Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 22 January 2019
Sec. Psychopathology

Decision-Making Deficits Are Associated With Learning Impairments in Female College Students at High Risk for Anorexia Nervosa: Iowa Gambling Task and Prospect Valence Learning Model

  • 1Department of Psychology, Sungshin Women's University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Jakwang Child & Family Clinic, Seoul, South Korea

This study investigated deficits in decision-making ability in female college students at high risk for anorexia nervosa (AN) using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) and the prospect valence learning (PVL) model. Based on scores on the Korean version of the Eating Attitude Test-26 (KEAT-26), participants were assigned to either the high risk for AN group (n = 42) or the control group (n = 43). The high risk for AN group exhibited significantly lower total net scores and block net scores on the third, fourth, and fifth blocks of the IGT than the control group did. The high risk for AN group selected cards significantly more often from the disadvantageous A and B decks and less often from the advantageous D deck than the control group did. In addition, the block net scores of the high risk for AN group did not differ across the five blocks, whereas those of the control group increased as the trials progressed. There was a significant negative correlation between IGT total net score and total score on the KEAT−26. The high risk for AN group had significantly lower values than the control group on the learning and response consistency parameters of the PVL model. These results indicate that female college students at high risk for AN have deficits in decision-making ability, and that these deficits are related to difficulties in remembering experience obtained from earlier trials and applying it to later trials. These difficulties further lead them to make decisions randomly.

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is characterized by distorted body image and an intense fear of gaining weight despite low actual body weight (1). Patients with AN restrict their calorie intake, exercise excessively to maintain or reduce their weight, and have rigid and stereotyped thinking or behavior regarding food and body shape (2, 3). AN has a poor prognosis (4) and the highest mortality rate among mental disorders (5). The etiologies of AN are not fully understood and evidence-based treatments for this disorder are lacking (6).

Patients with AN exhibit deficits in several cognitive domains, such as attention (7), visuospatial construction/visual memory (8, 9), and executive function (10). Deficits in decision-making ability are particularly pronounced, as patients with AN tend to prefer immediate rewards even if they incur in long-term negative consequences (11). Decision-making deficits have been found to predict the prognosis of AN (12). Additionally, poor decision-making ability is observed not only in patients with AN but also in their healthy relatives, and can therefore be regarded as a biological marker or endophenotype of AN (13).

The Iowa gambling task [IGT, (14)] is widely used to measure decision-making ability. The IGT requires selecting one deck from among four decks presented to the subject. Two decks (A and B) are disadvantageous, delivering more losses than gains, whereas the two other decks (C and D) are advantageous, delivering more gains than losses. To perform the IGT successfully, individuals should learn the expectancy values of each deck (15). Decision-making abilities are evaluated by total net score and block net scores. As trials progress, normal individuals begin to select advantageous decks more often than disadvantageous ones (16, 17).

Studies using the IGT to investigate decision-making in patients with AN have reported that these patients exhibit significantly lower total net scores and block net scores (except on the first or second block) than healthy controls (11, 18). These results indicate that patients with AN are unable to distinguish between advantageous and disadvantageous decks even as trials progress and are likely to pursue immediate gains even at the cost of greater long-term losses (1921). Evidence from neuroimaging research gives context to the impaired IGT performance seen in patients with AN. The ventromedial prefrontal cortex, including the orbitofrontal cortex, is involved in performance on the IGT (22). Patients with AN exhibit decreased orbitofrontal cortex volume, and greater reductions in volume correspond to lower IGT total net scores in these patients (20).

However, total net scores and block net scores on the IGT cannot provide a complete explanation of the mechanisms underlying decision-making deficits. Decision-making is a complex process consisting of several components, such as the formation of preference about possible choices, selection/execution of behavior, and evaluation of the behavior's probable outcomes (23). Several cognitive models have been developed to identify the mechanisms of processes involved in IGT performance. One of these models, the expectancy valence learning (EVL) model, assumes that three processes, namely motivation, memory/learning, and response consistency, are involved in performance on the IGT (24).

The EVL model suggests that deck selection in each trial of the IGT is made based on the expectation of valence, such as a positive or negative feeling for a certain deck or an implicit association between a certain deck and outcomes of gains/losses. This process is called the motivational parameter (25). Expectation of valence is also formed by memory or learning. For example, participants who have deficits of memory or learning cannot use information about gains/losses obtained from earlier trials for selecting decks in later trials. Therefore, the learning parameter reflects how well participants remember experience obtained from earlier trials and apply it to deck selection in later trials. Finally, the response consistency parameter reflects whether participants consistently select decks based on the expectation of valence for each deck or whether they select decks at random (25).

Ahn et al. (26) modified the EVL model and proposed the prospect valence learning (PVL) model. The PVL model separates the EVL model's motivational parameter into feedback sensitivity and loss aversion parameters. The PVL model thus analyzes IGT performance according to four parameters: feedback sensitivity, loss aversion, learning, and response consistency (25). Ahn et al. (26) suggested that the PVL model is better than the EVL model in analyzing IGT performance. For example, they suggested that the PVL model accounts for the gain-loss frequency effects on the formation of expectancy for each deck, which the EVL model cannot explain, since the PVL model uses a non-linear utility function instead of the linear function used by the EVL model (26).

The feedback sensitivity parameter reflects the non-linear relationship between the actual quantities of gains/losses and the prospect valence. A higher feedback sensitivity score means that the participant's subjective evaluation about outcomes of deck selection corresponds more closely to the actual quantities of gains/losses. The loss aversion parameter evaluates the tendency to avoid losses relative to the tendency to seek gains, while the learning parameter measures the formation or modification of preference for each deck based on recent experiences with a particular outcome. Finally, the response consistency parameter reflects the consistency of choice behavior.

The PVL model has been used to analyze the IGT performance in several clinical groups including people with schizophrenia (27) and substance abuse problems (28). It has proven to be a useful tool for revealing the mechanisms underlying decision-making deficits in these clinical groups. For example, Chan et al. (25) compared the IGT performances of patients with AN or bulimia nervosa with those of healthy controls using the PVL model. They found that both the AN and bulimia nervosa groups exhibited significantly impaired performance on the IGT relative to healthy controls. In addition, impaired performance on the IGT was related to memory deficits and sensitivity to gains in patients with AN and bulimia nervosa, respectively.

The investigation of cognitive functions in patients with AN may be affected by several factors such as symptom severity, duration of illness, or comorbidity (21, 29). One way to control these factors is by evaluating individuals at high risk for AN (30), since AN symptoms such as distorted body image (31) and deficits in set-shifting (30) frequently observed in patients with AN are also observed in these individuals. Investigation of high risk group for AN would provide valuable information about the risk factors and prevention programs for AN (32).

To this end, in this study we used the PVL model to analyze IGT results, to investigate decision-making abilities in female college students at high risk for AN. The objectives of the study were to investigate whether female college students at high risk for AN exhibited deficits in decision-making and, if so, to identify the specific decision-making processes underlying impaired IGT performance in these individuals.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We administered the Korean version of the Eating Attitude Test-26 (KEAT-26, 33, 34) to 652 female college students. Students who obtained total scores above 22 on the KEAT-26 were included in the high risk for AN group (n = 42) and those who obtained average scores were included in the control group (n = 43). Garner et al. (33) administered the EAT-26 to AN patients and female college students, and found that total scores above 20 predict the development of AN. Rhee et al. (34) administered the KEAT-26 to Korean women aged over 18 years, and suggested that total scores of above 22 reliably indicate a high-risk of development of AN.

To ensure that none of participants had histories of neurological disorders, mental disorders, or alcohol/drug abuse or addiction, we administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-Non Patient [SCID-NP, (35)]. The Self-Rating Depression Scale [SDS, (36)] and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [STAI, (37)] were administered to evaluate depression and anxiety, respectively. Finally, we also administered the Korean version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV [K-WAIS-IV, (38)]. All participants provided their written informed consent after receiving a description of the study. The students were paid for their participation, and this study was approved by the Sungshin Women's University Institutional Review Board (SSWUIRB 2017-069).

Eating Attitude Test-26 (EAT-26)

Although the EAT-26 (33) was developed to evaluate characteristics of behavior and attitude in patients with AN, it is also useful for the evaluation of eating behaviors in the general population (33). The EAT-26 consists of 11 items concerning anorexia, seven items concerning binging or overeating, and eight items concerning control of eating behavior. Each item is rated on a 6-level likert scale. In this study, we used the Korean version of the Eating Attitude Test-26 [KEAT-26, (34)].

Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)

The computerized IGT (39) was administered to evaluate decision-making. In this task, four decks are presented on a computer monitor. When a deck is selected, numbers representing gains and losses are presented on the upper part of the monitor. Gains occur whenever a deck is selected, but the losses occur in certain ratios. The quantities and frequencies of gain/loss are different for each deck (Table 1). Decks A and B deliver large immediate gains but more losses than gains in the long run, whereas decks C and D deliver small immediate gains but more gains than losses overall. No instruction about which decks are advantageous or disadvantageous is given to participants. Instead they are instructed to gain as much as possible before the completion of the task.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Magnitude and frequency of gain and loss in each block of IGT.

The IGT consists of 120 trials, including 20 practice trials, from which the total net score and block net scores are calculated. The total net score is calculated by subtracting the frequency with which disadvantageous decks are selected from the frequency of selecting advantageous decks [(C+D)–(A+B)]. The 100 experimental trials are divided into five blocks of 20 trials each, and the block net scores are calculated for each block in the same way as the total net score.

Prospect Valence Learning (PVL) Model

The PVL model suggests that deck selection in each trial of the IGT is based on expectancy valence, which is formed by the magnitude of gains/losses, loss aversion and feedback sensitivity.

u(t)={x(t)a           if x(t)0λ|x(t)|a       if x(t)<0.

This equation explains how a participant's subjective expectancy valence [u(t)] is formed, and x(t) means the net gain on the tth trial, i.e., sum of subtracting losses from gains on the tth trial. x(t)a determines the feedback sensitivity, i.e., the non-linear relation between actual net gain and expectancy valence, and has 0–1 value. If the value approaches 1, individual's expectancy valence is affected sensitively by the changes of actual amount of gains/losses, whereas if the value approaches 0, the expectancy valence is not affected by actual amount of gains/losses. When the net gain is below 0, motivation to avoid the losses occurs, and this motivation also affects the formation of a participant's subjective expectancy valence along with feedback sensitivity. In the above equation, λ is the loss aversion parameter, i.e., tendency to respond sensitively to losses relative to gains, which has 0–5 values. If λ = 0, participants do not consider the loss at all in the formation of expectancy valence, whereas if λ = 1 participants consider the losses and gains equally. If λ > 1, participants tend to focus more on the losses than gains in the formation of expectancy valence.

PVL model suggests that expectancy valence is also affected by learning, i.e., experience of gains/losses on earlier trials, and the learning parameter is calculated by the following equation.

Ej(t)=A·Ej(t-1)+δj(t)·u(t)

In the equation, learning parameter A determines how the earlier expectancy valence on the j card [Ej(t − 1)] is considered in the formation of expectancy valence on the j card on current trial. δj(t) is a pacifier variable and coded 1 if j card is selected on tth trial, whereas 0 if not selected. The learning parameter (0 < A < 1) indexes the learning rate, and the value approaches 1, the expectancy valence formed from earlier experience affects the deck selection on current trial.

The response consistency parameter of the PVL model is calculated by the following equation.

Pr[D(t+1)=j]=eθ(t)·Ej(t)k=14eθ(t)·Ek(t)

On the IGT performance, participants form the expectancy valence for each deck through the evaluation of decks, and during earlier trials they explore the decks to assure the expectancy valence for each deck. Later they consistently select the decks having high expectancy valence (26). In the equation, Pr[D(t + 1) = j] reflects the probability of choosing j card on other trials, and θ(t) reflects the degree of selecting decks based on the expectancy valence. Since the PVL model applies a trial-dependent choice rule, [θ(t) = 3c-1], θ increases or decreases as trials progress. The response consistency parameter (c) has 0–5 values, high values reflect consistent deck selection based on expectancy, whereas low values reflect random deck selection.

Statistical Analysis

The demographic characteristics of the high risk for AN and control groups were analyzed using independent t-tests. Total net IGT scores were analyzed using univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with SDS and STAI as covariates. Block net scores were analyzed with a mixed-design ANCOVA with block as a within-subject factor, group as a between–subject factor, and SDS and STAI as covariates. The relationships between performance on the IGT and severity of AN symptoms were analyzed using the Pearson product-moment correlation.

A Markov chain Monte Carlo sampling scheme in OpenBugs and BRugs [which provides an R interface for OpenBugs, (40)] was used to estimate PVL parameters. After a 500-sample burn-in with three chains, 1,000 samples were drawn and the estimated parameters were analyzed with a Mann–Whitney U-test. Relations between PVL parameters and severity of AN symptoms and between PVL parameters and IGT performance were analyzed by bootstrapped Pearson product correlation.

Results

Demographic Characteristics

The high risk for AN and control groups did not differ in terms of age, t(83) = 1.61, p = 0.112, educational level, t(83) = 0.52, p = 0.603, body mass index (BMI, kg/m2), t(83) = −0.88, p = 0.381, or IQ, t(83) = −0.90, p = 0.370. However, the two groups differed significantly in SDS, t(83) = 4.68, p < 0.001, state anxiety as measured by STAI, t(83) = 2.18, p < 0.05, trait anxiety as measured by STAI, t(83) = 5.68, p < 0.001, and KEAT-26 score, t(83) = 21.78, p < 0.001. The high risk for AN group exhibited significantly higher scores on these scales than the control group did. The mean scores of demographic characteristics, depression, anxiety, and eating disorder tests for the high risk for AN and control groups are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic characteristics of high risk for anorexia nervosa (AN) and control groups.

IGT Performance

The total net scores of the high risk for AN and control groups differed significantly, F(1, 80) = 9.24, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.104, with the high risk for AN group exhibiting significantly lower total net scores than the control group. We observed an interaction effect of group X block on block net scores, F(4, 320) = 2.69, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.033. The block net scores of the two groups for each block and the performance of each group over the five blocks were analyzed with univariate ANCOVA and repeated–measures ANOVA, respectively. Bonferroni corrections were used to reduce type 1 errors. The high risk for AN group exhibited significantly lower block net scores than the control group in the third block, F(1, 80) = 6.05, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.070, fourth block, F(1, 80) = 7.67, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.088, and fifth block, F(1, 80) = 9.54, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.107. However, the two groups did not differ in the first, F(1, 80) = 1.36, p = 0.247, ηp2 = 0.017, or second block, F(1, 80) = 1.35, p = 0.248, ηp2 = 0.017. In addition, the block net scores of the high risk for AN group did not differ across the five blocks, F(4, 164) = 1.64, p = 0.176, ηp2 = 0.038, whereas those of the control group increased as the blocks progressed, F(4, 168) = 12.26, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.226. The mean total net scores and block net scores of the high risk for AN and control groups are presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The mean total net scores and block net scores of the IGT in high risk for anorexia nervosa (AN) and control groups.

Analysis of deck selection showed that the high risk for AN group selected A, t(83) = 2.71, p < 0.01, and B decks, t(83) = 2.51, p < 0.05, more frequently than did the control group, and D deck, t(83) = −3.08, p < 0.01, less frequently than did the control group. The mean frequencies of deck selection by the two groups are presented in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mean numbers of deck selection of the IGT in high risk for anorexia nervosa (AN) and control groups.

Correlations Between IGT Performance and Severity of AN

There was a significant negative correlation between IGT total net score and KEAT-26 score, r(42) = −0.24, p < 0.05, with subjects in the high risk for AN group who displayed more AN symptoms receiving lower IGT total net scores. However, this correlation was not observed in the control group.

PVL Model Parameters

The high risk for AN group received significantly lower scores than the control group with regard to the learning (U = 579.00, p < 0.01) and response consistency (U = 633.00, p < 0.05) parameters. The two groups did not differ on the feedback sensitivity (U = 808.00, p = 0.404) and loss aversion (U = 738.50, p = 0.148) parameters.

There were significant negative correlations between IGT total net scores and the learning, r(42) = −0.36, p < 0.05 and response consistency parameters, r(42) = −0.35, p < 0.05 with individuals in the high risk for AN group, whereas there were significant positive correlations between IGT total net scores and the learning, r(43) = 0.53, p < 0.001, and response consistency parameters, r(43) = 0.35, p < 0.05, in the control group. There were no significant correlations between PVL model parameters and severity of AN symptoms. The mean values of the PVL model parameters for the high risk for AN and control groups are presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The mean values of PVL model parameters of high risk for anorexia nervosa (AN) and control.

Discussion

This study investigated deficits of decision-making in female college students at high risk for AN using the IGT and PVL model. Analysis of demographic characteristics showed that the high risk for AN group exhibited significantly higher levels of depression and anxiety than did the control group. These results are consistent with those of previous studies that found depression and anxiety disorders to be comorbid with AN (41, 42).

The total net IGT scores of the high risk for AN group were significantly lower than those of the control group, indicating that individuals at high risk for AN experience deficits in decision-making ability. Although these results are consistent with the findings of some studies (13, 21), other studies have produced contradictory results (43, 44). For example, Guillaume et al. (44) did not find significant differences in IGT performance between patients with AN and healthy controls after controlling for depression and medication, leading them to conclude that variables such as depression or medication contributed to impaired IGT performance in patients with AN. However, the present results, i.e., the finding that IGT performance is impaired in individuals at high risk for AN after controlling for depression and anxiety, indicate that the deficits in decision-making are trait characteristics which are not affected by depression or anxiety, and preexist the development of AN.

The high risk for AN group exhibited significantly lower block net scores in the third, fourth and fifth block of the IGT than the control group did. The controls learned which decks were advantageous or disadvantageous and selected advantageous decks more frequently than disadvantageous ones as the trials progressed, whereas this learning did not occur in the high risk for AN group. To perform the IGT successfully, participants must learn the contingencies of gains/losses for each deck. Bechara et al. (45) stated that healthy participants switch from one deck to another through trial and error at the outset, but that as trials progress (after about 50 trials, in the third block of the IGT) participants begin to recognize that decks A and B are disadvantageous and their preference becomes biased toward decks with higher net gain. In other words, healthy participants develop a preference for advantageous decks. The present results showed that this development of preference does not occur in the high risk for AN group. These results suggest that the high risk for AN group may have limited behavioral or mental flexibility, since difficulties in set-shifting are frequently observed in patients with AN (21) or individuals at high risk for AN (30), and significant associations between performances on the IGT and the reversal-learning task are observed (46).

The high risk for AN group selected cards from decks A and B more frequently and from deck D less frequently than did the control group. Decks A and B deliver larger gains but also larger losses than decks C and D (see Table 1), leading to lower net gains in the long run. The high frequency with which the high risk for AN group in this study selected decks A and B indicates that these individuals prefer immediate large gains despite greater losses in the long run. Additionally, the high risk for AN group selected deck D significantly less frequently than the control group did, while the two groups did not differ in the frequency with which they selected deck C. Although decks C and D are both advantageous, deck D delivers larger (–$250 vs. –$50) but less frequent (10 vs. 50%) losses than does deck C. Healthy participants tend to select the deck that delivers less frequent losses (47). However, individuals in the high risk for AN group avoided larger losses and were unable to simultaneously consider both the magnitude and frequency of gains/losses when selecting decks. Furthermore, results of deck selection indicate that the high risk for AN group selected decks based on immediate cues, such as large gains or losses, rather than long-term outcome.

There was a significant negative correlation between IGT total net score and AN symptoms measured by the KEAT-26 in the high risk for AN group. The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in performance on the IGT (22). Reduced orbitofrontal volume is observed in patients with AN and a relationship between reduced orbitofrontal volume and IGT performance has also been reported (20). Extrapolating from these data, the present results indicate that individuals at high risk for AN may have structural or functional dysfunctions of the orbitofrontal cortex.

Application of the PVL model revealed that poor contingency learning was related to poor IGT performance in individuals at high risk for AN. The high risk for AN group exhibited significantly lower values for the learning parameter of the PVL model than the control group, and this result is consistent with previous observation of AN patients (25). The learning parameter measures the formation or modification of preference for each deck based on recent experiences with a particular outcome (25, 26). Our results indicate that individuals at high risk for AN could not memorize or incorporate experiences from earlier trials into expectancies for subsequent trials, or modify preferences formed during earlier trials. In other words, individuals at high risk for AN may have difficulties with memory (48), working memory (49), or set-shifting (13, 21, 50).

The high risk for AN group also exhibited lower response consistency values than the control group. The response consistency parameter reflects the degree of consistency between deck selections and the expected outcomes associated with each deck (24, 25). In the present study, the high risk for AN group selected cards randomly throughout the trials without learning which decks were advantageous or disadvantageous, whereas the control group consistently selected cards from advantageous decks as trials progressed (51). Application of the PVL model revealed that the high risk for AN group had significantly lower values of the learning and response consistency parameters. These results indicate that individuals at high risk for AN have deficits in decision-making ability, possibly due to failures to convert experiences from previous trials into expectancies about options in subsequent trials.

This study has several limitations that should be addressed in future research. First, inclusion of only a small number of female participants in this study limits the generalizability of our findings. Second, menstrual cycle, which is known to affect decision-making ability in women (52), was not controlled in the present study. Third, as the PVL model is based on behavioral data from the IGT, its ability to reveal the mechanisms leading to poor IGT performance in individuals at high risk for AN is limited. Therefore, future studies should employ neuroimaging techniques to understand these mechanisms more fully. Fourth, performance on the IGT may be associated with performance on neuropsychological tests evaluating memory or set-shifting. Future studies should administer both the IGT and neuropsychological tests to provide a better understanding of the association between decision-making ability and other neuropsychological functions. Finally, subjects in the high risk for AN group could not be classified by AN subtype in the present study. Since characteristics such as impulsivity or perfectionism differ according to subtypes of AN (53), studies that classify high risk for AN group into subtypes would provide valuable information about the risk factors for AN.

In conclusion, individuals in the high risk for AN group had significantly lower total net scores and block net scores in the third, fourth, and fifth blocks of the IGT. They were more likely than controls to select cards from the disadvantageous A and B decks and less likely to select them from the advantageous D deck. Additionally, the block net scores of the control group increased as trials progressed, whereas the block net scores of the high risk for AN group did not change over the course of the IGT. Application of the PVL model showed that the high risk for AN group had significantly lower values for the learning and response consistency parameters. These results indicate that individuals at high risk for AN have deficits in decision-making, possibly due to failure to incorporate the experiences of outcomes on previous trials into card selection in subsequent trials.

Author Contributions

M-SK designed the study and revised the manuscript. EN collected the data and wrote the first draft. BK analyzed the data.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Sungshin Women's University Research Grant of 2017.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press (2013).

2. Merwin RM, Timko CA, Moskovich AA, Ingel KK, Bulik CM, Zucker NL. Psychological flexibility and symptom expression in anorexia nervosa. Eating Disord. (2010) 19:62–82. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2011.533606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Steinglass JE, Walsh BT, Stern Y. Set shifting deficit in anorexia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2006) 12:431–5. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Steinhausen HC. The outcome of anorexia nervosa in the 20th century. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159:1284–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Vitiello B, Lederhendler I. Research on eating disorders: current status and futureprospect. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 47:777–86. doi: 10.1016/S0006–3223(99)00319–4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hatch A, Madden S, Kohn M, Clarke S, Touyz S, Williams LM. Anorexia nervosa: towards an integrative neuroscience model. Eur Eating Disord Rev. (2010) 18:165–79. doi: 10.1002/erv.974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fassino S, Piero A, Daga GA, Leombruni P, Mortara P, Rovera GG. Attentional bias and frontal functioning in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eating Disord. (2002) 31:274–83. doi: 10.1002/eat.10028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Roberts ME, Tchanturia K, Treasure JL. Is attention to detail a similarly strong candidate endophenotype for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa? World J Biol Psychiatry (2013) 14:452–63. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.639804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Stedal K, Rose M, Frampton I, Landro NI, Lask B. The neuropsychological profile of children, adolescents, and young adults with anorexia nervosa. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2012) 27:329–37. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wilsdon A, Wade TD. Executive functioning in anorexia nervosa: exploration of the role of obsessionality, depression and starvation. J Psychiatric Res. (2006) 40:746–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Tchanturia K, Liao P, Forcano L, Fernandez-Aranda F, Uher R, Treasure J, et al. Poor decision making in male patients with anorexia nervosa. Eur Eating Disord Rev. (2012) 20:169–73. doi: 10.1002/erv.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Bassi T, Gorini A, Baraldi C, Ubbiali A, et al. Decision-making function as a predictor of treatment outcome in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. (2006) 145:179–87. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Galimberti E, Fadda E, Cavallini MC, Martoni RM, Erzegovesi S, Bellodi L. Executive functioning in anorexia nervosa patients and their unaffected relatives. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 208:238–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition (1994) 50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010–0277(94)90018–3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Clark L, Robbins TW. Decision-making. In: Wood SJ, Allen NB, Pantelis C, editors. The Neuropsychology of Mental Illness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2009). p. 138–56.

Google Scholar

16. Brand M, Grabenhorst F, Starcke K, Vandekerckhove MM, Markowitsch HJ. Role of the amygdala in decision under ambiguity and decisions under risk: evidence from patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuropsychologia (2007) 45:1305–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Struglia F, Stratta P, Gianfelice D, Pacifico R, Riccardi I, Rossi A. Decision-making impairment in schizophrenia: relationships with positive symptomatology. Neurosci Lett. (2011) 502:80–3. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cavedini P, Bassi T, Ubbiali A, Casolari A, Giordani S, Zorzi C, et al. Neuropsychological investigation of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. (2004) 127:259–66. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Adoue C, Jaussent I, Olie E, Beziat S, Van den Eynde F, Courtet P, et al. A further assessment of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Eur Psychiatry (2015) 30:121–7. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bodell LP, Keel PK, Brumm MC, Akubuiro A, Caballero J, Tranel D, et al. Longitudinal examination of decision-making performance in anorexia nervosa: before and after weight restoration. J Psychiatric Res. (2014) 56:150–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Danner UN, Sanders N, Smeets PA, van Meer F, Adan RA, Hoek HW, et al. Neuropsychological weakness in anorexia nervosa: set-shifting, central coherence, and decision-making in currently ill and recovered women. Int J Eating Disord. (2012) 45:685–94. doi: 10.1002/eat.22007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP. Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci. (1999) 19:5474–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19–13-05473.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ernst M, Paulus MP. Neurobiology of decision making: a selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 58:597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Busemeyer JR, Stout JC. A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychol Assess. (2002) 14:253–62. doi: 10.1037/1040–3590.14.3.253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chan TW, Ahn WY, Bates JE, Busemeyer JR, Guillaume S, Redgrave GW, et al. Differential impairments underlying decision making in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a cognitive modeling analysis. Int J Eating Disord. (2014) 47:157–67. doi: 10.1002/eat.22223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ahn WY, Busemeyer JR, Wagenmakers EJ, Stout JC. Comparison of decision learning models using the generalization criterion method. Cogn Sci. (2008) 32:1376–402. doi: 10.1080/03640210802352992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kim MS, Kang BN, Lim JY. Decision-making deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia: Iowa gambling task and prospect valence learning model. Neuropsychiatric Dis Treatment (2016) 12:1019–27. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S103821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fridberg DJ, Queller S, Ahn WY, Kim W, Bishara AJ, Busemeyer JR, et al. Cognitive mechanisms underlying risky decision-making in chronic cannabis users. J Mathemat Psychol. (2010) 54:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Vall E, Wade TD. Trail making task performance in inpatients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Eur Eating Disord Rev. (2015) 23:304–11. doi: 10.1002/erv.2364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Naor-Ziv R, Glicksohn J. Investigating cognitive deficits as risk factors for developing eating disorders during adolescence. Dev Neuropsychol. (2016) 41:107–24. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1170129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Touchette E, Henegar A, Godart NT, Pryor L, Falissard B, Tremblay RE, et al. Sublclinical eating disorders and their comorbidity with mood and anxiety disorders in adolescent girls. Psychiatry Res. (2011) 185:185–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Stice E, Desjardins CD. Interactions between risk factors in the prediction of onset of eating disorders: exploratory hypothesis generating analyses. Behav Res Ther. (2018) 105:52–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Garner DM, Olmsted MP, Bohr Y, Garfinkel PE. The eating attitudes test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychol Med. (1982) 12:871–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700049163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rhee MK, Lee YH, Park SH, Sohn CH, Chung YC, Hong SK, et al. A standardization study of the Korean version of eating attitude test-26 I: reliability and factor analysis. Korean J Psychosomatic Med. (1998) 6:155–75.

35. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbson M, Williams JB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorder, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I-NP). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute (1996).

36. Zung WW. A self-rating depression scale. Arch General Psychiatry (1965) 12:63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1970).

Google Scholar

38. Hwang S, Kim J, Park G, Chey J, Hong S. K-WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual. Daegu: Korea Psychology (2013).

39. Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task Professional Manual. Lutz: FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (2007).

40. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2009).

PubMed Abstract

41. Blinder BJ, Cumella EJ, Sanathara VA. Psychiatric comorbidities of female inpatients with eating disorders. Psychosomatic Med. (2006) 68:454–62. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221254.77675.f5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. O'Brien KM, Vincent NK. Psychiatric comorbidity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: nature, prevalence, and causal relationships. Clin Psychol Rev. (2003) 23:57–74. doi: 10.1016/S0272–7358(02)00201–5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bosanac P, Kurlender S, Stojanovska L, Hallam K, Norman T, McGrath C, et al. Neuropsychological study of underweight and “weight-recovered” anorexia nervosa compared with bulimia nervosa and normal controls. Int J Eating Disord. (2007) 40:613–21. doi: 10.1002/eat.20412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Guillaume S, Sang CN, Jaussent I, Raingeard I, Bringer J, Jollant F, et al. Is decision making really impaired in eating disorders? Neuropsychology (2010) 24:808–12. doi: 10.1037/a0019806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasion AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science (1997) 275:1293–5. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Yoo JY, Kim MS. Deficits in decision-making and reversal learning in college students who participate in binge drinking. Neuropsychiatry (2016) 6:321–30. doi: 10.4172/Neuropsychiatry.1000156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Steingroever H, Wetzels R, Horstmann A, Neumann J, Wagenmakers EJ. Performances of healthy participants on the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychol Assess. (2013) 25:180–93. doi: 10.1037/a0029929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Oltra-Cucarella J, Espert R, Rojo L, Jacas C, Guillen V, Moreno S. Neuropsychological impairments in anorexia nervosa: a Spanish sample pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol. (2014) 21:161–75. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2013.782030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Smith KE, Mason TB, Johnson JS, Lavender JM, Wonderlich SA. A systematic review of reviews of neurocognitive functioning in eating disorders: the state-of-the-literature and future directions. Int J Eating Disord. (2018) 51:1–24. doi: 10.1002/eat.22929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Holliday J, Tchanturia K, Landau S, Collier D, Treasure J. Is impaired set-shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? Am J Psychiatry (2005) 162:2269–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Vassileva J, Ahn WY, Weber KM, Busemeyer JR, Stout JC, Gonzalez R, et al. Computational modeling reveals distinct effects of HIV and history of drug use on decision making processes in women. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e68962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lazzaro SC, Rutledge RB, Burghart DR, Glimcher PW. The impact of menstrual cycle phase on economic choice and rationality. PLoS ONE (2016) 11:e0144080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fassino S, Piero A, Gramaglia C, Abbate-Daga G. Clinical, psychopathological and personality correlates of interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and obesity. Psychopathology (2004) 37:168–74. doi: 10.1159/000079420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: decision-making, eating disorder, high risk for anorexia nervosa, Iowa gambling task, prospect valence learning model

Citation: Na E, Kang B and Kim M-S (2019) Decision-Making Deficits Are Associated With Learning Impairments in Female College Students at High Risk for Anorexia Nervosa: Iowa Gambling Task and Prospect Valence Learning Model. Front. Psychiatry 9:759. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00759

Received: 13 October 2018; Accepted: 20 December 2018;
Published: 22 January 2019.

Edited by:

Qinghua He, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by:

Wan-Sen Yan, Guizhou Medical University (GMU), China
Paulo Jannuzzi Cunha, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2019 Na, Kang and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Myung-Sun Kim, kimms@sungshin.ac.kr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.