Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 April 2019
Sec. Mood Disorders

The Effects of Meditation, Yoga, and Mindfulness on Depression, Anxiety, and Stress in Tertiary Education Students: A Meta-Analysis

\r\nJosefien J. F. Breedvelt,*Josefien J. F. Breedvelt1,2*Yagmur AmanvermezYagmur Amanvermez3Mathias HarrerMathias Harrer4Eirini KaryotakiEirini Karyotaki3Simon GilbodySimon Gilbody5Claudi L. H. BocktingClaudi L. H. Bockting1Pim CuijpersPim Cuijpers3David D. EbertDavid D. Ebert4
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Research Department, Mental Health Foundation, London, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Clinical, Neuro-, and Developmental Psychology, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Erlangen, Germany
  • 5Mental Health and Addictions Research Group, Department of Health Sciences, University of York, York, United Kingdom

Background: Meditation, yoga, and mindfulness are popular interventions at universities and tertiary education institutes to improve mental health. However, the effects on depression, anxiety, and stress are unclear. This study assessed the effectiveness of meditation, yoga, and mindfulness on symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress in tertiary education students.

Methods: We searched Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), PubMed, PsycINFO and identified 11,936 articles. After retrieving 181 papers for full-text screening, 24 randomized controlled trials were included in the qualitative analysis. We conducted a random-effects meta-analysis amongst 23 studies with 1,373 participants.

Results: At post-test, after exclusion of outliers, effect sizes for depression, g = 0.42 (95% CI: 0.16–0.69), anxiety g = 0.46 (95% CI: 0.34–0.59), stress g = 0.42 (95% CI: 0.27–0.57) were moderate. Heterogeneity was low (I2 = 6%). When compared to active control, the effect decreased to g = 0.13 (95% CI: −0.18–0.43). No RCT reported on safety, only two studies reported on academic achievement, most studies had a high risk of bias.

Conclusions: Most studies were of poor quality and results should be interpreted with caution. Overall moderate effects were found which decreased substantially when interventions were compared to active control. It is unclear whether meditation, yoga or mindfulness affect academic achievement or affect have any negative side effects.

Introduction

Rationale

Every 12 months, between 7 and 16% of students in tertiary education experience a mood or anxiety disorder and a further 30% of students report experiencing moderate to severe levels of stress (14).

It is important to tackle poor mental health early as unattended symptoms can contribute to poorer clinical outcomes such as an increased risk of developing a clinical diagnosis or relapse (5). When in distress, few students seek or receive treatment (6). This is due to several barriers such as stigma and lack of awareness of services (6).

Mindfulness, meditation, and yoga have been coined as a non-stigmatizing alternative to traditional mental health support. They are highly popular tools at tertiary education institutes and used for stress reduction, improve productivity and general mental health (7). Yoga, mindfulness, and meditation are part of a suite of interventions called mind-body interventions (8). They are closely related practices and share underlying common principles and therapeutic elements grounded in religion and spirituality (912).

The most commonly known and offered mindfulness program is Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (13). MBSR includes a set of specific mindfulness practices including focused attention on the breath, “body-scanning,” prosocial meditation (e.g., loving kindness and compassion), and gentle hatha yoga. MBSR is different from Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) as it includes cognitive therapeutic elements such as cognitive restructuring and is aimed at reducing depressive relapse (14). Yoga is defined as a variety of practices which includes postures, breathing exercises, meditation, mantras, lifestyle changes spiritual beliefs, and/or rituals (15). A frequently practiced form of yoga is Hatha Yoga, which includes asanas (postures, pranayama (breathing exercises) and meditation, usually integrated throughout the practice (16).

Several reviews have been conducted to assess the effects of mindfulness and yoga-based interventions on a range of outcomes and populations. Reviews assessing the evidence for yoga have covered PTSD (17), depression (18, 19), anxiety (20), and physiological measures of stress (21, 22). For mindfulness and meditation interventions, reviews have assessed mood, and general functioning of students (23), employee mental health (24), stress management (25, 26), depression, stress and wellbeing (27), recurrent depression (28), and anxiety (27, 29). The reviews are wide-ranging in their conclusions and offer mixed results. Whilst the majority of reviews suggest preliminary evidence for their effectiveness, the authors often comment on the need for more rigorous research in this area.

The debate about the effects of these alternative medicine interventions thus remains. A recent review by Goyal et al. (27) found a pool of low-quality studies, with limited evidence for effect especially when compared to specific active treatment control conditions such as behavioral therapies, relaxation interventions, or exercise.

It is important to address the effects of these interventions for students, clinicians and commissioners to make evidence-based decisions about the provision of mental health support at university. Whilst widely accessed, it is unclear whether yoga, mindfulness, or meditation have a beneficial effect on mental health or academic achievement in young adults beyond placebo.

Objectives

This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to study the effectiveness of both yoga and mindfulness-based interventions on stress, depression, anxiety, and academic achievement for students in tertiary education.

Research Question

What are the effects of mindfulness, meditation, and yoga on depression, anxiety stress and academic achievement in tertiary education students vs. control?

Methods

Study Design

This study utilized a systematic review and meta-analysis in order to answer the above research question.

Participants, Interventions, Comparators, Outcomes

Included studies were randomized controlled trials, published in English, in which a meditation, yoga or mindfulness intervention was compared to an active or non-active control group (wait-list, treatment-as-usual, placebo or active treatment control). Participants had to be enrolled in tertiary education when they were randomized into treatment group in the study (i.e., a university, college or other postsecondary higher education). Studies in which measured depression, anxiety, stress (i.e., Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), academic achievement (i.e., productivity, GPA, absenteeism) or a combination of these, as measured via a validated questionnaire were included. Additionally, enough information needed to be provided to calculate the effect size. We contacted authors when we were unable to calculate effect sizes based on the information provided in the paper.

Systematic Review Protocol

The procedure for this systematic review is outlined according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocol (PRISMA-P) guidelines (30).

Search Strategy and Data Sources

Publications were identified by searching Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), MEDLINE, and PsycINFO by combining terms (text words, MeSH terms and subject headings) on (1) student population, (2) psychological interventions, (3) mental health or academic outcomes, and (4) randomized controlled trials. We conducted the searches on 27.04.17. We included studies with any date of publication which were either published, under review or “in press.” We contacted authors of study protocols that were suitable for inclusion to assess whether any unpublished results were available for inclusion. The search string can be found in Appendix 1 in Supplementary Material. Additionally, we searched for references in prior meta-analyses and included studies until 02.03.18.

Study Selection and Data Extraction

Study Selection

Titles and abstracts of articles identified through the database search were screened by two independent researchers. The researchers coded all retrieved results to separate categories, including one pool of “alternative medicine” studies. Please see Appendix 2 for an overview of study coding procedures. From the alternative medicine study pool, studies were coded as yoga, mindfulness or meditation and these were then retrieved for full-text screening. In this second step full texts of all studies that were deemed suitable were retrieved and reviewed for eligibility by two researchers (JB and YA). When there was any disagreement the authors convened for a discussion; senior researchers (DE or PC) were consulted if disagreement could not otherwise be resolved. Figure 1 shows the PRISMA-P flow chart.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Prisma-P flow chart.

Data Extraction

A standardized electronic data collection form following Cochrane Good practice guidance was used to extract data. The following variables were extracted: (1) bibliographical data, (2) study design, (3), sample characteristics (e.g., gender, % female, ethnicity), (4), intervention type (mindfulness, MBSR, yoga, meditation) (5) exposure to the intervention [e.g., duration of each session, duration of home practice, length of intervention period (weeks)]. Exposure was calculated as the duration of meditation each week (in session and home practice) (in minutes) × length of intervention (weeks). (6) Setting (country), (7) intervention modality (e.g., face to face, online, group setting) (9) outcomes (depression, anxiety, stress, academic achievement), and (10) drop-out and handling of missing data. Two reviewers (JB and YA) independently extracted the study data and resolved any discrepancies by consulting a third reviewer (MH). When studies conducted assessments for the above outcomes during an exam and in non-exam settings, we extracted the non-exam setting only. Please see Table 1 for an overview of interventions and intervention descriptions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Definitions of intervention categories.

Risk of Bias

Two researchers assessed the risk of bias (JB and YA), which was extracted using an approach based on the Cochrane Collaboration risk of bias assessment criteria for RCTs described by Furlan et al. (34). The criteria were: (1) random sequence generation, (2) allocation concealment, (3) blinding of participants, (4) blinding of personnel, (5) blinding of outcome assessors, (6) incomplete outcome data, (7) selective outcome reporting (which was established searching for the protocol on PubMed and Google Scholar and assess discrepancies between included outcomes and reported outcomes, if no protocol was available we examined the methods for any unreported outcomes), (8) serious flaw. Studies were scored as “low,” “high,” or “unclear” risk of bias on each of these domains. If the researchers scored six criteria as “low” and if there were no serious flaws detected, the study was scored to have a low risk of bias.

Researcher Allegiance

For researcher allegiance assessment, the reprint approach with criteria operationalized to assess the “belief [of the investigators] in the superiority of a treatment [and] … the superior validity of the theory of change that is associated with the treatment” was applied (35). A six-point rating scale indicating various degrees of risk for researcher allegiance derived and adapted from Wampold et al. (36) was used.

Convenience Sampling Rating

We defined studies as employing convenience samples when either: (1) the sample only contains students of the investigators institute, (2) the sample was recruited through an internal study recruitment system used for the recruitment of research participants, (3) course credit was given, or (4) the article states a convenience sample was used.

Data Analysis

Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) software (Biostat, Inc.) and STATA version 15 were used for the analysis (37). For each study included in the quantitative analysis, between-group effect sizes between intervention and control group at post-intervention were calculated (Hedges' g). For the main outcome analysis, three separate analyses were conducted to quantify the effects of studies on depression, anxiety, and stress. In the case of multiple treatment groups, the mean effect sizes were pooled for each study. When studies only recorded outcomes taken during an exam, we extracted these and conducted a sensitivity analysis to assess whether results were comparable.

A random effects model was used to pool effect sizes as we expect considerable heterogeneity amongst studies (38). To improve clinical interpretation, g-values were converted into the numbers-needed-to-treat (NNT) using Furakawa's method (39). The assumed response rate (50% reduction in symptoms) in the control group was 19% (40). The response rate was estimated from studies that assessed psychotherapy for depression and we assumed similar rates could be achieved in these studies. The NNT reflects the number of participants that need to receive the intervention in order for a positive outcome for one participant (41). 95% confidence intervals and two-sided P-values for each outcome were calculated.

The I2 statistic was used to determine heterogeneity (42). I2 heterogeneity of 25% was deemed low, 50% moderate, and 75% as substantial heterogeneity (43). The 95% confidence intervals were calculated using the STATA module heterogi (44). In this, a non-central chi-square based approach was used. Sensitivity analyses were conducted to assess whether study quality was related to effect sizes by comparing studies indicating a low risk of bias with all other studies. In addition, we examined the association between researcher allegiance, the use of a convenience sample and the treatment effects. Publication bias was tested by inspection of the funnel plot on primary outcome measures. Egger's test, a test for asymmetry of the funnel plot, was performed to attain quantitative results on publication bias (45).

When funnel plot inspection or Egger's test suggested the presence of bias, we applied the Duval and Tweedie trim and fill-procedure. This procedure estimates the number of missing studies and adjusts the effect size accordingly to attain a more unbiased estimate of the pooled effect size (46).

Subgroup Analysis and Meta-Regression

Subgroup analyses and bivariate regression analyses were conducted to explore the following moderators; type of control, researcher allegiance, risk of bias, country of study, intervention type, exposure of intervention [duration of meditation each week (in session and home practice) (in minutes) x length of intervention (weeks)], and delivery of intervention (therapist, group, self-help).

Results

Selection and Inclusion of Studies

After screening 11,936 abstracts, 181 studies were retrieved and coded. Of these 181 studies, 58 studies covered a meditation, yoga or mindfulness intervention. Subsequently, we identified 24 studies as fitting our inclusion criteria, for further detail on study selection, please see Figure 1.

Study Characteristics

Out of 24 included studies in both the quantitative and qualitative analysis, nine were conducted on the North American continent, 12 in Asia, and three in Europe. Eighty-three percent of participants were female. All studies used a “convenience sample” and most studies were conducted with participants from a medical faculty (N = 14). With regards to symptom levels in the sample, only one study excluded participants with low scores on the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (47). All other studies were aimed at a healthy or subclinical population. A further overview of study characteristics can be found in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study characteristics.

Out of 24 studies, the average rating of research allegiance was 2.63 and three studies scored 5/5. Eight studies provided information on ethnicity, of these, most participants were Caucasian (68%, N = 484), followed by Asian (12%, N = 88) and African/ African American (10%, N = 72).

The average length of the intervention was ~7 weeks. On average, participants practiced meditation yoga or mindfulness for 153 min each week, totalling to overall average exposure at 19 h and 36 min. All studies but two were offered in a group setting, with two offered as self-help, one of these approaches was an internet-based intervention. Four treatment-control comparisons utilized an active control, 10 studies used wait-list control and 10 provided no treatment. Please see Table 3 for a further specification of intervention characteristics.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Intervention characteristics.

In two comparisons symptom scores were higher in the intervention group at post-test. In one case this was when the intervention was compared to an inactive control (69). In the other, the intervention performed worse compared to an active control (66). No studies reported any further adverse effects.

Synthesized Findings

In the quantitative analysis, we included a total of 23 studies. The studies included 1,373 participants with 660 in the intervention and 713 in control. For the quantitative analysis, we could not include academic outcomes as there were only two eligible studies reporting these (54, 61). The study by Paholpak et al. (61) assessed breathing meditation on memory, academic functioning and psychiatric symptoms in medical students (N = 58). No significant difference was found between intervention and control group (effect size and significance here). Nemati (54) did find a significant difference between intervention (pranayama yoga) and control (N = 107) on academic functioning (effect size and significance here). Test anxiety (i.e., anxiety related to performing a particular test) was measured in two studies (50, 54). We thought the sample too small to pool in a sensitivity analysis and a further analysis was not conducted.

The overall post-treatment effect in the 23 comparisons between yoga and mindfulness-based therapies and control groups was g = 0.61 (95% CI: 0.40–0.81) with an NNT of 5 (please see Table 4). The heterogeneity was high (I2 = 74%, 95% CI: 61–83). Three studies were potential positive outliers with an extremely high effect size (g >1) (53, 55, 56). After exclusion of these studies, the effect size decreased to g = 0.42 (95% CI: 0.31–0.52), NNT = 7 for depression, anxiety, and stress combined. Heterogeneity also decreased substantially (I2 = 6%, 95% CI: 0–40).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of meditation based interventions on depression, anxiety, and stress compared to control with Hedges g.

Five studies had more than two groups. Three studies used a second “active control” (relaxation, bio-feedback or dog therapy) condition aside from a no treatment control (49, 66, 67) and one study included two versions of yoga (56). A further study compared the effects of mindfulness or yoga techniques to control (47) (hatha yoga and body scan vs. wait-list control). Including multiple groups in the analysis may artificially reduce heterogeneity and thus introduce bias. To address this, we conducted an analysis where we first only included the study with the strongest effect size. A second analysis only included the lowest effect size. As Table 4 shows, effect sizes were similar, and heterogeneity increased (I2 high = 0% (95% CI: 0–48%), I2 low = 16% (95% CI: 0–51).

Effects of Mindfulness and Yoga Interventions on Depression, Anxiety and Stress

Ten studies reported a depression outcome, 15 anxiety, and 10 stress. Figure 2 shows three forest plots on the separate outcome categories. Considering outcomes for depression, anxiety, and stress separately, the mean effect size for depression was g = 0.42 (95% CI: 0.16–0.69, I2 = 62%, 95% CI: 24–81), anxiety g = 0.46 (95% CI: 0.34–0.59, I2 = 0%, 95% CI: 0–54), and stress g = 0.42 (95%CI: 0.27–0.57, I2 = 5%, 95% CI: 0–64), respectively. There was no significant difference between these outcome categories in subgroup analysis (p = 0.41). Because depression, anxiety, and stress are highly interrelated and moderator characteristics were evenly distributed across outcomes, we conducted further subgroup analyses with pooled effect sizes across depression, anxiety, and stress outcomes.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot for intervention effects on anxiety, depressing and stress symptoms with Hedges g.

Long Term Follow-Up

Six studies provided long term follow-up data (i.e., any assessment after post-intervention) ranging between 1 and 24 months. The pooled effect size was small to medium g = 0.39 (95% CI: 0.17–0.61), and heterogeneity was low (I2 = 11%, 95% CI: 0–77).

Subgroup Analysis

We conducted several subgroup analyses, for all results please see Table 4. Stronger effects were found when questionnaires were taken during an exam setting. When compared to active and inactive controls, the continent in which the study was conducted was not associated with effect size (p = 0.06). However, when we compared the effects to no-treatment control, we did identify a significant effect (p = 0.03), with studies conducted in Asia g = 0.54 (95% CI: 0.34–0.74) yielding strongest effects compared to America g = 0.49 (95% CI: 0.34–0.64), and Europe g = 0.13 (95% CI: −0.13 to −0.39). A subgroup analysis which compared yoga, mindfulness meditation, and MBSR did not find any significant subgroup differences between the intervention types.

Meta-Regression Analyses

We conducted regression analyses on research allegiance, exposure to intervention, and overall Risk of Bias score. As a result, we did not find any significant explanatory value in these variables. Research allegiance (coefficient: 0; 95% CI: −0.11 to 0.08; p = 0.76) or the exposure to mindfulness, meditation or yoga (coefficient: 0; 95% CI: 0–0; p = 0.50). A further bivariate regression analysis on total RoB score and effect size did not identify significant subgroup differences either (coefficient: 0; 95% CI: −0.08 to 0.09; p = 0.91).

Risk of Bias

Overall, only one study was scored to have a low risk of bias (60). Most studies had an unclear risk of selection bias. Six studies reported adequate sequence generation, the remaining studies did not report methods or used inappropriate methods. Adequate blinding of participants and personnel was rare. None of the studies adequately blinded participants, which is common amongst psychological research trials. Two studies reported that outcome assessors were blinded and three reported blinding of personnel. Only four studies reported conducting an ITT analysis. In other studies, this was either unclear or a completer analysis was conducted. See Table 5 for an overview of the risk of bias assessments.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Risk of Bias.

We conducted subgroup analyses on each risk of bias category but did not identify any significant association between the risk of bias and effect size. A subgroup analysis comparing “high” and “low” risk did not identify a significant difference p = 0.80. A further bivariate analysis on total risk of bias score and effect size did not identify significant subgroup differences either (coefficient: 0; 95% CI: −0.08 to 0.09; p = 0.91). Because almost all studies had a high risk of bias we were unable to assess the impact of risk of bias on the outcomes as we had too little power to identify differences between the groups.

Publication Bias

A visual inspection of the funnel plots for three separate outcomes (Figure 3) indicated no indication for publication bias for depression and anxiety but some risk of publication bias for the effects of mindfulness, yoga and meditation interventions on stress. Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill procedure under the random effects model did not impute any study effects, and the mean effect size remained unchanged except for studies assessing the effects of the intervention on stress from g = 0.44 (95%CI: 0.28–0.59) to g = 0.34 (95%CI: 0.16–0.51). The Egger's test did not find evidence for significant asymmetry of the funnel plot (p = 0.12; Intercept: 1.47, 95% CI: −1.25 to 4.18).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Visual inspection of the funnel plots for stress, anxiety, and depressive symptoms.

Discussion

Summary of Main Findings

In this study, we set out to assess the efficacy of mindfulness, meditation, and yoga on student mental health and academic achievement. This is the first study to assess the effects of meditation-based interventions in this population across all outcomes specified above.

We found moderate positive effects for mindfulness, yoga or meditation-based interventions on symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress, however, the quality of the studies included in this review was low.

Subgroup analyses did not identify any differences were between yoga, mindfulness or meditation interventions. Two studies collected academic achievement data, making it impossible to render conclusions on the effects of such interventions on academic achievement. Similar to other reviews on mindfulness in this population, there were few studies with a long term follow up (23).

Comparison to Previous Literature

When comparing our results to the previous literature on other interventions such as CBT or exercise, we find few studies that cover the same age range, inclusion criteria, similar outcomes and vs. similar controls.

From the few comparisons that we are able to make, we find that effects for CBT and exercise in non-clinical populations are more similar to our results compared to effects found in clinical interventions. A recent meta-analysis on preventative interventions for depression found g = 0.53 (95% CI: 0.38–0.68) for CBT (70). The effects of CBT on anxiety in a mixed (clinical and non-clinical population) are also within the range of our results g = 0.43 (95% CI: 0.14–0.73) (71). Effects of exercise on depression in non-clinical adolescent populations compared to control were not significant d = −0.52, (95% CI: −1.30 to 0.26) (72). For clinical populations, we identified slightly larger effect sizes for psychological treatment g = 0.89 (95% CI: 0.66–1.11) (73) and for exercise on depression g = −0.72, (95% CI: −1.15 to −0.30) (74).

In contrast, internet interventions for clinical and non-clinical populations overall had lower effects for depression g = 0.18, (95% CI: 0.08–0.27), anxiety g = 0.27 (95% CI: 0.13–0.40), and stress g = 0.20, (95% CI: 0.02–0.38) (75). Finally, comparing these results to mindfulness in students, mindfulness had somewhat similar effects depression in a previous analysis in a non-clinical sample g = 0.31 (95% CI: 0.15–0.42) (70). In another meta-analysis amongst healthy individuals, MBSR showed higher effect sizes across stress g = 0.74 (95% CI = 0.41–1.07), depression g = 0.80 (95% CI = 0.49–1.12), and anxiety g = 0.64 (95% CI: 0.33–0.94). However, across outcomes a subgroup analysis found more similar results for students g = 0.47 (95% CI = 0.30–0.64) (76).

To summarize, it seems our results are somewhat more in line with studies conducted in non-clinical samples compared to a clinical sample. Due to the heterogeneity found in the meta-analytic literature, it is not yet possible to compare exercise vs. mind-body interventions in a vis-à-vis manner although the above might give an indication of the comparative effectiveness of interventions.

Other similarities with previous include the high risk of bias which was similarly high in reviews on yoga (18, 77), exercise and psychotherapy (73, 74). A mixed risk of bias was found for internet interventions (75), it might be that such interventions carry a slightly lower risk to bias as blinding of participants and personnel might be easier compared to a face to face intervention.

Other similarities with previous include the high risk of bias which we noted, which was similar to reviews on yoga (18, 77), exercise and psychotherapy (73, 74). A mixed risk of bias was found for internet interventions (75), it might be that such interventions carry a slightly lower risk to bias as blinding of participants and personnel might be easier compared to a face to face intervention.

In addition, no adverse events were recorded and intervention elements were inadequately reported on, which is a common feature of the evidence base for yoga and meditation (27, 7779).

Comparison to Active Controls

In line with prior research on meditation programs, there was no evidence that meditation, yoga or mindfulness was more effective than active control (52, 76, 80). We had a limited number of subgroups so were unable to segment our analysis by active treatment (drugs, exercise or CBT) or active non-specific control (27). All our controls were deemed specific controls and thus are in line with (52) who found that meditation-based interventions were only more effective when compared to non-specific active control.

Research on psychotherapy in a similar population found that effects were also not more effective compared to active (specific) control (73). For exercise, these interventions were more effective compared to placebo control although further comparison with specific controls was not possible (74). Similar results were found for interventions (75) and other mental health interventions (81). The above summary indicates that the results are in line with previous research on both mindfulness and meditation interventions and behavioral interventions. As far as we are aware, we have not yet identified similar comparisons to specific or non-specific control conditions as detailed in Goyal et al. (27) for behavioral interventions, which would be an interesting element to explore in future research.

Length and Duration

We did not find that the number of hours of meditation, yoga, and mindfulness is associated with effect size. Other meta-analyses on internet interventions did not find an effect on treatment length (weeks) (75). Meta-analyses with a majority of interventions based on CBT did find that interventions with a longer duration (hours) were associated with effect size (71, 81). In our analysis, we included both home practice as well as treatment duration, whilst the above analyses only specified the amount of treatment, this difference in coding might have altered our results. Also we were not always able to exactly specify the amount of home practice, which might again affect the validity of our result.

Limitations

In contrast to psychotherapeutic interventions, there remains a substantial degree of uncertainty about the robustness of our effect. Our study found that most studies were of lower quality and those improvements to reporting and procedures of studies in this area are required. High risk of bias is a concern as this impacts the validity of the findings and our confidence in making any conclusions from our analysis (82). Whilst risk of bias was not associated with effect size, this might be due to low power to detect such a difference. Due to the high risk of bias in most studies, it is difficult to determine the true value of these approaches and more rigorous research is clearly needed to assess for improving mental health in tertiary education. Whilst the quality of studies across behavioral interventions is low, we note that there are specific elements to meditation, yoga and mindfulness interventions that warrant improvement.

We found that intervention effects diminished when compared to active control, which implies that non-specific intervention elements such as peer-support, or activity scheduling, might have driven our results. To further study the differential effect of mindfulness, meditation, and yoga, a comprehensive typology of intervention elements is necessary. A shared understanding of differential intervention elements will allow development of adequate placebo control conditions to identify whether the contribution of mindfulness, meditation or yoga improves mental health or whether they are equally effective as non-specific placebo interventions.

Interestingly, we did not find an association between the exposure to yoga, mindfulness or meditation through treatment or home practice and effect size. This is surprising given the premise that yoga and mindfulness are seen as practices that improve over time (83). One might then expect to see a positive association between the amount of recent mindfulness practice and effect sizes. There is a caveat however as the amount of home practice was not always reported on consistently, thus due to a lack of clear reporting, we may have been unable to accurately estimate the exposure to such interventions.

Conclusions

To improve the evidence base, the conduct and reporting of studies on meditation, yoga, and mindfulness needs to be more rigorous to allow the delivery of results that are closer to their empirical truth. Furthermore, we recommend that a common typology for meditation, yoga and mindfulness interventions is developed and that future research includes comparisons between active placebo and control. This will allow us to determine the true differential effects between mindfulness, meditation, and yoga and in comparison, to other approaches to improve mental health. Ultimately, this will allow us to further our understanding of delivering effective non-clinical solutions for protecting and promoting mental health in student populations.

Author Contributions

JB selected and extracted the studies, conducted the analysis, prepared tables and prepared the first draft for review. YA selected and extracted the studies and prepared the tables for the first draft. Revising work critically for important content and accuracy. MH led on data acquisition and design of the work. Contributed strongly to the methods section and revised work critically after the first draft was prepared. EK led on conception and design of the work. Critically revised the first and second draft for accuracy and intellectual content. SG critically revised for intellectual content and reviewed first and second draft. Contributed to analysis plan. CB critically revised for intellectual content and contributed to plan of analysis. PC led on conception and design and provided support with analytical decisions in analysis stage critically reviewed the first and second draft for intellectual and conceptual relevance. DE led on conception and design of the work, critically revised the first and second draft for intellectual and conceptual relevance.

Conflict of Interest Statement

JB is employed by the Mental Health Foundation which offers an online Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy course titled BeMindful. She is not directly affiliated to the delivery or development of this programme.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Mental Health Foundation for their contribution to the publication fee.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00193/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Auerbach RP, Mortier P, Bruffaerts R, Alonso J, Benjet C, Cuijpers P, et al. The WHO world mental health surveys international college student project: prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. J Abnormal Psychol. (2018) 127:623–38. doi: 10.1037/abn0000362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bayram N, Bilgel N. The prevalence and socio-demographic correlations of depression, anxiety and stress among a group of university students. Social Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2008) 43:667–72. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0345-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Blanco C, Okuda M, Wright C, Hasin DS, Grant BF, Liu S-M, et al. Mental health of college students and their non–college-attending peers. Arch General Psychiatry. (2008) 65:1429. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Verger P, Guagliardo V, Gilbert F, Rouillon F, Kovess-Masfety V. Psychiatric disorders in students in six French universities: 12-month prevalence, comorbidity, impairment and help-seeking. Social Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. (2010) 45:189–99. doi: 10.1007/s00127-009-0055-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Patton GC, Coffey C, Romaniuk H, Mackinnon A, Carlin JB, Degenhardt L, et al. The prognosis of common mental disorders in adolescents: a 14-year prospective cohort study. Lancet. (2014) 383:1404–11. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736%2813%2962116-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hunt J, Eisenberg D. Mental health problems and help-seeking behavior among college students. J Adolescent Health. (2010) 46:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Upchurch DM, Gill M, Jiang L, Prelip M, Slusser W. Use of mind-body therapies among young adults aged 18-24 years : findings from the 2012 national health interview survey. J Adolescent Health. (2018) 014:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth,.2018.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. US National Library of Medicine. National Institutes of Health Collection Development Manual, Complementary and Alternative Medicine (2018). Available online at: https://www.nlm.nih.gov/tsd/acquisitions/cdm/subjects24.html (accessed June 25, 2018).

9. Ospina MB, Bond K, Karkhaneh M, Tjosvold L, Vandermeer B, Liang Y, et al. Meditation practices for health: state of the research. Evidence Report/Technology Assess. (2007) 155:1–263.

Google Scholar

10. Van Dam NT, van Vugt MK, Vago DR, Schmalzl L, Saron CD, Olendzki A, et al. Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 13:36–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691617709589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. Meditation. (2017). Available online at: https://nccih.nih.gov/health/meditation (accessed July 5, 2018).

12. National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. Yoga. (2017) Available online at: https://nccih.nih.gov/health/yoga (accessed July 5, 2018)

13. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clin Psychol. (2003) 10:144–56. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2002).

Google Scholar

15. Birdee GS, Legedza AT, Saper RB, Bertisch SM, Eisenberg DM, Phillips RS. Characteristics of yoga users: results of a national survey. J General Internal Med. (2008) 23:1653–8. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0735-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Riley D. Hatha Yoga and the treatment of ilness. Alternat Therapies. (2004) 10:20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

17. Cramer H, Anheyer D, Saha FJ, Dobos G. Yoga for posttraumatic stress disorder - a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry. (2018) 18:72. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1650-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cramer H, Lauche R, Langhorst J, Dobos G. Yoga for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Depression Anxiety. (2013) 30:1068–83. doi: 10.1002/da.22166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Pilkington K, Kirkwood G, Rampes H, Richardson J. Yoga for depression: the research evidence. J Affect Dis. (2005) 89:13–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Duan-Porter W, Coeytaux RR, McDuffie JR, Goode AP, Sharma P, Mennella H Evidence map of yoga for depression anxiety and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Phys Activ Health. (2016) 13:281–8. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2015-0027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pascoe MC, Bauer IE. A systematic review of randomised control trials on the effects of yoga on stress measures and mood. J Psychiatr Res. (2015) 68:270–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Pascoe MC, Thompson DR, Ski CF. Yoga, mindfulness-based stress reduction and stress-related physiological measures: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2017) 86:152–68. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. O'Driscoll M, Byrne S, Mc Gillicuddy A, Lambert S, Sahm LJ. The effects of mindfulness-based interventions for health and social care undergraduate students – a systematic review of the literature. Psychol Health Med. (2017) 22:851–65. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2017.1280178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Janssen M, Heerkens Y, Kuijer W, Van Der Heijden B, Engels J. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on employees' mental health: a systematic review. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e191332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. J Alternat Complement Med. (2009) 15:593–600. doi: 10.1089/acm.2008.0495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Regehr C, Glancy D, Pitts A. Interventions to reduce stress in university students: A review and metaanalysis. Eur Psychiatry. (2013) 148:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Goyal M, Singh S, Sibinga E, Gould N, Rowland-Seymour A, Sharma R, et al. Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being : a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. (2014) 174:357–68. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018.Meditation

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kuyken W, Warren FC, Taylor RS, Whalley B, Crane C, Bondolfi G, Dalgleish T. Efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in prevention of depressive relapse an individual patient data meta-analysis from randomized trials. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73:565–74. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Chen KW, Berger CC, Manheimer E, Forde D, Magidson J, Dachman L, et al. Meditative therapies for reducing anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Depression Anxiety. (2012) 29:545–62. doi: 10.1002/da.21964.Meditative

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberatî A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. (2015) 4:1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Creswell JD. Mindfulness interventions. Ann Rev Psychol. (2017) 68:491–516. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-042716-051139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Shapiro SL, Schwartz GE, Bonner G. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J Behav Med. (1998) 21:581–99. doi: 10.1023/A:1018700829825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Walsh R, Shapiro SL. The meeting of meditative disciplines and western psychology: A mutually enriching dialogue. Am Psychol. (2006) 61:227–39. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.3.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Furlan AD, Pennick V, Bombardier C, van Tulder M. 2009 updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the cochrane back review group. Spine. (2009) 34:1929–41. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181b1c99f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Leykin Y, DeRubeis RJ. Allegiance in psychotherapy outcome research: Separating association from bias. Clin Psychol. (2009) 16:54–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01143.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Wampold BE, Budge SL, Laska KM, Del Re AC, Baardseth TP, Fluckiger C, et al. Evidence-based treatments for depression and anxiety versus treatment-as-usual: a meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Clin Psychol Rev. (2011) 31:1304–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC (2017).

38. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Controlled Clin Trials. (1986) 7:177–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

39. Furukawa TA, Leucht S. How to obtain nnt from cohen's d: comparison of two methods. PLOS ONE. (2011) 6:e19070. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cuijpers P, Karyotaki E, Weitz E, Andersson G. The effects of psychotherapies for major depression in adults on remission, recovery and improvement : a meta-analysis. J Affect Dis. (2014) 159:118–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Laupacis A, Sackett DL, Roberts RS. An assessment of clinically useful measures of the consequences of treatment. N Engl J Med. (1988) 318:1728–33. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198806303182605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Higgins J, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statist Med. (2002) 21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Orsini N, Bottai M, Higgins J, Buchan I. HETEROGI: Stata Module to Quantify Heterogeneity in a Meta-Analysis. Statistical Software Components (2006).

Google Scholar

45. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C, Mulrow C, Egger M, et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br Med J. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Taylor S, Tweedie R. Trim and Fill: A Simple Funnel Plot Based Method of Testing and Adjusting for Publication Bias in Meta-Analyses. Fort Collins, CO: Colorado State University (1998).

Google Scholar

47. Call D, Miron L, Orcutt H. Effectiveness of brief mindfulness techniques in reducing symptoms of anxiety and stress. Mindfulness. (2014) 5:658–68. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0218-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Malathi A, Damodaran A. Stress due to exams in medical students–role of yoga. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. (1999) 43:218–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Tloczynski J. A preliminary study of opening-up meditation college adjustment, and self-actualization. Psychol Rep. (1994) 75:449–50. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1994.75.1.449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Chang JC-W. Effect of meditation on music performance anxiety. Disserta Abstr Int Sect A. (2001) 62:1765. doi: 10.16953/deusbed.74839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Nidich SI, Rainforth MV, Haaga DAF, Hagelin J, Salerno JW, Travis F, et al. A randomized controlled trial on effects of the Transcendental Meditation program on blood pressure, psychological distress, and coping in young adults. Am J Hyperte. (2009) 22:1326–31. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2009.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gopal A, Mondal S, Gandhi A, Arora S, Bhattacharjee J. Effect of integrated yoga practices on immune responses in examination stress - a preliminary study. Int J Yoga. (2011) 4:26–32. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.7817810.4103/0973-6131.78178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Kim SD. Effects of yogic exercises on life stress and blood glucose levels in nursing students. J Phys Ther Sci. (2014) 26:2003–6. doi: 10.1589/jpts.26.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Nemati A. The effect of pranayama on test anxiety and test performance. Int J Yoga. (2013) 6:55–60. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.105947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Shankarapillai R, Nair MA, George R. The effect of yoga in stress reduction for dental students performing their first periodontal surgery: a randomized controlled study. Int J Yoga. (2012) 5:48–51. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.9171410.4103/0973-6131.91714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sharma VK, Trakroo M, Subramaniam V, Rajajeyakumar M, Bhavanani AB, Sahai A. Effect of fast and slow pranayama on perceived stress and cardiovascular parameters in young health-care students. Int J Yoga. (2013) 6:104–10. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.113400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Erogul M, Singer G, McIntyre T, Stefanov DG. Abridged Mindfulness Intervention to Support Wellness in First-Year Medical Students. Teach Learn Med. (2014) 26:350–6. doi: 10.1080/10401334.2014.945025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Song Y, Lindquist R. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on depression, anxiety, stress and mindfulness in Korean nursing students. Nurse Edu Today. (2015) 35:86–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Esch T, Sonntag U, Esch SM, Thees S. Stress management and mind-body medicine: a randomized controlled longitudinal evaluation of students' health and effects of a behavioral group intervention at a middle-size German University (SM-MESH). Forschende Komplementarmedizin. (2013) 20:129–37. doi: 10.1159/000350671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. van Dijk I, Lucassen PLBJ, Akkermans RP, van Engelen BGM, van Weel C, Speckens AEM, et al. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of clinical clerkship students: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Acad Med. (2017) 92:1012–21. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000001546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Paholpak S, Piyavhatkul N, Rangseekajee P, Krisanaprakornkit T, Arunpongpaisal S, Pajanasoontorn N, et al. Breathing meditation by medical students at Khon Kaen University: effect on psychiatric symptoms, memory, intelligence and academic achievement. J Med Assoc Thail. (2012) 95(3):461–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

62. Danilewitz M, Bradwejn J, Koszycki D. A pilot feasibility study of a peer-led mindfulness program for medical students. Can Med Edu J. (2016) 7:e31–7. doi: 10.3946/kjme.2014.26.3.217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Greeson J, Juberg M, Maytan M, James K, Rogers H. A randomized controlled trial of Koru: a mindfulness program for college students and other emerging adults. J Am College Health. (2014) 62:222–33. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2014.887571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kvillemo P, Brandberg Y, Bränström R. Feasibility and outcomes of an internet-based mindfulness training program: a pilot randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mental Health. (2016) 3:e33. doi: 10.2196/mental.5457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Kang YS, Choi SY, Ryu E. The effectiveness of a stress coping program based on mindfulness meditation on the stress, anxiety, and depression experienced by nursing students in Korea. Nurse Edu Today. (2009) 29:538–43. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2008.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ratanasiripong P, Park JF, Ratanasiripong N, Kathalae D. Stress and anxiety management in nursing students: biofeedback and Mindfulness Meditation. J Nurs Edu. (2015) 54:520–4. doi: 10.3928/01484834-20150814-07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Shearer A, Hunt M, Chowdhury M, Nicol L. Effects of a brief mindfulness meditation intervention on student stress and heart rate variability. Int J Stress Manage. (2016) 23:232–54. doi: 10.1037/a0039814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Yazdani M, Esmaeilzadeh M, Pahlavanzadeh S, Khaledi F. The effect of laughter Yoga on general health among nursing students. Iran J Nurs Midwifery Res. (2014) 19:36–40.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

69. Chen Y, Yang X, Wang L, Zhang X. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of brief mindfulness meditation on anxiety symptoms and systolic blood pressure in Chinese nursing students. Nurse Edu Today. (2013) 33:1166–72. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2012.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Breedvelt JJF, Kandola A, Kousoulis AA, Brouwer ME, Karyotaki E, Bockting CLH, et al. What are the effects of preventative interventions on major depressive disorder (MDD) in young adults? a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials J Affect Disord. (2018) 239. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Conley CS, Durlak JA, Shapiro JB, Kirsch AC, Zahniser E. A meta-analysis of the impact of universal and indicated preventive technology-delivered Interventions for higher education students. Prevent Sci. (2016) 17:659–78. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0662-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Carter T, Morres ID, Meade O, Callaghan P. The effect of exercise on depressive symptoms in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 55:580–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Cuijpers P, Cristea IA, Ebert DD, Koot HM, Auerbach RP, Bruffaerts R, et al. Psychological treatment of depression in college students: a meta-analysis. Depress Anxiety. (2016) 33:400–14. doi: 10.1002/da.22461.PSYCHOLOGICAL

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Bailey AP, Hetrick SE, Rosenbaum S, Purcell R, Parker AG. Treating depression with physical activity in adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Psychol Med. (2017) ;48:1068–83. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Harrer M, Adam SH, Baumeister H, Cuijpers P, Karyotaki E, Auerbach RP, et al. Internet Interventions for Mental Health in University students: a systematic review and meta-analysis (under review). Int J Methods Psychiatric Res. (2018) 26:e1759. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Khoury B, Lecomte T, Fortin G, Masse M, Therien P, Bouchard V, et al. Mindfulness-based therapy: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2013) 33:763–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Cramer H, Lauche R, Dobos G. Characteristics of randomized controlled trials of yoga: a bibliometric analysis. BMC Complement Alternat Med. (2014) 14:328. doi: 10.1186/1472-6882-14-328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Cramer H, Ward L, Saper R, Fishbein D, Dobos G, Lauche R. The safety of yoga: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am J Epidemiol. (2015) 182:281–93. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwv071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Elwy AR, Groessl EV, Eisen S, Riley K, Maya M, et al. A systematic scoping review of yoga intervention components and study quality. Am J Prevent Med. (2014) 47:220–32. doi: 10.1002/ar.20849.3D

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Zoogman S, Goldberg SB, Hoyt WT, Miller L. Mindfulness Interventions with Youth: A Meta-Analysis. Mindfulness. (2015) 6:290–302. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0260-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Huang J, Nigatu YT, Smail-Crevier R, Zhang X, Wang J. Interventions for common mental health problems among university and college students: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychiatric Res. (2018) 107:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD. The cochrane collaboration' s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Br Med J. (2011) 343:1–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Kabat-zinn J. Mindfulness for Beginners. Boulder, CO: Sounds True (2012).

Google Scholar

Keywords: tertiary education, meditation, yoga, mindfulness, anxiety, depression, stress, university

Citation: Breedvelt JJF, Amanvermez Y, Harrer M, Karyotaki E, Gilbody S, Bockting CLH, Cuijpers P and Ebert DD (2019) The Effects of Meditation, Yoga, and Mindfulness on Depression, Anxiety, and Stress in Tertiary Education Students: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychiatry 10:193. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00193

Received: 03 December 2018; Accepted: 18 March 2019;
Published: 24 April 2019.

Edited by:

Xenia Gonda, Semmelweis University, Hungary

Reviewed by:

Rafael Ferreira Garcia, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Veruska Andrea Dos Santos, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Copyright © 2019 Breedvelt, Amanvermez, Harrer, Karyotaki, Gilbody, Bockting, Cuijpers and Ebert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Josefien J. F. Breedvelt, josefienbreedvelt@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.