Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 October 2020
Sec. Mood Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Grief Disorders: Clinical, Cultural, and Epidemiological Aspects View all 17 articles

Different Trajectories of Prolonged Grief in Bereaved Family Members After Terror

  • 1Center for Crisis Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2Faculty of Health and Social Sciences, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Bergen, Norway
  • 3Research Department, Division of Mental Health, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway

Introduction: The loss of a loved one in a terror incident is associated with elevated risk for mental health disorders such as prolonged grief disorder (PGD) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but the long- term adaptation after such losses are not well understood. This study aims to explore the trajectories of PGD among parents and siblings (n = 129) after the 2011 terror attack on Utøya Island, Norway.

Methods: The 19-item Inventory of Complicated grief (ICG) was used to measure PGD at 18, 28, and 40 months post-loss. Latent class growth analysis (LCGA) was used to identify trajectories of grief and a multinomial regression analysis was conducted to examine predictors of class membership.

Results: The analysis identified three grief trajectories; moderate/decreasing class (23%), high/slow decreasing class (64%), and a high/chronic class (13%). Predictors of high/slow recovery or chronic grief was female gender, previous depressive symptoms, and intrusion and avoidance symptoms.

Conclusion: The findings highlights the difficult grief process and slow recovery that characterizes the majority of close family members bereaved by a terror-incident. Community mental health programs should strive for both early outreach and long-term follow-up after such incidents.

Introduction

Prolonged grief disorder (PGD) is a chronic, unrelenting grief that was included in the revised International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) in 2018 (1) and is now also proposed for inclusion in DSM-5. PGD is distinguishable from other mental disorders such as depression and PTSD (2, 3), and is associated with poor physical health, reduced quality of life, and functional impairment (4). The core symptoms of PGD are intense yearning or preoccupation with the deceased, combined with severe emotional pain related to the loss (e.g., difficulties accepting the death, anger or bitterness, feeling that life is meaningless). PGD affects ~10% after expected losses, but certain subgroups show higher prevalence rates. A recent meta-analysis found that 50% of bereaved after unnatural losses (e.g., suicide, homicide, accident or combat-related deaths) experience PGD (5).

Persons who lose a loved one to terror-incidents are more vulnerable for developing PGD and other mental health disorders than persons bereaved by other types of disasters (5). High prevalence of PGD (43–83%) and comorbid PTSD (43–85%) have been found after different terror-incidents such as 9/11 (6), the 2011 Utøya-killings (7), and the 2015 terror attack in Paris (8). Long-term studies are limited, but one recent study by Cozza et al. (9) examined the mental health status of bereaved family members 15 years after the 9/11 incident and concluded that the majority of family members could be considered healthy.

Both degree of exposure to the incident and closeness in relationship to the deceased affects the level of mental health difficulties. In a study of 704 adults bereaved by the 9/11 terror attacks Neria, Gross et al. (6) found elevated levels of PGD among persons that witnessed the attacks on television and among parents who had lost a child.

The co-existence of trauma reactions or PTSD symptoms and grief may create a synergy effect that delays recovery (10). A common assumption is that traumatic imagery or intrusions may hinder normal grief from progressing. However, recent studies have also suggested that increase in grief levels predict later levels of PTSD and not vice versa (11, 12). Overall, the combination of PGD and PTSD is associated with functional impairment (13).

Longitudinal bereavement studies have traditionally relied on mean scores to measure grief reactions across time. While these studies are informative, mean scores do not capture the diversity of grief reactions over time (14). More advanced quantitative methods, latent class growth analysis are able to identify subpopulations or different pathways to adjustment, also termed trajectories. Few studies have investigated trajectories of grief or PGD after disasters. In a study of 170 adults bereaved by the South East-Asian tsunami in 2004 Sveen et al. (15) examined the course of PGD on three different occasions up to 6 years after the disaster. Three distinct trajectories were identified: recovery (48% of the sample), resilient (41%), and chronic (11%). The strongest predictor of chronic grief was loss of a child. Lenferinket al. (16) examined trajectories of PGD, depression, and PTSD on four occasions up to 2½ years post-loss among 172 adults bereaved by a plane disaster in Ukraine in 2014. Two trajectories of grief were identified: mild (82%) and chronic (18%). Low education was associated with the chronic grief trajectory. These studies demonstrate that the majority of disaster-bereaved adults will adjust to their loss, but a significant minority continue to struggle with chronic grief reactions several years after the death.

While studies of disaster-bereaved populations suggest that resilience and recovery are the two most common trajectories, no study has, as far as we know, examined trajectories of PGD among close family members after a terror-incident. Subsequently, our knowledge of their long-term adaptation after terror is scarce.

Aim of the Study

The main aim of this study was to examine the trajectories of PGD among close family members bereaved by the 2011 terror attack at Utøya, Norway. A second aim was to investigate predictors of the different grief trajectories. We chose factors that has shown to predict PGD in other studies such as gender (17), type of loss (18), exposure to the incident (18), previous losses (19) and pre-loss mental health difficulties (20). In addition we chose to explore whether trauma reactions such as intrusions and avoidance would affect recovery from their loss (21). Satisfaction of social support was also included since it is regularly mentioned among the bereaved as important for recovery. These factors were all measured on timepoint 1.

The Event

On July 22nd, 2011 a Norwegian born terrorist detonated a 950 kg car bomb close to several governmental buildings in the city of Oslo, killing 8 persons and severely injuring 10. Just before the bomb went off the terrorist drove to Utøya, a small island outside of Oslo, where over 500 adolescents and young adults were attending a political youth summer camp for the Norwegian Labor party. The terrorist, dressed as a police officer, chased people all over the island for 1 h and 20 min with the aim of killing as many as possible. Before he was arrested, he had killed 69 persons between 14 and 51 years of age, and physically injured 56.

Materials and Methods

This project is derived from a longitudinal study, which examines the mental health effects of the loss of a child or sibling in the 2011 terror attack at Utøya Island, Norway. Participants completed a self-report questionnaire 18 (T1), 28 (T2), and 40 months (T3) after the attack.

Procedures and participants

Biological parents and siblings, as well as step-parents and step-siblings of the adolescents and adults who were killed at Utøya were invited to participate in the study. Public records of the deceased were linked to the National Population Register (NPR) in order to obtain the names of the biological parents and siblings. The parents and siblings were first sent an informational letter regarding participation in the study. Those who wanted to participate returned their informed consent forms and received the questionnaire by ordinary mail or filled it in using Survey Monkey (a digital platform). Parents and/or siblings who were on Utøya during the terror attack were excluded from the study since they were invited to participate in a project on the direct survivors' mental health. As two of the deceased were non-Norwegian citizens and not registered in the NPR, their families were also excluded from the study. Moreover, one of the deceased had no parents alive.

A total of 208 biological parents and siblings were identified through the NPR, and 121 of them participated (58%). In addition, seven step-parents and one step-sibling participated in the study. Thus, the total sample consisted of 129 bereaved, that is, 86 parents and 43 siblings. At T1 103 persons participated, at T2 123 persons participated (including 25 new persons), and at T3 111 persons participated.

A total of 83 (64%) persons participated at all three time points. A total of 17 persons (13.2%) dropped-out from the study either after T1 (n = 5) or T2 (n = 12). There were no significant differences between ‘drop-outs’ and ‘completers’ neither on the sumscore on the ICG (M = 39.18 vs. M = 36.62), t = 0.75 (96), p = 0.46), on the intrusion subscale (M = 17.12 vs. M = 16.15, t = 0.51 (96), p = 0.61) nor on the avoidance subscale (M = 15.35 vs. M = 13.06, t = 1.28 (96), p = 0.20). The participants came from 51 families representing 74% of the deceased, with one-to-six members belonging to the same family.

The project was approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Norway.

Measures

The survey consisted of questions mapping socio-demographics, loss-related questions, questions related to exposure, previous depressive symptoms, previous losses, psychosocial help after the terror-incident, PTSD and PGD.

Exposure was measured with the following question: “Were you in contact with your child or sibling either by telephone or SMS during the terror attack”? (yes vs. no). Previous depressive symptoms were measured with the following question: Have you ever before July 22nd 2011 had low mood, felt depressed or had feelings of hopelessness for 4 weeks or more (yes vs. no)? Previous losses were measured with the following question: Have you any time before July 22nd 2011 lost someone through death whom you were closely attached to? (yes vs. no). Received psychosocial help was measured with the following question: Have you received help from mental health care after your loss? (yes vs. no).

The Inventory of Complicated Grief (ICG) (22) was used to measure PGD. The ICG consists of 19 items; answers were ranked on a 5-point scale ranging from never (0) to always (4). Scores vary from 0 to 76 with a high score indicating a higher level of PG. The ICG has shown high internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and concurrent validity (22). The Norwegian version of ICG has shown good psychometric properties (23), and the internal reliability was found to be good across all three time points (α = 0.89–0.92). A cut-off score >25 indicates probable PGD (24).

The Impact of Event Scale-Revised (IES-R) (24) was used to measure symptoms of post-traumatic stress. IES-R consists of 22 items divided into three subscales: intrusion (re-experiencing the traumatic event), avoidance (avoiding trauma reminders), and hyperarousal (disturbed sleep, irritability, hypervigilance). Replies were given on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all (0) to extremely (4). The Norwegian version has shown good psychometric properties in a non-clinical sample (25). The internal reliability was found to be good across the three time points for intrusion (α = 0.83–0.90), for avoidance (α = 0.83–0.85), and arousal (α = 0.86–0.88).

One item from The Crisis Support Scale (26) was used to measure satisfaction with social support: “Overall, are you satisfied with the social support you have received after the terror incident?” This question has been used in other disaster studies (27). The respondents rated their overall satisfaction on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always).

Statistical Analysis

SPSS 23.0 was used for descriptive statistics, frequency, cross tabulation, student t-test, generalized mixed logistic regression, and linear mixed models (28). These two mixed (multilevel) models were used in order to account for family clustering in the data. The p-level was set to 0.05 (two-tailed). Mplus 7.4 was used for analyzing level and change in PGD as latent contrast difference score models (29). This model, which is a restricted latent growth curve model, analyzing each interval as separate pieces of change, was combined with mixture modeling in order to explore data heterogeneity and identify different sub-groups (latent classes) with different levels and change profiles of PGD (30). The mixture model was specified as a Latent Class Growth Analysis (LCGA) which constrain the variances in intercept and slope factors constrained to zero. This model may give one more class than the growth mixture model (GMM), with these variances estimated freely.

Based on an unconditional latent difference score in PGD, LCGA were analyzed (31). Evaluation of the number of classes was based on the entropy index, and the model fit indices from Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and sample-adjusted BIC (SA-BIC). Lower AIC, BIC, and SA-BIC values indicate a better model fit (32, 33), while entropy values should preferably be over 0.80, at least 0.70 (31). BIC and bootstrap LRT have been found to perform best out of all these indices across several simulated models (34).

Statistically significant improvement as a result of adding classes was tested by the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT), and by a parametric bootstrapped likelihood ratio test for k-1(H0) vs. k classes with 1,000 bootstrap draws (35). The evaluation was in addition based on the estimated class size and relative class frequencies. A sample size of 25 classified subjects (5%) has been proposed (36).

The Maximum Likelihood Estimator with robust standard errors (MLR) was chosen in order to account for non-normality (32). Due to observation within families, the clustering option was used in order to achieve correct standard errors. The Full Information Maximization Likelihood method uses all available data under the “Missing at Random” assumption (32, 37). Thus, the effect of missing data is minimized (38), in contrast to listwise deletion methods, and ensures stronger statistical power and generalizability to the results. However, this do not rule out that missingness may be related to the values that would be observed if given, the “missing not at random” situation.

After the model with the best fit was determined, a multinominal logistic regression analysis examined potential predictors of class membership, including gender, type of loss, previous losses, previous depressive symptoms, exposure to the attack, received psychosocial help after the attack, social support (measured at T1), intrusion (measured at T1), and avoidance (measured at T1). The multinominal regression compares each group with a reference category.

Results

The characteristics of the sample and descriptive statistics of PG is shown in Table 1. A total of 67% had lost a child among the participants. About half the parents were female (52.3%). Their mean age was 51.6 years (SD = 6.9), and almost all parents were married or cohabitants (85.5%). The mean age of the deceased children was 18.7 years (SD = 5.4). In the sibling sub-group 74.4% were female. Their mean age was 22.3 years (SD = 7.6), and 55.8% had lost a sister. The mean age of the deceased siblings was 18.6 years (SD = 5.2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics and mean sum scores on ICG and IES-R (n = 129).

Based on the fit measures and the number of subjects and frequencies, a three-class solution was decided to be the final model (Table 2). The six-class model represented the variability in the observed scores even better but could not be used for further analyses due to the very low sample size in several classes. The three-class model is presented in Figure 1. The subjects in the moderate/decreasing class (N = 29/22.5%) started with a PG score of 25.58, and showed a statistically significant reduction in the first period (−0.78, p < 0.01), but not in the second period (0.01, p = 0.972). The high/slow decreasing class (N = 83/64.3%) started with a PG score of 39.30 and showed statistically significant reduction in both time intervals (T1–T2 = −0.32, p < 0.01; T2–T3 = −0.25, p < 0.05). The high/chronic grief class (N = 17/13.2%) started with a PG score of 50.95, increased their scores significantly in the first interval (T1–T2 = 0.62, p < 0.05), but showed no further change (T2–T3 = −0.12, p = 0.526).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Latent class growth analysis (LCGA) of the PG sum score with fit measures for evaluation (n = 129).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Different grief trajectories among bereaved family members after the 2011 terror attack on Utøya lsland.

The generalized mixed multinomial regression results are presented in Table 3. Adjusted predictor estimates showed an increased probability for being in the high/slow decreasing class in contrast to the moderate/decreasing class (reference), if the subject was female, reported symptoms of depression before the attack, and had higher scores on intrusion (measured at T1). We found increased probability for being in the high/chronic grief class in contrast to the moderate/decreasing grief class (reference) if the subject was a female, reported symptoms of depression before the attack, and had higher scores on intrusion or avoidance (both measured at T1). The type of loss was not related to being in the high/chronic grief class.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Trajectory group (class) prediction based on generalized mixed multinomial regression analyses.

Discussion

This is, to our knowledge, the first study of trajectories of PGD following a terror-incident, and we found that a three-class model best represented the data. All three groups started above the recommended threshold for probable PGD (22), but had different developments across time. The classes included a high/slow recovery trajectory of (64%) with initial high level of grief which attenuated slowly from 1 to 3½ years after the incident; a moderate/decreasing trajectory (23%) with an initial moderate to high level of grief, which decreased below the threshold for PGD and a chronic trajectory (13%) which started with very high level of grief which even increased slightly from 1 to 3 1/2 years after the loss.

The lack of a a distinct resilient group is noteworthy, since most disaster-studies find resilience to be among, if not the most common trajectory (15, 16). Still, it is consistent with studies that suggest that resilience is far from normative among grieving parents (39). It is worth noting that the sample in the current study was homogenous constituting only of parents and siblings of those who were killed. Closeness in the relationship to the deceased is a stronger risk factor for PGD than both the circumstances of the death and time since the loss (40, 41). Also, almost all of those who were killed were adolescents between 14 and 19 years of age, which is an age that has been associated with more intense parental grief than loss of either a younger child or adult child (42). Also, some suggest that vulnerable persons find more meaning in participating in research (43). If this is true we may have a biased sample. On the other hand, those who struggle most with their grief may also refuse to participate in order to avoid reminders of the loss (44).

The finding that almost 80% of the participants displayed either a high grief level and a slow recovery or chronic grief is alarming, and suggest that the large majority of terror-bereaved family members may struggle for several years with adapting to their loss. Several factors may account for this. In addition to losing a child or a sibling, which is a devestating experience in itself, the terror-bereaved need to cope both with the traumatic circumstances of the loss and with secondary stressors such as media coverage and a potential trial which function as constant reminders of the incident. This affects their opportunity to grieve privately, but also to take time off from their grief (45).

Our finding that pre-loss depressive symptoms predicted chronic grief is regularly documented in studies of PGD, and should be routinly screened for after traumatic losses (4). Also, our finding that comorbid PTSD symptoms intrusion and avoidance predicted a slow recovery and chronic grief are consistent with a recent meta-analysis (21). Horowitz stated that intrusions are common after sudden traumatic events, and that avoidance is a strategy that is used to ward off the painful emotions related to these intrusions (46). While avoidance can be adaptive in the early stages of a loss, if it is used as the main coping strategy to regulate grief it may hinder the bereaved from confronting the reality of their loved one's death or processing the loss (47). Intrusive thoughts or re-enactment fantasies of how their loved one was killed can interfere with positive reminiscing of the deceased (48). This suggest that PTSD symptoms may hinder or delay the resolution of grief. Recent studies have, however, found that an increase in grief levels can predict later PTSD levels and not vice versa (11). This suggest that PGD and PTSD may influence each other reciprocally, which requests a thorough assessment in clinical practice since it may have implications for the choice of treatment.

Limitations

The current study has several limitations. Due to confidentiality, the ethical committee did not permit inquiries about non-responders beyond the information of gender and age of the deceased. However, there were no statistically significant group differences between participants and non-participants concerning these demographic variables. Furthermore, the study suffer from low sample size. In the LCGA two classes had a relatively small number of predicted persons. The estimated relationships are a result of the included predictors, but also a result of the potential predictors not included in the model. Also, we used single non-validated questions in the assessment of previous depressive symptoms and psychosocial help after the attack. These measures may be subject to reporting bias. The use of ICG to tap grief symptoms does not capture the current symptoms or criteria for of PGD as defined in either DSM-5 or ICD-11 (1). Comparative studies show that it matters which criteria are used to define disturbed grief in terms of prevalence and predictive validity (49).

Clinical Implications

There are several implications of these findings. First of all, the high level of PGD and slow recovery characterizing the majority of bereaved suggest that an early out-reach in the follow-up of family members bereaved by terror is warranted (50). Use of adequate screening measures may be helpful in reaching those who need help most. As studies have shown, persons with PGD may not seek adequate help due to different barriers, and not all will accept help in the early stages of the loss. A plan for long-time follow-up of the bereaved is also indicated. Some bereaved will need help first several months after their loss when there is a marked erosion of support from social network. Collective gatherings are highly valued among bereaved families were a combination of psychoeducation and grief groups are organized (51). Some bereaved also need specialized help for unrelenting PGD. Clinicians need to be familiar with assessment and treatment of both trauma-related disorders and different grief disorders. Several treatment models of traumatic bereavement are promising and have shown good effects (52, 53).

Conclusion

Terror-bereaved family members are vulnerable for a long-lasting and difficult grief process, comprising of both trauma symptoms and PGD. This suggest that there is a need for both early interventions and interventions capturing those who need long-term help. Also, mental health personnel working with terror-bereaved families should have be trained in both assessment of and treatment of trauma or PTSD and PGD.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Norway. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

PK: lead author of introduction, materials and methods section, and discussion. KD: project leader, commented on manuscript. RG conducted analysis, written statistical analysis section, results section, commented on manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The project was funded by Center for crisis psychology, Bergen, Norway and The Danish Egmont Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Killikelly C, Maercker A. Prolonged grief disorder for ICD-11: the primacy of clinical utility and international applicability. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2017) 8(Suppl 6):1476441. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2018.1476441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kristensen P, Dyregrov K, Dyregrov A. What distinguishes prolonged grief disorder from depression? J Norwegian Med Association. (2017) 137:538–9. doi: 10.4045/tidsskr.16.0629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Shear K, Simon N, Wall M, Zisook S, Neimeyer R, Duan N, et al. Complicated grief and related bereavement issues for DSM-5. Depress Anxiety. (2011) 28:103–17. doi: 10.1002/da.20780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Prigerson HG, Horowitz MJ, Jacobs SC, Parkes CM, Aslan M, Goodkin K, et al. Prolonged grief disorder: psychometric validation of criteria proposed for DSM-V and ICD-11. PLoS Med. (2009) 6:e1000121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Djelantik A, Smid GE, Mroz A, Kleber RJ, Boelen PA. The prevalence of prolonged grief disorder in bereaved individuals following unnatural losses: systematic review and meta regression analysis. J Affect Disord. (2020) 265:146–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Neria Y, Gross R, Litz B, Maguen S, Insel B, Seirmarco G, et al. Prevalence and psychological correlates of complicated grief among bereaved adults 2.5–3.5 years after September 11th attacks. J Traumat Stress. (2007) 20:251–62. doi: 10.1002/jts.20223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Dyregrov K, Dyregrov A, Kristensen P. Traumatic bereavement and terror: the psychosocial impact on parents and siblings 1 1/2 year after the 22.07.11 terror-killings in Norway. J Loss Trauma. (2014) 20:556–76. doi: 10.1080/15325024.2014.957603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pirard P, Motreff Y, Lavalette C, Vandentorren S, Baubet T, Messiah A. Mental health impact and health care use of the bereaved persons 8–11 months after the November 2015 Paris terror attacks. Eur J Public Health. (2018) 28:635. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cky212.635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cozza SJ, Fisher JE, Fetchet MA, Chen S, Zhou J, Fullerton CS, et al. Patterns of comorbidity among bereaved family members 14 years after the September 11th, 2001, terrorist attacks. J Trauma Stress. (2019) 32:526–35. doi: 10.1002/jts.22407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Armour M. Violent death: understanding the context of traumatic and stigmatized grief. J Human Behavi Social Environment. (2007) 14:53–90. doi: 10.1300/J137v14n04_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Djelantik AMJ, Smid GE, Kleber RJ, Boelen PAJCP. Do prolonged grief disorder symptoms predict post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms following bereavement? A cross-lagged analysis. Compr Psychiatry. (2018) 80:65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. O'Connor M, Nickerson A, Aderka IM, Bryant RA. The temporal relationship between change in symptoms of prolonged grief and posttraumatic stress following old age spousal bereavement. Depress Anxiety. (2015) 32:335–40. doi: 10.1002/da.22349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kristensen P, Weisæth L, Hussain A, Heir T. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders and functional impairment after loss of a family member: a longitudinal study after the 2004 Tsunami. Depress Anxiety. (2015) 32:49–56. doi: 10.1002/da.22269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Orcutt HK, Bonanno GA, Hannan SM, Miron LR. Prospective trajectories of posttraumatic stress in college women following a campus mass shooting. J Trauma Stress. (2014) 27:249–56. doi: 10.1002/jts.21914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sveen J, Bergh Johannesson K, Cernvall M, Arnberg FK. Trajectories of prolonged grief one to six years after a natural disaster. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0209757. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lenferink L, Nickerson A, de Keijser J, Smid GE, Boelen PA. Trajectories of grief, depression, and posttraumatic stress in disaster-bereaved people. Depress Anxiety. (2020) 37:35–44. doi: 10.1002/da.22850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kristensen P, Weisæth L, Heir T. Predictors of complicated grief after a natural disaster: a population study two years after the 2004 South-East Asian tsunami. Death Stud. (2010) 34:137–50. doi: 10.1080/07481180903492455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Johannesson KB, Lundin T, Hultman CM, Frojd T, Michel PO. Prolonged grief among traumatically bereaved relatives exposed and not exposed to a tsunami. J Trauma Stress. (2011) 24:456–64. doi: 10.1002/jts.20668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Goldstein RD, Petty CR, Morris SE, Human M, Odendaal H, Elliott A, et al. Pre-loss personal factors and prolonged grief disorder in bereaved mothers. Psychol Med. (2019) 49:2370–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718003264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shear K. Clinical practice. Complicated grief. N Engl J Med. (2015) 372:153–60. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp1315618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Heeke C, Kampisiou C, Niemeyer H, Knaevelsrud C. A systematic review and meta-analysis of correlates of prolonged grief disorder in adults exposed to violent loss. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2019) 10:1583524. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2019.1583524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Prigerson HG, Maciejewski PK, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Bierhals AJ, Newsom JT, Fasiczka A, et al. Inventory of Complicated Grief: a scale to measure maladaptive symptoms of loss. Psychiatry Res. (1995) 59:65–79. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02757-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Thimm JC, Davidsen M, Elsness MM, Vara H. Reliability, factor structure, and validity of the Inventory of Complicated Grief (ICG) in a general bereaved sample in Norway. Scand Psychol. (2019) 6:e7. doi: 10.15714/scandpsychol.6.e7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Weiss DS, Marmar CB. The impact of event scale-revised. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM, editors. Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. New York, NY: Guildford Press (1997). p. 399–411.

PubMed Abstract

25. Eid J, Larsson G, Johnsen BH, Laberg JC, Bartone PT, Carlstedt B. Psychometric properties of the Norwegian impact of event scale-revised in a non-clinical sample. Nordic J Psychiatry. (2009) 63:426–32. doi: 10.1080/08039480903118190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Joseph S, Andrews B, Williams R, Yule W. Crisis support and psychiatric symptomatology in adult survivors of the Jupiter cruise ship disaster. British J Clin Psychol. (1992) 31(Pt 1):63–73. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00968.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Johannesson KB, Arinell H, Arnberg FK. Six years after the wave. Trajectories of posttraumatic stress following a natural disaster. J Anxiety Disord. (2015) 36:15–24. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Version 23.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. (2015).

29. Newsom JT. Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group (2015). p. 411.

Google Scholar

30. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus 7.4. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (2015).

Google Scholar

31. Wang J, Wang X. Structural Equation Modeling: Applications Using Mplus. West Sussex, UK: Wiley, A John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Publication (2012). doi: 10.1002/9781118356258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 3 ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2010). p. 425.

Google Scholar

33. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus. Statistical Analysis With Latent Variables. User's Guide. Version 7. 7 ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (2007).

Google Scholar

34. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a monte carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model. (2007) 14:535–69. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide. Version 8. 7 ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (2007).

36. Wickrama KAS, Lee TK, O'Neal CW, Lorenz FO. Higher-Order Growth Curves and Mixture Modeling With Mplus. New York, NY: Routledge (2016). p. 320.

Google Scholar

37. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus 7.3. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (1998).

Google Scholar

38. Bollen KA, Curran PJ. Latent Curve Models: A Structural Equation Perspective. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience (2006).

Google Scholar

39. Infurna FJ, Luthar SS. Parents' adjustment following the death of their child: resilience is multidimensional and differs across outcomes examined. J Res Pers. (2017) 68:38–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2017.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lobb EA, Kristjanson LJ, Aoun SM, Monterosso L, Halkett GK, Davies A. Predictors of complicated grief: a systematic review of empirical studies. Death Stud. (2010) 34:673–98. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2010.496686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Fernández-Alcántara M, Zech E. One or multiple complicated grief (s)? The role of kinship on grief reactions. Clin Psychol Sci. (2017) 5:851–7. doi: 10.1177/2167702617707291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Wijngaards-de Meij L, Stroebe M, Schut H, Stroebe W, van den Bout J, van der Heijden P, et al. Couples at risk following the death of their child: predictors of grief versus depression. J Cons Clin Psychol. (2005) 73:617–23. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.4.617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Aldwin DF. How good is survey measurement? Assessing the reliability and validity of survey measures. In: Marsden PV, Wright JV, editors. Handbook of Survey Research. Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing (2010). p. 405–37.

Google Scholar

44. Grievink L, van der Velden PG, Yzermans CJ, Roorda J, Stellato RK. The importance of estimating selection bias on prevalence estimates shortly after a disaster. Ann Epidemiol. (2006) 16:782–8. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Stroebe M, Schut H. The dual process model of coping with bereavement: rationale and description. Death Stud. (1999) 23:197–224. doi: 10.1080/074811899201046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Horowitz MJ. Psychological processes induced by illness, injury, and loss. In: Green C, Meagher R, Million T, editors. Handbook of Clinical Health Psychology. New York, NY: Springer US; Plenum Press (1982). p. 53–67.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. Dyregrov K, Dyregrov A, Kristensen P. In what ways do bereaved parents after terror go on with their lives, and what seems to inhibit or promote adaptation during their grieving process? A qualitative study. Omega-J Death Dying. (2016) 73:374–99. doi: 10.1177/0030222816653851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Layne CM, Kaplow JB, Oosterhoff B, Hill RM, S Pynoos R. The interplay between posttraumatic stress and grief reactions in traumatically bereaved adolescents: when trauma, bereavement, and adolescence converge. Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 7:266–85. doi: 10.2174/2210676608666180306162544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lenferink LI, Boelen PA, Smid GE, Paap MC. The importance of harmonising diagnostic criteria sets for pathological grief. Br J Psychiatry. (2019) 216:1–4. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Allsopp K, Brewin CR, Barrett A, Williams R, Hind D, Chitsabesan P, et al. Responding to mental health needs after terror attacks. BMJ. (2019) 366:l4828. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Dyregrov A, Dyregrov K, Straume M, Grønvold Bugge R. Weekend family gatherings for bereaved after the terror killings in Norway in 2011. Scand Psychol. (2014) 1:e8. doi: 10.15714/scandpsychol.1.e8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lenferink L, de Keijser J, Smid GE, Boelen PA. Cognitive therapy and EMDR for reducing psychopathology in bereaved people after the MH17 plane crash: findings from a randomized controlled trial. Traumatology. (2020). doi: 10.1037/trm0000253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pearlman LA, Wortman CB, Feuer CA, Farber CH, Rando TA. Treating Traumatic Bereavement: A Practitioner's Guide. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2014).

Google Scholar

Keywords: bereavement, terror, prolonged grief, trajectories, latent class growth analysis

Citation: Kristensen P, Dyregrov K and Gjestad R (2020) Different Trajectories of Prolonged Grief in Bereaved Family Members After Terror. Front. Psychiatry 11:545368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.545368

Received: 24 March 2020; Accepted: 14 September 2020;
Published: 14 October 2020.

Edited by:

Birgit Wagner, Medical School Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Sebastian Trautmann, Medical School Hamburg, Germany
Lonneke Lenferink, University of Groningen, Netherlands
Jos De Keijser, University of Groningen, Netherlands

Copyright © 2020 Kristensen, Dyregrov and Gjestad. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pål Kristensen, pal.kristensen@uib.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.