata Compound on the Grade of Learning and Memory and Antihypoxia in Mice. Lishizhen Med. Mater. Med. Res. 5, 392–393. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1008-0805.2000.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, S., Liu, M., Gu, X., and Ding, F. (2008). Neuroprotective effects of salidroside in the PC12 cell model exposed to hypoglycemia and serum limitation. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 28, 1067–1078. doi: 10.1007/s10571-008-9284-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Zhen, Y. F., Ren, P. C. B., Song, L. G., Kong, W. N., Shao, T. M., et al. (2013). Salidroside attenuates beta amyloid-induced cognitive deficits via modulating oxidative stress and inflammatory mediators in rat hippocampus. Behav. Brain Res. 1, 70–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Yu, H., Sun, Y., Lin, X., Chen, B., Tan, C., et al. (2007). Protective effects of salidroside on hydrogen peroxide-induced apoptosis in SH-SY5Y human neuroblastoma cells. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 564, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.01.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Zeng, Z. F., Chu, Z. Y., and Wang, J. C. (2012). The improving effect and mechanisms of Rhodiola compound on memory-damaged mice. Chin. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 9, 797–799. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1674-6554.2012.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X. D., Yu, Z. Y., and Zhang, R. (2006). Progress in the chemical composition of Rhodiola. Aerosp. Med. 17, 61–63. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.2095-1434.2006.01.060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X. X., Zheng, H., Ma, Z. S., Shi, L. X., and Zeng, S. L. (2012). Effects of Rhodiola rosea on learning and memory in rats after sleep deprivation and the dissection of its mechanism. Chin. J. Clin. Pharm. Ther. 6, 634–639. Available online at: http://www.wanfangdata.com.cn/details/detail.do?_type=perio&id=zglcylxyzlx201206006

Google Scholar

Zhao, L. Y., Cheng, Y. J., and Zhang, Y. (2010). Cognitive function Effect of Salidroside on Diabetic Encephalopathy Rats. Chin. J. Labora Diag. 11, 1722–1724. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-4287.2010.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, Y. Q., Cai, Z. Y., Mao, Y. F., Li, J. B., Deng, X. M., et al. (2009). Effects of salidroside-pretreatment on neuroethology of rats after global cerebral ischemia-reperfusion. J. Chin. Integr. Med. 2, 130–134. doi: 10.3736/jcim20090207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Rhodiola rosea L., salidroside, learning and memory, cognition, preclinical evidence, possible mechanisms

Citation: Ma G, Zheng Q, Xu M, Zhou X, Lu L, Li Z and Zheng G (2018) Rhodiola rosea L. Improves Learning and Memory Function: Preclinical Evidence and Possible Mechanisms. Front. Pharmacol. 9:1415. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.01415

Received: 26 February 2018; Accepted: 16 November 2018;
Published: 04 December 2018.

Edited by:

Farhat Ullah, University of Malakand, Pakistan

Reviewed by:

Ikram Ullah, International Islamic University, Pakistan
Haroon Khan, Abdul Wali Khan University Mardan, Pakistan

Copyright © 2018 Ma, Zheng, Xu, Zhou, Lu, Li and Zheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Lu, lulinlc@126.com
Zuo-xiao Li, lzx3235@sina.com
Guo-Qing Zheng, gq_zheng@sohu.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.