Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 October 2015
Sec. Psychological Therapy and Psychosomatics
This article is part of the Research Topic Endophenotypes for schizophrenia and mood disorders: implications from genetic, biochemical, cognitive, behavioral and neuroimaging studies View all 5 articles

Is schizotypy per se a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia? – do not forget to distinguish positive from negative facets

  • Biological Psychology and Individual Differences, Department of Psychology, Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Giessen, Germany

In 2003, Gottesman and Gould advocated using the endophenotype concept in psychiatry genetics; describing it as a measurable component “along the pathway between disease and distal genotype” (p. 636). Especially in schizophrenia research, the idea of endophenotypes has gathered a wide following. Meehl’s definition of schizotaxia [Ref. (1), p. 829], as “an ‘integrative neural defect’ as the only direct phenotypic consequence produced by the genetic mutation” and “This neural integrative defect […] is all that can properly be spoken of as inherited. The imposition of a social learning history upon schizotaxic individuals results in a personality organization which I shall call, following Rado, the schizotype” (p. 830; my emphasis) reminds clearly of the aforementioned endophenotype description.

Although there are a number of ongoing debates regarding the nature of schizotypy, its distribution throughout the population and its relation with schizophrenia, a few elements may be considered “agreed upon”: (1) schizotypy is a latent trait (partly) harboring individual risk for schizophrenia (2). (2) Schizotypy is (partly) influenced by genes considered relevant for schizophrenia; q.v. (3). (3) Schizotypic variance is best explained through gene–environment interactions of a kind also relevant for schizophrenia (4). (4) Schizotypy shares a factor-structure with symptoms of schizophrenia; with the most commonly replicated factors being the positive, negative, and disorganized facets (5, 6). It therefore appears that schizotypy and schizophrenia qualitatively share various similarities, albeit in a quantitatively different fashion.

It is not surprising that a number of measures considered endophenotypes of schizophrenia [q.v. (7)] have also been shown to be associated with schizotypy (8). The question at hand (whether schizotypy per se may qualify as a suitable endophenotype for schizophrenia) has, to my knowledge, not been answered as yet. I have briefly argued thusly elsewhere (9) and will, here, further elaborate on this and continue discussing the implications of the debated link between schizophrenia-genetics and schizotypy in an attempt to provide a suitable answer to this question.

First, it is necessary to distinguish two pairs of entities: on the one hand, although often used pseudo-synonymously, schizophrenia and psychosis are not the same. Schizophrenia is a psychotic disorder; featuring psychosis as a primary phenomenon, but being defined furthermore by having Krankheitswert. It has been shown repeatedly that (extremely) high schizotypic features and/or psychotic experiences are not limited to the clinical domain and, contrapositively, that psychotic experiences are found with considerably higher prevalence within the population [between 30 and 70% (10, 11)] than possible, if existing only in patient groups. Furthermore, it has been convincingly argued [review in Ref. (12)] that individuals, christened “happy schizotypes” (13), with considerably high positive schizotypy and often repeated psychotic experiences but significantly lower negative/disorganized schizotypic features (compared to the population!) are not only “not sick” but actually benefit from these experiences and are “more healthy.”

The second necessary distinction is between positive and negative/disorganized schizotypy: it is often stated that “negative schizotypy is the most heritable,” although this is better amended to “most heritable in biological relatives of schizophrenics” [q.v. (14)]. In the population, heritability estimates do not differ between positive and negative schizotypy, and the data suggest that the two facets are influenced genetically by two distinct latent genetic factors (15). It would, thus, seem that while positive schizotypy is the underlying dimension explaining psychotic features, it is not necessarily related to Krankheitswert. Negative schizotypy, however, appears closely related to schizophrenia regarding its heritability as well as a major factor that differentiates “happy schizotypes” from schizophrenic patients.

Returning to endophenotypes, Gottesman and Gould (16) clearly state that configured self-report data may also constitute an endophenotype. They propose five criteria that qualify an endophenotype. I shall aim to show that schizotypy meets these criteria:

(1) Schizotypy is quite clearly associated with the clinical condition of schizophrenia. Apart from aforementioned similarities, patients with schizophrenia have repeatedly shown to have higher values in schizotypy (17), and schizotypy is elevated more highly in schizophrenic patients than in those with other psychotic disorders (18).

(2) Schizotypy is heritable (~50%) (15); although there are differences between individual schizotypy facets; with negative schizotypy being most highly heritable in patients’ relatives (14).

(3) Schizotypy as a stable trait with high re-test reliability does not solely manifest during acute phases of illness [9, 19]. There are, however, intraindividual variations in acute schizotypal sensitivities, which are explained (mostly) by environmental influences, whereby interindividual differences in susceptibility to environmental influences is again a major factor of trait schizotypy (4, 19, 20). This is unsurprising when dealing with complex traits and strengthens the case for (state) schizotypy.

(4) Schizotypy co-segregates with schizophrenia; i.e., schizophrenic relatives of schizophrenic patients have higher values in (especially negative) schizotypy than healthy relatives of schizophrenic patients (q.v., criterion 1).

(5) Healthy biological relatives (especially offspring) of schizophrenic patients have repeatedly been shown to have higher values in (especially negative) schizotypy (14), and offspring of healthy but highly schizotypic individuals have greater risk of developing schizophrenia (21, 22).

It should be emphasized that especially heritability, co-segregation, and familial association are key points that differentiate between “true” endophenotypes and “mere” biomarkers or intermediate phenotypes [e.g., Ref. (23)]. The strong associations found between schizotypy and schizophrenia regarding the respective criteria 2, 4, and 5, thus, make a strong case toward the acceptance of psychometric schizotypy as an endophenotype of schizophrenia.

There are, however, a number of findings that need further discussion before a final conclusion may be drawn. (1) A number of genetic factors associated with schizotypy are no longer considered relevant in schizophrenia genetics. (2) A number of genetic factors associated with schizophrenia are not (or even inversely) associated with schizotypy.

To address these points, it is necessary to keep the aforementioned distinctions “psychosis vs. schizophrenia” and “positive vs. negative/disorganized schizotypy” in mind. In other words, it is expected that all schizophrenics are highly schizotypic, but not all highly schizotypic individuals are schizophrenic; both parts of this expectation hold true [q.v. (13)].

This latter observation, referred to by Claridge (24) as benign schizotypy, is an important point to consider with respect to the aforementioned two “critical points.” There is currently no general consensus regarding the exact nature of schizotypy in the entire population or the relation with schizophrenia genes. The Meehlian model proposes the group of schizotypes to be discrete, making up 10% of the population and defined qua the presence of (few) specific genetic risk factors. The more common view in schizotypy research, however, is that schizotypy is continuously distributed throughout the population and is genetically based on a multitude of genetic loci, whereof each have only small effect sizes, but these effects interact (9) and are additive (25); similar to recent conceptions of schizophrenia genetics (26, 27). This would give rise to an Eysenckian view (28) that schizophrenia be equal to extremely high schizotypy (or rather psychoticism in the Eysenckian model of personality) [q.v. (29)]. This view is, however, incompatible with the existence of benign schizotypy or happy schizotypes. I have, thus, suggested and discussed in more detail (3, 4) that the fully dimensional model of schizotypy (24) has the best fit regarding more recently shown genetic associations of both schizotypy and schizophrenia.

This model explains the discrepancies mentioned above in my “critical points”: it proposes that schizotypy is normally distributed in the population, but interacts with a second dimension; i.e., health or resilience. High schizotypy is in and of itself, thus, not equal to schizophrenia; when coinciding with low health/resilience, however, schizophrenia is the outcome. Schizophrenia may therefore be viewed upon as resulting from low resilience to the high [probably dopaminergic; q.v. (30)] strain on the nervous system conferred by (positive) schizotypy. The lack of association between individual schizotypy-related genetic variants and schizophrenia (e.g., the COMT-polymorphism rs4680) is therefore not surprising and does not disqualify schizotypy as an endophenotype. This is quite simply due to the fact that small effects of single schizotypy-related polymorphisms may disappear in case–control schizophrenia studies; under the assumption of the fully dimensional model.

The second critical point is also explained easily when assuming this model. Those genetic loci most strongly associated with the distinction between cases and controls cannot be associated with high schizotypy within healthy individuals. In fact, and this has been shown [e.g., Ref. (31, 32)], they must be inversely related to schizotypy or related measures (like psychosis-like experiences). In other words, should a person high in (especially positive) schizotypy additionally carry substantial numbers of alleles that distinguish between cases and controls (and, thus, probably load on the aforementioned dimension resilience), this person would not be found in a healthy sample. Furthermore, increasingly high levels of schizotypy must, in healthy individuals, coincide with increasingly lower genetic factors conveying risk for schizophrenia (i.e., with higher resilience).

To summarize and in conclusion, at first glance, schizotypy meets all the criteria as proposed by Gottesman and Gould (16) for endophenotypes in psychiatry. One could, thus, stop here and consider schizotypy in general a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia. This might be jumping the gun, however, as, upon closer examination, there are a number of discrepancies between schizotypy and schizophrenia; especially related to genetics and the distinction between positive and negative/disorganized traits. I would, therefore, suggest (sensu 30) that (high) positive schizotypy be better described as an endophenotype of psychosis-in-schizophrenia (as well as in other psychiatric conditions or the otherwise healthy), while (high) negative schizotypy may be viewed upon as an endophenotype of schizophrenia-in-psychosis (i.e., tapping more into Krankheitswert). The latter is important, as (high) negative schizotypy is not exclusive to psychotic disorders, but also found to share considerable variance with another personality trait linked to other (e.g., affective or anxiety) disorders; i.e., Neuroticism (33). Schizophrenia would, thus, be considered as the overlap of (very) high positive and negative schizotypy. This interaction between different aspects of personality (and inherent cognitive patterns) regarding the development of schizophrenia has been clearly shown by Gaweda and Prochwicz (34): while a cognitive bias typically associated with positive schizotypy (jumping to conclusions) did not differ between highly delusion-prone healthy controls and delusional schizophrenics, another bias strongly associated with negative schizotypy (catastrophizing) clearly differentiated both groups of patients (schizophrenics with and without delusions) from both groups of healthy controls (high and low delusion-proneness).

This makes schizotypy no less useful in schizophrenia research, however. I would state that it actually strengthens the case made by schizotypy researchers when saying that it is a useful and “the most influential, comprehensive psychological construct in schizophrenia research” [(35); p. S363; my emphasis]. This is quite simply due to the fact that in schizophrenia genetics it is not clear with what the identified genes are actually associated (proneness for psychosis or risk for schizophrenia; i.e., illness), whereas this is not the case in schizotypy. In other words, accepting discrete facets of schizotypy per se as suitable endophenotypes of psychosis-in-schizophrenia vs. schizophrenia-in-psychosis may not only help researching the (biological) basis of psychosis in eneral (including, of course, psychosis-in-schizophrenia), but also help us examine why certain (highly schizotypic) individuals develop schizophrenia while others benefit from their perceived “oddness.” Schizotypy research is, in my opinion, better suited for both aforementioned aspects of schizophrenia research than simple case–control designs both due to the possibility of differentiation between individual schizotypic facets and due to schizotypy’s naturally dimensional nature compared to the (artificial) dichotomy in the comparison between schizophrenic patients and “healthy” controls.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am Psychol (1962) 17:827–38. doi: 10.1037/h0041029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lenzenweger MF. Schizotypy – an organizing framework for schizophrenia research. Curr Dir Psychol (2006) 15:162–6. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Grant P. Genetic associations: the basis of schizotypy. In: Mason O, Claridge G, editors. Schizotypy – New Dimensions. London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group (2015).

Google Scholar

4. Barrantes-Vidal N, Grant P, Kwapil TR. The role of schizotypy in the Study of the Etiology of Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41:S408–16. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Vollema MG, van den Bosch RJ. The multidimensionality of schizotypy. Schizophr Bull (1995) 21:19–31. doi:10.1093/schbul/21.1.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N, Silvia PJ. The dimensional structure of the Wisconsin schizotypy scales: factor identification and construct validity. Schizophr Bull (2008) 34:444–57. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Allen AJ, Griss ME, Folley BS, Hawkins KA, Pearlson GD. Endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res (2009) 109:24–37. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ettinger U, Mohr C, Gooding DC, Cohen AS, Rapp A, Haenschel C, et al. Cognition and brain function in schizotypy: a selective review. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41:S417–26. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Grant P, Kuepper Y, Mueller E, Wielpuetz C, Mason O, Hennig J. Dopaminergic foundations of schizotypy as measured by the German version of the oxford-liverpool inventory of feelings and experiences (O-LIFE) – a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:1. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Jackson M. Bening schizotypy? The case of spiritual experience. In: Claridge G, editor. Schizotypy – Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1997). p. 227–50.

Google Scholar

11. Hay D. Exploring Inner Space, Penguin. Harmondsworth: Penguin (1987).

Google Scholar

12. Mohr C, Claridge G. Schizotypy-do not worry, it is not all worrisome. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41:S436–43. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. McCreery C. Hallucinations and arousability: pointers to a theory of psychosis. In: Claridge G, editor. Schizotypy – Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1997). p. 251–73.

Google Scholar

14. Tarbox SI, Pogue-Geile MF. A multivariate perspective on schizotypy and familial association with schizophrenia: a review. Clin Psychol Rev (2011) 31:1169–82. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER, MacDonald AM, Rijsdijk F, Sham PC. A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits. Psychol Med (2003) 33:803–16. doi:10.1017/S0033291703007906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160:636–45. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hodgekins J, Coker S, Freeman D, Ray-Glover K, Bebbington P, Garety P, et al. Assessing levels of subthreshold psychotic symptoms in the recovery phase: the schizotypal symptoms inventory (SSI). J Exp Psychopathol (2012) 3: 582–93. doi:10.5127/jep.021211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Heron J, Jones I, Williams J, Owen MJ, Craddock N, Jones LA. Self-reported schizotypy and bipolar disorder: demonstration of a lack of specificity of the kings schizotypy questionnaire. Schizophr Res (2003) 65:153–8. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00004-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mason O, Claridge G. The oxford-liverpool inventory of feelings and experiences (O-LIFE): further description and extended norms. Schizophr Res (2006) 82:203–11. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.12.845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Barrantes-Vidal N, Chun CA, Myin-Germeys I, Kwapil TR. Psychometric schizotypy predicts psychotic-like, paranoid, and negative symptoms in daily life. J Abnorm Psychol (2013) 122:1077–87. doi:10.1037/a0034793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Battaglia M, Gasperini M, Sciuto G, Scherillo P, Diaferia G, Bellodi L. Psychiatric disorders in the families of schizotypal subjects. Schizophr Bull (1991) 17:659–68. doi:10.1093/schbul/17.4.659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kendler KS, Walsh D. Schizotypal personality disorder in parents and the risk for schizophrenia in siblings. Schizophr Bull (1995) 21:47–52. doi:10.1093/schbul/21.1.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Glahn DC, Blangero J. Why endophenotype development requires families. Chin Sci Bull (2011) 56:3382–4. doi:10.1007/s11434-011-4740-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Claridge G. Theoretical background and issues. In: Claridge G, editor. Schizotypy – Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1997). p. 3–18.

Google Scholar

25. Grant P, Munk AJL, Kuepper Y, Wielpuetz C, Hennig J. Additive genetic effects for schizotypy support a fully-dimensional model of psychosis-proneness. J Individ Differ (2015) 36:87–92. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, Moran JL, Kähler AK, Akterin S, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet (2013) 45:1150–9. doi:10.1038/ng.2742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature (2014) 511:421–7. doi:10.1038/nature13595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Eysenck HJ. The definition and measurement of psychoticism. Pers Individ Dif (1992) 13:757–85. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90050-Y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Plomin R, Haworth CM, Davis OS. Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nat Rev Genet (2009) 10:872–8. doi:10.1038/nrg2670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Howes OD, Kapur S. The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III – the final common pathway. Schizophr Bull (2009) 35:549–62. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zammit S, Hamshere M, Dwyer S, Georgiva L, Timpson N, Moskvina V, et al. A population-based study of genetic variation and psychotic experiences in adolescents. Schizophr Bull (2014) 40(6):1254–62. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hatzimanolis, et al. Personal communication from Nikolaos Smyrnis. (Forthcoming).

Google Scholar

33. Macare C, Bates CB, Heath AC, Martin NG, Ettinger U. Substantial genetic overlap between schizotypy and neuroticism: a Twin Study. Behav Genet (2012) 42:732–42. doi:10.1007/s10519-012-9558-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gaweda L, Prochwicz K. A comparison of cognitive biases between schizophrenia patients with delusions and healthy controls with delusion-like experiences. Eur Psychiatry (2015) 30:943–9.

Google Scholar

35. Debbane M, Mohr C. Integration and development in schizotypy research: an introduction to the special supplement. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41:S363–5. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizotypy, psychosis, schizophrenia, endophenotypes, psychosis proneness, genetic predisposition to disease

Citation: Grant P (2015) Is schizotypy per se a suitable endophenotype of schizophrenia? – do not forget to distinguish positive from negative facets. Front. Psychiatry 6:143. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00143

Received: 11 August 2015; Accepted: 22 September 2015;
Published: 23 October 2015

Edited by:

Blazej Misiak, Wroclaw Medical University, Poland

Reviewed by:

Lukasz Gaweda, Medical University of Warsaw, Poland

Copyright: © 2015 Grant. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Phillip Grant, phillip.grant@psychol.uni-giessen.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.