Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychiatry, 27 June 2019
Sec. Mood Disorders

Depression Reappraisal and Treatment Effect: Will Response Shift Help Improve the Estimation of Treatment Efficacy in Trials for Mood Disorders?

  • 1INSERM-U1246 SPHERE University of Nantes and University of Tours, Nantes, France
  • 2CHU Nantes, Department of Addictology and Psychiatry, Nantes, France
  • 3Laboratory “Movement, Interactions, Performance” (E.A. 4334), Faculty of Sport Sciences, University of Nantes, Nantes, France
  • 4INSERM-U1028, CNRS-UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ΨR2 Team, University of Lyon, CH Le Vinatier, Lyon, France
  • 5Department of Emergency Psychiatry, Edouard Herriot Hospital, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • 6CHU Nantes, Department of Methodology and Biostatistics, Nantes, France

Major depressive disorder (MDD) has the highest burden of all mental health disorders. It affects more than 300 million people worldwide and is a leading cause of disability. Beyond the classical focus on increased negative emotions, self-referential thoughts may play a crucial role in depression (1, 2). Indeed, individuals with MDD tend to interpret their experiences with a negative cognitive bias toward the self, have higher levels of self-consciousness in relation to depression severity, and have enhanced memory for one’s own negative personality traits (3, 4). Moreover, the neural architecture underpinning the tendency to overgeneralize self-blaming in MDD has recently been elucidated (5), and this aberrant self-referential processing may be an important factor in the mechanism of action of antidepressants (while shifting the focus from the self to the external environment and enhancing positive self-referential processing) (6).

Changes in self-perception and changes in the disease or treatment effect reappraisal over time may have an impact on self-assessment of MDD.

The most widely used self-report scale to measure depression and assess treatment efficacy is the BDI (Beck Depression Inventory). Its psychometric properties allow accurate discrimination between depressed and non-depressed populations and a good estimation of depression severity (7). Longitudinal data are often collected to assess changes in the BDI scores, and it is usually assumed that the scale measures MDD in the same way over time (longitudinal measurement invariance. However, the assumption of longitudinal measurement invariance may sometimes be compromised and impact conclusions (8).

Our belief is that treatment has a direct effect on outcomes but also an indirect impact via treatment-induced changes in appraisal in the interpretation of the items of the scale, or even the concept of being measured (depression). For example, Fokkema and colleagues (9) suggested, using confirmatory factor analysis on the sample of the National Institute Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program (n = 155), that participants got better at assessing their levels of depressive symptoms and have become also more aware of them after treatment.

This is supported by the recent demonstration, on the basis of influential randomized controlled trials in the field such as sequenced treatment alternatives to relieve depression (STAR*D) or Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA) studies (n = 3,509). These studies showed that the instruments analyzed in this report [Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression, Inventory of Depression Symptomatology (IDS)-Clinician rated, Quick Inventory of Depression Symptomatology, and IDS-Self Rated] did not assess a single underlying construct and did not measure the same (set of) construct(s) in the same way across time. This could be indicative of reconceptualization explained by changes in depression’s perception and meaning before and after treatment (10). Hence, we support the opinion that treatment effect (considered as “true change”) may be underestimated (or overestimated) due to a reappraisal of the disease and treatment impact.

This raises an important question regarding reliable and unbiased depression assessment since the influence of MDD and treatment on self-evaluation changes over time may lead to draw erroneous conclusions regarding treatment efficacy. If we study treatment efficacy using a method assuming no appraisal modifications over time, we might not optimally assess outcomes.

Response shift (RS) reflects the patients’ changes in the meaning of their self-evaluation [beyond the evaluation of the construct the patient-reported outcomes (PRO) aims to measure]. It allows disentangling true change and modification of self and disease perception over treatment course, and offers better understanding of those changes in PRO assessment (1113).

The study of RS can help to detect changes in patient’s reappraisal of his/her symptomatology and to identify patient’s subgroups that may be vulnerable to poor unchanged perceived improvement despite objective improvement observed by the clinician. RS has already been widely studied in quality of life research in several clinical studies in oncology or neurology, but to date there are very few empirical studies in MDD (14, 15).

RS is currently operationalized in three different forms:

i. Recalibration: change in the respondent’s internal standards of measurement (e.g., change in the interpretation of the response categories of items of the scale). For instance, after 6 months of treatment, “feeling guilty most of the time” does not correspond to the same level of guilt than at diagnosis. This can also occur when patients get better at assessing their level of depressive symptoms. For example, three recent studies (10, 16, 17) using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), which depicts relations among observed variables and latent variables (construct), demonstrated the impact of recalibration on self-rated depression levels. In a clinical trial comparing antidepressants and psychotherapy (n = 155), recalibration RS caused overestimation of the depressive symptomatology after treatment (i.e., underestimation of true change) as compared to pre-treatment assessment (10). Furthermore, the authors reported that patients who received a psychotherapy experienced RS to a greater extent than patients who received medication. Elhai and colleagues found clear recalibration signs suggesting underestimation of true change in a sample of 1,025 depressed adult patients (16). Wu and colleagues evidenced recalibration on BDI scores (primarily in negative attitude factor) in a sample of 320 adolescents but in an opposite direction (BDI-II factor scores underestimated in that study) (17).

ii. Reprioritization: change in the importance of different factors of depression, more precisely changes in relative importance of component domains (e.g., performance impairment becoming more important than sad mood) in constituting the target construct (depression). Reprioritization might occur when a subject experiences a particular life or health event that changes his/her priorities or the attention drawn to several particular symptoms rather than others. The results reported in the literature can be heterogeneous. While Fokkema and colleagues (9) and Elhai and colleagues (16) showed reprioritization, Wu and colleagues (17) did not. This discrepancy may be explained by the differing population samples with a more prominent perception of treatment effect as a relevant health event in clinical sample of depressed adults (in the two former studies) than in non-clinical student sample (in the latter).

iii. Reconceptualization: change in the patient’s definition of what is being measured (respondents redefine the concept of depression and which components are contained in the construct). For example, after depression remission, a patient considers the items related to previously unrecognized somatic factors as an important part of depression. Literature about reconceptualization in MDD is rare so that it is difficult to assume how it can influence true change in MDD trials.

Two main approaches have been proposed to detect and estimate RS:

1. Methods based on specific study design including either specific measurement tools such as the Quality of Life Appraisal Profile (QOLAP) or retrospective ratings of baseline assessment (thentest), and qualitative interviews (1820),

2. Statistical methods, including SEM, Item Response Theory (IRT), and Rasch Measurement Theory models (RMT), that can be used to analyze data and do not require a specific design (12, 21).

A recent scoping review (22) has shown that the most commonly used approaches were SEM and the thentest. SEM enables detecting all forms of RS (recalibration, reprioritization, reconceptualization). The thentest can detect recalibration RS thanks to a retrospective evaluation of a previous assessment, but is prone to recall bias. Other methods have been proposed such as IRT and RMT for recalibration and reprioritization RS detection at the item-level, or relative importance analysis based on logistic regression for reprioritization RS detection at the domain level (23). SEM operationalizes RS as changes in SEM parameters over time as follows: change in intercepts (uniform recalibration), residual variances (non-uniform recalibration), factor loadings (reprioritization), or factor structure (reconceptualization).

We wish to underline the value of RS analysis in former or future clinical data in order to estimate and account for under- or over-estimation of true change as well as to identify clusters of patients more prone to have a self-reported under- or over-estimation of their depressive symptomatology. Randomized clinical trial (RCT) methodology could be improved by integrating more baseline descriptive data to get more insight into patients’ experience over the treatment course using RS analyses. According to the model of Sprangers and Schwartz (11), RS usually depends on a catalyst (e.g., health event) on several individual’s characteristics, such as existing beliefs, expectations, goals, and psychological mechanisms (e.g., behavioral, cognitive, and affective coping processes). For example, paying attention to personal goals, through personal goal attainment scaling (GAS), may be of importance in RCT for MDD since goal definition and perception is relevant for MDD prognosis and is likely to be linked with RS (24, 25). Hence, it could be of value to collect variables related to underlying personality, potential malignant self-regard, insight, illness perception, and beliefs related to medical history (chronicity, resistance, etc.) to better interpret self-reported change and RS occurrence, if appropriate. One can assume that some patients with some types of personality are more prone to change their evaluation over time, while other may be more “persistent” in their evaluation, and that baseline coping style has a great influence on reappraisal process. After treatment, an analysis of lived experience perspective with qualitative approaches (e.g., semi-structured interviews) may help to find narrative descriptions reflecting and explaining changes in values and internal standard (26).

Moreover, studying RS effect (especially reprioritization or reconceptualization) may help in focusing on residual symptoms whose relative importance depends on the patient’s personal objectives and priorities and may change over time. Detection of reprioritization or reconceptualization RS may indeed highlight changes in the importance of some clinically relevant residual symptoms that may otherwise go undetected and increase risk of relapse. Eventually, RS may depend on the factors that are more sensitive to it (e.g., negative attitude, performance impairment, or self-blame) and on the type of therapeutic intervention (cognitive behavioral approach, medication, brain stimulation targeting specific networks). Investigating RS effect in clinical studies may help in identifying subgroups of patients in need for more support or more prone to benefit from some synergistic approaches (e.g., cognitive remediation and antidepressants) and thus contribute to precision psychiatry.

Author Contributions

SB and VS analyzed literature and wrote the manuscript. AV and VS afforded methodological expertise. EP, JB, JMV and AS gave their viewpoint as clinicians and researchers and contributed to manuscript improvement.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Zahn R, Lythe KE, Gethin JA, Green S, Deakin JF, Young AH, et al. The role of self-blame and worthlessness in the psychopathology of major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord (2015) 186:337–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Smith TW, Greenberg J. Depression and self-focused attention. Motiv Emot (1981) 5(4):323–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00992551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Baños R, Medina P, Pascual J. Explicit and implicit memory biases in depression and panic disorder. Behav Res Therap (2001) 39(1):61–74. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00158-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Green S, Lambon Ralph MA, Moll J, Zakrzewski J, Deakin JF, Grafman J, et al. The neural basis of conceptual-emotional integration and its role in major depressive disorder. Soc Neurosci (2013) 8:417–33. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.810171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Komulainen E, Heikkilä R, Nummenmaa L, Raij TT, Harmer CJ, Isometsä E, et al. Short-term escitalopram treatment normalizes aberrant self-referential processing in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord (2018) 236:222–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wang YP, Gorenstein C. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory-II: a comprehensive review. Braz J Psychiatry (2013) 35(4):416–31. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2012-1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Edwards MC, Houts CR, Wirth RJ. Measurement invariance, the lack thereof, and modeling change. Qual Life Res (2018) 27(7):1735–43. doi: 10.1007/s11136-017-1673-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fokkema M, Smits N, Kelderman H, Cuijpers P. Response shifts in mental health interventions: an illustration of longitudinal measurement invariance. Psychol Assess (2013) 25:520–31. doi: 10.1037/a0031669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Epskamp S, Schoevers RA, Tuerlinckx F, Borsboom D. Measuring depression over time … Or not? Lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychol Assess (2016) 28(11):1354–136. doi: 10.1037/pas0000275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sprangers MAG, Schwartz CE. Integrating response shift into health-related quality-of-life research: a theoretical model. Soc Sci Med (1999) 48:1507–15. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00045-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Oort FJ, Visser M, Sprangers M. Formal definitions of measurement bias and explanation bias clarify measurement and conceptual perspectives on response shift. J Clin Epidemiol (2009) 62:1126–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Verdam MGE, Oort FJ, Sprangers MAG. Structural equation modeling–based effect-size indices were used to evaluate and interpret the impact of response shift effects. J Clin Epidemiol (2017) 85:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2017.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sajobi TT, Speechley KN, Liang Z, Goodwin SW, Ferro MA, Wiebe S. Response shift in parents’ assessment of health-related quality of life of children with new-onset epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav (2017) 75:97–101. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Salmon M, Blanchin M, Rotonda C, Guillemin F, Sébille V. Identifying patterns of adaptation in breast cancer patients with cancer-related fatigue using response shift analyses at subgroup level. Cancer Med (2017) 6(11):2562–75. doi: 10.1002/cam4.1219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Elhai JD, Contractor AA, Biehn TL, Allen JG, Oldham J, Ford JD, et al. Changes in the Beck Depression Inventory-II’s underlying symptom structure over 1 month of inpatient treatment. J Nerv Ment Dis (2013) 201(5):371–6. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31828e1004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wu PC. Response shifts in depression intervention for early adolescents. J Clin Psychol (2016) 72(7):663–75. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rapkin BD, Schwartz CE. Toward a theoretical model of quality-of-life appraisal: implications of findings from studies of response shift. Health Qual Life Outcomes (2004) 2:14. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-2-14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Howard GS, Ralph KM, Gulanick NA, Maxwell SE, Nance DW, Gerber SK. Internal invalidity in pretest-posttest self-report evaluations and a re-evaluation of retrospective pretests. Appl Psychol Meas (1979) 3:1–23. doi: 10.1177/014662167900300101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Schwartz CE, Sprangers MAG. Methodological approaches for assessing response shift in longitudinal health-related quality-of-life research. Soc Sci Med (1999) 48:1531–48. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00047-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Guilleux A, Blanchin M, Vanier A, Guillemin F, Falissard B, Schwartz CE, et al. RespOnse Shift ALgorithm in Item response theory (ROSALI) for response shift detection with missing data in longitudinal patient-reported outcome studies. Qual Life Res (2015) 24:553–64. doi: 10.1007/s11136-014-0876-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sajobi TT, Brahmbatt R, Lix LM, Zumbo BD, Sawatzky R. Scoping review of response shift methods: current reporting practices and recommendations. Qual Life Res (2018) 27(5):1133–46. doi: 10.1007/s11136-017-1751-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lix LM, Sajobi TT, Sawatzky R, Liu J, Mayo NE, Huang Y, et al. Relative importance measures for reprioritization response shift. Qual Life Res (2013) 22(4):695–703. doi: 10.1007/s11136-012-0198-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Dickson JM, Moberly NJ, O’Dea C, Field M. Goal Fluency, Pessimism and disengagement in depression. PloS One (2016) 30:11(11). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Carver CS, Scheier MF. Scaling back goals and recalibration of the affect system are processes in normal adaptive self-regulation: understanding “response shift” phenomena. Soc Sci Med (2000) 50(12):1715–22. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00412-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Beeken RJ, Eiser C, Dalley C. Health-related quality of life in haematopoietic stem cell transplant survivors: a qualitative study on the role of psychosocial variables and response shifts. Qual Life Res (2011) 20(2):153–60. doi: 10.1007/s11136-010-9737-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, reappraisal, patient-reported outcomes, response shift, efficacy

Citation: Bulteau S, Sauvaget A, Vanier A, Vanelle J-M, Poulet E, Brunelin J and Sebille V (2019) Depression Reappraisal and Treatment Effect: Will Response Shift Help Improve the Estimation of Treatment Efficacy in Trials for Mood Disorders? Front. Psychiatry 10:420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00420

Received: 08 March 2019; Accepted: 28 May 2019;
Published: 27 June 2019.

Edited by:

Ciara McCabe, University of Reading, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Martina Di Simplicio, Imperial College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Bulteau, Sauvaget, Vanier, Vanelle, Poulet, Brunelin and Sebille. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Samuel Bulteau, sabulteau@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.