Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 04 August 2020
Sec. Mood Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic The Complex Biopsychosocial Interactions that Create Stress Resilience View all 12 articles

Effects Of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors on Depression, Anxiety, and Anger

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Korean University Anam Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Gachon University Gil Medical Center, Incheon, South Korea
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea

Background: Early life traumatic events and recent stressful events are known to have especially strong effects on emotional wellbeing. However, little is known about the interaction of early and recent stressors on emotions. We aimed to examine the interactive effects of early trauma and recent stressors on depression, anxiety, and anger.

Methods: One hundred and seventy adults were recruited and asked to complete the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), the state anxiety subscale of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S), and the state anger subscale of the State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI-S). Early traumas and recent stressors were assessed during face-to-face interviews. Multiple regression analysis was performed to test whether early trauma, recent stressors, and the interaction of the two would predict CES-D, STAI-S, and STAXI-S scores.

Results: In the multiple regression models, STAI-S scores were predicted only by recent stressors (R2 = 0.063, p = 0.001). In contrast, CES-D and STAXI-S scores were predicted only by the synergistic interaction of early trauma with recent stressors (R2 = 0.075, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.039, p = 0.01, respectively).

Conclusions: A synergistic interaction effect between early trauma and recent stressful events on current depression and anger was observed, indicating that the combined effects of early trauma and recent stressors are stronger than their individual effects. In contrast, anxiety was affected mainly by recent stressors. Our findings suggest that the form that emotional disturbance takes can vary depending on the timing of stressors.

Introduction

Stressful life events can contribute to various emotional disturbances such as depression, anxiety, and anger (14). Stressful life events seem to affect emotion via various mechanisms, including variations in neurological development, epigenetic modifications, neuroendocrine modulation, or changes in brain circuits (512). Recent stressful life events have been suggested to stimulate conditions for subsequent depression, anxiety, or anger (1, 1317). In addition, early traumatic events are known to have strong, long-term effects on emotional well-being (18, 19), being associated depression, anxiety (2022), and anger (23, 24). A previous study reported that childhood adversity leads to higher levels of anger and aggression (23). Childhood adversity and anger have also been reported to be associated with neurobiological changes, such as altered activity in the amygdala and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (24). Recent stressors can also provoke anger (4, 25). Anger is an important issue when studying the emotional effects of trauma or stress, and should be assessed separately from anxiety and depression. The effects of childhood traumatic events on emotional disturbances may continue into adulthood (26).

As stressful events have cumulative effects (2730), the long-term effect of early trauma and the short-term effects of recent stressors may simply be additive. However, it may also be that the early trauma and recent stressors have synergistic effects on emotional responses such as depression, anxiety, and anger. Given that the neurobiological mechanism by which early life trauma affects subsequent emotion may be different from that for recent stressful life events, these two different mechanisms may have synergistic or interacting effects on the development of emotional disturbances (12).

Also from a psychological viewpoint, children with early adversity exhibit more severe emotional disturbances when they experience subsequent stressful events. Individuals who have experienced trauma during childhood may show emotional dysregulation (31). Emotional regulation plays an important role in coping with stress. Dysfunctional emotion regulation strategies, such as high expressive suppression, are associated with emotional difficulties (32). Childhood traumatic experience can be a risk factor for negative emotions through the mechanism of emotional dysregulation. Individuals with early traumatic experiences may be more likely to exhibit emotional disturbances during subsequent stressful situations (3336). In other words, childhood traumatic experience can exacerbate the emotional disturbances caused by subsequent stressful life events (37, 38).

Several studies have investigated both early trauma and recent stressors in a single study. However, only few have explored the interactive effects of early trauma and recent stressors on depression, and those produced controversial results. One study reported the interactive effects of childhood adversity and recent life stressors on suicidal behavior among Chinese college students (39). Others reported that young Americans (37) and psychiatric outpatients in Germany (38) who reported childhood adversity became more depressed after experiencing recent stressors. However, another study found no effect of the interaction between early and recent stressors on depression in older people (26). These discrepancies may be due to differences in sample characteristics, such as age or psychiatric illness. Furthermore, no studies have investigated such interactive effects on anxiety or anger.

In the current study, we aimed to explore the effects of early trauma, recent stressors, and their interaction on depression, anxiety, and anger in a community sample covering a broad range. We hypothesized that the interaction between early trauma and recent stressors would exert a synergistic effect on subsequent depression, anxiety, and anger. In other words, early trauma is posited to exacerbate the effects of recent stressors, thus promoting emotional disturbances, while recent stressors should in turn exacerbate the effects of early trauma.

Methods

Subjects

Community-dwelling adults (≥19 years of age) were recruited in the Incheon area, which is the third largest metropolitan area in South Korea. Participants were recruited through posters and brochures placed in hospitals, apartment buildings, churches, and public health centers. All participants were asked to self-report their levels of depression, anxiety, and anger.

A total of 170 subjects participated in the study, including 73 men and 97 women. The mean age of subjects was 41.1 ± 8.2 years; there was no significant difference in age between men and women (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Comparison of depression, anxiety, and anger Between subjects with and without early trauma or recent stressors.

The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board of Gachon University of Medicine and Science. All subjects provided written informed consent.

Assessment of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors

Information on early trauma was collected through face-to-face interviews using the Early Life Stress Questionnaire (ELSQ). ELSQ is a questionnaire based on the Child Abuse and Trauma Scale (40). With ELSQ, responders are asked to indicate whether they experienced any of 19 negative events, such as physical and sexual abuse and neglect, in early life (41).

Information regarding recent negative stressors was also obtained through face-to-face interviews. During the interview, subjects were asked whether any of the items extracted from the Life Experience Survey (LES) described their own experience. The questions asked about (1) bereavement involving family or close friends; (2) serious illness in oneself or a family member; (3) serious economic crisis; (4) unwanted retirement or being fired; (5) severe conflicts with family, friends, relatives, or colleagues; and (6) divorce or unwanted end of a relationship (42, 43). To minimize the possibility of recall bias, participants were asked to report only experiences that had happened during the 6 months prior to their participation in the study.

All subjects were divided into four groups based on the presence of early trauma and/or recent stressors: those with both early trauma and recent stressors (ET-RS); those with early trauma but no recent stressors (ET-NRS); those with recent stressors but no early trauma (NET-RS); and those with neither early trauma nor recent stressors (NET-NRS). Subjects were also divided into two groups according to the presence/absence of early trauma and the presence/absence of recent stressors, independently of whether they had experienced the other form of stressor.

Assessments of Emotional Disturbances

To measure depressive symptoms, the Korean version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) was used. The severity of depression is reflected in CES-D scores, with higher scores indicating more severe depression. The Korean version of the CES-D is well verified. It consists of 20 questions and has an internal reliability coefficient of 0.89. The Korean version of the CES-D tends to produce higher scores than the original version (44, 45).

The Korean version of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) was used to measure current anxiety. A high score indicates high levels of anxiety. STAI is divided into subscales assessing state anxiety (the current anxiety level) and trait anxiety (intrinsic emotional characteristics (46, 47). In this study, the state anxiety subscale (STAI-S) was used to measure the current anxiety level. The scale comprises 20 questions designed to measure state anxiety, and has an internal reliability coefficient of 0.87.

The Korean version of the State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) was used to measure current anger. High scores indicate more severe anger symptoms. As with STAI, STAXI is divided into subscales assessing state and traits (48, 49). We used the state anger subscale (STAXI-S) to measure current anger in this study. This questionnaire consists of 24 questions and has an internal reliability coefficient of 0.89.

Statistical Analysis

Independent t-tests and Pearson’s correlations were used to compare continuous variables, and chi-square tests to compare categorical data. Non-parametric statistical tests such as the Kruskal–Wallis test or the Mann–Whitney U-test were used when it was not possible to assume regularity based on the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Stepwise multiple regression analysis was performed to test whether the interaction between early trauma on ELSQ (ET) and recent stressors on LES (RS) was a statistically significant predictor of CES-D, STAI-S, and STAXI-S scores. The dependent variables were CES-D, STAI-S, and STAXI-S scores. The independent variables were age, gender, ET, RS, and ET × RS. ET × RS was added as an independent variable to test the interaction of early trauma and recent stressors. The depressed group included subjects with a CES-D score ≥ 16; those with a score of ≤ 15 were classified into the non-depressed group. A regression analysis was performed on the depressed and non-depressed groups. SPSS Statistics ver. 25 was used for statistical analysis.

Results

Characteristics of the Study Subjects

In total, 170 subjects participated in the current study. Among them, 73 (42.9%) were men. There were no statistically significant sex differences in CES-D, STAI-S, or STAXI-S scores. The mean age of subjects was 41.1 ± 8.2 years. Age was not significantly correlated with CES-D, STAI-S, or STAXI-S scores (Table 1). There was no significant difference in age or sex between those with and without early trauma or between those with and without recent stressors. There was no significant association between the presence of early trauma and the occurrence of recent stressors.

Sixty-five (38.2%) subjects reported early traumatic experiences, and 40 (23.5%) reported recent stressful life events. Sixteen subjects (9.4%) were included in the ET-RS group, 49 (28.8%) in the ET-NRS group, 24 (14.1%) in the NET-RS group, and 81 subjects (47.6%) were included in the NET-NRS group (Table 1).

For all subjects, the mean ( ± SD) CES-D score was 11.7 ± 9.2, and the mean STAI-S and STAXI-S scores were 48.7 ± 7.7 and 12.7 ± 4.4, respectively (Table 1). Fifty-two subjects with a score ≥ 16 on the CES-D were assigned to the depressed group. CES-D was significantly correlated with both STAI-S (r = 0.658, p < 0.001) and STAXI-S (r = 0.564, p < 0.001) scores. The STAI-S and STAXI-S scores were also significantly correlated (r = 0.482, p < 0.001).

Effects of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors on Depression

The mean CES-D score of those who had experienced early trauma was significantly higher than that of subjects who had not (U = 2759.500, p = 0.036). Similarly, those who had experienced recent stressors had higher mean CES-D scores than those who had not (U = 2020.000, p = 0.033). The mean CES-D scores for the four groups were as follows: ET-RS, 19.4 ± 12.8; ET-NRS, 11.7 ± 8.2; NET-RS, 12.1 ± 10.3; NET-NRS, 10.1 ± 8.0 (Table 1). The difference among the groups was significant (H = 9.995, p = 0.019). Post hoc tests showed that the mean CES-D score of the ET-RS group was significantly higher than that of the ET-NRS (U = 245.500, p = 0.026) and NET-NRS groups (U = 332.500, p = 0.002) (Figure 1A). In the multiple regression models, the CES-D score was predicted by the interaction of early trauma and recent stressors (R2 = 0.075, p < 0.001), but not by early trauma or recent stressors alone (Table 2). Similar results were found in the regression model after logarithmic transformation of CES-D.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Depression, anxiety, and anger among subjects with or without early trauma and/or recent stressors. ET, presence of early trauma on the Early Life Stress Questionnaire; RS, presence of recent stressors on the Life Experience Survey; ET-RS, those with both early trauma and recent stressors; ET-NRS, those with early trauma but without recent stressors; NET-RS, those with recent stressors but without early trauma; NET-NRS, those with neither early trauma nor recent stressors; CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale; STAI-S, state anxiety subscale of Korean version of State–Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAXI-S, The state anger subscale of the Korean version of the State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.005. (A) Depression classified according to ET and RS. (B) Anxiety classified according to ET and RS. (C) Anger classified according to ET and RS.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Effects of early trauma, recent stressors, and the early trauma–recent stressors interaction on depression, anxiety, and anger.

Effects of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors on Anxiety

Participants who had experienced recent stressors had higher average STAI-S scores than those who had not (t = 3.374, p = 0.001). Early trauma, however, had no significant effect on anxiety. The mean STAI-S scores in the four groups were as follows: ET-RS, 53.0 ± 7.6; ET-NRS, 48.7 ± 6.5; NET-RS, 51.6 ± 8.1; and NET-NRS, 46.9 ± 7.8 (Table 1). The difference among the groups was significant (H = 12.279, p = 0.006). Post hoc tests showed that the mean STAI-S scores of the ET-RS (U = 358.500, p = 0.005) and NET-RS groups (U = 647.500, p = 0.013) were significantly higher than that of the NET-NRS group (Figure 1B). In the multiple regression model, the only significant predictor of STAI-S scores was recent stressors (R2 = 0.063, p; = 0.001) (Table 2). This result did not change with logarithmic transformation of STAI-S scores.

Effects of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors on Anger

Participants who had experienced early trauma had higher STAXI-S scores than those who had not (U = 2826.000, p = 0.048). However, recent stressors showed no significant effect on anger. The mean STAXI-S scores in the four groups were as follows: ET-RS, 14.8 ± 6.0; ET-NRS, 13.3 ± 4.6; NET-RS, 13.0 ± 5.7; and NET-NRS, 11.9 ± 3.3 (Table 1). Overall, the groups did not differ significantly (Figure 1C), but a direct comparison of the ET-RS and NET-NRS groups revealed a significant difference (U = 440.000, p = 0.033). No other group differences were found. STAXI-S scores were predicted by the interaction of early trauma with recent stressors (R2 = 0.039, p = 0.010), but not by early trauma or recent stressors alone (Table 2). After logarithmic transformation of STAXI-S scores, similar results were found in the regression model.

Predictors of Questionnaire Scores According to Depression Status

The CES-D scores of the depressed group were predicted by the interaction between early trauma and recent stressors (R2 = 0.096, p = 0.025), but not by early trauma or recent stressors alone. No variables significantly predicted the STAI-S or STAXI-S scores in the depressed group. In contrast, the STAI-S scores of the non-depressed group were significantly predicted by recent stressors (R2 = 0.065, p = 0.005). No other variables significantly predicted the CES-D or STAXI-S score in the non-depressed group.

Discussion

The current study investigated the effect of early trauma, recent stressors, and their interaction on depression, anxiety, and anger in a community sample representing a broad age range. The results varied with the type of emotional disturbance examined. Depression and anger were mainly predicted by the interaction of early trauma and recent stressors, supporting our a priori hypothesis. However, contrary to our hypothesis, anxiety was predicted only by recent stressors and not by the early trauma–recent stressor interaction.

As expected, in the current study, both early trauma and recent stressors were associated with greater depression. When subjects were divided into four groups based on the presence or absence of early trauma and recent stressors, those who reported both early trauma and recent stressors were more depressed than those who had experienced only early trauma. Furthermore, depressive symptoms were predicted only by the interaction between early trauma and later stressors. The current findings suggest that depressive mood may be affected by synergistic effects of past childhood trauma and a current stressful environment.

Our finding corresponds to the stress sensitization model of depression, which proposes that the first depressive episode makes the individual more prone to subsequent depressive episodes following stressors (50, 51). Recently, stress itself has been suggested to produce sensitization for depression. According to this sensitization model, people who experienced early trauma are more easily depressed when they experience later stressors (37). Those without early trauma can fend off depression even under currently stressful conditions, and those with early trauma experiences can avoid depression if they are not currently experiencing stress.

The current findings correspond to those of previous studies reporting synergistic effects of early trauma and later stressors on the development of depression in young populations (37, 38). However, one study reported that childhood adversity did not modify the effect of recent events on depression of people 55–85 years of age (26). These discrepancies in findings may be due to differences between populations. The effect of early traumatic events may diminish as people get older, due to the longer time since the event rather than to age itself. Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms after trauma tend to decrease with time, although there are some exceptions to this. Psychiatric symptoms caused by trauma decrease over time in veterans (52), young adults (53), and children (54). In addition, memories of a trauma tend to be vary depending on when they are evaluated (55, 56); thus, it appears that traumatic memories can change significantly over time. As the mean age of participants in our study was 41 years, our results suggest that the interactive effects of early trauma and current stress on depression might persist through middle age.

In the current study, as expected, anxiety was affected by recent stressors; however, it was not affected by early trauma (14, 16). The absence of a relationship between early trauma and current anxiety was contrary to previous findings showing that childhood adversity is linked to anxiety in adults (20, 57, 58). This discrepancy may reflect differences among studies in the severity of childhood trauma. Most previous studies focused on more serious trauma, such as physical or sexual abuse, whereas ours included any childhood adversity reported on the ELSQ. In addition, compared to depression or anger, anxiety may be highly affected by the present situation. Moreover, adulthood anxiety in victims of early trauma has been reported to vary depending on the person’s resilience (5961). Those with low resilience become sensitized to emotional difficulties by childhood adversity, whereas others may even achieve posttraumatic growth after similar experiences (62, 63).

Anger was predicted only by the interaction between early trauma and recent stressors. Anger, like depression, is likely to be provoked when people who experienced childhood trauma encounter subsequent negative life events. Experiences of childhood trauma might result in a character structure that makes it easier to feel angry in stressful situations (24).

Although statistical insignificance necessarily means no real effects, some of the results in this study lost significance when the participants were divided into the depressed and non-depressed groups. The synergistic effects of early trauma and recent stressors on depression were found only in the depressed group. In contrast, the effects of recent stressors on anxiety were more prominent in the non-depressed group. These findings suggest that the interaction effect between early trauma and recent stressors on depression may be stronger in depressed than non-depressed groups. Non-depressed people may respond to current stress with anxiety, regardless of past childhood traumatic experience.

Usually, the CES-D is used for psychopathological evaluation of symptoms of depressive disorder while the STAI-S and STAXI-S are concerned with emotional problems. Our results may be related to the different dimensions measured by the questionnaires. The psychopathological problems measured by the CES-D may be more affected by the synergistic interaction between early trauma and recent stressors, while the emotional responses measured by the STAI-S are more related to current stressful events. However, we found that the STAXI-S scores were also associated with the synergistic interaction between early trauma and recent stressors, similar to the CES-D which is psychopathological evaluation. These complex results may be due to the infeasibility of distinguishing between psychopathological and emotional aspects in many situations. Severe anxiety or anger may be not only problematic but also pathological by themselves.

Previous studies have shown the effects of the interaction between early trauma and recent stressors on depression or suicidality in younger groups (3739). To the best of our knowledge, the current study is the first to explore the interaction effect between early trauma and recent stressors on negative emotions such as anxiety or anger in a community sample with a wide age range.

The current study found that the effects of early and recent stressors and their interaction varied with the type of emotional disturbance considered. This finding may have some clinical implications. When individuals with early trauma encounter new stressful events, special clinical attention should be paid to the development or aggravation of depression or anger. For the management of anxiety, focusing on and coping with current stressful events rather than early experiences would be more helpful.

This study has several limitations. First, the number of subjects, especially of individuals who experienced recent stressors, was small. Thus, the impact of recent stressors may have been underestimated. Besides, convenience sampling was used to recruit the subjects. Thus, the generalizability of the result may be limited. Second, given that the information about early and recent stressors was obtained only by self-report, the data may be subject to recall bias or selective reporting and attempts to hide shameful or painful memories. Third, PTSD disease group was not analyzed separately. However, there were no subjects who reported diagnosis of PTSD at the time of the investigation. Besides, traumatic experiences do not always lead to PTSD (64). Especially, the ELSQ covers mild as well as severe traumatic experiences. The possibility of the recall bias of the early childhood experience would also be considered. Fourth, as this study was cross sectional, it could not show causal relationships or the mechanism underlying stressor interactions and their effects on emotions. Fifth, because stressful events were measured only in terms of their presence or absence, differences among individuals in the nature and severity of those experiences could not be considered. A future cohort study examining the detailed nature of such events in a large population may be needed to confirm the interaction effects of early and recent stressors on emotions.

In conclusion, our results showed a synergistic interaction between early trauma and recent stressors on current depression and anger. Anxiety, however, was affected mainly by recent stressors in the present study. Clinicians should be aware that clients’ emotional difficulties may depend on when they experienced traumatic or stressful experiences.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by institutional review board of Gachon University of Medicine and Science. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: SK, S-JC. Data curation: SK, S-JC, YL, JL, BS. Formal analysis: SJ, BS. Funding acquisition: SK. Investigation: SK, BS. Methodology: SK, S-JC, YL, JL, BS. Project administration: SK. Resources: SK, S-JC, YL. Supervision: SK. Software: YL, BS. Validation: SJ, JL. Visualization: BS. Writing—original draft: BS. Writing—review and editing: SJ, SK.

Funding

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea, funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (No. 2016M3C7A1904336) and National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean government (MEST) (No. 2016R1A2B4011561).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea, funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT, & Future Planning (No. 2016M3C7A1904336) and a National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean Government (MEST) (No. 2016R1A2B4011561).

References

1. Brown RL. The Influence of Stressor Exposure and Psychosocial Resources on the Age-Anger Relationship. J Aging Health (2016) 28:1465–87. doi: 10.1177/0898264315624900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hassanzadeh A, Heidari Z, Feizi A, Hassanzadeh Keshteli A, Roohafza H, Afshar H, et al. Association of Stressful Life Events with Psychological Problems: A Large-Scale Community-Based Study Using Grouped Outcomes Latent Factor Regression with Latent Predictors. Comput Math Methods Med (2017) 2017:3457103. doi: 10.1155/2017/3457103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Mall S, Mortier P, Taljaard L, Roos J, Stein DJ, Lochner C. The relationship between childhood adversity, recent stressors, and depression in college students attending a South African university. BMC Psychiatry (2018) 18:63. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1583-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Riggs DS, Dancu CV, Gershuny BS, Greenberg D, Foa EB. Anger and post-traumatic stress disorder in female crime victims. J Traumatic Stress (1992) 5:613–25. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490050410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bolton JL, Molet J, Ivy A, Baram TZ. New insights into early-life stress and behavioral outcomes. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2017) 14:133–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Chaney A, Carballedo A, Amico F, Fagan A, Skokauskas N, Meaney J, et al. Effect of childhood maltreatment on brain structure in adult patients with major depressive disorder and healthy participants. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2014) 39:50–9. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chen Y, Baram TZ. Toward Understanding How Early-Life Stress Reprograms Cognitive and Emotional Brain Networks. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41:197–206. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Hanson JL, Knodt AR, Brigidi BD, Hariri AR. Heightened connectivity between the ventral striatum and medial prefrontal cortex as a biomarker for stress-related psychopathology: understanding interactive effects of early and more recent stress. Psychol Med (2018) 48:1835–43. doi: 10.1017/s0033291717003348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Heim C, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, Nemeroff CB. The link between childhood trauma and depression: insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2008) 33:693–710. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kim MJ, Elliott ML, d’Arbeloff TC, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, Brigidi BD, et al. Microstructural integrity of white matter moderates an association between childhood adversity and adult trait anger. Aggress Behav (2019) 45:310–8. doi: 10.1002/ab.21820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10:434–45. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Poletti S, Aggio V, Brioschi S, Bollettini I, Falini A, Colombo C, et al. Impact of early and recent stress on white matter microstructure in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord (2018) 225:289–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bonanno GA, Kennedy P, Galatzer-Levy IR, Lude P, Elfstrom ML. Trajectories of resilience, depression, and anxiety following spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol (2012) 57:236–47. doi: 10.1037/a0029256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fan LB, Blumenthal JA, Watkins LL, Sherwood A. Work and home stress: associations with anxiety and depression symptoms. Occup Med (Lond) (2015) 65:110–6. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqu181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. LeMoult J, Ordaz SJ, Kircanski K, Singh MK, Gotlib IH. Predicting first onset of depression in young girls: Interaction of diurnal cortisol and negative life events. J Abnorm Psychol (2015) 124:850–9. doi: 10.1037/abn0000087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Michl LC, McLaughlin KA, Shepherd K, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol (2013) 122:339–52. doi: 10.1037/a0031994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schuler KL, Ruggero CJ, Goldstein BL, Perlman G, Klein DN, Kotov R. Diurnal Cortisol Interacts With Stressful Events to Prospectively Predict Depressive Symptoms in Adolescent Girls. J Adolesc Health (2017) 61:767–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Park S, Hong JP, Jeon HJ, Seong S, Cho MJ. Childhood exposure to psychological trauma and the risk of suicide attempts: the modulating effect of psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Invest (2015) 12:171–6. doi: 10.4306/pi.2015.12.2.171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Br J Psychiatry (2010) 197:378–85. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wang D, Lu S, Gao W, Wei Z, Duan J, Hu S, et al. The Impacts of Childhood Trauma on Psychosocial Features in a Chinese Sample of Young Adults. Psychiatry Invest (2018) 15:1046–52. doi: 10.30773/pi.2018.09.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A. Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169:141–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) III: associations with functional impairment related to DSM-IV disorders. Psychol Med (2010) 40:847–59. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Carver CS, Johnson SL, McCullough ME, Forster DE, Joormann J. Adulthood personality correlates of childhood adversity. Front Psychol (2014) 5:1357:1357. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kim MJ, Scult MA, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, d’Arbeloff TC, Brigidi BD, et al. A Link Between Childhood Adversity and Trait Anger Reflects Relative Activity of the Amygdala and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex. Biol Psychiatry Cognit Neurosci Neuroimaging (2018) 3:644–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Amstadter AB, Vernon LL. Emotional Reactions During and After Trauma: A Comparison of Trauma Types. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma (2008) 16:391–408. doi: 10.1080/10926770801926492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Comijs HC, Beekman AT, Smit F, Bremmer M, van Tilburg T, Deeg DJ. Childhood adversity, recent life events and depression in late life. J Affect Disord (2007) 103:243–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hanson JL, Albert D, Iselin AM, Carre JM, Dodge KA, Hariri AR. Cumulative stress in childhood is associated with blunted reward-related brain activity in adulthood. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci (2016) 11:405–12. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Karam EG, Friedman MJ, Hill ED, Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Petukhova M, et al. Cumulative traumas and risk thresholds: 12-month PTSD in the World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Depress Anxiety (2014) 31:130–42. doi: 10.1002/da.22169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lee JH, Kim S. Exposure to Negative Life Events and Post-Traumatic Embitterment Symptoms in Young Adults in Korea: Cumulative and Differential Effects. Psychopathology (2019) 52:18–25. doi: 10.1159/000496501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Osvath P, Voros V, Fekete S. Life events and psychopathology in a group of suicide attempters. Psychopathology (2004) 37:36–40. doi: 10.1159/000077018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dvir Y, Ford JD, Hill M, Frazier JA. Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities. Harv Rev Psychiatry (2014) 22:149–61. doi: 10.1097/hrp.0000000000000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol (2003) 85:348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Briere J, Hodges M, Godbout N. Traumatic stress, affect dysregulation, and dysfunctional avoidance: a structural equation model. J Trauma Stress (2010) 23:767–74. doi: 10.1002/jts.20578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Eftekhari A, Zoellner LA, Vigil SA. Patterns of emotion regulation and psychopathology. Anxiety Stress Coping (2009) 22:571–86. doi: 10.1080/10615800802179860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Goldsmith RE, Chesney SA, Heath NM, Barlow MR. Emotion regulation difficulties mediate associations between betrayal trauma and symptoms of posttraumatic stress, depression, and anxiety. J Trauma Stress (2013) 26:376–84. doi: 10.1002/jts.21819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Linehan MM. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2018).

Google Scholar

37. Shapero BG, Black SK, Liu RT, Klugman J, Bender RE, Abramson LY, et al. Stressful life events and depression symptoms: the effect of childhood emotional abuse on stress reactivity. J Clin Psychol (2014) 70:209–23. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Vogt D, Waeldin S, Hellhammer D, Meinlschmidt G. The role of early adversity and recent life stress in depression severity in an outpatient sample. J Psychiatr Res (2016) 83:61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. You Z, Chen M, Yang S, Zhou Z, Qin P. Childhood adversity, recent life stressors and suicidal behavior in Chinese college students. PloS One (2014) 9:e86672. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sanders B, Becker-Lausen E. The measurement of psychological maltreatment: early data on the Child Abuse and Trauma Scale. Child Abuse Negl (1995) 19:315–23. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(94)00131-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cohen RA, Hitsman BL, Paul RH, McCaffery J, Stroud L, Sweet L, et al. Early life stress and adult emotional experience: an international perspective. Int J Psychiatry Med (2006) 36:35–52. doi: 10.2190/5r62-9pqy-0nel-tlpa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lee Y. (The) Relations between attributional style, life events, event attribution, hopelessness and depression. Seoul National University Ph.D. Dissertation (1993).

Google Scholar

43. Sarason IG, Sarason BR, Potter E, Antoni MH. Life events, social support, and illness. Psychosom Med (1985) 47:156–63. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198503000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cho MJ, Kim KH. Use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) Scale in Korea. J Nerv Ment Dis (1998) 186:304–10. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199805000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Radloff L. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Measurement (1977) 1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hahn D-W. Korean adaptation of Spielberger’s STAI (K-STAI). Kor J Health Psychol (1996) 1:1–14.

Google Scholar

47. Spielberger CD. STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Self-Evaluation Questionnaire (1970), 1:1–24.

Google Scholar

48. Chon K-K, Hahn D-W, Lee C-H, Spielberger C. Korean adaptation of the state-trait anger expression inventory: anger and blood pressure. Korean J Health Psychol (1997) 2:60–78.

Google Scholar

49. Spielberger CD. Professional manual for the state-trait anger expression inventory-2 (STAXI-2). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1999).

Google Scholar

50. Lewinsohn PM, Allen NB, Seeley JR, Gotlib IH. First onset versus recurrence of depression: differential processes of psychosocial risk. J Abnorm Psychol (1999) 108:483–9. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry (1992) 149:999–1010. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.8.999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Marmar CR, Schlenger W, Henn-Haase C, Qian M, Purchia E, Li M, et al. Course of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder 40 Years After the Vietnam War: Findings From the National Vietnam Veterans Longitudinal Study. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:875–81. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Galatzer-Levy IR, Ankri Y, Freedman S, Israeli-Shalev Y, Roitman P, Gilad M, et al. Early PTSD symptom trajectories: persistence, recovery, and response to treatment: results from the Jerusalem Trauma Outreach and Prevention Study (J-TOPS). PloS One (2013) 8:e70084. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Panter-Brick C, Grimon MP, Kalin M, Eggerman M. Trauma memories, mental health, and resilience: a prospective study of Afghan youth. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2015) 56:814–25. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hepp U, Gamma A, Milos G, Eich D, Ajdacic-Gross V, Rössler W, et al. Inconsistency in reporting potentially traumatic events. Br J Psychiatry (2006) 188:278–83. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.104.008102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Waller M, Charlson FJ, Ireland RE, Whiteford HA, Dobson AJ. Time-course of PTSD symptoms in the Australian Defence Force: a retrospective cohort study. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci (2016) 25:393–402. doi: 10.1017/s2045796015000517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lindert J, von Ehrenstein OS, Grashow R, Gal G, Braehler E, Weisskopf MG. Sexual and physical abuse in childhood is associated with depression and anxiety over the life course: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Public Health (2014) 59:359–72. doi: 10.1007/s00038-013-0519-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Spinhoven P, Elzinga BM, Hovens JG, Roelofs K, Zitman FG, van Oppen P, et al. The specificity of childhood adversities and negative life events across the life span to anxiety and depressive disorders. J Affect Disord (2010) 126:103–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Poole JC, Dobson KS, Pusch D. Anxiety among adults with a history of childhood adversity: Psychological resilience moderates the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation. J Affect Disord (2017) 217:144–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.03.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Seery MD, Holman EA, Silver RC. Whatever does not kill us: cumulative lifetime adversity, vulnerability, and resilience. J Pers Soc Psychol (2010) 99:1025–41. doi: 10.1037/a0021344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Wu LM, Sheen JM, Shu HL, Chang SC, Hsiao CC. Predictors of anxiety and resilience in adolescents undergoing cancer treatment. J Adv Nurs (2013) 69:158–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2012.06003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Latham RM, Meehan AJ, Arseneault L, Stahl D, Danese A, Fisher HL. Development of an individualized risk calculator for poor functioning in young people victimized during childhood: A longitudinal cohort study. Child Abuse Negl (2019) 98:104188. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J Trauma Stress (1996) 9:455–71. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490090305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Bonanno GA. Loss, trauma, and human resilience: have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? Am Psychol (2004) 59:20–8. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.59.1.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, anxiety, anger, trauma, stress

Citation: Seok BJ, Jeon S, Lee J, Cho SJ, Lee YJ and Kim SJ (2020) Effects Of Early Trauma and Recent Stressors on Depression, Anxiety, and Anger. Front. Psychiatry 11:744. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00744

Received: 08 April 2020; Accepted: 16 July 2020;
Published: 04 August 2020.

Edited by:

Juliana Nery Souza-Talarico, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Amit Shrira, Bar-Ilan University, Israel
Marion Trousselard, Institut de Recherche Biomédicale des Armées (IRBA), France

Copyright © 2020 Seok, Jeon, Lee, Cho, Lee and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Seog Ju Kim, ksj7126@skku.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.