Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 March 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Etiology and Treatment for Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder View all 18 articles

Autism Spectrum and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders in Children of Immigrants: A Brief Review of Current Evidence and Implications for Clinical Practice

  • 1INSERM U1136, Institut Pierre Louis d'Epidémiologie et de Santé Publique, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
  • 2École des Hautes Études en Santé Publique, Rennes, France
  • 3Department of Interdisciplinary Social Science, Utrecht Centre for Child and Adolescent Studies, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Reference Centre for Rare Psychiatric Diseases, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
  • 5Institute for Intelligent Systems and Robotics, CNRS UMR 7222, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
  • 6Pôle Hospitalo-Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l'Enfant et de l'Adolescent, Université de Rennes 1 and Centre Hospitalier Guillaume-Régnier, Rennes, France
  • 7Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, CNRS UMR 8002 and University of Paris, Paris, France

Children of immigrants may have higher neurodevelopmental risks than those of non-immigrant populations. Yet, some evidence suggests that this group may receive late diagnosis, and therefore miss beneficial early interventions. Clinicians may misattribute symptoms of disorders to other social, behavioral or language problems. Likewise, there might be cultural differences in parents' likelihood of perceiving or reporting first developmental concerns to clinicians. Population-based standardized screening may play an important role in addressing ethnic inequalities in the age at diagnosis, although further research focusing on cross-cultural use is necessary. Once children are diagnosed, clinicians may rely on culturally sensitive procedures (translation services, cultural mediators) to increase the accessibility of interventions and improve adherence among immigrant families. In this brief review, we provide an overview about what is currently known about the epidemiology and risk factors of neurodevelopmental disorders, paying special attention to autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in children of immigrants and suggest the necessity of population-based screening and culturally sensitive care.

Introduction

There is increasing evidence based on literature reviews that children of immigrants are at higher risk of neurodevelopmental disorders, particularly autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (14). Other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), intellectual disability, and specific learning disabilities, have been studied less frequently and results are less consistent, with some studies reporting higher risks and others reporting lower or equal risks (58). Some evidence suggests that children of immigrants may face delays in diagnosis and increased difficulties in accessing appropriate health care (911). Especially in relation to ASD this is very concerning, as children of immigrants may be at risk of missing early interventions, which have been shown to be highly beneficial in terms of clinical prognosis (12). In this brief review, we provide a short overview on what is currently known about inequalities in neurodevelopmental health in children of immigrants and discuss implications for screening and clinical practice, paying special attention to ASD.

Epidemiology of Neurodevelopmental Disorders in Children of Immigrants

Increased risks of ASD in children of immigrants were first reported in Sweden, the UK, and Australia as early as the 1970 and 1980s (3, 13), and have since been confirmed in various countries (14). The main results are summarized in Table 1. This table does not present an exhaustive list of all studies on ASD in children of immigrants.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies on ASD in children of immigrants [based on Augereau et al. (14)].

Although there is variability in the strength of the reported associations, systematic reviews have found support of increased risks of ASD in children of immigrant parents (14). Particularly maternal immigrant status has been reported as an environmental risk factor for ASD (23, 24). Parental immigrant status seems to be mainly associated with low-functioning ASD (20) and other ASD phenotypes characterized by intellectual disability and communication disorders (25). Indeed, studies reported generally lower risks of high-functioning ASD (20) and Asperger's syndrome (26) in children of immigrants (25, 27), as compared to the host population. Neurodevelopmental disorders other than ASD have been examined less frequently, and results are less consistent. When considering ADHD, divergent results have been found, partially due to differences in informants, as well as in the accessibility of mental healthcare across countries. In Finland, Lehti et al. found higher odds of ADHD in children of immigrants in a case-control study (5). Similarly, in Germany, immigrant parents reported more ADHD symptoms in their children (28) and stronger increases in symptoms over time (29). However, German children born to immigrants had lower odds of receiving a diagnosis of ADHD (7). In Sweden, only non-European immigrants reported more symptoms of ADHD in their children (30). In Denmark, teachers reported similar numbers of ADHD symptoms in children of immigrants and non-immigrants, whereas immigrant parents reported lower levels of symptoms among their children (31). In Belgium, adolescents with a migrant background tended to self-report fewer symptoms of ADHD than their non-immigrant peers (32), whereas no differences in self-reported symptoms were found in Norway (33). In the Netherlands, Zwirs et al. found lower prevalence estimates of ADHD in children of immigrants, as compared to those of the native Dutch (34). In the USA, several population-based studies found lower risks of receiving an ADHD diagnosis in ethnic minority groups, including Hispanic Americans (3541). However, a study specifically focusing on the Public Mental Health System of New York State found higher prevalences of ADHD in Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks than in Whites (42). Importantly, it must be noted that in the US, many studies on ethnic minority groups did not distinguish between immigrants and non-immigrants (e.g., in the case of Blacks or Hispanics), and few provide detailed information on individuals' country of origin (22).

Also when considering intellectual and learning disabilities, divergent results were observed. In Australia, Abdullahi et al. found higher prevalence rates of cerebral palsy with ID in children of mothers born in upper-middle-income countries only. They did not find evidence of increased risks of ID without cerebral palsy or cerebral palsy without ID in children of immigrant mothers (8). In two other Australian studies, mothers born abroad had no significant differences in their risk of having a child with severe ID, and when considering mild to moderate ID, foreign-born women tended to have lower risks (43, 44). A British study found lower prevalence rates of intellectual disabilities in children of immigrants, except those of Bangladeshi descent, who had higher odds of profound multiple learning difficulties, of Pakistani descent who had higher odds of profound multiple and severe learning difficulties, as well as Travelers of Irish heritage, who had significantly higher odds of moderate and severe learning difficulties (45). In France, the prevalence of severe ID was higher in areas with a high proportion of immigrants (46). In Finland, Lehti et al. found that children of immigrant parents had higher odds of developmental disorders related to speech and language, academic skills and coordination (6). Similarly, a German study found that children with foreign nationalities had higher odds of delays in gross motor skills, fine and grapho-motor coordination, grammar, memory and concentration, perseverance, abstraction, visual perception and arithmetic (47). In the US, two studies found no differences in the odds of learning disabilities in children of Latinos and Whites (37, 48), and a third study found lower odds of developmental delay, learning disabilities, and speech problems in children of immigrants (49). A Taiwanese study found lower risks of developmental delays in children of immigrants (50).

Inequalities in neurodevelopmental health may already be present at a very young age, long before children are typically diagnosed, though currently few studies on very young infants are available. In a population-based birth cohort in France, Schmengler et al. found that immigrant mothers more frequently observe early signs of neurodevelopmental problems in their 2-year-old children, as measured by a standardized parental-report screening instrument [Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT)], compared to non-immigrant French mothers (51). In a recent Italian study on pre-term infants, children of migrant mothers from countries with a low Human Development Index (HDI) showed increased risks of general neurodevelopmental impairment at 24 months, as determined by the Revised Griffiths Mental Development Scale 0–2 years, particularly if they were exclusively fed with formula milk (52).

The timing and circumstances of migration seem to play an important role in neurodevelopmental inequalities in immigrant populations. For example, Magnusson et al. (20) reported that recent migration was particularly associated with risks of low-functioning ASD in the offspring, and children of mothers who migrated during the year they gave birth had the greatest risks. However, elevated risks are also observed in children of descendants of immigrants, which may suggest transgenerational effects of migration (22, 51). Importantly, risks differ strongly by parental region of origin. For example, in the USA, children of women born in the Philippines, Vietnam, and Central/South America have increased risks of ASD, compared to children of US-born White mothers, whereas no higher risks were found in children of women born in China, Japan, Korea, or Mexico (22). In France, children of mothers born in North and French-speaking Sub-Saharan Africa scored significantly higher on the M-CHAT than children of non-immigrant French mothers. However, no differences in M-CHAT scores were found when comparing children of non-immigrant French women and those born in other EU countries (51). In the UK, higher risks of intellectual and learning disabilities were mainly found in children of immigrants from Bangladesh and Pakistan, while most other immigrant groups had lower or similar risks of intellectual and learning disabilities, as compared to the non-immigrant British (45). Lehti et al. found higher odds of childhood autism in children of mothers and fathers born in Asia, the former Soviet Union, and former Yugoslavia. No higher odds were observed in children of parents born in Western countries, North Africa and the Middle East, and Sub-Saharan Africa (53). When considering ADHD, maternal migration was only associated with higher odds for children of mothers born in the former Soviet Union and Yugoslavia, Sub-Saharan Africa, and North Africa and the Middle East, whilst no higher odds were found in children of women born in Asia, Latin America, and Western countries. Paternal migration was associated with increased odds of ADHD regardless of fathers' region of birth (5). Regarding developmental disorders of speech and language, academic skills or coordination, Lehti et al. (6) found higher odds in children of women born in the former Soviet Union and former Yugoslavia, Sub-Saharan Africa, North Africa and the Middle East, Asia, and Latin America, but not other Western countries. Paternal migration was associated with higher odds of these developmental disorders regardless of fathers' region of birth, except for men born in Latin America, where no significant differences were found with non-immigrant Finnish men (6).

Differential Exposure to Neurodevelopmental Risk Factors

It is not clear which risk factors underlie the association between parental immigrant status and offspring neurodevelopment. Several explanations have been suggested, which include an increased prevalence of known pre-, peri-, and post-natal risk factors for neurodevelopmental disorders (5458). For example, immigrants frequently differ from the host population on risk factors, such as obesity (59, 60), and low socioeconomic status, as well as associated risk factors, such as inadequate housing and poor nutrition (46, 6165). Furthermore, they may be more frequently exposed to environmental pollutants, such as dioxin, PCBs, traffic-related air pollution, or heavy metals (53, 6569). In France, children born to immigrant women of African descent have significantly higher blood lead levels than children of non-immigrant French mothers (70). Etchevers et al. hypothesized that this may partially be related to cultural habits involving the use of contaminated goods, such as imported lead-releasing ceramic cookware, herbs, or cosmetics, such as surma or kohl (70). Immigrant mothers may face language and cultural barriers, as well as discrimination when accessing prenatal care (71), which could lead to deferred treatment of pregnancy complications, which are a known risk factor of neurodevelopmental problems (55, 56, 58, 72). Indeed, pregnancy complications seem more common in some immigrant populations (7375), yet were unable to explain higher risks of ASD in children of immigrant mothers in Sweden (20) and more early signs of neurodevelopmental problems children of immigrant mothers in France (51). Another common, yet controversial, hypothesis is that elevations in neurodevelopmental risk might be due to increased risks of vitamin D deficiency in darker skinned populations (3). Gestational vitamin D deficiency was associated with ASD-related traits at age 6 in a recent analysis of a Dutch multiethnic cohort (76). In a Swedish registry-based study, vitamin D-deficiency in pregnancy predicted ASD with, but not without, intellectual disability (77). In another Swedish study, Somali women were more likely to be vitamin D deficient than Swedish women. However, no significant differences were observed when comparing Somali mothers with and without children with autism (78). In the Netherlands, no elevation in ASD risk was found in groups known to have high levels of vitamin D deficiency, such as the Moroccan Dutch (79). Associations between vitamin D deficiency in pregnancy and increased risks of ADHD have also been reported in studies (80, 81). Other explanations focus on psychosocial stress experienced during the premigration, migration, or post-migration periods, which may adversely impact neural development of the fetus, for example through epigenetic mechanisms (82, 83). Immigrants sometimes flee from armed conflict or severe economic insecurity in their countries of origin, and may have high levels of exposure to combat, poverty and sexual violence (84). Common post-migration psychosocial stressors include acculturative stress and discrimination (85, 86).

Some evidence suggests that risks are highest if parents originate from regions with low human development, which can be considered as a proxy of the extent of adversity in the country of origin. In Finland, children of both immigrant mothers and fathers from countries with low HDI had higher odds of ADHD, compared to children of immigrant parents from countries with very high, high, or medium HDI (5). However, no such associations were found when considering developmental disorders of speech and language, academic skills or coordination (6). In Sweden, only children of mothers from medium and low HDI countries had significantly higher risks of low-functioning ASD, whereas no differences were found between children of immigrant mothers from countries with high or very high HDI and those of non-immigrant Swedish women (20). In Australia, children with ASD born to immigrants from lower income countries were more likely to have comorbid intellectual disability, and showed more social impairments and communication difficulties (87). In France, the HDI values of native countries were significantly lower for parents and grandparents of ASD boys compared to controls (parents and grandparents of non-ASD boys with language disorders), especially for paternal grandparents, whereas no differences were found between the two groups when considering only (grand) parental immigrant status. Furthermore, HDI levels from the paternal line (father and especially paternal grandparents) were negatively correlated with autism severity, particularly for social interaction impairments (14). The authors hypothesized that social adversity-related stress experienced during the premigration period, especially by paternal grandparents, may be a factor of vulnerability for ASD (14). Furthermore, associations between low human development and the incidence of schizophrenia (which is nowadays considered a neurodevelopmental disorder), including childhood-onset schizophrenia, have been observed in immigrant populations (8891). Unfortunately, currently very little is known about neurodevelopmental health in countries with low human development, such as those in Sub-Saharan Africa (92, 93). Importantly, bilingualism does not seem to be a determinant of developmental problems in children with and without clinical disorders, such as ASD (9498), nor is it able to explain associations between parental immigrant status and neurodevelopmental disorders (51, 87).

Differences in the Detection of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Health Care Access, and Treatment Retention

Most available literature on the neurodevelopment of immigrants' children is based on disease registries and case-control studies of children diagnosed with a clinical disorder, such as ASD. These studies consider often only one neurodevelopmental disorder (e.g., ASD), despite that comorbidity is the rule rather than the exception among neurodevelopmental disorders (99101). Furthermore, these studies miss very young children or those that remain undiagnosed (51). This is especially problematic considering that there is some evidence of ethnic diagnostic/referral bias in clinical practice, as clinicians may incorrectly attribute symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders to other social, behavioral or language problems (102). For example, in an experimental study, Dutch pediatricians were more likely to consider an ASD diagnosis when evaluating clinical vignettes of children of non-immigrant parents, as compared to vignettes of children of Moroccan and Turkish-origin parents (102). Diagnostic bias may be less problematic when studying ethnic inequalities in ADHD, as ADHD symptoms are featured in many behavioral questionnaires, such as the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (103, 104). These instruments are routinely included in population-based cohort studies, which also include undiagnosed children. Nevertheless, in studies based on clinical samples, underdiagnosis can lead to underestimation of the true extent of neurodevelopmental inequalities in the population. In clinical practice, immigrants' children with ASD may be at higher risk of not accessing highly beneficial early interventions (105).

The extent to which late or under-diagnosis is an issue for children of immigrants is not entirely clear and may vary across settings, by type of condition, and clinical severity. In several studies from the USA, children of ethnic minorities, including African Americans and Hispanics were on average diagnosed later with ASD than children of White Americans or remained undiagnosed (106, 107), suggesting that developmental problems in children from these populations may systematically be missed. Other studies found no differences or yielded opposite associations. A Dutch population-based cohort study did not find differences in the age of ASD diagnosis between children of immigrants and non-immigrants (79), whereas Abdullahi et al. found that Australian children of immigrants were diagnosed with ASD at earlier ages than their non-immigrant peers (87). However, children of immigrants in Australia were more likely to show more severe ASD accompanied with intellectual disability (87). It is therefore possible that an earlier age at diagnosis in this group is mainly explained by a higher prevalence of comorbidities and more severe phenotypes that are easier to detect in clinical practice (87). Importantly, diagnostic bias may still be present concerning children with less severe or more ambiguous symptoms, which may, for instance, explain the underrepresentation of children of immigrants in studies of children diagnosed with high-functioning ASD (20).

Disparities may exist not only in the timing of diagnosis and initial contact to health care providers, but also concern retention in high-quality neurodevelopmental care. For example, in a US study, autistic children of Latino parents with limited English proficiency received fewer hours of therapy and had higher unmet therapy needs than non-Latino White children (108). In another US study, children with ASD from immigrant families had more than twice the risk of not having a usual source of care (9). However, a study from the UK did not find ethnic differences in ASD-related care use (109). Currently, less is known about inequalities in access to ASD-related care in other European countries, where data on ethnicity are often not routinely collected (110). Nevertheless, in France, two sociological studies conducted in districts of Paris (111) and Marseille (112) that are characterized by a high prevalence of immigrant families show increased difficulties in accessing adequate care. These difficulties occurred despite the fact that France has universal healthcare access and that the principle of justice/equity within the healthcare system is inscribed in law (113). This suggests that current efforts may still be insufficient to address disparities in access to neurodevelopmental care for immigrant families in France. When considering ADHD care, studies have consistently shown lower use of ADHD medication in children of immigrants (114). For example, a study on Dutch ADHD patients aged <19 years found that higher proportions of patients with Turkish or Moroccan backgrounds reported never having used ADHD medication compared to non-immigrant Dutch patients. Furthermore, these groups showed higher medication discontinuation rates (115). Higher rates of medication discontinuation and treatment disengagement have also been found in ethnic minorities in the US, including Latin Americans (116, 117).

Barriers to the Detection of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Health Care Access, and Treatment Retention

Potential barriers to diagnosis, access to and retention in specialized care reported in studies conducted among immigrants are similar across disorders and include both structural/socioeconomic factors (e.g., financial barriers, underinsurance, problems with transportation, fragmented services, and language barriers) and social/cultural factors (e.g., lack of social support, stigma and discrimination, insufficient understanding of the host country's health system, low health literacy, and differences in values and expectations between health service providers and parents) (9, 10, 116, 118126).

This mismatch in expectations may often stem from the fact that cultures differ substantially in their perceptions of the etiology and symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders, on their prognosis, as well as on the effectiveness and acceptability of treatments. For example, studies suggest that the likelihood that parents attribute certain behaviors to a medical condition differs across ethnic groups (125). Some behaviors typical of ASD, such as avoiding eye contact, may be perceived as a way of expressing respect toward authority figures rather than a sign of atypical development in some Asian cultures (11, 118, 125). Likewise, the extent to which children engage in imaginative play differs across cultural groups (125). For example, Korean-Americans may be less likely to engage their children in pretend play, as compared to White Americans (127). Hence, the absence of imaginative play may be a more salient warning sign in some cultures than others (125). Such cultural differences in norms of optimal child development and parenting may influence whether and when early warning signs are recognized and reported (125). In a US study, Latina mothers were less likely to report developmental concerns and ASD symptoms than White mothers of children potentially with ASD, although children of Latino mothers who met the diagnostic criteria for ASD tended to have more severe ASD symptoms (128). Somali parents in Britain correctly identified clinical vignettes of children with autism, but had difficulties recognizing other developmental problems, which they misclassified sometimes as ASD-like symptoms (129). Indian parents may be first concerned by social difficulties, whereas US parents notice first delays in language and general development (11). Ethnic differences in reporting were also found when considering ADHD, suggesting that immigrant parents may be less likely to identify symptoms of ADHD in their children. In the Netherlands, Moroccan, Turkish, and Surinamese parents showed lower sensitivity in correctly identifying ADHD in their children than non-immigrant Dutch parents. At the same time, Moroccan and Turkish parents showed higher specificity in detecting ADHD than the non-immigrant Dutch (130). In line with this, in a Danish study, immigrant parents reported fewer symptoms of ADHD than non-immigrant parents in their children, whilst no such differences were found when considering teacher report (31). This could be because cultures may differ in the extent to which they view behavioral problems typical of ADHD as a medical issue rather than a social or spiritual problem (42, 131). A US study found that traditional gender roles and cultural values of familism were related to sociological and spiritual beliefs about the etiology of ADHD amongst Latinos (131).

Furthermore, immigrant parents may have less support from friends and family networks, and may even face stigma from their own community, which can lead to social isolation of these parents (121). For example, in some cultures, having a child with ASD is perceived as a divine punishment or a consequence of evil spirits or witchcraft (118). Perhaps more frequently, communities are not aware of the challenges of raising a child with a long-term neurodevelopmental disability, such as ASD. In a study within the Somali community in Britain, many families were unfamiliar with the term “autism” when their child was diagnosed, as ASD is not recognized in Somalia and no corresponding word exists in Somali language (132). As a result of lack of ASD awareness, parents sometimes received conflicting messages from their community and medical professionals. For example, parents were often told by members of their extended family to disregard the diagnosis of ASD, as their child may grow out of the symptoms on his or her own (132). This led frequently to a confusion between ASD as a long-term diagnosis and temporary delays in development (132). Furthermore, some parents tended to delay accessing care and to hide their children from their community for fear of negative responses to the child's disruptive behavior (132). Also immigrant parents of children with ADHD may face difficulties finding social support from within their communities. For example, a qualitative study on US Latinos reported that families often blamed parents for problematic child behavior rather than viewing ADHD as a mental health condition. As a result, some parents had a tendency to conceal their child's condition to their extended family (133). Preferences concerning treatment modalities differ also across cultural groups. For example, Latino and African American parents tend to rate behavioral treatments for ADHD more favorably than White Americans, whilst being more reluctant to accept the use of psychotropic drugs on their children (134).

Screening May Address Inequities in Access to Early Interventions for ASD

The use of standardized screening instruments in unselected populations may help address potential referral bias and improve early detection of neurodevelopmental disorders in children of immigrants and other underserved communities, although further research focusing on multiethnic contexts is necessary. Early detection is especially important in the case of ASD, as intervention in early childhood is a critical determinant of long-term clinical prognosis (12). Some initial results on the multiethnic use of standardized screeners in toddlers (e.g., M-CHAT) seem promising, though studies also have found differences in the performance of questionnaires across language and cultural groups. In a US study, children of ethnic minorities showed inflated screen-positive rates on the M-CHAT-R questionnaire. However, this effect disappeared when additionally considering the follow-up interview M-CHAT-R/F or subsequent clinical evaluations (135). No significant differences in the positive predictive values (PPVs) were found across ethnic groups for the M-CHAT-R with follow up (135), which was similar to the findings reported in another US study by Herlihy et al. (136). Furthermore, for children who screened at-risk for developmental delay based on the M-CHAT, Herlihy et al. found only small differences in age at evaluation between White and ethnic minority children, which suggests that standardized screening instruments may address inequalities in age at diagnosis (137). Seif-Eldin et al. found that the test performance of the M-CHAT in nine Arab-speaking countries was similar to Western contexts (138). However, some results from other studies were less encouraging, which highlights the importance of additional research on the cross-cultural validity and adaptation of neurodevelopmental screening instruments. For example, in the USA, Scarpa et al. reported poor internal consistency of the M-CHAT in children of mothers with ethnic minority background (139), and Guthrie et al. observed higher PPVs and specificity in children of Whites, as compared to those from other ethnic groups (140). Many other commonly-used ASD screeners have been assessed in terms of their cross-cultural validity, such as the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ)-Child questionnaire, the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), and the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Overall, studies support the cross-cultural use of these scales, though similar to the M-CHAT, some differences were found across settings. Carruthers et al. compared the predictive ability of individual items of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ)-Child questionnaire across samples in Japan, India, and the UK. Whereas they detected cultural differences on some of the items, they found considerable cross-cultural overlap in the items that predicted most strongly ASD diagnosis (141). Wang et al. found similar sensitivity, specificity, and total scores in the Mandarin-adaptation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS) in Taiwan, compared to Western contexts (142). Similarly, Bölte et al. found satisfactory-to-good reliability and validity regarding the German adaptation of the SRS (143). However, whereas the raw SRS scores in controls in Taiwan and Germany were similar, these scores were lower than in controls from the US (142). Adequate performance of the SRS has also been found in South Korea (144) and for the Farsi adaptation of the scale in Iran (145). Studies that support the cross-cultural use of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) have been conducted in Taiwan (146), Greece (147), Germany (148), the Gulf countries (149), and Turkey (150). Detailed reviews on cultural adaptations of autism screening instruments have been performed by Soto et al. (151) and Al Maskari et al. (152).

Whereas current adaptations of screening instruments are often limited to language translations, the accuracy of these instruments could potentially be improved by making further adjustments to incorporate the norms, values, and beliefs of the adapted culture (152). For example, in Iran, Samadi and McConkey found that a screening instrument developed specifically for the Iranian context outperformed language adaptations of the M-CHAT (153). Additional difficulties relate to poor implementation of screening in clinical practice. For example, Rea et al. reported low screening completion rates and referral practices that were inconsistent with screening results in a retrospective medical chart review across three American pediatric clinics (154).

Conclusion

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has by far been the most studied neurodevelopmental disorder in terms of its relation with parental migration. It is also the disorder for which inequalities in risk have been most consistently found (14). Less is known about other neurodevelopmental disorders, on which studies have found heterogeneous results, with some studies reporting higher and others lower risks in children of immigrants (58). Importantly, there is a profound lack of research on children of immigrants living in non-Western host countries. Differences in the exposure to environmental risk factors may contribute to inequalities in neurodevelopmental health between children of immigrants and non-immigrants. Although the contribution of these risk factors to the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders is thought to be substantial (up to 40–50% in case of ASD), research in this area is still in its infancy (155). Common limitations of existing studies include, for example, lack of prospective longitudinal designs, small sample sizes, lack of direct measurement of environmental exposures, and difficulties ascertaining the timing of exposures (155). Particularly little is known about environmental risk factors in developing countries, which may differ substantially in their prevalence and distribution from high-income countries (92). Nevertheless, some environmental risk factors (e.g., ambient air pollution, certain pregnancy complications) have been repeatedly associated with neurodevelopmental disorders in studies (55, 155). Yet, the contributions of individual risk factors tend to be small and findings are not specific enough to derive interventions to prevent neurodevelopmental disorders that go beyond the provision timely diagnosis and adequate medical care (55). There is also a scarcity of studies on the experiences of immigrant families with children with neurodevelopmental disorders in the healthcare system, and, to our knowledge, there is currently no systematic review available summarizing what is currently known about these families' interactions with providers or the utility of existing interventions. Nevertheless, the limited available evidence suggests that many barriers to detection, health care access, and treatment retention are similar across neurodevelopmental disorders and include structural/socioeconomic factors, such as underinsurance and service fragmentation, as well as social/cultural factors, such as lack of social support from the same-ethnic community and insufficient availability of culturally appropriate care (9, 10, 116, 118126).

Several recommendations can be made to improve diagnosis and access to neurodevelopmental healthcare for children of immigrants. When addressing disparities in the age of diagnosis, it is important to be aware that different cultural groups may not be equally likely to raise concerns about their child's development. Likewise, clinicians may misattribute certain symptoms to language difficulties or cultural differences (102). The implementation of standardized screening instruments in routine medical practice may be helpful as they prompt clinicians to look specifically for early signs of ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders that otherwise may be missed (102). Screening at day-care centers, where many young children spend a significant amount of time, may also help address barriers in access to early detection and intervention (156). Childcare professionals could be trained to identify signs of neurodevelopmental problems and ASD as early as possible and inform parents in a culturally sensitive manner. Once children are diagnosed, clinicians should be encouraged to use culturally sensitive procedures (translation services, cultural mediators) to make treatment more accessible and enhance adherence among immigrant families (11, 157, 158). In settings where sufficient resources are not available, home-based early interventions may be helpful, which are implemented by caregivers in close collaboration with healthcare professionals (159).

Author Contributions

HS and MM conceived and developed an initial draft manuscript in consultation with DC and ST, who regularly provided extensive feedback. All authors contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This review was funded by a government grant managed by the French National Research Agency (ANR)—SOCIALRISK_MH project under the ANR Social Determinants of Health programme (2012). Heiko Schmengler received a fellowship for his Master's degree leading to this publication from the Congrès Français de Psychiatrie.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Abdullahi I, Leonard H, Cherian S, Mutch R, Glasson EJ, de Klerk N, et al. The risk of neurodevelopmental disabilities in children of immigrant and refugee parents: current knowledge and directions for future research. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 5:29–42. doi: 10.1007/s40489-017-0121-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Crafa D, Warfa N. Maternal migration and autism risk: systematic analysis. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2015) 27:64–71. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2014.995601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Dealberto MJ. Prevalence of autism according to maternal immigrant status and ethnic origin. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2011) 123:339–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01662.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kawa R, Saemundsen E, Jonsdottir SL, Hellendoorn A, Lemcke S, Canal-Bedia R, et al. European studies on prevalence and risk of autism spectrum disorders according to immigrant status—a review. Eur J Public Health. (2017) 27:101–10. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckw206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lehti V, Chudal R, Suominen A, Gissler M, Sourander A. Association between immigrant background and ADHD: a nationwide population-based case-control study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:967–75. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lehti V, Gyllenberg D, Suominen A, Sourander A. Finnish-born children of immigrants are more likely to be diagnosed with developmental disorders related to speech and language, academic skills and coordination. Acta Paediatr. (2018) 107:1409–17. doi: 10.1111/apa.14308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Huss M, Hölling H, Kurth B-M, Schlack R. How often are German children and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD? Prevalence based on the judgment of health care professionals: results of the German health and examination survey (KiGGS). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 17:52–8. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-1006-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Abdullahi I, Wong K, Mutch R, Glasson EJ, de Klerk N, Cherian S, et al. Risk of developmental disorders in children of immigrant mothers: a population-based data linkage evaluation. J Pediatr. (2019) 204:275–84.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.08.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lin SC, Yu SM, Harwood RL. Autism spectrum disorders and developmental disabilities in children from immigrant families in the United States. Pediatrics. (2012) 130(Suppl. 2):S191–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0900R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Angell AM, Empey A, Zuckerman KE. Chapter four—a review of diagnosis and service disparities among children with autism from racial and ethnic minority groups in the United States. In: Hodapp RM, Fidler DJ, editors. International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (2018). p. 145–80.

Google Scholar

11. Sritharan B, Koola MM. Barriers faced by immigrant families of children with autism: a program to address the challenges. Asian J Psychiatr. (2019) 39:53–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2018.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Chlebowski C, Robins DL, Barton ML, Fein D. Large-scale use of the modified checklist for autism in low-risk toddlers. Pediatrics. (2013) 131:e1121–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gillberg C, Steffenburg S, Borjesson B, Andersson L. Infantile autism in children of immigrant parents. A population-based study from Goteborg, Sweden. Br J Psychiatry. (1987) 150:856–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Augereau N, Lagdas I, Kermarrec S, Gicquel L, Martin V, Xavier J, et al. Premigration social adversity and autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry Open. (2020) 6:e92. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2020.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Gillberg C, Schaumann H, Gillberg IC. Autism in immigrants: children born in Sweden to mothers born in Uganda. J Intellect Disabil Res. (1995) 39:141–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1995.tb00482.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism in immigrants: a population-based study from Swedish rural and urban areas. J Intellect Disabil Res. (1996) 40:24–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1996.tb00599.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: a nationwide register-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2005) 46:963–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00391.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Keen DV, Reid FD, Arnone D. Autism, ethnicity and maternal immigration. Br J Psychiatry. (2010) 196:274. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.065490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Barnevik-Olsson M, Gillberg C, Fernell E. Prevalence of autism in children of Somali origin living in Stockholm: brief report of an at-risk population. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2010) 52:1167–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03812.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Magnusson C, Rai D, Goodman A, Lundberg M, Idring S, Svensson A, et al. Migration and autism spectrum disorder: population-based study. Br J Psychiatry. (2012) 201:109–15. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.095125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Schieve LA, Boulet SL, Blumberg SJ, Kogan MD, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Boyle CA, et al. Association between parental nativity and autism spectrum disorder among US-born non-Hispanic white and Hispanic children, 2007 National Survey of Children's Health. Disabil Health J. (2012) 5:18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Becerra TA, von Ehrenstein OS, Heck JE, Olsen J, Arah OA, Jeste SS, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and race, ethnicity, and nativity: a population-based study. Pediatrics. (2014) 134:e63–71. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tordjman S, Cohen D, Anderson GM, Botbol M, Canitano R, Coulon N, et al. Reframing autism as a behavioral syndrome and not a specific mental disorder: implications of genetic and phenotypic heterogeneity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 89:132–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Tordjman S, Somogyi E, Coulon N, Kermarrec S, Cohen D, Bronsard G, et al. Gene × environment interactions in autism spectrum disorders: role of epigenetic mechanisms. Front Psychiatry. (2014) 5:53. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dealberto M-J. Are different subtypes of autism spectrum disorders associated with different factors? Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2013) 128:1–2. doi: 10.1111/acps.12063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lehti V, Cheslack-Postava K, Gissler M, Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, Brown AS, Sourander A. Parental migration and Asperger's syndrome. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2015) 24:941–8. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0643-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Murray MJ. Children of migrant parents may be at greater risk of low-functioning autism spectrum disorder, but lower risk of high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Evid Based Ment Health. (2013) 16:24. doi: 10.1136/eb-2012-101025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hölling H, Kurth BM, Rothenberger A, Becker A, Schlack R. Assessing psychopathological problems of children and adolescents from 3 to 17 years in a nationwide representative sample: results of the German health interview and examination survey for children and adolescents (KiGGS). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 17(Suppl. 1):34–41. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-1004-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wüstner A, Otto C, Schlack R, Hölling H, Klasen F, Ravens-Sieberer U. Risk and protective factors for the development of ADHD symptoms in children and adolescents: results of the longitudinal BELLA study. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0214412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Rydell AM. Family factors and children's disruptive behaviour: an investigation of links between demographic characteristics, negative life events and symptoms of ODD and ADHD. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2010) 45:233–44. doi: 10.1007/s00127-009-0060-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sahuric A, Hohwü L, Bang Madsen K, Christensen AF, Snefstrup MV, Obel C, et al. Differential parent and teacher reports of ADHD symptoms according to the child's country of origin: a quantitative study from Denmark exploring the implication for diagnosis. J Atten Disord. (2019). doi: 10.1177/1087054719895309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Derluyn I, Broekaert E, Schuyten G. Emotional and behavioural problems in migrant adolescents in Belgium. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 17:54–62. doi: 10.1007/s00787-007-0636-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sagatun Å, Lien L, Søgaard AJ, Bjertness E, Heyerdahl S. Ethnic Norwegian and ethnic minority adolescents in Oslo, Norway. A longitudinal study comparing changes in mental health. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2008) 43:87–95. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0275-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Schulpen TWJ, Wiznitzer M, Fedder H, Buitelaar JK. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders among children of different ethnic origin. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2007) 35:556–66. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9112-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Morgan PL, Hillemeier MM, Farkas G, Maczuga S. Racial/ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis by kindergarten entry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2014) 55:905–13. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Morgan PL, Staff J, Hillemeier MM, Farkas G, Maczuga S. Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis from kindergarten to eighth grade. Pediatrics. (2013) 132:85. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Pastor PN, Reuben CA. Racial and ethnic differences in ADHD and LD in young school-age children: parental reports in the National Health Interview Survey. Public Health Rep. (2005) 120:383–92. doi: 10.1177/003335490512000405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Coker TR, Elliott MN, Toomey SL, Schwebel DC, Cuccaro P, Emery ST, et al. Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis and treatment. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20160407. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Bax AC, Bard DE, Cuffe SP, McKeown RE, Wolraich ML. The association between race/ethnicity and socioeconomic factors and the diagnosis and treatment of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2019) 40:81–91. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Collins KP, Cleary SD. Racial and ethnic disparities in parent-reported diagnosis of ADHD: national survey of children's health (2003, 2007, and 2011). J Clin Psychiatry. (2016) 77:52–9. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Chung W, Jiang SF, Paksarian D, Nikolaidis A, Castellanos FX, Merikangas KR, et al. Trends in the prevalence and incidence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adults and children of different racial and ethnic groups. JAMA Netw Open. (2019) 2:e1914344. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.14344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Siegel CE, Laska EM, Wanderling JA, Hernandez JC, Levenson RB. Prevalence and diagnosis rates of childhood ADHD among racial-ethnic groups in a public mental health system. Psychiatr Serv. (2016) 67:199–205. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201400364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Leonard H, Glasson E, Nassar N, Whitehouse A, Bebbington A, Bourke J, et al. Autism and intellectual disability are differentially related to sociodemographic background at birth. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e17875. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Leonard H, Petterson B, De Klerk N, Zubrick SR, Glasson E, Sanders R, et al. Association of sociodemographic characteristics of children with intellectual disability in Western Australia. Soc Sci Med. (2005) 60:1499–513. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Emerson E. Deprivation, ethnicity and the prevalence of intellectual and developmental disabilities. J Epidemiol Community Health. (2012) 66:218–24. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.111773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Delobel-Ayoub M, Ehlinger V, Klapouszczak D, Maffre T, Raynaud JP, Delpierre C, et al. Socioeconomic disparities and prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0141964. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stich HL, Baune BT, Caniato RN, Mikolajczyk RT, Krämer A. Individual development of preschool children-prevalences and determinants of delays in Germany: a cross-sectional study in Southern Bavaria. BMC Pediatr. (2012) 12:188. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-12-188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Altarac M, Saroha E. Lifetime prevalence of learning disability among US children. Pediatrics. (2007) 119(Suppl. 1):S77–83. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2089L

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Singh GK, Yu SM, Kogan MD. Health, chronic conditions, and behavioral risk disparities among U.S. immigrant children and adolescents. Public Health Rep. (2013) 128:463–79. doi: 10.1177/003335491312800606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tseng Y-C, Guo H-R, Lai D-C. Maternal nationality and developmental delays in young children: analysis of the data from the national registry in Taiwan. Res Dev Disabil. (2016) 53-54:127–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Schmengler H, El-Khoury Lesueur F, Yermachenko A, Taine M, Cohen D, Peyre H, et al. Maternal immigrant status and signs of neurodevelopmental problems in early childhood: the French representative ELFE birth cohort. Autism Res. (2019) 12:1845–59. doi: 10.1002/aur.2181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gibertoni D, Sansavini A, Savini S, Locatelli C, Ancora G, Perrone E, et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of preterm infants of Italian native-born and migrant mothers and role of neonatal feeding. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:4588. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lehti V, Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, Cheslack-Postava K, Gissler M, Brown AS, Sourander A. The risk of childhood autism among second-generation migrants in Finland: a case-control study. BMC Pediatr. (2013) 13:171. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ng M, de Montigny JG, Ofner M, Docé MT. Environmental factors associated with autism spectrum disorder: a scoping review for the years 2003-2013. Health Promot Chronic Dis Prev Can. (2017) 37:1–23. doi: 10.24095/hpcdp.37.1.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Guinchat V, Thorsen P, Laurent C, Cans C, Bodeau N, Cohen D. Pre-, peri- and neonatal risk factors for autism. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. (2012) 91:287–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0412.2011.01325.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Serati M, Barkin JL, Orsenigo G, Altamura AC, Buoli M. Research review: the role of obstetric and neonatal complications in childhood attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder—a systematic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2017) 58:1290–300. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Thapar A, Cooper M, Eyre O, Langley K. Practitioner review: what have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2013) 54:3–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Huang J, Zhu T, Qu Y, Mu D. Prenatal, perinatal and neonatal risk factors for intellectual disability: a systemic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0153655. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Martin-Fernandez J, Grillo F, Tichit C, Parizot I, Chauvin P. Overweight according to geographical origin and time spent in France: a cross sectional study in the Paris metropolitan area. BMC Public Health. (2012) 12:937. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sanchez CE, Barry C, Sabhlok A, Russell K, Majors A, Kollins SH, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and child neurodevelopmental outcomes: a meta-analysis. Obes Rev. (2017) 19:464–84. doi: 10.1111/obr.12643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Douzet F, Robine J. “Les jeunes des banlieues”: neighborhood effects on the immigrant youth experience in France. J Cult Geog. (2015) 32:40–53. doi: 10.1080/08873631.2015.1004854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Racape J, Schoenborn C, Sow M, Alexander S, De Spiegelaere M. Are all immigrant mothers really at risk of low birth weight and perinatal mortality? The crucial role of socio-economic status. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. (2016) 16:75. doi: 10.1186/s12884-016-0860-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Miller LL, Gustafsson HC, Tipsord J, Song M, Nousen E, Dieckmann N, et al. Is the association of ADHD with socio-economic disadvantage explained by child comorbid externalizing problems or parent ADHD? J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2018) 46:951–63. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0356-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Marques A, O'Connor T, Roth C, Susser E, Bjørke-Monsen A-L. The influence of maternal prenatal and early childhood nutrition and maternal prenatal stress on offspring immune system development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Neurosci. (2013) 7:120. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rauh VA, Margolis AE. Research review: environmental exposures, neurodevelopment, and child mental health—new paradigms for the study of brain and behavioral effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:775–93. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Gorini F, Muratori F, Morales MA. The role of heavy metal pollution in neurobehavioral disorders: a focus on autism. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 1:354–72. doi: 10.1007/s40489-014-0028-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Volk HE, Lurmann F, Penfold B, Hertz-Picciotto I, McConnell R. Traffic-related air pollution, particulate matter, and autism. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:71–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Grineski SE, Collins TW, Morales DX. Asian Americans and disproportionate exposure to carcinogenic hazardous air pollutants: a national study. Soc Sci Med. (2017) 185:71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.05.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Aghaei M, Janjani H, Yousefian F, Jamal A, Yunesian M. Association between ambient gaseous and particulate air pollutants and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children; a systematic review. Environ Res. (2019) 173:135–56. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2019.03.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Etchevers A, Bretin P, Lecoffre C, Bidondo M-L, Le Strat Y, Glorennec P, et al. Blood lead levels and risk factors in young children in France, 2008-2009. Int J Hyg Environ Health. (2014) 217:528–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Heaman M, Bayrampour H, Kingston D, Blondel B, Gissler M, Roth C, et al. Migrant women's utilization of prenatal care: a systematic review. Matern Child Health J. (2013) 17:816–36. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-1058-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Ben Amor L, Grizenko N, Schwartz G, Lageix P, Baron C, Ter-Stepanian M, et al. Perinatal complications in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2005) 30:120–6. Available online at: http://jpn.ca/vol30-issue2/30-2-120/

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

73. Johnson EB, Reed SD, Hitti J, Batra M. Increased risk of adverse pregnancy outcome among Somali immigrants in Washington state. Am J Obstet Gynecol. (2005) 193:475–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2004.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Lubotzky-Gete S, Shoham-Vardi I, Sheiner E. Comparing pregnancy outcomes of immigrants from Ethiopia and the former Soviet Union to Israel, to those of native-born Israelis. J Immigr Minor Health. (2017) 19:1296–303. doi: 10.1007/s10903-016-0484-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Urquia ML, Glazier RH, Gagnon AJ, Mortensen LH, Nybo Andersen AM, Janevic T, et al. Disparities in pre-eclampsia and eclampsia among immigrant women giving birth in six industrialised countries. BJOG. (2014) 121:1492–500. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.12758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Vinkhuyzen AAE, Eyles DW, Burne THJ, Blanken LME, Kruithof CJ, Verhulst F, et al. Gestational vitamin D deficiency and autism-related traits: the Generation R Study. Mol Psychiatry. (2018) 23:240–6. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Magnusson C, Kosidou K, Dalman C, Lundberg M, Lee BK, Rai D, et al. Maternal vitamin D deficiency and the risk of autism spectrum disorders: population-based study. BJPsych Open. (2016) 2:170–2. doi: 10.1192/bjpo.bp.116.002675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Fernell E, Barnevik-Olsson M, Bagenholm G, Gillberg C, Gustafsson S, Saaf M. Serum levels of 25-hydroxyvitamin D in mothers of Swedish and of Somali origin who have children with and without autism. Acta Paediatr. (2010) 99:743–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01755.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. van der Ven E, Termorshuizen F, Laan W, Breetvelt EJ, van Os J, Selten JP. An incidence study of diagnosed autism-spectrum disorders among immigrants to the Netherlands. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2013) 128:54–60. doi: 10.1111/acps.12054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Sucksdorff M, Brown AS, Chudal R, Surcel H-M, Hinkka-Yli-Salomäki S, Cheslack-Postava K, et al. Maternal vitamin D levels and the risk of offspring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 60:142–51.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Morales E, Julvez J, Torrent M, Ballester F, Rodríguez-Bernal CL, Andiarena A, et al. Vitamin D in pregnancy and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-like symptoms in childhood. Epidemiology. (2015) 26:458–65. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, Braeken M, de Rooij SR, Entringer S, et al. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: the influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 117:26–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Wadhwa PD, Buss C, Entringer S, Swanson JM. Developmental origins of health and disease: brief history of the approach and current focus on epigenetic mechanisms. Semin Reprod Med. (2009) 27:358–68. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1237424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Kalt A, Hossain M, Kiss L, Zimmerman C. Asylum seekers, violence and health: a systematic review of research in high-income host countries. Am J Public Health. (2013) 103:e30–42. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Ikram UZ, Snijder MB, Fassaert TJL, Schene AH, Kunst AE, Stronks K. The contribution of perceived ethnic discrimination to the prevalence of depression. Eur J Public Health. (2015) 25:243–8. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cku180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. D'Anna-Hernandez KL, Hoffman MC, Zerbe GO, Coussons-Read M, Ross RG, Laudenslager ML. Acculturation, maternal cortisol, and birth outcomes in women of Mexican descent. Psychosom Med. (2012) 74:296–304. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318244fbde

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Abdullahi I, Wong K, Bebbington K, Mutch R, de Klerk N, Cherian S, et al. Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder according to maternal race-ethnicity and country of birth: a register-based study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:3611–24. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04068-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Bonnot O, Tanguy M-L, Consoli A, Cornic F, Graindorge C, Laurent C, et al. Does catatonia influence the phenomenology of childhood onset schizophrenia beyond motor symptoms? Psychiatry Res. (2008) 158:356–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Cantor-Graae E, Selten J-P. Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis and review. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:12–24. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Selten J-P, Cantor-Graae E, Kahn RS. Migration and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2007) 20:111–5. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328017f68e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Bulla J, Hoffmann K, Querengässer J, Ross T. Socioeconomic disadvantage and schizophrenia in migrants under mental health detention orders. Int J Soc Psychiatry. (2017) 63:550–8. doi: 10.1177/0020764017716696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Franz L, Chambers N, von Isenburg M, de Vries PJ. Autism spectrum disorder in Sub-Saharan Africa: a comprehensive scoping review. Autism Res. (2017) 10:723–49. doi: 10.1002/aur.1766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh Y-J, Kim YS, Kauchali S, Marcín C, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. (2012) 5:160–79. doi: 10.1002/aur.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Dai YG, Burke JD, Naigles L, Eigsti IM, Fein DA. Language abilities in monolingual- and bilingual-exposed children with autism or other developmental disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2018) 55:38–49. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2018.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Petersen JM, Marinova-Todd SH, Mirenda P. Brief report: an exploratory study of lexical skills in bilingual children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 42:1499–503. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1366-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Gonzalez-Barrero AM, Nadig A. Bilingual children with autism spectrum disorders: the impact of amount of language exposure on vocabulary and morphological skills at school age. Autism Res. (2018) 11:1667–78. doi: 10.1002/aur.2023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Uljarević M, Katsos N, Hudry K, Gibson JL. Practitioner review: multilingualism and neurodevelopmental disorders—an overview of recent research and discussion of clinical implications. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:1205–17. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. De Houwer A, Bornstein MH, Putnick DL. A bilingual-monolingual comparison of young children's vocabulary size: evidence from comprehension and production. Appl Psycholinguist. (2014) 35:1189–211. doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Lai M-C, Kassee C, Besney R, Bonato S, Hull L, Mandy W, et al. Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 6:819–29. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Mannion A, Leader G. Comorbidity in autism spectrum disorder: a literature review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2013) 7:1595–616. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Muskens JB, Velders FP, Staal WG. Medical comorbidities in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorders: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 26:1093–103. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1020-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Begeer S, Bouk SE, Boussaid W, Terwogt MM, Koot HM. Underdiagnosis and referral bias of autism in ethnic minorities. J Autism Dev Disord. (2009) 39:142–8. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0611-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Chang L-Y, Wang M-Y, Tsai P-S. Diagnostic accuracy of rating scales for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2016) 137:e20152749. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Algorta GP, Dodd AL, Stringaris A, Youngstrom EA. Diagnostic efficiency of the SDQ for parents to identify ADHD in the UK: a ROC analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 25:949–57. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0815-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Pierce K, Courchesne E, Bacon E. To screen or not to screen universally for autism is not the question: why the task force got it wrong. J Pediatr. (2016) 176:182–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Mandell DS, Listerud J, Levy SE, Pinto-Martin JA. Race differences in the age at diagnosis among medicaid-eligible children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2002) 41:1447–53. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Mandell DS, Wiggins LD, Carpenter LA, Daniels J, DiGuiseppi C, Durkin MS, et al. Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of children with autism spectrum disorders. Am J Public Health. (2009) 99:493–8. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.131243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Zuckerman KE, Lindly OJ, Reyes NM, Chavez AE, Macias K, Smith KN, et al. Disparities in diagnosis and treatment of autism in Latino and non-Latino White families. Pediatrics. (2017) 139:e20163010. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-3010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Barrett B, Byford S, Sharac J, Hudry K, Leadbitter K, Temple K, et al. Service and wider societal costs of very young children with autism in the UK. J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 42:797–804. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1306-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Salomone E, Beranová Š, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Briciet Lauritsen M, Budisteanu M, Buitelaar J, et al. Use of early intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorder across Europe. Autism. (2016) 20:233–49. doi: 10.1177/1362361315577218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Chamak B, Jeandidier B. L'autisme au quotidien: témoignage d'un pédiatre en Seine-Saint-Denis. Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence. (2016) 64:360–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neurenf.2016.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Chamak B. L'autisme à Marseille Nord: inégalités territoriales, précarité et pénurie de services publics. Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence. (2019) 67:304–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neurenf.2019.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Ministère des Solidarités et de la Santé. La mise en place de la Protection Universelle Maladie. (2019). Available online at: https://solidarites-sante.gouv.fr/affaires-sociales/securite-sociale/article/la-mise-en-place-de-la-protection-universelle-maladie (accessed September 9, 2020).

114. Arat A, Östberg V, Burström B, Hjern A. ADHD medication in offspring of immigrants—does the income level of the country of parental origin matter? BMC Psychiatry. (2018) 18:3. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1572-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. van den Ban EF, Souverein PC, van Engeland H, Swaab H, Egberts TCG, Heerdink ER. Differences in ADHD medication usage patterns in children and adolescents from different cultural backgrounds in the Netherlands. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2015) 50:1153–62. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1068-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Slobodin O, Masalha R. Challenges in ADHD care for ethnic minority children: a review of the current literature. Transcult Psychiatry. (2020) 57:468–83. doi: 10.1177/1363461520902885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Cummings JR, Ji X, Allen L, Lally C, Druss BG. Racial and ethnic differences in ADHD treatment quality among Medicaid-enrolled youth. Pediatrics. (2017) 139:e20162444. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Millau M, Rivard M, Mello C. Immigrant families' perception of the causes, first manifestations, and treatment of autism spectrum disorder. J Child Fam Stud. (2018) 27:3468–81. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1180-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Zuckerman KE, Sinche B, Mejia A, Cobian M, Becker T, Nicolaidis C. Latino parents' perspectives on barriers to autism diagnosis. Acad Pediatr. (2014) 14:301–8. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2013.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Rivard M, Millau M, Magnan C, Mello C, Boulé M. Snakes and ladders: barriers and facilitators experienced by immigrant families when accessing an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis. J Dev Phys Disabil. (2019) 31:519–39. doi: 10.1007/s10882-018-9653-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Khanlou N, Haque N, Mustafa N, Vazquez LM, Mantini A, Weiss J. Access barriers to services by immigrant mothers of children with autism in Canada. Int J Ment Health Addict. (2017) 15:239–59. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9732-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Parish S, Magaña S, Rose R, Timberlake M, Swaine JG. Health care of Latino children with autism and other developmental disabilities: quality of provider interaction mediates utilization. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. (2012) 117:304–15. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-117.4.304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Nilses Å, Jingrot M, Linnsand P, Gillberg C, Nygren G. Experiences of immigrant parents in Sweden participating in a community assessment and intervention program for preschool children with autism. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2019) 15:3397–410. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S221908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Abdullahi I, Kruger E, Tennant M. Service access and perceptions for Somali Australian migrants at risk of autism. Int J Migr Health Soc Care. (2017) 13:119–25. doi: 10.1108/IJMHSC-09-2015-0031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. de Leeuw A, Happé F, Hoekstra RA. A conceptual framework for understanding the cultural and contextual factors on autism across the globe. Autism Res. (2020) 13:1029–50. doi: 10.1002/aur.2276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Paidipati CP, Brawner B, Eiraldi R, Deatrick JA. Parent and family processes related to ADHD management in ethnically diverse youth. J Am Psychiatr Nurses Assoc. (2017) 23:90–112. doi: 10.1177/1078390316687023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Farver JAM, Shin YL. Social pretend play in Korean- and Anglo-American preschoolers. Child Dev. (1997) 68:544–56. doi: 10.2307/1131677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Blacher J, Cohen SR, Azad G. In the eye of the beholder: reports of autism symptoms by Anglo and Latino mothers. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2014) 8:1648–56. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Hussein AM, Pellicano E, Crane L. Understanding and awareness of autism among Somali parents living in the United Kingdom. Autism. (2018) 23:1408–18. doi: 10.1177/1362361318813996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Buitelaar JK, Schulpen TWJ. Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2006) 15:418–26. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0550-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Lawton KE, Gerdes AC, Haack LM, Schneider B. Acculturation, cultural values, and Latino parental beliefs about the etiology of ADHD. Adm Policy Ment Health. (2014) 41:189–204. doi: 10.1007/s10488-012-0447-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Fox F, Aabe N, Turner K, Redwood S, Rai D. “It was like walking without knowing where I was going”: a qualitative study of autism in a UK Somali migrant community. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:305–15. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2952-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Perry CE, Hatton D, Kendall J. Latino parents' accounts of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Transcult Nurs. (2005) 16:312–21. doi: 10.1177/1043659605278938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Pham AV, Carlson JS, Kosciulek JF. Ethnic differences in parental beliefs of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and treatment. J Atten Disord. (2010) 13:584–91. doi: 10.1177/1087054709332391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Khowaja MK, Hazzard AP, Robins DL. Sociodemographic barriers to early detection of autism: screening and evaluation using the M-CHAT, M-CHAT-R, and follow-up. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:1797–808. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2339-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Herlihy LE. Racial/ethnic and socioeconomic differences in screening toddlers for autism spectrum disorders using the M-CHAT (dissertation). Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut (2014). Available from: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations/454/ (accessed March 03, 2021).

Google Scholar

137. Herlihy LE, Brooks B, Dumont-Mathieu T, Barton ML, Fein D, Chen C-M, et al. Standardized screening facilitates timely diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in a diverse sample of low-risk toddlers. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2014) 35:85–92. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Seif Eldin A, Habib D, Noufal A, Farrag S, Bazaid K, Al-Sharbati M, et al. Use of M-CHAT for a multinational screening of young children with autism in the Arab countries. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2008) 20:281–9. doi: 10.1080/09540260801990324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Scarpa A, Reyes NM, Patriquin MA, Lorenzi J, Hassenfeldt TA, Desai VJ, et al. The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: reliability in a diverse rural American sample. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:2269–79. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1779-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Guthrie W, Wallis K, Bennett A, Brooks E, Dudley J, Gerdes M, et al. Accuracy of autism screening in a large pediatric network. Pediatrics. (2019) 144:e20183963. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-3963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Carruthers S, Kinnaird E, Rudra A, Smith P, Allison C, Auyeung B, et al. A cross-cultural study of autistic traits across India, Japan and the UK. Mol Autism. (2018) 9:52. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0235-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Wang J, Lee L-C, Chen Y-S, Hsu J-W. Assessing autistic traits in a Taiwan preschool population: cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 42:2450–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1499-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Bölte S, Poustka F, Constantino JN. Assessing autistic traits: cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Res. (2008) 1:354–63. doi: 10.1002/aur.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Cheon K-A, Park J-I, Koh Y-J, Song J, Hong H-J, Kim Y-K, et al. The social responsiveness scale in relation to DSM IV and DSM5 ASD in Korean children. Autism Res. (2016) 9:970–80. doi: 10.1002/aur.1671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Tehrani-Doost M, Shahrivar Z, Torabi N, Ansari S, Haji-Esmaeelzadeh M, Saeed-Ahmadi S. Cross-cultural validation and normative data of the social responsiveness scale in a group of Iranian general child population. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:2389–96. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3773-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Gau SS-F, Lee C-M, Lai M-C, Chiu Y-N, Huang Y-F, Kao J-D, et al. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the social communication questionnaire. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:809–18. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Zarokanellou V, Kolaitis G, Vlassopoulos M, Papanikolaou K. Brief report: a pilot study of the validity and reliability of the Greek version of the social communication questionnaire. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2017) 38:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Bölte S, Holtmann M, Poustka F. The social communicaton questionnaire (SCQ) as a screener for autism spectrum disorders: additional evidence and cross-cultural validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 47:719–20. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816c42bd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Aldosari M, Fombonne E, Aldhalaan H, Ouda M, Elhag S, Alshammari H, et al. Validation of the Arabic version of the social communication questionnaire. Autism. (2019) 23:1655–62. doi: 10.1177/1362361318816065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Avcil S, Baykara B, Baydur H, Münir KM, Inal Emiroglu N. The validity and reliability of the social communication questionnaire—Turkish form in autistics aged 4-18 years. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. (2015) 26:56–64.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

151. Soto S, Linas K, Jacobstein D, Biel M, Migdal T, Anthony BJ. A review of cultural adaptations of screening tools for autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2014) 19:646–61. doi: 10.1177/1362361314541012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Al Maskari TS, Melville CA, Willis DS. Systematic review: cultural adaptation and feasibility of screening for autism in non-English speaking countries. Int J Ment Health Syst. (2018) 12:22. doi: 10.1186/s13033-018-0200-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Samadi SA, McConkey R. Screening for autism in Iranian preschoolers: contrasting M-CHAT and a scale developed in Iran. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:2908–16. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2454-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Rea KE, Armstrong-Brine M, Ramirez L, Stancin T. Ethnic disparities in autism spectrum disorder screening and referral: implications for pediatric practice. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2019) 40:493–500. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Modabbernia A, Velthorst E, Reichenberg A. Environmental risk factors for autism: an evidence-based review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Mol Autism. (2017) 8:13. doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0121-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

156. Janvier YM, Harris JF, Coffield CN, Louis B, Xie M, Cidav Z, et al. Screening for autism spectrum disorder in underserved communities: early childcare providers as reporters. Autism. (2015) 20:364–73. doi: 10.1177/1362361315585055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Mandell DS, Novak M. The role of culture in families' treatment decisions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2005) 11:110–5. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Welterlin A, LaRue RH. Serving the needs of immigrant families of children with autism. Disabil Soc. (2007) 22:747–60. doi: 10.1080/09687590701659600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Lee JD, Meadan H. Parent-mediated interventions for children with ASD in low-resource settings: a scoping review. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. (2020). doi: 10.1007/s40489-020-00218-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental disabilities, autism spectrum disorder, child development, migration, neurodevelopment, migrant health, maternal and child health

Citation: Schmengler H, Cohen D, Tordjman S and Melchior M (2021) Autism Spectrum and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders in Children of Immigrants: A Brief Review of Current Evidence and Implications for Clinical Practice. Front. Psychiatry 12:566368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.566368

Received: 27 May 2020; Accepted: 24 February 2021;
Published: 18 March 2021.

Edited by:

Roberto Canitano, Siena University Hospital, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marco Carotenuto, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy
Peng-Chou Tsai, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Schmengler, Cohen, Tordjman and Melchior. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria Melchior, maria.melchior@inserm.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.