Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Pharmacol., 17 February 2021
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Novel Pain Therapeutics: From Basic Research to Clinical Translation and Rehabilitation View all 20 articles

Addressing Pain for a Proper Rehabilitation Process in Patients With Severe Disorders of Consciousness

  • 1Research in Advanced NeuroRehabilitation, Istituto Sant’Anna, Crotone, Italy
  • 2Department of Physics and Astronomy, Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 3Research in Advanced NeuroRehabilitation, Istituto Sant’Anna, Crotone, Italy

Consciousness constitutes a fundamental prerequisite in the individual appraisal and experience of pain. In the same way, a person needs to be able to report on pain perception. Patients who suffered a severe brain injury with disorders of consciousness (DOC) represent a spectrum of pathologies affecting patients' capacity to interact with the external world. In these patients, the most relevant aspects in response to pain are physiologic and behavioral. The treatments and management of pain are challenging issues in these patients, arising serious ethical concerns and bringing emotional load among medical staff, caregivers, and relatives. In this review, we report the importance of having a correct pain management in DOC patients, to individuate the best pharmacological treatment that can make the difference in detecting a behavioral response, indicative of a change in the level of consciousness, and in planning a more effective rehabilitative approach.

Introduction

In 1979, the IASP approved the following definition of pain: "An unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage" coupling the sensory and emotional dimensions of the experience, as well as the association between tissue injury and pain (IASP, 1979).

The emotional experience can be described by a complex system of interacting processes characterized by affective (i.e., subjective experienced feeling), expressive (e.g., mimics, behaviors), cognitive (e.g., thoughts), and physiological (e.g., heart rate) components (Scherer et al., 2001).

In 1999 McCaffrey and Pasero reported a similar definition: "Pain is whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever the experiencing person says it does" denoting the subjectivity of the pain experience (McCaffrey and Pasero, 1999, p 63). Such definition implies not only that pain may be detected when a patient reports its manifestation but that consciousness constitutes a fundamental prerequisite in the individual appraisal and experience of pain. In 2007, at the Kyoto annual meeting, the publication of the modification of the IASP Basic Pain Terminology (Loeser and Treede, 2008) was approved, with the introduction of the terms nociceptive neuron, nociception, nociception stimulus, nociceptive pain, sensitization, peripheral and central sensitization (Table 1). Independently from a more accurate terminology, a key aspect of pain remains the subjective experience and the necessity to report on it (“…the experiencing person says it is … ”). The importance of reporting on the pain sensation is described in a study by Clarke and colleagues on the chronic pain in older adults, recommending the narrative approach to describe and discuss the experience of pain. If this approach could represent an useful tool to assess pain in subjects who are able to refer on it (Clarke et al., 2012), it highlights the issues in assessing pain in non-communicative patients.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. International association for the study of the pain–terminology.

Nociception Versus Pain

In the assessment of non-communicative patients, it is essential to discriminate a reflex from higher-order behavioral responses.

Noxious stimulation implies a response of the Autonomic Nervous System (ANS). Typical physiological responses are observable in the cardiovascular reactivity, respiration, skin conductance and pupil dilatation (Kyle and McNeil, 2014; Mischkowski et al., 2018).

The nociception (i.e., the neural process of encoding noxious stimuli) refers to the perception (conscious or not) of nociceptive stimuli (an actually or potentially tissue-damaging event transduced and encoded by nociceptors) (Loeser and Treede, 2008), eliciting the activation of an extensive cortical network (i.e. somatosensory, insular, and cingulate areas, as well as frontal and parietal areas) (Coghill et al., 2003; Chatelle et al., 2014). The transmission of the information of the nociceptive stimulation follows the via spinothalamic tract to reach the thalamus and the cortex (Loeser and Treede, 2008; Morton et al., 2016). The reflex response is thought to be modulated by midbrain and thalamus (Morton et al., 2016), while part of the sensory–discriminative features of the pain processing entails the secondary somatosensory (S2) cortex, with the posterior insula (lateral network) (Ploner et al., 2002; Lockwood et al., 2013).

The conscious experience of pain requires a more complex network, generally called Pain Matrix (Iannetti and Mouraux, 2010; Salomons et al., 2016) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Simplified scheme of the Pain Matrix.

The Pain Matrix involves two main subsystems: the Lateral Neuronal Network (LNN) and the Medial Network (MN). The LNN encompassing S2 cortex, lateral thalamus, and posterior insula (Mutschler et al., 2011) encodes the sensory discriminative information; the MN encompassing anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and prefrontal cortex encodes affective-cognitive information (Medford and Critchley, 2010). Also, the cerebellum (Moulton et al., 2011) and motor areas (e.g., the striatum, cerebellum, and the supplementary motor area) (Barceló et al., 2012) are involved in pain perception and processing.

Several studies investigating the dynamics of activation (connectivity) of the pain matrix have shown that nociceptive input is first processed in the posterior insula, wherein it is coded in terms of intensity and anatomical location, and then transmitted to the anterior insula, where the emotional reaction to pain is elaborated (Ploner et al., 2002; Tracey, 2008; Frot et al., 2014).

For the "mind-body" theory, the pain experience necessitates of a body and a mental component (Duncan, 2000). The first encompasses the phenomena leading to perception and response, such as pain pathway and central processing, while the second encompasses perception and interpretation of pain, including the cognitive and affective components (Sarno, 2001). The mind-body approach shows the impossibility to separate mind and body in the pain experience, then the importance of self-report. Nevertheless, the IASP stated that “Verbal description is the only one of several behaviors to express pain; inability to communicate does not negate the possibility that a human or a nonhuman animal experiences pain” (IASP, 2020).

In non-communicative patients, the most relevant aspects in response to pain are physiologic (i.e., modification in the vital parameters such as heart rate and respiration) and behavioral (i.e., modification in the facial expression, motor and visual response).

To assess pain in non-communicative patients several behavioral scales were developed, with each of them oriented to assess a specific typology of patients. As an example, the Behavioral Pain Scale (Payen et al., 2001) which is commonly used in trauma or post-operative care unit to assess pain in critically sedated and mechanically ventilated patients; the Faces, Legs Cry and Consolability scale (FLACC) (Merkel et al., 2002; Malviya et al., 2006), which was developed for the pediatric population to measure pain severity; or the Pain Assessment in Advanced Dementia scale (PAINAD) (Warden et al., 2003), developed for patients affected by dementia.

Disorders of Consciousness (DOC) represent a spectrum of pathologies affecting the capacity of patients to interact with the external world. It can be either due to a traumatic or a non-traumatic cause and sometime to a combination of both (Giacino et al., 2018).

Among the different definition of consciousness, the most accepted viewpoint refers to the brain's ability to form cognition of the world, by the perception of self and the environment. A requisite for conscious behaviors is the presence of adequate arousal (i.e., wakefulness) and awareness of content (i.e., sensory, cognitive, and affective experience) (Giacino et al., 2018). The first is referred to the level of consciousness and the second to the content of consciousness (Xie et al., 2017; Giacino et al., 2018).

The two possible conditions following the acquired brain injury (i.e. a terrible event disrupting the arousal and awareness systems, mediated respectively by the brainstem and cortex) are either the Vegetative State/Unresponsive Wakefulness Syndrome (UWS/VS) or the Minimally Conscious State (MCS) (Giacino and Kalmar, 2005; Laureys et al., 2010; Giacino et al., 2018). The first is characterized by spontaneous opening of the eyes and no sign of consciousness, with only residual reflexive responses to external stimuli; the second by minimal but discernible signs of non-reflex behaviors (i.e., response to visual, auditory, tactile, or noxious stimuli) which occur in a reproducible even if inconsistent manner (Giacino et al., 2002; Schnakers et al., 2009).

The clinical assessment is based on clinical consensus and behavioral scales such as the Coma Recovery Scale (Giacino et al., 2004; Seel et al., 2010).

For the assessment of pain in patients with DOC a specific scale, the Nociception Coma Scale (Schnakers et al., 2010; Riganello et al., 2014) has been developed. It is based on the observation of the motor response (non/flaccid, abnormal posturing, flexion withdrawal, and localization to noxious stimulation), verbal response (non-verbalization, groaning, vocalization, and intelligible verbalization), visual response (none, startle, eyes movement and fixation) and facial expression (non-oral reflexive/startle response, grimace and cry), following a noxious stimulation (i.e., pressure on the fingernail bed using an algometer). Each subscale ranges from 0 (no response) to 3 (appropriate response), for a total score ranging from 0 to 12. A revised version, characterized by the absence of the visual subscale, was developed by Chatelle et al. (2016), but the two versions maintain the same clinimetric properties (Vink et al., 2017). Higher values for these scales indicate a more complex response to the noxious stimulus and content of consciousness.

A study by Sattin et al. (2018) reported lower pain pressure thresholds in DOC patients compared to healthy participants suggesting further investigations. Hyperestesia, hypoesthesia and anesthesia, conditions frequently present after acquired brain injury, may in fact alter responses to pain stimuli. Formisano and colleagues (Formisano et al., 2020) proposed for the evaluation of the response to painful stimuli by NCS and NCS-R, different and personalized stimuli (e.g., hand opening, upper limb abduction, head mobilization), because altered pain pathway may affect the searched responses by standard pressure on the fingernail bed.

Pain and Consciousness in Disorders of Consciousness Patients

The treatments and management of pain is a challenging issue in patients with DOC. The condition of suffering in DOC patients is a very controversial question. Generally, caregivers and relatives believe in the possibility that VS/UWS patients might feel pain, influencing end-of-life decisions. However, there is not a unanimous consensus about whether non-responsive patients might have a sufferance condition or might feel pain (Demertzi et al., 2013; Demertzi, 2018), implying increasing ethical questions (Riganello et al., 2016).

Neuroimaging studies have shown different processing of pain between UWS/VS and MCS patients (Boly et al., 2008; Chatelle and Thibaut, 2014; Garcia-Larrea and Bastuji, 2018). In a seminal Oxigen 15 (O-H2O) PET study, pain induced activation of the midbrain, contralateral thalamus, and primary somatosensory cortex in UWS/VS patients (Laureys et al., 2002). Kassubek and colleagues, using the same PET technique in DOC patients, found the activation of the secondary somatosensory cortex, in the cingulate cortex contralateral to the stimulus, and the posterior insula ipsilateral to the stimulus (Kassubek et al., 2003). These findings suggest that DOC patients might have a residual perception and partial sensory-discriminative pain processing. However, the activation of the pain network resulted incomplete, with the primary somatosensory cortex functionally disconnected from the secondary somatosensory, bilateral posterior parietal, premotor, polysensory superior temporal, and prefrontal cortices (Laureys et al., 2004). The isolation of primary cortical activation from higher-order associative cortical activity suggests a non-integrated pain processing with a consequent less conscious experience (Boly et al., 2008).

Compared to the UWS/VS patients, MCS patients present higher metabolism in associative areas, principally in the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (Laureys et al., 2005), and a restoration of the correlation between these areas and the thalamus (Laureys et al., 2000). Boly and colleagues found similar brain area activation to noxious stimuli in MCS patients compared to controls (Boly et al., 2008) (i.e., thalamus, the primary somatosensory cortex, the secondary somatosensory cortex or insula, the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus, and the anterior cingulate area). Another fMRI study performed by Markl and colleagues (Markl et al., 2013), demonstrated the significant activation of the sensory and affective components of the pain matrix in patients clinically diagnosed as UWS/VS, suggesting the possibility of a painful experience in some of these patients.

The neuroimaging, although it is a powerful tool of investigation, remains a complicated, expensive, time-consuming approach and of difficult use in the routine of clinical practice. In this frame, the behavioral pain assessment is still widely recognized as the most accessible and easiest approach. However, the risk of misdiagnosis remains high, considering that patients with DOC might not show any overt response to painful stimulation even if perceived (Schnakers et al., 2012; Chatelle and Thibaut, 2014; Calabrò et al., 2017; Cortese et al., 2020).

Different approaches of investigation based on Heart Variability Analysis (HRV), Galvanik Skin Response (GSR), or Laser Evocated Potential (LEP) have shown the possibility to observe pain processing in UWS/VS patients (de Tommaso et al., 2015; Riganello et al., 2018a; Cortese et al., 2020).

HRV is the fluctuation in the time intervals between adjacent heartbeats (interbeat interval - IBI) and represents the output of a complex brain-heart two-way interaction system (Riganello et al., 2012). The Central Autonomic Network (CAN), an integrative model where neural structures and heart function are involved and functionally linked in the affective, cognitive and autonomic regulation, describes this interaction (Benarroch, 2007; Thayer and Lane, 2009; Riganello, 2016). The principal neural structure of the CAN cover the brainstem (periaqueductal gray matter, nucleus ambiguous, and ventromedial medulla), limbic structures (amygdala and hypothalamus), prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate, insula, orbitofrontal, and ventromedial cortex) and cerebellum (Benarroch, 2006, 2007; Lane et al., 2009; Thayer and Lane, 2009).

To describe the sympathovagal modulation, the HRV is generally analyzed in the time and frequency domains (Berntson et al., 1997). However, the physiological phenomena that characterize the biological events are dynamic and complex (Billman, 2011). For this reason, the non-linear analysis represents a useful approach to understand the brain-heart two-way interaction (Riganello, 2016). The HRV entropy quantifies the unpredictability and complexity of the IBI series. Higher and lower entropy indicate respectively higher or lower unpredictable IBI sequence, and correspondingly a higher or lower Heart-Brain two-way interaction (Riganello et al., 2018b). In a study based on noxious and non-noxious stimuli, lower HRV entropy was observed in UWS/VS compared to MCS patients and lower in MCS patients compared to healthy controls (Riganello et al., 2018a). Cortese and colleagues, through the GSR and HRV entropy measures, observed a trace conditioning of the nociceptive stimulus (i.e., a conditioning protocol where the Conditioned Stimulus - pain - is presented, terminated, and followed after some intervening period by the Unconditioned Stimulus - Music -) in patients diagnosed as UWS/VS and without any oriented or reflex behavioral response to the nociceptive stimulation (Cortese et al., 2020). The trace conditioning is considered an appropriate method to assess consciousness's presence without a verbal report (Bekinschtein et al., 2009). The GSR is an indicator of psychological or physiological arousal, measured by the skin conductance that is controlled by the sweat glands, that are controlled by the sympathetic nervous system (Critchley et al., 2000; Cernat et al., 2017). The GSR signal, used to observe the presence of the trace conditioning, was observed only in patients with UWS/VS who changed the level of consciousness within thirty days from the first assessment (suggesting the possibility in these patients to perceive and learn the pain stimulus). Moreover, the HRV entropy was higher in these patients compared to those that remain with the diagnosis of UWS/VS (Cortese et al., 2020).

In two different LEP studies, authors found that brain-injured UWS/VS patients might process the painful stimuli (de Tommaso et al., 2013; de Tommaso et al., 2015). In a subsequent study by Naro et al. (2016) on MCS and UWS/VS patients, authors reported the modulation of the γ-band oscillation power induced by nociceptive repetitive laser stimulations and its correlation with the NCS-R. The results showed a strong positive correlation between γ-band oscillation power and NCS-R in all MCS and some of the UWS/VS patients, suggesting that, also in the presence of a lower NCS-R total score, the UWS/VS patients may have had a covert pain’s experience. In a successive study, Calabrò and colleagues found γ-oscillations within the limbic system related to pain perception in some of the screened UWS/VS patients, evidencing that they might have perceived the affective component of pain (Calabrò et al., 2017).

Pain in Disorders of Consciousness and Treatment

The above-cited results put in evidence two relevant points: firstly, the assessment of nociceptive stimulation as mean to detect possible content of consciousness in patients diagnosed as UWS/VS; secondly, also if not capable of exhibiting oriented behavior to the painful stimuli, UWS/VS patients might perceive pain. In a recent work, Cortese and colleagues (Cortese et al., 2020) showed that the increase of the score in the NCS anticipates the increase of the score in the CRS-R. This finding highlights the importance of pain assessment in these patients, and how the behavioral response to pain could precede other responsive behavioral aspects. However, the oriented behavioral response to the nociceptive stimuli could be covered by a necessary pharmacotherapy for the treatments of the suspected pain condition (Pistoia et al., 2015).

Pain could be present in the acute phase and in the successive period of intensive rehabilitation (Schnakers et al., 2012; Schnakers and Zasler, 2015). The cause of pain might arise from multiple factors such as skin lesions, surgical wounds, neuropathic pain, or injury of various types (i.e., abdominal, chest, fractures) as well as nursing-maneuvers with devices used during the hospitalization period (i.e., percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy, nasogastric tube, bladder catheter replacement, venous and arterial blood sampling) (Ivanhoe and Hartman, 2004; Crooks et al., 2007; Baron, 2009; Popernack et al., 2015; Bexkens et al., 2017). In the rehabilitation as well as in the chronic phase, pain can arise from peripheral nerve lesions, central pain, diffuse spasticity, joint limitations, bedsores, paraosteoarthropathy, constipation, post-traumatic headache (Olver et al., 1996; Khan et al., 2003; Sherman et al., 2006; Hoffman et al., 2007; Ofek and Defrin, 2007; Baron, 2009; Gironda et al., 2009).

The Central Nervous System damage might be the cause of chronic pain (e.g., thalamic pain following a traumatic brain injury with diffuse axonal injury (Munivenkatappa and Agrawal, 2016; Irvine and Clark, 2018)). These conditions may lead to changes in the central nervous system pain processing and to a Complex Regional Pain Syndrome (CPRS), a neuropathic pain disorder characterized by distinct clinical features including allodynia, hyperalgesia, sudomotor and vasomotor abnormalities, and trophic changes (Schnakers et al., 2012; Guthmiller and Varacallo, 2020). Mechanism underlying CPRS is multifactorial, involving abnormal neuronal transmission, autonomic dysregulation, and central sensitization. The proinflammatory and immunological response increase production of interleukins, bradykinin, substance P, and osteoprotegerin, with consequent peripheral sensitization, alteration of the sympathetic nervous system and increasing expression of adrenergic receptors on nociceptive fibers (Guthmiller and Varacallo, 2020).

The presence of painful symptoms might interfere with the rehabilitation processes limiting and/or delaying its effect. It is crucial to intervene with appropriate early measure to prevent the appearance of secondary damage associated with pain and functional limitation such as bedsore or muscle-tendon retraction (Schnakers and Zasler, 2015).

In DOC patients, there is no general agreement on pharmacological pain treatment (Bartolo et al., 2016). Generally, it should be administered in the presence of behavioral signs of pain. The accurate pharmacotherapy dosage is crucial to avoid interferences with the assessment and treatment plan for the recovery of consciousness. Ineffective control of pain could affect or inhibit the emergence of intentional behavioral responses, while over-treatment could limit cognitive recovery and attention (Fins et al., 2008; Bartolo et al., 2016).

Brain lesions in these patients are extensive, affecting the nervous system at the cortical, subcortical, intracortical and spinal level. It is essential to provide basic care, managing the insurgence of the secondary medical complication that could increase the risk of further disability (Sazbon and Groswasser, 1990; Sazbon and Groswasser, 1991; Seel et al., 2013) and complicate their treatment and pain management.

Most of these patients are characterized by spasticity. The spasticity, due to a lesion of the pyramidal tract, is defined as “a motor disorder, characterized by a velocity-dependent increase in tonic stretch reflexes (muscle tone) with exaggerated tendon jerks, resulting from hyper-excitability of the stretch reflex as one component of the upper motor neuron syndrome.” (Lance, 1980). It is present in the 89% of DOC patients (Thibaut et al., 2015) and associated with pain and other symptoms such as increased hypertonia and altered sensorimotor control and muscle spasms (Burke et al., 2013). Infiltration of botulinum is advised in case of focal spasticity and to treat severe or worsening cases (Childers et al., 2004; Verplancke et al., 2005). In the case of dystonia and diffuse spasticity, an improvement in their management was observed by the intrathecal baclofen (Pistoia et al., 2015). The improvement of the level of consciousness in DOC was associated to the use of the intrathecal baclofen (Margetis et al., 2014; Pistoia et al., 2015), due probably to the reduced overload of the dysfunctional sensory stimuli reaching the brain or to the stabilization of the circadian rhythms (Margetis et al., 2014).

The symptomatic pain treatment follows the criteria of proportionality and graduality, assessing the interaction with the current therapy (Bartolo et al., 2016). The therapies approaches are generally based on aspirin, paracetamol, nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, opioid and γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic agents (Mura et al., 2013; Bartolo et al., 2016). Aspirin, paracetamol, and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs should be administered in case of presumed mild pain (Schnakers and Zasler, 2007).

In case of suspected moderate pain and neuropathic pain, it is suggested a high-dose of aspirin or paracetamol, oral NSAIDs, GABAergic agents (Czuczwar and Patsalos, 2001; Enna and McCarson, 2006; Schnakers and Zasler, 2015; Bartolo et al., 2016). GABAergic agents are also indicated in case of psychomotor agitation or opposition to mobilization associable to pain. GABA is widely distributed throughout the neuraxis playing a central role in mediating or modulating most central nervous system functions. GABAA and GABAB receptors and GABAergic neurons are present in spinal cord and brain areas associated with the mediation and perception of pain (Enna and McCarson, 2006). Behavioral and physiological responses to pain are regulated by GABAergic projections from the ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra to the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray and dorsal medullary raphe nucleus (Kirouac et al., 2004). Both inhibitors of GABA uptake and metabolism and GABA receptor agonists display significant antinociceptive activity in animal models of acute, inflammatory, and neuropathic pain (Malan et al., 2002; Sa et al., 2004). Further, the antinociceptive response was observed to be induced by the activation of GABAA receptors in the parafasciculus thalami (Reyes-Vazquez et al., 1986). The pharmacotherapy based on GABAergic agents may be accompanied by adverse effects such as drowsiness, fatigue, depression or constriction of the visual field (Czuczwar and Patsalos, 2001).

In the case of presumed severe pain, it is advised to consider the use of parenteral opioids, mixed agonists/antagonists, partial agonist opioids, antidepressants, anticonvulsants, and atypical agents (Schnakers and Zasler, 2015; Bartolo et al., 2016; Seal et al., 2018; Adams et al., 2020). The opioids act by binding proteins called opioid receptors that are widely distributed. Those involved in pain modulation are localized in the central and peripheral nervous system. These receptors also bind endorphins involved not only in pain modulation but also in other body functions such as reinforcement and reward mechanisms, mood and stress, mediated by deep structures of the brain (Russo and Nestler, 2013). The neural proliferation is also modulated by the opioid system (Sargeant et al., 2008) inducing, for example, neural degeneration (Atici et al., 2004; van Dijk et al., 2011) and apoptosis (Hu et al., 2002). Nevertheless, the use of opioids to treat analgesia may be accompanied by side effects, which will depend on the dose, such as somnolence, mental clouding, and respiratory depression (Rosenblum et al., 2008; Rogers et al., 2013) that might interfere with a correct diagnosis of the level of consciousness.

It is evident the current difficulty for pain treatment in patients with DOC, and the impossibility for the patient to refer on the pain perception makes the choice of the correct pharmacological approach a challenge.

At the light of these concerns, the guideline of the physicians should be based on the cost/benefit, intended as to follow the ethical principle of nonmaleficence/beneficence of the treatments.

Conclusion

Pain is not only a perceptual phenomenon. The initial injury, cause of the pain, disrupts the body’s homeostatic systems which, in turn, produce stress. Pain involves a dynamic interaction among biological, psychological, and social factors. These components may modulate pain perception and disability (Duncan, 2000; Gatchel and Kishino, 2008).

The assessment and management of pain in patients with a DOC remain a challenge. The perception of pain in these patients arises rehabilitative problems with ethical issues extending beyond the boundaries of end-of-life decisions (Miller-Smith et al., 2019; Wolf-Meyer, 2020). To date, the correct assessment of DOC patients has a high rate of misdiagnosis (Bosco et al., 2010; van Erp et al., 2015), and the misinterpretation of the behavioral signs may lead to a non-fully appropriate rehabilitative approach.

The correct pain management and the capability to individuate the best pharmacological treatment can make the difference in detecting a behavioral response indicative of a change in the level of consciousness in DOC patients, and in planning a more effective rehabilitative approach.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed and agreed on the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adams, R. S., Corrigan, J. D., and Dams-O’Connor, K. (2020). Opioid use among individuals with traumatic brain injury: a perfect storm? J. Neurotrauma 37, 211–216. doi:10.1089/neu.2019.6451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atici, S., Cinel, L., Cinel, I., Doruk, N., Aktekin, M., Akca, A., et al. (2004). Opioid neurotoxicity: comparison of morphine and tramadol in an experimental rat model. Int. J. Neurosci. 114, 1001–1011. doi:10.1080/00207450490461314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barceló, A. C., Filippini, B., and Pazo, J. H. (2012). The striatum and pain modulation. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 32, 1–12. doi:10.1007/s10571-011-9737-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. (2009). “Neuropathic pain: a clinical perspective,” in Sensory nerves handbook of experimental pharmacology. Editors B. J. Canning, and D. Spina (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 3–30.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolo, M., Chiò, A., Ferrari, S., Tassorelli, C., Tamburin, S., Avenali, M., et al. (2016). Assessing and treating pain in movement disorders, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, severe acquired brain injury, disorders of consciousness, dementia, oncology and neuroinfectivology. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 52, 14.

Google Scholar

Bekinschtein, T. A., Shalom, D. E., Forcato, C., Herrera, M., Coleman, M. R., Manes, F. F., et al. (2009). Classical conditioning in the vegetative and minimally conscious state. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1343–1349. doi:10.1038/nn.2391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (2006). Pain-autonomic interactions. Neurol. Sci. 27, s130–s133. doi:10.1007/s10072-006-0587-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (2007). The autonomic nervous system: basic anatomy and physiology. Continuum Lifelong Learn. Neurol. 13, 13–32. doi:10.1212/01.CON.0000299964.20642.9a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 34, 623–648.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bexkens, R., Washburn, F. J., Eygendaal, D., van den Bekerom, M. P. J., and Oh, L. S. (2017). Effectiveness of reduction maneuvers in the treatment of nursemaid’s elbow: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Emerg. Med. 35, 159–163. doi:10.1016/j.ajem.2016.10.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billman, G. E. (2011). Heart rate variability – a historical perspective. Front. Physiol. 2, 86. doi:10.3389/fphys.2011.00086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boly, M., Faymonville, M.-E., Schnakers, C., Peigneux, P., Lambermont, B., Phillips, C., et al. (2008). Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: an observational study. Lancet Neurol. 7, 1013–1020. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70219-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosco, A., Lancioni, G. E., Belardinelli, M. O., Singh, N. N., O’Reilly, M. F., and Sigafoos, J. (2010). Vegetative state: efforts to curb misdiagnosis. Cognit. Process. 11, 87–90. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0355-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D., Wissel, J., and Donnan, G. A. (2013). Pathophysiology of spasticity in stroke. Neurology. 80, S20–S26. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827624a7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calabrò, R. S., Naro, A., Manuli, A., Leo, A., Luca, R. D., Buono, V. L., et al. (2017). Pain perception in patients with chronic disorders of consciousness: what can limbic system tell us? Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 454–462. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2016.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cernat, R. A., Speriatu, A. M., Taralunga, D. D., Hurezeanu, B. E., Nicolae, I. E., Strungaru, R., et al. (2017). Stress influence on drivers identified by monitoring galvanic skin resistance and heart rate variability 2017 E-health and bioengineering conference. Sinaia: EHB. 2017, 261–264. doi:10.1109/EHB.2017.7995411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatelle, C., De Val, M.-D., Catano, A., Chaskis, C., Seeldrayers, P., Laureys, S., et al. (2016). Is the nociception coma scale-revised a useful clinical tool for managing pain in patients with disorders of consciousness? Clin. J. Pain. 32, 321–326. doi:10.1097/AJP.0000000000000259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatelle, C., Thibaut, A., Bruno, M.-A., Boly, M., Bernard, C., Hustinx, R., et al. (2014). Nociception coma scale–revised scores correlate with metabolism in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neurorehabilitation Neural Repair. 28, 149–152. doi:10.1177/1545968313503220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatelle, C., and Thibaut, A. (2014). Pain issues in disorders of consciousness. Brain Inj. 28 (9), 1202–1208. doi:10.3109/02699052.2014.920518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childers, M. K., Brashear, A., Jozefczyk, P., Reding, M., Alexander, D., Good, D., et al. (2004). Dose-dependent response to intramuscular botulinum toxin type A for upper-limb spasticity in patients after a stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 1063–1069. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2003.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, A., Anthony, G., Gray, D., Jones, D., McNamee, P., Schofield, P., et al. (2012). “I feel so stupid because I can’t give a proper answer” How older adults describe chronic pain: a qualitative study. BMC Geriatr. 12, 78. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-12-78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coghill, R. C., McHaffie, J. G., and Yen, Y.-F. (2003). Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 8538–8542. doi:10.1073/pnas.1430684100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, D., Riganello, F., Arcuri, F., Lucca, L., Tonin, P., Schnakers, C., et al. (2020). The trace conditional learning of the noxious stimulus in UWS patients and its prognostic value in a GSR and HRV entropy study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14, 97. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2020.00097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Elliott, R., Mathias, C. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 20, 3033–3040. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.20-08-03033.2000 Available at: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/20/8/3033 (Accessed January 30, 2014).

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crooks, C. Y., Zumsteg, J. M., and Bell, K. R. (2007). Traumatic brain injury: a review of practice management and recent advances. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. 18, 681–710. doi:10.1016/j.pmr.2007.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czuczwar, S. J., and Patsalos, P. N. (2001). The new generation of GABA enhancers: potential in the treatment of epilepsy. CNS Drugs. 15, 339–350. doi:10.2165/00023210-200115050-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Navarro, J., Ricci, K., Lorenzo, M., Lanzillotti, C., Colonna, F., et al. (2013). Pain in prolonged disorders of consciousness: laser evoked potentials findings in patients with vegetative and minimally conscious states. Brain Inj. 27, 962–972. doi:10.3109/02699052.2013.775507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Navarro, J., Lanzillotti, C., Ricci, K., Buonocunto, F., Livrea, P., et al. (2015). Cortical responses to salient nociceptive and not nociceptive stimuli in vegetative and minimal conscious state. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 17. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demertzi, A., Racine, E., Bruno, M.-A., Ledoux, D., Gosseries, O., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2013). Pain perception in disorders of consciousness: neuroscience, clinical care, and ethics in dialogue. Neuroethics. 6, 37–50. doi:10.1007/s12152-011-9149-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demertzi, A. (2018). “The ethics in the management of patients with disorders of consciousness,” in Coma and Disorders of consciousness. Editors C. Schnakers, and S. Laureys (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 225–234.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. (2000). Mind-body dualism and the biopsychosocial model of pain: what did descartes really say?. J. Med. Philos. 25, 485–513. doi:10.1076/0360-5310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enna, S. J., and McCarson, K. E. (2006). The role of GABA in the mediation and perception of pain. Adv. Pharmacol. 54, 1–27. doi:10.1016/S1054-3589(06)54001-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fins, J. J., Illes, J., Bernat, J. L., Hirsch, J., Laureys, S., and Murphy, E. (2008). Neuroimaging and disorders of consciousness: envisioning an ethical research agenda. Am. J. Bioeth. 8, 3–12. doi:10.1080/15265160802318113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Formisano, R., Contrada, M., Aloisi, M., Ferri, G., Schiattone, S., Iosa, M., et al. (2020). Nociception Coma Scale with personalized painful stimulation versus standard stimulus in non-communicative patients with disorders of consciousness. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 30, 1893–1904. doi:10.1080/09602011.2019.1614464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frot, M., Faillenot, I., and Mauguière, F. (2014). Processing of nociceptive input from posterior to anterior insula in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5486–5499. doi:10.1002/hbm.22565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Larrea, L., and Bastuji, H. (2018). Pain and consciousness. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatr. 87, 193–199. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatchel, R. J., and Kishino, N. D. (2008). The biopsychosocial approach to pain management. Available at: https://rc.library.uta.edu/uta-ir/handle/10106/4999 (Accessed December 21, 2020).

Google Scholar

Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., et al. (2002). The minimally conscious state Definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 58, 349–353. doi:10.1212/WNL.58.3.349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacino, J. T., and Kalmar, K. (2005). Diagnostic and prognostic guidelines for the vegwive and minimally conscious states. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 15, 166–174. doi:10.1080/09602010443000498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., and Whyte, J. (2004). The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 2020–2029. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacino, J. T., Katz, D. I., Schiff, N. D., Whyte, J., Ashman, E. J., Ashwal, S., et al. (2018). Comprehensive systematic review update summary: disorders of consciousness: report of the guideline development, dissemination, and implementation subcommittee of the American academy of neurology; the American congress of rehabilitation medicine; and the national institute on disability, independent living, and rehabilitation research. Neurology. 91, 461–470. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000005928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gironda, R. J., Clark, M. E., Ruff, R. L., Chait, S., Craine, M., Walker, R., et al. (2009). Traumatic brain injury, polytrauma, and pain: challenges and treatment strategies for the polytrauma rehabilitation. Rehabil. Psychol. 54, 247–258. doi:10.1037/a0016906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guthmiller, K. B., and Varacallo, M. (2020). “Complex regional pain syndrome,” in StatPearls (treasure island (FL): StatPearls publishing). Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK430719/ (Accessed December 21, 2020).

Google Scholar

Hoffman, J. M., Pagulayan, K. F., Zawaideh, N., Dikmen, S., Temkin, N., and Bell, K. R. (2007). Understanding pain after traumatic brain injury: impact on community participation. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 86, 962–969. doi:10.1097/PHM.0b013e31815b5ee5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, S., Sheng, W. S., Lokensgard, J. R., and Peterson, P. K. (2002). Morphine induces apoptosis of human microglia and neurons. Neuropharmacology. 42, 829–836. doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(02)00030-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., and Mouraux, A. (2010). From the neuromatrix to the pain matrix (and back). Exp. Brain Res. 205, 1–12. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2340-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IASP (1979). Pain terms: a list with definitions and notes on usage. Recommended by the IASP Subcommittee on Taxonomy. Pain. 6, 249.

PubMed AbstractGoogle Scholar

IASP (2020). IASP terminology–IASP. Available at: https://www.iasp-pain.org/Education/Content.aspx?ItemNumber=1698 (Accessed September 28, 2020).

Google Scholar

Irvine, K.-A., and Clark, J. D. (2018). Chronic pain after traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology and pain mechanisms. Pain Med. 19, 1315–1333. doi:10.1093/pm/pnx153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanhoe, C. B., and Hartman, E. T. (2004). Clinical caveats on medical assessment and treatment of pain after TBI. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 19, 29–39. doi:10.1097/00001199-200401000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassubek, J., Juengling, F. D., Els, T., Spreer, J., Herpers, M., Krause, T., et al. (2003). Activation of a residual cortical network during painful stimulation in long-term postanoxic vegetative state: a 15O-H2O PET study. J. Neurol. Sci. 212, 85–91. doi:10.1016/s0022-510x(03)00106-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, F., Baguley, I. J., and Cameron, I. D. (2003). 4: rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury. Rehabil. Med. 178, 6. doi:10.5694/j.1326-5377.2003.tb05199.x

Google Scholar

Kirouac, G. J., Li, S., and Mabrouk, G. (2004). GABAergic projection from the ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra to the periaqueductal gray region and the dorsal raphe nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 469, 170–184. doi:10.1002/cne.11005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyle, B. N., and McNeil, D. W. (2014). Autonomic arousal and experimentally induced pain: a critical review of the literature. Pain Res. Manag. 19, 159–167. doi:10.1155/2014/536859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lance, J. W. (1980). “Pathophysiology of spasticity and clinical experience with Baclofen.” in Spasticity: disordered motor control. Editors J. W. Lance, R. G. Feldman, R. R. Young, and W. P. Koella. Chicago: Year Book, 185–204.

Google Scholar

Lane, R., Mcrae, K., Reiman, E., Chen, K., Ahern, G., and Thayer, J. (2009). Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage. 44, 213–222. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Celesia, G. G., Cohadon, F., Lavrijsen, J., León-Carrión, J., Sannita, W. G., et al. (2010). Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 8, 68. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-8-68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Faymonville, M.-E., Luxen, A., Lamy, M., Franck, G., and Maquet, P. (2000). Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from persistent vegetative state. Lancet. 355, 1790–1791. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(00)02271-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Faymonville, M. E., Peigneux, P., Damas, P., Lambermont, B., Del Fiore, G., et al. (2002). Cortical processing of noxious somatosensory stimuli in the persistent vegetative state. Neuroimage. 17, 732–741. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Perrin, F., Faymonville, M.-E., Schnakers, C., Boly, M., Bartsch, V., et al. (2004). Cerebral processing in the minimally conscious state. Neurology. 63, 916–918. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000137421.30792.9b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Perrin, F., Schnakers, C., Boly, M., and Majerus, S. (2005). Residual cognitive function in comatose, vegetative and minimally conscious states. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 18, 726–733. doi:10.1097/01.wco.0000189874.92362.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockwood, P. L., Iannetti, G. D., and Haggard, P. (2013). Transcranial magnetic stimulation over human secondary somatosensory cortex disrupts perception of pain intensity. Cortex. 49, 2201–2209. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeser, J. D., and Treede, R.-D. (2008). The Kyoto protocol of IASP basic pain terminology. Pain. 137, 473–477. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malan, T. P., Mata, H. P., and Porreca, F. (2002). Spinal GABAAand GABABReceptor Pharmacology in a rat model of neuropathic pain. Anesthesiology. 96, 1161–1167. doi:10.1097/00000542-200205000-00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malviya, S., Voepel‐Lewis, T., Burke, C., Merkel, S., and Tait, A. R. (2006). The revised FLACC observational pain tool: improved reliability and validity for pain assessment in children with cognitive impairment. Pediatric Anesthesia. 16, 258–265. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9592.2005.01773.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Margetis, K., Korfias, S. I., Gatzonis, S., Boutos, N., Stranjalis, G., Boviatsis, E., et al. (2014). Intrathecal baclofen associated with improvement of consciousness disorders in spasticity patients. Neuromodulation Technol. Neural Interface. 17, 699–704. doi:10.1111/ner.12147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markl, A., Yu, T., Vogel, D., Müller, F., Kotchoubey, B., and Lang, S. (2013). Brain processing of pain in patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome. Brain Behav. 3, 95–103. doi:10.1002/brb3.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCaffrey, M., and Pasero, C. (1999). Pain: clinical manual. St Louis, MO: Mosby.

Google Scholar

Medford, N., and Critchley, H. D. (2010). Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 535–549. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merkel, S., Voepel-Lewis, T., and Malviya, S. (2002). Pain assessment in infants and young children: the FLACC Scale: a behavioral tool to measure pain in young children. AJN Am. J. Nursing. 102, 55–58. doi:10.1097/00000446-200210000-00024 Available at: https://journals.lww.com/ajnonline/Citation/2002/10000/Pain_Assessment_in_Infants_and_Young_Children__The.24.aspx (Accessed September 28, 2020).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller-Smith, L., Finnsdóttir Wagner, Á., and Lantos, J. D. (2019). “The difficulty with determining whether someone is dead, finnsdóttir”, in Bioethics in the pediatric ICU: ethical dilemmas Encountered in the Care of critically ill children international library of ethics, law, and the new medicine. Editors L. Miller‐Smith, Á. Finnsdóttir Wagner, and J. D. Lantos (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 45–68. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-00943-4_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischkowski, D., Palacios-Barrios, E. E., Banker, L., Dildine, T. C., and Atlas, L. Y. (2018). Pain or nociception? Subjective experience mediates the effects of acute noxious heat on autonomic responses. Pain. 159, 699. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morton, D. L., Sandhu, J. S., and Jones, A. K. (2016). Brain imaging of pain: state of the art. J. Pain Res. 9, 613–624. doi:10.2147/JPR.S60433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moulton, E. A., Elman, I., Pendse, G., Schmahmann, J., Becerra, L., and Borsook, D. (2011). Aversion-related circuitry in the cerebellum: responses to noxious heat and unpleasant images. J. Neurosci. 31, 3795–3804. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6709-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munivenkatappa, A., and Agrawal, A. (2016). Role of thalamus in recovery of traumatic brain injury. J. Neurosci. Rural Pract. 7, S76–S79. doi:10.4103/0976-3147.196468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mura, E., Pistoia, F., Sara, M., Sacco, S., Carolei, A., and Govoni, S. (2013). Pharmacological modulation of the state of awareness in patients with Disorders of Consciousness: an Overview. CPD. 999, 5–6. doi:10.2174/13816128113196660658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutschler, I., Wankerl, J., Seifritz, E., and Ball, T. (2011). P02-405 - the role of the human insular cortex in pain processing. Eur. Psychiatr. 26, 1001. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(11)72706-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naro, A., Leo, A., Cannavò, A., Buda, A., Bramanti, P., and Calabrò, R. S. (2016). Do unresponsive wakefulness syndrome patients feel pain? Role of laser-evoked potential-induced gamma-band oscillations in detecting cortical pain processing. Neuroscience. 317, 141–148. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ofek, H., and Defrin, R. (2007). The characteristics of chronic central pain after traumatic brain injury. Pain. 131, 330–340. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olver, J. H., Ponsford, J. L., and Curran, C. A. (1996). Outcome following traumatic brain injury: a comparison between 2 and 5 years after injury. Brain Inj. 10, 841–848. doi:10.1080/026990596123945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payen, J.-F., Bru, O., Bosson, J.-L., Lagrasta, A., Novel, E., Deschaux, I., et al. (2001). Assessing pain in critically ill sedated patients by using a behavioral pain scale. Critical Care Medicine. Available at: https://journals.lww.com/ccmjournal/Abstract/2001/12000/Assessing_pain_in_critically_ill_sedated_patients.4.aspx (Accessed September 28, 2020).

Google Scholar

Pistoia, F., Sacco, S., Sarà, M., Franceschini, M., and Carolei, A. (2015). Intrathecal baclofen: effects on spasticity, pain, and consciousness in disorders of consciousness and locked-in syndrome. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 19, 466. doi:10.1007/s11916-014-0466-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploner, M., Gross, J., Timmermann, L., and Schnitzler, A. (2002). Cortical representation of first and second pain sensation in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 12444–12448. doi:10.1073/pnas.182272899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popernack, M. L., Gray, N., and Reuter-Rice, K. (2015). Moderate-to-Severe traumatic brain injury in children: complications and rehabilitation strategies. J. Pediatr. Health Care. 29, e1–e7. doi:10.1016/j.pedhc.2014.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes-Vazquez, C., Enna, S. J., and Dafny, N. (1986). The parafasciculus thalami as a site for mediating the antinociceptive response to GABAergic drugs. Brain Res. 383, 177–184. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)90018-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F. (2016). “Responsiveness and the autonomic control–CNS two-way interaction in disorders of consciousness,” in Brain Function and Responsiveness in Disorders of consciousness. Editors M. M. Monti, and W. G. Sannita (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 145–155. Available at: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-3-319-21425-2_11 (Accessed December 4, 2015).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Garbarino, S., and Sannita, W. G. (2012). Heart rate variability, homeostasis, and brain function: a tutorial and review of application. J. Psychophysiol. 26, 178–203. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Cortese, M. D., Arcuri, F., Candelieri, A., Guglielmino, F., Dolce, G., et al. (2014). A study of the reliability of the nociception coma scale. Clin. Rehabil. 29 (4), 388–393. doi:10.1177/0269215514546767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Macrì, S., Alleva, E., Petrini, C., Soddu, A., Leòn-Carriòn, J., et al. (2016). Pain perception in unresponsive wakefulness syndrome may challenge the interruption of artificial nutrition and hydration: neuroethics in action. Front. Neurol. 7, 202. doi:10.3389/fneur.2016.00202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Chatelle, C., Schnakers, C., and Laureys, S. (2018a). Heart Rate Variability as an indicator of nociceptive pain in disorders of consciousness?. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 57 (1), 47–56. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2018.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riganello, F., Larroque, S. K., Bahri, M. A., Heine, L., Martial, C., Carrière, M., et al. (2018b). A heartbeat away from consciousness: heart rate variability entropy can discriminate disorders of consciousness and is correlated with resting-state fMRI brain connectivity of the central autonomic network. Front. Neurol. 9, 769. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, E., Mehta, S., Shengelia, R., and Reid, M. C. (2013). Four strategies for managing opioid-induced side effects in older adults. Clin geriatr 21. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4418642/ (Accessed October 6, 2020).

Google Scholar

Rosenblum, A., Marsch, L. A., Joseph, H., and Portenoy, R. K. (2008). Opioids and the treatment of chronic pain: controversies, current status, and future directions. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 16, 405–416. doi:10.1037/a0013628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, S. J., and Nestler, E. J. (2013). The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 609–625. doi:10.1038/nrn3381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sa, S., Ke, M., and Sj, E. (2004). Relationship between the antinociceptive response to desipramine and changes in GABAB receptor function and subunit expression in the dorsal horn of the rat spinal cord. Biochem. Pharmacol. 67, 743–749. doi:10.1016/j.bcp.2003.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomons, T. V., Iannetti, G. D., Liang, M., and Wood, J. N. (2016). The “pain matrix” in pain-free individuals. JAMA Neurol. 73, 755–756. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.0653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sargeant, T. J., Miller, J. H., and Day, D. J. (2008). Opioidergic regulation of astroglial/neuronal proliferation: where are we now? J. Neurochem. 107, 883–897. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05671.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarno, J. E. (2001). The mindbody prescription: healing the body, healing the pain. Warner Books: Hachette UK.

Google Scholar

Sattin, D., Schnakers, C., Pagani, M., Arenare, F., Devalle, G., Giunco, F., et al. (2018). Evidence of altered pressure pain thresholds in persons with disorders of consciousness as measured by the Nociception Coma Scale–Italian version. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 28, 1295–1310. doi:10.1080/09602011.2017.1290532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sazbon, L., and Groswasser, Z. (1990). Outcome in 134 patients with prolonged posttraumatic unawareness. Part 1: parameters determining late recovery of consciousness. J. Neurosurg. 72, 75–80. doi:10.3171/jns.1990.72.1.0075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sazbon, L., and Groswasser, Z. (1991). Prolonged coma, vegetative state, post-comatose unawareness: semantics or better understanding? Brain Inj. 5, 1–2.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., and Johnstone, T. (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: theory, methods, research. Oxford University Press.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., Chatelle, C., Demertzi, A., Majerus, S., and Laureys, S. (2012). What about pain in disorders of consciousness? AAPS J. 14, 437–444. doi:10.1208/s12248-012-9346-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., Chatelle, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Majerus, S., Ledoux, D., Boly, M., et al. (2010). The Nociception Coma Scale: a new tool to assess nociception in disorders of consciousness. Pain. 148, 215–219. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J., Ventura, M., Boly, M., Majerus, S., et al. (2009). Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol. 9, 35. doi:10.1186/1471-2377-9-35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., and Zasler, N. (2015). Assessment and management of pain in patients with disorders of consciousness. PM&R. 7, S270–S277. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2015.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., and Zasler, N. D. (2007). Pain assessment and management in disorders of consciousness. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 20, 620–626. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e3282f169d9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seal, K. H., Bertenthal, D., Barnes, D. E., Byers, A. L., Gibson, C. J., Rife, T. L., et al. (2018). Traumatic brain injury and receipt of prescription opioid therapy for chronic pain in Iraq and Afghanistan veterans: do clinical practice guidelines matter? J. Pain. 19, 931–941. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2018.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seel, R. T., Sherer, M., Whyte, J., Katz, D. I., Giacino, J. T., Rosenbaum, A. M., et al. (2010). Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 91, 1795–1813. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2010.07.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seel, R. T., Douglas, J., Dennison, A. C., Heaner, S., Farris, K., and Rogers, C. (2013). Specialized early treatment for persons with disorders of consciousness: program components and outcomes. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 94, 1908–1923. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2012.11.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, K. B., Goldberg, M., and Bell, K. R. (2006). Traumatic brain injury and pain. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. 17, 473–490. doi:10.1016/j.pmr.2005.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 81–88. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibaut, F. A., Chatelle, C., Wannez, S., Deltombe, T., Stender, J., Schnakers, C., et al. (2015). Spasticity in disorders of consciousness: a behavioral study. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 51, 389–397.

PubMed AbstractGoogle Scholar

Tracey, I. (2008). Imaging pain. Br. J. Anaesth. 101, 32–39. doi:10.1093/bja/aen102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijk, M., Ceelie, I., and Tibboel, D. (2011). Endpoints in pediatric pain studies. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 67, 61–66. doi:10.1007/s00228-010-0947-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Erp, W. S., Lavrijsen, J. C. M., Vos, P. E., Bor, H., Laureys, S., and Koopmans, R. T. C. M. (2015). The vegetative state: prevalence, misdiagnosis, and treatment limitations. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 16, 85.e9–85.e14. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2014.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verplancke, D., Snape, S., Salisbury, C. F., Jones, P. W., and Ward, A. B. (2005). A randomized controlled trial of botulinum toxin on lower limb spasticity following acute acquired severe brain injury. Clin. Rehabil. 19, 117–125. doi:10.1191/0269215505cr827oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vink, P., Lucas, C., Maaskant, J. M., van Erp, W. S., Lindeboom, R., and Vermeulen, H. (2017). Clinimetric properties of the nociception coma scale (‐Revised): a systematic review. Eur. J. Pain. 21, 1463–1474. doi:10.1002/ejp.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warden, V., Hurley, A. C., and Volicer, L. (2003). Development and psychometric evaluation of the pain assessment in advanced dementia (PAINAD) scale. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 4, 9–15. doi:10.1097/01.jam.0000043422.31640.f7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf-Meyer, M. (2020). Neurological disorders, affective bioethics, and the nervous system: reconsidering the Schiavo case from a materialist perspective. Med. Humanit. 46, 166–175. doi:10.1136/medhum-2018-011568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, Q., Ni, X., Yu, R., Li, Y., and Huang, R. (2017). Chronic disorders of consciousness. Exp. Ther. Med. 14, 1277–1283. doi:10.3892/etm.2017.4639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: disorders of consciousness, pain, rehabilitation, assessment, pharmacotherapeutic approaches

Citation: Riganello F, Soddu A and Tonin P (2021) Addressing Pain for a Proper Rehabilitation Process in Patients With Severe Disorders of Consciousness. Front. Pharmacol. 12:628980. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2021.628980

Received: 13 November 2020; Accepted: 08 January 2021;
Published: 17 February 2021.

Edited by:

Adam Halberstadt, University of California, San Diego, United States

Reviewed by:

Robert L. Barkin, Rush University Medical Center, United States
Francesca G. Magnani, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologio Carlo Besta, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Riganello, Soddu and Tonin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: F. Riganello, f.riganello@istitutosantanna.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.