Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 February 2020
Sec. Sleep Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Childhood Sleep Assessment: Tools for Specific Populations View all 15 articles

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Measurements in Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Stphanie Bioulac,,*Stéphanie Bioulac1,2,3*Jacques Taillard,Jacques Taillard2,3Pierre Philip,,Pierre Philip1,2,3Patricia Sagaspe,Patricia Sagaspe1,3
  • 1CHU Pellegrin, Clinique du Sommeil, Bordeaux, France
  • 2Université de Bordeaux, Sommeil, Attention et Neuropsychiatrie, USR 3413, Bordeaux, France
  • 3CNRS, SANPSY, USR 3413, Bordeaux, France

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most commonly diagnosed neurodevelopmental disorder in childhood. It is a heterogeneous disorder in terms of clinical presentation that is probably due to the frequent occurrence of comorbidity. Children with ADHD more frequently report sleep disorders (notably delayed sleep phase syndrome) and excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) than typically developing children. The aim of this article is to propose a narrative review of the assessment of EDS in the context of ADHD with first a summary of the subjective and objective tools used to measure it. Secondly, perspectives in terms of electroencephalogram (EEG) markers and neurofeedback are proposed. Then, possibilities for new kinds of evaluation are discussed (virtual reality, ecological momentary assessment, etc.). Lastly, we discuss specific clinical situations with EDS in the context of ADHD as links with narcolepsy, the comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders, and the context of sluggish cognitive tempo.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most commonly diagnosed neurodevelopmental disorder in childhood with a worldwide estimated prevalence of about 5% (1). It is characterized by inappropriate levels of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (2). Follow-up studies have documented the persistence of ADHD into adulthood in 50% to 65% of cases (3).

Sleep disorders have been extensively investigated in patients with ADHD, and their prevalence is reported to be in the range of 25–55% (46). Children with ADHD commonly exhibit additional mental health and neurodevelopmental comorbidities (7). Taurines R (8) introduces the notion of “developmental comorbidity” to highlight the importance of considering the age- and development-dependent occurrence of comorbidity in this disorder. Concerning the chronological order of occurrence, he posits that psychiatric conditions may be present before the appearance of first definite ADHD symptoms (“pre-comorbidity,” such as temperament factors, sleep disturbance, autism spectrum disorders, and atopic eczema). This work underlines the importance of sleep in the context of ADHD.

The most dominant sleep disturbance observed in patients with ADHD is delayed sleep phase syndrome (DSPS) associated with difficulties in falling asleep, difficulties in awakening, and/or excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS), characterized by difficulties in maintaining adequate alertness for daily activities with sleep occurring unintentionally or at inappropriate times almost daily.

Children with ADHD report EDS and more sleep problems than typically developing children (9). Other studies confirmed this subjective EDS (1012) and notably described insufficient sleep on school days and during weekends and a higher level of EDS (13). Contrary to subjective data, there is significantly less evidence supporting group differences in objective measures of sleepiness (major findings in Table 1). Moreover, both meta-analyses of Cortese in 2006 and 2009 (9, 19) demonstrated that children with ADHD showed a tendency to be sleepier than controls during the daytime but without reaching pathological levels. Thus, while all these data point to the existence of objective EDS, the nature of this EDS remains to be determined. It might be a primary disorder or the consequence of other sleep disorders. Indeed, most studies with objective measures using the maintenance sleep latency test (MSLT) excluded patients with primary sleep disorders. An exception is the study by Prihodova (17), where 10 patients presented sleep-disordered breathing and 9 had periodic limb movements (PLMD) when asleep. Furthermore, the methodology used for the MSLT differed from one study to another (see Table 1). The mean age of the patients must also be taken into account, with younger patients in the work by Lecendreux (15) and older ones in the study by Golan (16). Indeed, a decrease in mean sleep latency with Tanner stages has been reported in healthy subjects. Moreover, even though patients were free of treatment and had stopped treatment at least 2 days before the test, there may have been an “after-effect” on sleepiness assessment. Since ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder, identification of its phenotypes would allow better understanding of its clinical diversity. Sleep and EDS are dimensions that may be involved in specific phenotypes.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Major findings of MSLT in studies with ADHD children compared to control children.

The aim of this article is to propose a narrative review of the assessment of EDS in the context of ADHD with first a summary of the subjective and objective tools used to measure it. Secondly, perspectives in terms of electroencephalogram (EEG) markers and neurofeedback are proposed. Then, possibilities for new kinds of evaluation are discussed [virtual reality, ecological momentary assessment (EMA), etc.]. Lastly, we discuss specific clinical situations with EDS in the context of ADHD as links with narcolepsy, the comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders, and the context of sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT).

Methods

We used the following search strategy. We considered papers examining the assessment of EDS in ADHD children. Only data published in English were included. We conducted a narrative review using the PubMed database up to June 2019 with the following keywords combination: “ADHD children” and (“sleepiness” or “excessive daytime sleepiness” or “hypoarousal”) and “assessment”. For perspectives, the search in PubMed used the following keywords: “ADHD children” and (“sleepiness” or “excessive daytime sleepiness” or “hypoarousal” or “EEG” or “arousal” or “vigilance”) or “neurofeedback”.

Assessment of Sleepiness in Children

Clinical Assessment

The clinical expression of EDS in children is very variable and differs from that exhibited by adults. Sleepiness can occur intermittently, often during passive activities such as reading and watching TV. At first, sleepy children may exhibit inattention, hyperactivity, or behavioral problems. This clinical presentation may mimic a patient with ADHD, which raises the issue of the differential diagnosis.

Clinical History

A clinical detailed history is crucial for the initial evaluation of a child with EDS (20, 21). First, the sleep history should include a review of the child’s wake/sleep schedule on weekdays, weekends, and during summer. It is important to report the child’s sleep habits and estimate his/her sleep quantity (total daily 24 h sleep time with naps), which may be compared and interpreted with the norms for age and overall level development (21). Secondly, the clinical presentation frequently provides enough data to suggest a sleep disorder. Symptoms should be screened for sleep-disordered breathing, circadian disorder, PLMD, narcolepsy symptoms, and medical causes of EDS (21). Finally, a detailed sleep diary recording at least 2 weeks of the child’s wake/sleep schedule confirms the information provided by the parents and the child. It is also important to probe the use of caffeine or other stimulants, and whether the child is taking a sedative. Sedentary habits such as watching TV, playing video games, and/or excessive snacking must be explored.

In the event of ADHD, it is important to explore the clinical history of the sleep symptoms and compare it with the prescription of stimulants. In fact, stimulants can induce insomnia but also can improve sleep disorders in the context of ADHD. Moreover, in a subgroup of children with ADHD taking methylphenidate, EDS increased a few hours after taking it. The study by Cockcroft K (22) showed that children with ADHD treated by methylphenidate demonstrated significantly higher levels of subjectively EDS 6 h after taking their stimulants both in the morning and in the afternoon.

Physical Examination

The physical examination is often normal in children with EDS. This examination can provide indications on sleep disorders such as sleep-disordered breathing in children, as in those with adenoid facies.

Subjective Evaluation Tools

There is no specific questionnaire to assess sleepiness for children with ADHD. The questionnaires adapted for children can be used in patients with ADHD.

Self-Reported Sleepiness Questionnaires

Even if EDS is a frequent symptom in sleep disorders, validated tools in pediatric populations are lacking. Three clinical rating scales have been used to assess sleepiness specifically: the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (23), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) (24), and the Pediatric Sleepiness Scale (11). The recent review by Benmedjahed (25) on the evaluation of sleepiness in children concluded that the ESS (24) or ESS versions modified for pediatric populations was the most frequently used measure of EDS. The ESS is the most commonly used measure of EDS. The eight questions assess sleep propensity in eight different daily-life activities. This questionnaire was validated in a population of adults with narcolepsy (26). Various modified versions of the EES are currently in use in pediatric populations with situations adapted to children. Melendres et al. (27) proposed a modified ESS (two items modified from the adult version to be more applicable to children), and Johns (28) proposed another modified version of this scale for children and adolescents called the ESS-CHAD. Scores range from 0 to 32. A score > 10 indicates the need for further evaluation.

The Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS) is a validated measure for assessing daytime sleepiness in children. It was developed to assess EDS in young school-age populations. It is an eight-item questionnaire evaluating subjective experiences of daytime sleepiness (items are scored from 0 to 4). Scores range from 0 to 32 (normal score <15, cutoff for sleepiness >20).

The Teacher’s Daytime Sleepiness Questionnaire (TSDQ) (29) is a teacher-reported measure. It is a six-item questionnaire rated on a three-point scale.

Self-Reported Sleep Questionnaires for Children

There are other questionnaires that assess sleep in children. In these questionnaires, some questions concern sleepiness. The most frequently used questionnaires are the following: the Sleep Disturbances Scale for children (30) (with the subscale excessive somnolence), the Sleep Disorder Inventory for children and adolescents (31), the Cleveland Adolescent Sleepiness Questionnaire (32), the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, and the Pediatric Sleep Questionnaire (PSQ) (12). The reviews by Moreira GA (33) and Benmedjahed (25) confirm the dearth of available validated measures for assessing EDS in pediatric populations.

Objective Measures

Multiple Sleep Latency Test

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) is the recommended test to assess objective sleepiness both adults and children (32). This test assesses the time it takes to fall asleep (objective by the first epoch scored as sleep) during the daytime. The MSLT instructs the subject to fall asleep (protocol defined in AASM Practice Parameters for Clinical Use of MSLT, 2005) (34) while lying in bed in a dark and quiet room for five 20 min periods spaced at 2 h intervals. It is technically feasible and provides clear results in developmentally normal children aged 5 years and older (3537), which showed that average MSLT sleep latency of 10 min or less is statistically significant for healthy children 5–16 years of age on bedtime and rise time schedules that allow at least 10 h of sleep per night.

As stated in these guidelines (36): “The MSLT, preceded by nocturnal PSG, is indicated in children suspected of having hypersomnia from causes other than narcolepsy to assess excessive sleepiness and to aid in differentiation from narcolepsy” (option level). Indeed, normative data indicate that children who were prepubertal or at early pubertal stages were less likely to fall asleep during the MSLT than older adolescents. These data suggest that the standard protocol may underestimate mild degrees of sleepiness (36). Thus, some researchers have modified the standard protocol by using 30 min nap opportunities instead of 20 min for prepubertal children, but more research is needed to confirm the impact of this increased time for nap opportunities (38, 39). In children with narcolepsy, the standard MSLT protocol appears adapted. The recent work from Pizza (40) exhibited that a mean sleep latency ≤8.2 min at the MSLT are valid and reliable markers for pediatric NT1 diagnosis. Nevertheless, no normative data for sleep latency have been found in the preschool population in whom daytime napping is customary.

Actigraphy

Actigraphy monitors rest/activity cycle by integrating the occurrence and degree of arm movement activity over time. It provides information about daytime activity, night/sleep activity (sleep patterns estimated by scoring algorithms) and activity circadian rhythm. This information can be useful in assessing sleep problems in adolescents with ADHD, especially to determine sufficient and insufficient sleep duration or stability or variability in sleep/wake patterns (41, 42). Martin-Martinez (43) propose a novel methodology for the automatic diagnosis of the combined type of ADHD based on nonlinear signal processing of actigraphy by combining a feature from the 24 h analysis, with features from the first part of sleep and the afternoon activity interval. Actigraphy cannot measure daytime sleepiness. The AASM recommendations suggest using actigraphy to assess pediatric patients with circadian rhythm sleep/wake disorder, to monitor total sleep time prior to testing with MSLT in pediatric patients with suspected central disorders of hypersomnolence and to estimate total sleep time in “adult” patients with suspected insufficient sleep syndrome. As it has been shown that children, especially boys, move during sleep more often than adults, actigraphy should be used with caution with the understanding that actigraphy has both strengths (multiday assessment, ability to detect sleep) and limitations (overestimation of wake during the sleep period) (44).

Perspectives in Terms of Evaluation of Sleepiness With EEG Characteristics and Related Treatment

EEG Characteristics

Sleepiness can be evaluated objectively by power density of waking EEG. The sleepier the subject, the more alpha (8–12 Hz) (45) and/or theta (4–8 Hz) (45, 46) EEG activity with eyes open will be evident, thus giving objective information about sleepiness. The evolution of theta/alpha power (6.25–9 Hz) of waking EEG across the 24 h day reflects the sleep homeostatic process (evolution of sleep pressure during 24 h) (47).

In children and adolescents with ADHD, the most dominant waking EEG features are increased power of slow waves and/or decreased power of fast waves recorded in resting state and over the fronto-central region in comparison with healthy children/adolescents [reviewed in (4850)]. These EEG abnormalities could represent a cortical “slowing,” characterized by an increase in theta band activity (4–7 Hz) and by an increase in delta band activity (0–4 Hz) coupled with a decrease in beta band activity (14–30 Hz) [reviewed in (4850)]. In adults with ADHD, the cortical “slowing” was maintained and was characterized by an increase in theta band activity in comparison with healthy adults (50).

This increase in theta band activity can reflect sleepiness (51) and/or cortical hypoarousal (5254) both in children/adolescents and adults with ADHD, thereby confirming the hypoarousal model described above (55, 56). However, using the skin conductance level (SCL) as an unusual marker of drowsiness, Barry (57) suggested that alpha activity is the major band associated with arousal and not theta or beta activity in patients with ADHD. Clarke et al. (58) demonstrated that a subgroup with excess beta activity was not hyperaroused (assessed by SCL) and that the theta-to-beta ratio (TBR) was not associated with arousal.

Other studies have investigated the TBR [ratio of theta band (4–7 Hz) power divided by beta band (13–30 Hz) power [reviewed by (48, 49)]. In children/adolescents with ADHD, this ratio is abnormally higher than in healthy subjects. The meta-analysis of Snyder (59) demonstrated that TBR is remarkably robust with an effect size of 3.08 and a sensitivity and specificity greater than 90%. In view of these results, the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approved the Neuropsychiatric EEG-Based Assessment Aid (NEBA) System (using a single electrode recording from central-midline location (Cz) and a ground electrode at the frontal-midline location (Fz) with eyes closed to obtain the TBR) as a diagnostic biomarker of ADHD in 2013. Recent studies did not confirm elevated TBR in adults with ADHD (60, 61) and suggest that it is age-dependent (50).

The review by Newson (50), which identified 65 studies in children and adults with ADHD, showed that the results differ greatly depending on the EEG power used (relative or absolute) and the recording conditions used (eyes open or closed). Consistency and validation scores in children were better in the eyes-closed condition with than with eyes open. Moreover, the studies analyzed absolute or relative power band, but the mean effect size was better with relative band than with absolute band (62, 63). Bussalb et al. (63) proposed a standardized method for calculating the TBR.

The heterogeneity in the EEG characteristics of these children could be related especially with the heterogeneity of the clinical profiles of ADHD. Already in 1996, Chabot and Serfontein (64) demonstrated three different EEG profiles in children with ADHD: one with a generalized increase in theta/alpha activity, another with an increase in alpha activity, and a third small group with an increase in beta activity.

Clarke et al. (65) identified three subgroups: the cortical hypoarousal group with elevated theta waves and decreased beta waves was coupled with symptoms of delinquent behaviors, the maturational lag group with elevated slow-wave (delta and theta) activity and decreased alpha waves, and the hyperarousal group with excessive beta activity was coupled with markers of ritualistic obsessive behaviors. These three subgroups were replicated, and an additional group was subsequently added (increased alpha and beta band power) (66).

Loo et al. (67), in a large sample of children, identified five subgroups of children with ADHD according to their resting EEG characteristics. Those with elevated slow-wave activity (delta and theta band) had higher levels of externalizing behaviors and cognitive deficits. Latent subgroups with elevated alpha and beta power had higher levels of internalizing behaviors, emotion dysregulation, and intact cognitive functioning. They demonstrated that there was no resting EEG abnormality that specifically characterizes children with ADHD or without ADHD and that there are age and gender differences in resting EEG characteristics (68). Boys with the combined subtype had higher theta power and lower alpha power than those with the inattentive type (68). Loo et al. (51) reported that the TBR was higher in the combined subtype than in the attentive subtype. Recently, Bussalb et al. (63) demonstrated two distinct subgroups in 363 children: one characterized by an elevated TBR and the other with a normal TBR.

The results analyzing resting state cortical activity in patients with ADHD are very heterogeneous, and the identification of distinct EEG subgroups is not consensual. Nevertheless, since EEG analysis is considered to be the gold standard to quantify sleepiness, evidence is accumulating of a subgroup of patients characterized by cortical hypoarousal.

In a different approach, the group of Hegerl (69, 70) using an automatic resting EEG classification of sleepiness [Vigilance Algorithm Leipzig software (VIGALL, seven EEG-vigilance stages)] demonstrated unstable arousal regulation in children and adults with ADHD. This arousal instability was characterized by a faster decline to the low EEG-vigilance stages and more fluctuations in their stages of vigilance, i.e., higher number of stage switches. A vigilant state instability [unstable vigilant state that fluctuates (71)] is observed in healthy sleep-deprived subjects: increasing sleep drive brings about escalating state instability in attention (the effort to recover and maintain attention increases), making neurobehavioral performance increasingly variable. This state instability could explain the higher sleepiness observed in patients with ADHD.

Despite years of research and the identification of clusters, quantitative EEG is not helpful in classifying ADHD patients and cannot be considered as a diagnostic biomarker for ADHD. However, the identification of EEG subgroups may help to optimize treatments (stimulant and non-stimulant treatment, neurofeedback, exercise, and dietary intervention) and/or measure the response to them. Indeed, stimulant treatment was more efficient in the hypoarousal subgroup [for review, see Kirkland (72)] or unstable alertness (70) and was not efficient in ADHD patients with a slowed individual alpha peak frequency (73).

Treatment With Neurofeedback

Neurofeedback training (NFT) analyzes EEG activity and transforms it into instant feedback (visual and/or auditory signal) received by the user. By learning, the user self-regulates the amplitude of specific EEG activity. Neurofeedback is considered a non-pharmacological treatment option for ADHD [for review, see van Doren (74)].

The efficacy of NFB for children with ADHD is controversial. Meta-analyses published in the past decade have been contradictory (i.e., 7578). For some authors like Thibault (75), the mechanisms involved raise questions. They suggest that neurofeedback could be an especially powerful form of placebo intervention, a kind of “superplacebo.” The most common NFT protocols in the treatment of ADHD are theta/beta training, and sensorimotor rhythm (SMR, increase activation at 12–15 Hz) and slow cortical potential (SCP) protocols. SCP neurofeedback has been compared to alpha-enhancement neurofeedback. Theta/beta training aims at reinforcing reductions in theta activity and increases in beta activity and could be more appropriate and efficient in the hypoarousal subgroup of patients with ADHD (76). SMR aims to decrease delta and beta activity. In light of the hyperarousal theory of insomnia, SMR reduces the symptoms of insomnia, especially the hyperarousal. SMR could be more appropriate and efficient in the hyperarousal subgroup of patients with ADHD. Bussalb et al. (63), who, in analyzing TBR, differentiated two distinct subgroups of ADHD patients, consider that neurofeedback should be divided into two subgroups: TBR downward and SMR upward. In all cases (diagnostic biomarker, prognostic biomarker, and/or biomarker of therapeutic response), more research is needed before EEG activity can be applied in clinical settings.

Neuropsychological Markers: Links Between Attention and Sleepiness

Attentional Process and Arousal Level

Arousal and attention are heterogeneous processes that interact with each other (77).

On one hand, attention corresponds to the appropriate allocation of processing resources to relevant stimuli. Attention is sub-divided into: (a) attentional orientation (the simple direction of attention to a particular stimulus); (b) selective (or focused) attention (giving attentional priority to one stimulus instead of another); (c) divided attention (dividing attention between two or more different stimuli); and (d) sustained attention (attending to one stimulus over an increasing period of time). On the other hand, arousal can be defined as the state of physiological reactivity, ranging on a continuum from sleep to alert wakefulness. Sleepiness increases the probability of the transition from wakefulness to sleep.

The arousal level controlled by homeostatic sleep pressure and the waking systems has repercussions on the attentional system, which modulates cognitive components such as executive functioning. The fundamental biological rhythm of sleep/wakefulness controlling the level of arousal is essential for maintaining optimal brain functions. Normal sleep ensures alert wakefulness, which is essential for appropriate decision-making and optimal adaptation to the environment, and is beneficial for global cognition (78).

The level of arousal can have an impact on cognitive functioning, in particular on attention and executive functions, via the modulation of the level of vigilance. Vigilance is a state of high efficiency of the central nervous system and is considered in a behavioral perspective as sufficient preparedness to detect and respond to critical events that occur rarely, and which are difficult to discriminate in extremely monotonous situations (e.g., detection tasks) (79). Vigilance could be the behavioral correlate of physiological alertness.

Links With Executive Functions

This hypothesis echoes the model of attention proposed by Van Zomeren and Brouwer (80), which is widely used in neuropsychology and distinguishes an “intensity” component (vigilance/alert) and a “selectivity” component (selective attention and divided). In this model, the alert component refers to the ability to respond quickly and appropriately to the demands of the environment. It can be linked with the mobilization of energy so that the nervous system responds better. Tonic alert corresponds to a general state of arousal, which varies during the day. Phasic alert relates to the voluntary capacity to rapidly increase the general level of attention to anticipate an expected event. This vigilance/alert component modulates attention, which is a prerequisite to any other cognitive function. Thus, the influence of arousal level on cognitive functioning operates through the influence of the intensity component on component selectivity. This vigilance/alert component via modulation of attention also impacts executive functions. Executive functions govern all cognitive functions necessary to control and execute complex non-routine activities (81). Executive functions are a set of processes involving flexibility, decision-making, inhibition, planning, coordination, and control of thoughts and actions. Their main function is to facilitate the adaptation of the subject to the requirements and sudden fluctuations of the environment, especially when faced with new situations for which the action routines are no longer sufficient. Impairment of executive functioning results in disturbances in the complex activities of daily living.

New Dimensional Tools to Assess the Impact of Sleepiness on Cognitive Functions

Impairments in sleep and alertness frequently occur in children with ADHD (19). Moreover, children and adolescents with ADHD often exhibit symptoms of inattention. Cognitive disorders affecting vigilance, attention, cognitive control, and executive functions are usually observed (8285), leading to difficulties in performing complex daily life activities (86). Daily life is punctuated by a variety of complex activities related to different areas for children and adolescents (education, pedestrian safety), which all require adequate levels of alertness and attention.

Therefore, the clinical evaluation of sleepiness in ADHD could be achieved in the future by measuring the influence of arousal level on cognitive functioning (vigilance, attention, executive functions), including by measuring these functions when performing complex activities of daily living. The measurement of the level of sleepiness could be considered from a physiological to a psychological–behavioral sphere.

Neuropsychological assessment, notably involving vigilance, could be informative about the level of sleepiness. Calhoun, Fernandez-Mendoza, et al. (87) showed, in a large general population of young children, that parent-reported EDS was associated with impairment in cognitive and behavioral functioning. The Continuous Performance Test (CPT) is the most widely used task to assess vigilance, attention and impulsivity in children with ADHD (88). The KITAP (Attention Assessment Battery—Child Version) allows assessment of the attentional and executive performance of school-age children with validated standardized tests. The tests allow the evaluation of the “intensity” component of attention such as alertness and sustained attention, and the “selectivity” component such as selective attention and divided attention, as well as executive functions such as mental flexibility or inhibition.

An innovative approach could be to use the technology of virtual reality to determine the level of sleepiness as a function of cognitive alteration in ADHD children and adolescents immersed in the ecological tasks of everyday life. A study using a simulated driving task showed that not only the objective level of alertness but also inhibitory control deficits contribute independently to highway driving impairment in adults with ADHD (86). Virtual reality simulation is a technology that could allow the influence of arousal level on cognitive functioning (attention and executive functions) to be measured through vigilance and its impact on complex activities of daily life. In parallel, the monitoring of eyelid frequency and pupil diameter (eye-tracking system) or facial dynamic changes (89) could be promising (90).

Another innovative approach could be the use of EMA to evaluate daily and diurnal variations in sleepiness and repercussions on daily life such as in the emotional, cognitive, behavioral, social, and physical domains (91, 92) in ADHD.

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Children With ADHD

The links between ADHD and sleep disorders remain unclear. While specific sleep disorders are a frequent comorbid condition associated with ADHD according to a categorical approach, they can also induce ADHD-like symptoms if considered dimensionally and are thought to be the consequence of EDS. Given the complexity of the interaction between ADHD and sleep, the team of Miano (9395) identified five sleep phenotypes in ADHD: a sleep phenotype characterized by a hypo-arousal state “narcolepsy-like,” another associated with delayed sleep onset latency, a phenotype related with sleep-disordered breathing, another associated with restless legs syndrome and/or PLMD, and a phenotype related to epilepsy/or EEG interictal discharges.

Links With Narcolepsy

Narcolepsy, which is characterized by EDS and abnormal REM sleep, is a chronic sleep disorder caused by a deficiency of hypocretin/orexin-producing neurons within the lateral hypothalamus (type 1 narcolepsy with cataplexy). In type 2 narcolepsy, where cataplexy is absent and orexin levels normal, the physiopathology is unknown. The “narcolepsy-like” phenotype (ADHD and hypersomnia) described by Miano could be part of a subtype of type 2 narcolepsy (96). A body of evidence reinforces the links between ADHD and narcolepsy, and ADHD symptoms are more frequent in children (97) or adults (98) with narcolepsy than in control subjects. A systematic review demonstrated that the prevalence of ADHD symptoms in narcolepsy was >30% (99).

In addition, adult narcoleptics present more childhood ADHD than controls (100, 101). Using MSLT or EEG features, some authors have hypothesized the existence of a dysfunction in arousal in the pathophysiology in ADHD (15, 65, 100). A physiopathological model for ADHD involves a deficit in arousal (hypo-arousal model). Children with ADHD have greater difficulty in falling asleep, awakening, and/or maintaining adequate daytime alertness than control children, and use hyperactivity as a strategy to stay awake and alert in order to counteract the tendency to fall asleep (55, 56).

Moreover, high levels of EDS and shorter REM latency are classically both potential signs of narcolepsy (102). In the study by Diaz-Roman (10), polysomnographic recordings showed that children with ADHD presented significantly greater general sleep problems than control subjects and shorter REM latency in the ADHD group. Nevertheless, other studies failed to find differences in REM latency between children with and without ADHD in polysomnography (PSG) (9, 103). A recent study found that serum orexin A levels were significantly lower in drug-naive children with ADHD, especially in the attention deficit dominant subgroup (104). As suggested by Cortese (105), orexin neurons located in the perifornical and dorsomedial hypothalamic areas (implicated in arousal) could be hypoactivated, while those located in the lateral hypothalamus (involved in reward processing, stimulating feeding, and other reward-seeking behaviors) could be overactivated in patients with ADHD.

Specific Clinical Situations

“Sluggish cognitive tempo” (SCT) is a behavioral construct characterized by a set of symptoms as sluggish, under-motivated, lethargic, slowed, and/or forgetful behavior (106, 107). This condition is strongly associated with ADHD (notably with the inattentive subtype), although the meta-analysis by Becker et al. (106) identified SCT symptoms that were distinguishable from ADHD inattentive symptoms. Some authors hypothesized that SCT symptoms are specifically related with sleep problems and EDS (106, 108, 109). SCT symptoms were found to overlap with EDS in patients with ADHD but were distinct from sleep problems. Objective assessments of EDS may help to better phenotype these subjects and to propose other therapeutic strategies, to the extent that this condition predicts non-response or poorer response to methylphenidate in children with ADHD (110). Pharmacological treatment such as wakefulness drugs might be more indicated in this subgroup of patients.

Comorbidity

Moreover, SCT is strongly associated with internalizing symptoms, especially depressive symptoms. It is important to evaluate sleep in the context of comorbidity (internalizing and externalizing disorders), as mentioned by Spruyt (111) and by Diaz-Roman (10). Comorbid conditions may also play a role in EDS in children. Diaz-Roman (10) found significant correlations between psychopathology and sleep measures with a significant correlation between scores on the Child Behavior Checklist (a questionnaire completed by the parents which evaluates internalizing and externalizing problems) and EDS and general sleep problems. This finding was in line with previous data supporting the link between sleep problems and children with ADHD with comorbid anxiety and depression (112), and with a study by Mulraney M et al. (113) who reported links between emotional problems and sleep disturbances in ADHD. When assessing sleep in children with ADHD, it is also necessary to evaluate other psychiatric comorbidities, notably internalizing problems such as depression and anxiety that seem to play a role in the expression of EDS in these children.

In conclusion, ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder in terms of its clinical presentation and underlying physiopathological mechanisms. EDS, a usual complaint in children with ADHD, seems to be a pertinent clinical dimension. Thus, the identification of a specific “sleepy ADHD” phenotype appears to be both a clinical and objective maker. The identification of EEG subgroups would help in defining various phenotypes and optimizing treatments. More studies are needed in children with ADHD to assess EDS. The interactions between EDS and the attentional process and executive functions should also be probed.

Author Contributions

SB wrote sections Introduction and Assessment of Sleepiness in Children of the manuscript. JT wrote section Perspectives in Terms of Evaluation of Sleepiness With EEG Characteristics and Related Treatment of the manuscript. PS wrote section Neuropsychological Markers: Links Between Attention and Sleepiness of the manuscript. PP wrote the Introduction and did a rereading of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieling C, Rohde LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol (2014) 43(2):434–42. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

3. Lara C, Fayyad J, de Graaf R, Kessler RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Angermeyer M, et al. Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 65(1):46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hvolby A. Associations of sleep disturbance with ADHD: implications for treatment. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord (2015) 7(1):1–18. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0151-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Yoon SY, Jain U, Shapiro C. Sleep in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: past, present, and future. Sleep Med Rev (2012) 16(4):371–88. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cortese S, Brown TE, Corkum P, Gruber R, O'Brien LM, Stein M, et al. Assessment and management of sleep problems in youths with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2013) 52(8):784–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Larson K, Russ SA, Kahn RS, Halfon N. Patterns of comorbidity, functioning, and service use for US children with ADHD, 2007. Pediatrics (2011) 127(3): 462–70. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Taurines R, Schmitt J, Renner T, Conner AC, Warnke A, Romanos M. Developmental comorbidity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord (2010) 2(4):267–89. doi: 10.1007/s12402-010-0040-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cortese S, Faraone SV, Konofal E, Lecendreux M. Sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of subjective and objective studies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2009) 48(9):894–908. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Diaz-Roman A, Buela-Casal G. Shorter REM latency in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res (2019) 278:188–93. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Drake C, Nickel C, Burduvali E, Roth T, Jefferson C, Pietro B. The pediatric daytime sleepiness scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school outcomes in middle-school children. Sleep (2003) 26(4):45–58.

Google Scholar

12. Chervin RD, Hedger K, Dillon JE, Pituch KJ. Pediatric sleep questionnaire (PSQ): validity and reliability of scales for sleep-disordered breathing, snoring, sleepiness, and behavioral problems. Sleep Med (2000) 1(1):21–32. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(99)00009-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Becker SP, Epstein JN, Tamm L, Tilford AA, Tischner CM, Isaacson PA, et al. shortened sleep duration causes sleepiness, inattention, and oppositionality in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: findings from a crossover sleep restriction/extension study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2019) 58(4):433–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.09.439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Palm L, Persson E, Bjerre I, Elmqvist D, Blennow G. Sleep and wakefulness in preadolescent children with deficits in attention, motor control and perception. Acta Paediatr (1992) 81(8):618–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

15. Cortese S, Konofal E, Yateman N, Mouren MC, Lecendreux M. Sleep and alertness in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review of the literature. Sleep (2006) 29(4):504–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Prihodova I, Paclt I, Kemlink D, Skibova J, Ptacek R, Nevsimalova S. Sleep disorders and daytime sleepiness in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a two-night polysomnographic study with a multiple sleep latency test. Sleep Med (2010) 11(9):922–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lecendreux M, Konofal E, Bouvard M, Falissard B, Mouren-Simeoni MC. Sleep and alertness in children with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2000) 41(6):803–12. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Wiebe S, Carrier J, Frenette S, Gruber R. Sleep and sleepiness in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and controls. J Sleep Res (2013) 22(1):41–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Golan N, Shahar E, Ravid S, Pillar G. Sleep disorders and daytime sleepiness in children with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder. Sleep (2004) 27(2):261–6. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Palm L, Persson E, Elmqvist D, Blennow G. Sleep and wakefulness in normal preadolescent children. Sleep (1989) 12(4):299–308. doi: 10.1093/sleep/12.4.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kothare SV, Pomeroy SL. Pediatric sleep medicine in 2008: state-of-the-art. Curr Opin Pediatr (2008) 20(6):639–40. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0b013e328317ee99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hoban TF, Chervin RD. Assessment of sleepiness in children. Semin Pediatr Neurol (2001) 8(4):216–28. doi: 10.1053/spen.2001.29043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Cockcroft K, Ashwal J, Bentley A. Sleep and daytime sleepiness in methylphenidate medicated and un-medicated children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Afr J Psychiatry (Johannesbg) (2009) 12(4):27–59. doi: 10.4314/ajpsy.v12i4.49043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement WC. Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology (1973) 10(4):431–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep (1991) 14(6):540–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Benmedjahed K, Wang YG, Lambert J, Evans C, Hwang S, Black J, et al. Assessing sleepiness and cataplexy in children and adolescents with narcolepsy: a review of current patient-reported measures. Sleep Med (2017) 32:14–39. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.12.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Melendres MC, Lutz JM, Rubin ED, Marcus CL. Daytime sleepiness and hyperactivity in children with suspected sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics (2004) 114(3):768–75. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Johns MW. Sensitivity and specificity of the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT), the maintenance of wakefulness test and the epworth sleepiness scale: failure of the MSLT as a gold standard. J Sleep Res (2000) 9(1):5–11. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00177.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Johns M. The assessment of sleepiness in children and adolescents. Sleep Biol Rhythm (2015) 13(Suppl. 1):97.

Google Scholar

30. Owens JA, Maxim R, Nobile C, McGuinn M, Msall M. Parental and self-report of sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med (2000) 154(6):549–55. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.154.6.549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bruni O, Ottaviano S, Guidetti V, Romoli M, Innocenzi M, Cortesi F, et al. The Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC). Construction and validation of an instrument to evaluate sleep disturbances in childhood and adolescence. J Sleep Res (1996) 5(4):251–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1996.00251.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Luginbuehl M, Kohler WC. Screening and evaluation of sleep disorders in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am (2009) 18(4):825–38. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Spilsbury JC, et al. The Cleveland adolescent sleepiness questionnaire: a new measure to assess excessive daytime sleepiness in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med (2007) 3(6):603–12. doi: 10.1037/t65526-000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Moreira GA, Pradella-Hallinan M. Sleepiness in children: an update. Sleep Med Clin (2017) 12(3):407–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2017.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Littner MR, Kushida C, Wise M, Davila DG, Morgenthaler T, Lee-Chiong T, et al. Practice parameters for clinical use of the multiple sleep latency test and the maintenance of wakefulness test. Sleep (2005) 28(1):113–21. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.1.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Aurora RN, Lamm CI, Zak RS, Kristo DA, Bista SR, Rowley JA, et al. Practice parameters for the non-respiratory indications for polysomnography and multiple sleep latency testing for children. Sleep (2012) 35(11):1467–73. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Aurora RN, Kristo DA, Bista SR, Rowley JA, Zak RS, Casey KR, et al. The treatment of restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movement disorder in adultsan update for 2012: practice parameters with an evidence-based systematic review and meta-analyses: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Clinical Practice Guideline. Sleep (2012) 35(8):1039–62. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Fallone G, Seifer R, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. How well do school-aged children comply with imposed sleep schedules at home? Sleep (2002) 25(7):739–45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.7.739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Gozal D, Wang M, Pope DW Jr. Objective sleepiness measures in pediatric obstructive sleep apnea. Pediatrics (2001) 108(3):693–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.3.693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Pizza F, Barateau L, Jaussent I, Vandi S, Antelmi E, Mignot E, et al. Validation of multiple sleep latency test for the diagnosis of pediatric narcolepsy type 1. Neurology (2019) 93(11):e1034–44. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Becker SP, Langberg JM, Eadeh HM, Isaacson PA, Bourchtein E. Sleep and daytime sleepiness in adolescents with and without ADHD: differences across ratings, daily diary, and actigraphy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2019) 60(9):1021–31. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Langberg JM, Breaux RP, Cusick CN, Green CD, Smith ZR, Molitor SJ, et al. Intraindividual variability of sleep/wake patterns in adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2019) 60(11):1219–29. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Martin-Martinez D, Casaseca-de-la-Higuera P, Alberola-Lopez S, Andres-de-Llano J, Lopez-Villalobos JA, Ardura-Fernandez J, et al. Nonlinear analysis of actigraphic signals for the assessment of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Med Eng Phys (2012) 34(9):1317–29. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2011.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Meltzer LJ, Wong P, Biggs SN, Traylor J, Kim JY, Bhattacharjee R, et al. Validation of actigraphy in middle childhood. Sleep (2016) 39(6):1219–24. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci (1990) 52(1–2):29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Strijkstra AM, Beersma DG, Drayer B, Halbesma N, Daan S. Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8-12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4-8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram. Neurosci Lett (2003) 340(1):17–20. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00033-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cajochen C, Brunner DP, Krauchi K, Graw P, Wirz-Justice A. Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep (1995) 18(10):890–4. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.10.890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Newson JJ, Thiagarajan TC. EEG Frequency Bands in psychiatric disorders: a review of resting state studies. Front Hum Neurosci (2018) 12:521. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Loo SK, Makeig S. Clinical utility of EEG in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a research update. Neurotherapeutics (2012) 9(3):569–87. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0131-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Satterfield JH, Cantwell DP, Saul RE, Yusin A. Intelligence, academic achievement, and EEG abnormalities in hyperactive children. Am J Psychiatry (1974) 131(4):391–5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

51. Mann CA, Lubar JF, Zimmerman AW, Miller CA, Muenchen RA. Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatr Neurol (1992) 8(1):30–6. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(92)90049-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lubar JF. Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedback for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Biofeedback Self Regul (1991) 16(3):201–25. doi: 10.1007/BF01000016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Brown TE, McMullen WJ Jr. Attention deficit disorders and sleep/arousal disturbance. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2001) 931:271–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05784.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Weinberg WA, Harper CR, Brumback RA. Neuroanatomic substrate of developmental specific learning disabilities and select behavioral syndromes. J Child Neurol (1995) 10 Suppl 1:S78–80. doi: 10.1177/08830738950100S115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Barry RJ, Clarke AR, Johnstone SJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. Electroencephalogram theta/beta ratio and arousal in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence of independent processes. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66(4):398–401. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, Dupuy FE, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Johnstone SJ. Excess beta activity in the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a disorder of arousal? Int J Psychophysiol (2013) 89(3):31–49. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Snyder SM, Hall JR. A meta-analysis of quantitative EEG power associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Neurophysiol (2006) 23(5):440–55. doi: 10.1097/01.wnp.0000221363.12503.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Markovska-Simoska S, Pop-Jordanova N. Quantitative EEG in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: comparison of absolute and relative power spectra and Theta/Beta ratio. Clin EEG Neurosci (2017) 48(1):20–32. doi: 10.1177/1550059416643824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Saad JF, Kohn MR, Clarke S, Lagopoulos J, Hermens DF. Is the Theta/Beta EEG marker for ADHD inherently Flawed? J Atten Disord (2018) 22(9):815–26. doi: 10.1177/1087054715578270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Boutros N, Fraenkel L, Feingold A. A four-step approach for developing diagnostic tests in psychiatry: EEG in ADHD as a test case. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2005) 17(4):455–64. doi: 10.1176/jnp.17.4.455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bussalb A, Collin S, Barthelemy Q, Ojeda D, Bioulac S, Blasco-Fontecilla H, et al. Is there a cluster of high theta-beta ratio patients in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Clin Neurophysiol (2019) 130(8):1387–96. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Chabot RJ, Serfontein G. Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biol Psychiatry (1996) 40(10):951–63. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00576-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. Excess beta activity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an atypical electrophysiological group. Psychiatry Res (2001) 103(2–3):205–18. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00277-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, Dupuy FE, Heckel LD, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, et al. Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Clin Neurophysiol (2011) 122(7):1333–41. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Loo SK, McGough JJ, McCracken JT, Smalley SL. Parsing heterogeneity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder using EEG-based subgroups. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2018) 59(3):223–31. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Dupuy FE, Barry RJ, Clarke AR, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. Sex differences between the combined and inattentive types of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an EEG perspective. Int J Psychophysiol (2013) 89(3):320–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Strauss M, Ulke C, Paucke M, Huang J, Mauche N, Sander C, et al. Brain arousal regulation in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatry Res (2018) 261:102–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Sander C, Arns M, Olbrich S, Hegerl U. EEG-vigilance and response to stimulants in paediatric patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol (2010) 121(9):1511–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Doran SM, Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol (2001) 139(3):253–67.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

70. Kirkland AE, Holton KF. Measuring treatment response in pharmacological and lifestyle interventions using electroencephalography in ADHD: a review. Clin EEG Neurosci (2019) 50(4):256–66. doi: 10.1177/1550059418817966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Arns M, Gunkelman J, Breteler M, Spronk D. EEG phenotypes predict treatment outcome to stimulants in children with ADHD. J Integr Neurosci (2008) 7(3):421–38. doi: 10.1142/S0219635208001897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Van Doren J, Arns M, Heinrich H, Vollebregt MA, Strehl U, K.L S. Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2019) 28(3):293–305. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Thibault RT, Lifshitz M, Raz A. Neurofeedback or neuroplacebo? Brain (2017) 140(4):862–4. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Arns M, Heinrich H, Strehl U. Evaluation of neurofeedback in ADHD: the long and winding road. Biol Psychol (2014) 95:108–15. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Coull JT. Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Prog Neurobiol (1998) 55(4):343–61. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sagaspe P, Taillard J, Akerstedt T, Bayon V, Espié S, Chaumet G, et al. Extended driving impairs nocturnal driving performances. PloS One (2008) 3(10):e34–93. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Mackworth JF. Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. Psychol Rev (1968) 75(4):308–22. doi: 10.1037/h0025896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Van Zomeren AH, Brouwer WH. New York: Oxford University Press (1994).

Google Scholar

79. Rabbitt P. Methodology of frontal and executive function. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis (1997).

Google Scholar

80. Barkley RA, Grodzinsky G, DuPaul GJ. Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: a review and research report. J Abnormal Child Psychol (1992) 20(2):163–88. doi: 10.1007/BF0091654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Schachar R, Mota VL, Logan GD, Tannock R, Klim P. Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Abnormal Child Psychol (2000) 28(3):227–35. doi: 10.1023/a:1005140103162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Nigg JT. Neuropsychologic theory and findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the state of the field and salient challenges for the coming decade. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57(11):1424–35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Lecendreux M, Cortese S. Sleep problems associated with ADHD: a review of current therapeutic options and recommendations for the future. Expert Rev Neurother (2007) 7(12):1799–806. doi: 10.1586/14737175.7.12.1799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Bioulac S, Sagaspe P, Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Altena E, Taillard J, Schröder C, et al. Objective level of alertness and inhibitory control predict highway driving impairment in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord (2016). doi: 10.1177/1087054716633751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Calhoun SL, Mendoza JF, Vgontzas AN, Mayes SD, Tsaoussoglou M, Rodriguez-Munoz A. Learning, attention/hyperactivity, and conduct problems as sequelae of excessive daytime sleepiness in a general population study of young children. Sleep (2012) 35(5):627–32. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Huang-Pollock CL, Karalunas SL, Tam H, Moore AN. Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: a meta-analysis of CPT performance. J Abnorm Psychol (2012) 121(2):360–71. doi: 10.1037/a0027205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Poursadeghiyan M, Mazloumi A, Nasl Saraji G, Niknezhad A, Akbarzadeh A, Ebrahimi MH. Determination the levels of subjective and observer rating of drowsiness and their associations with facial dynamic changes. Iran J Public Health (2017) 46(1):93–102.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

88. Rhle KH, Franke KJ, Nilius G. [Microsleep, sleepiness and driving performance in patients with sleep apnoea syndrome]. Pneumologie (2008) 62(10):595–601. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1038203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Kaplan KA, Talavera DC, Harvey AG. Rise and shine: a treatment experiment testing a morning routine to decrease subjective sleep inertia in insomnia and bipolar disorder. Behav Res Ther (2018) 111:106–12. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Dong L, Gumport NB, Martinez AJ, Harvey AG. Is improving sleep and circadian problems in adolescence a pathway to improved health? A Mediation Anal J Consult Clin Psychol (2019) 87(9):757–71. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Miano S, Amato N, Foderaro G, Pezzoli V, Ramelli GP, Toffolet L, et al. Sleep phenotypes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sleep Med (2019) 60:123–31. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Miano S, Parisi P, Villa MP. The sleep phenotypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the role of arousal during sleep and implications for treatment. Med Hypotheses (2012) 79(2):147–53. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2012.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Miano S, Peraita-Adrados R. Pediatric insomnia: clinical, diagnosis, and treatment. Rev Neurol (2014) 58(1):35–42. doi: 10.33588/rn.5801.2013398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Ito W, Komada Y, Okajima I, Inoue Y. Excessive daytime sleepiness in adults with possible attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a web-based cross-sectional study. Sleep Med (2017) 32:4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Lecendreux M, Lavault S, Lopez R, Inocente CO, Konofal E, Cortese S, et al. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms in pediatric narcolepsy: a cross-sectional study. Sleep (2015) 38(8):1285–95. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Filardi M, Pizza F, Tonetti L, Antelmi E, Natale V, Plazzi G. Attention impairments and ADHD symptoms in adult narcoleptic patients with and without hypocretin deficiency. PloS One (2017) 12(8):e0182085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Kim MJ, Park I, Lim MH, Paik KC, Cho S, Kwon HJ, et al. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and its comorbidity among korean children in a community population. J Korean Med Sci (2017) 32(3):401–6. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2017.32.3.401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Modestino EJ, Winchester J. A retrospective survey of childhood ADHD symptomatology among adult narcoleptics. J Atten Disord (2013) 17(7):574–82. doi: 10.1177/1087054713480033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Lopez R, Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Camodeca L, Gachet M, Jaussent I, Dauvilliers Y. Association of inattention, hyperactivity, and hypersomnolence in two clinic-based adult cohorts. J Atten Disord (2018) 1087054718775826. doi: 10.1177/1087054718775826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Sateia MJ. American Academy of Sleep Medicine, International Classification of Sleep Disorders 3rd ed. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2014).

Google Scholar

101. Diaz-Roman A, Hita-Yanez E, Buela-Casal G. Sleep characteristics in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: systematic review and meta-analyses. J Clin Sleep Med (2016) 12(5):747–56. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.5810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Baykal S, Albayrak Y, Durankus F, Guzel S, Abbak O, Potas N, et al. Decreased serum orexin a levels in drug-naive children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Neurol Sci (2019) 40(3):593–602. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3692-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Cortese S, Konofal E, Lecendreux M. Alertness and feeding behaviors in ADHD: does the hypocretin/orexin system play a role? Med Hypotheses (2008) 71(5):770–5. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2008.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Becker SP, Leopold DR, Burns GL, Jarrett MA, Langberg JM, Marshall SA, et al. The Internal, external, and diagnostic validity of sluggish cognitive tempo: a meta-analysis and critical review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2016) 55(3):163–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Koriakin TA, Mahone EM, Jacobson LA. Sleep difficulties are associated with parent report of sluggish cognitive tempo. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2015) 36(9):717–23. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Becker SP, Marshall SA, McBurnett K. Sluggish cognitive tempo in abnormal child psychology: an historical overview and introduction to the special section. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42(1):1–6. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9825-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Langberg JM, Becker SP, Dvorsky MR, Luebbe AM. Are sluggish cognitive tempo and daytime sleepiness distinct constructs? Psychol Assess (2014) 26(2):586–97. doi: 10.1037/a0036276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Froehlich TE, Becker SP, Nick TG, Brinkman WB, Stein MA, Peugh J, et al. Sluggish cognitive tempo as a possible predictor of methylphenidate response in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry (2018) 79(2). doi: 10.4088/JCP.17m11553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Spruyt K, Gozal D. Sleep disturbances in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Rev Neurother (2011) 11(4):565–77. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Bixler EO, Vgontzas AN, Mahr F, Hillwig-Garcia J, et al. ADHD subtypes and comorbid anxiety, depression, and oppositional-defiant disorder: differences in sleep problems. J Pediatr Psychol (2009) 34(3):328–37. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Mulraney M, Giallo R, Lycett K, Mensah F, Sciberras E. The bidirectional relationship between sleep problems and internalizing and externalizing problems in children with ADHD: a prospective cohort study. Sleep Med (2016) 17:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Bussalb A, Congedo M, Barthelemy Q, Ojeda D, Acquaviva E, Delorme R, et al. Clinical and experimental factors influencing the efficacy of neurofeedback in ADHD: a meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry (2019) 10:35. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Riesco-Matias P, Yela-Bernabe JR, Crego A, Sanchez-Zaballos E. What do meta-analyses have to say about the efficacy of neurofeedback applied to children with adhd? Review of previous meta-analyses and a new meta-analysis. J Atten Disord (2019) 1087054718821731. doi: 10.1177/1087054718821731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Children, excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS), assessment, neuropsychological markers

Citation: Bioulac S, Taillard J, Philip P and Sagaspe P (2020) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Measurements in Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 11:3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00003

Received: 13 September 2019; Accepted: 02 January 2020;
Published: 26 February 2020.

Edited by:

Lino Nobili, University of Genoa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sona Nevsimalova, Charles University, Czechia
Andrea Romigi, Mediterranean Neurological Institute (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2020 Bioulac, Taillard, Philip and Sagaspe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stéphanie Bioulac, stephanie.bioulac@chu-bordeaux.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.