Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 March 2021
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Recent Empirical Research and Methodologies in Defense Mechanisms View all 17 articles

Increasing Mentalization to Reduce Maladaptive Defense in Patients With Mental Disorders

\nMarkus C. Hayden,
Markus C. Hayden1,2*Pia K. MüllauerPia K. Müllauer1Klea J. P. BeyerKlea J. P. Beyer1Richard GaugelerRichard Gaugeler3Birgit SenftBirgit Senft4Maria C. DehoustMaria C. Dehoust5Sylke AndreasSylke Andreas1
  • 1Institut für Psychologie, Alpen-Adria-Universität Klagenfurt, Klagenfurt, Austria
  • 2Klinik Bad Reichenhall der Deutschen Rentenversicherung Bayern-Süd, Bad Reichenhall, Germany
  • 3Öffentliches Krankenhaus Waiern, Feldkirchen in Kärnten, Austria
  • 4Reha-Klinik für Seelische Gesundheit und Prävention, Klagenfurt, Austria
  • 5Psychosomatische Klinik Ginsterhof, Rosengarten, Germany

Background: There are indications of associations between the ability to mentalize and psychological defense mechanisms. However, only a few studies have focused on these associations, and even fewer have included empirical analyses. In the present study, we aimed to fill this research gap by analyzing the link between the ability to mentalize and psychological defense mechanisms in patients with mental disorders. We examined whether changes in defense mechanisms are predicted by an increase in mentalization or whether such changes are only related to reductions in psychopathology and interpersonal problems.

Methods: A clinical sample of N = 89 patients was studied during and after inpatient psychiatric rehabilitation. Repeated-measures analyses of variance were performed to determine changes in mentalization, psychological defense, psychopathology, and interpersonal problems over the course of therapy and post-treatment. Linear regression analyses were used to predict the change in defense patterns based on an increase in mentalization.

Results: Maladaptive defense mechanisms were significantly reduced during inpatient therapy and remained low until follow-up, whereas neurotic and adaptive defense mechanisms did not change significantly. The results of the regression analyses indicated that mentalization played an important role in the reduction in maladaptive defense during and after inpatient rehabilitation for mental disorders, whereas reductions in psychopathology and interpersonal distress were only partially associated with a reduction in maladaptive defense.

Conclusion: We conclude that mentalization is vital for reducing maladaptive defense mechanisms, which are commonly associated with mental disorders. In therapy, an increase in patients' capacity to mentalize may be a practicable approach to diminish maladaptive defense mechanisms.

Introduction

Mentalization is a form of mostly preconscious imaginative mental activity that is defined by the ability to understand and interpret one's own and others' behavior in terms of underlying mental states. These states go beyond thoughts, feelings, and emotions and include needs, beliefs, goals, purposes, and reasons (13). High levels of mentalization are characterized by a differentiated understanding of the inner world that affects human beings and include the awareness that the mind, especially the mind of another person, cannot fully be accessed or read (2). Mentalizing enables humans to reflect upon their own and other people's perceptions and understand and anticipate associated patterns of behavior. Therefore, it plays a key role in interpersonal behavior (4, 5). Furthermore, it has proven to be a substantial factor influencing the transfer of attachment security from parents to their children (6). Mentalization is a broad concept that encompasses aspects of the self vs. others as well as both implicit and explicit and both cognitive and affective dimensions (3). Therefore, there are several conceptual overlaps between mentalization and other models, such as mindfulness, empathy, affect consciousness, and theory of mind (3, 7). The most common way to operationalize and measure the capacity to mentalize is through reflective functioning (8). To date, various instruments, such as interviews and questionnaires, have been developed for the assessment of reflective functioning (9). Mentalization is positively intercorrelated with mental stability and attachment security. A growing number of studies have emphasized the importance of mentalization as a protective factor against mental disorders (1012). On the other hand, impairments in the ability to mentalize are predictors of psychopathology and mental instability (3, 8). In the last decade, several studies have clarified the associations between a lack of mentalization and various kinds of mental disorders e.g. (1316). In particular, mental disorders that involve a pathology of the self, such as borderline personality disorder, are characterized by a distinct pattern of impairments in the ability to mentalize (3, 8).

In contrast to the relatively new field of mentalization research, studies on psychological defense have been conducted since the late 19th century (17). Sigmund Freud published his first work on defense in 1894 (18) and continued his research in the field for several decades e.g. (1921). His studies and the research published by his daughter, Anna Freud (22), described the main characteristics of psychological defense and most of the defense styles that are known today (23). Psychological defense mechanisms are characterized as unconscious mental processes that provide important self-protective effects by reducing or masking anxiety arising from unacceptable or potentially harmful stimuli (24, 25). In particular, defense mechanisms maintain psychological homeostasis, i.e., the organization of personality, in both pathological and healthy individuals (25, 26). In the absence of defense mechanisms, humans are persistently confronted with negative emotions, such as anger, sadness, and anxiety (24). Defense mechanisms are vital for a healthy relationship with the self, others, and the environment. However, these mechanisms have the ability to be potentially harmful as well, depending on the manner, frequency, and circumstances in which they are unconsciously used (27). Various forms of defense styles evolve from infancy to adolescence and adulthood, making the individual more flexible in defending himself or herself against negative stimuli (22). Contemporary psychology has adapted a hierarchical understanding of different forms of defense mechanisms based on their level of adaptiveness (28). Healthy individuals can draw on a variety of defense mechanisms that match the circumstances in which they are used. People with mental disorders, however, tend to use only a limited range of defense mechanisms that may not be adapted to the situation, for example, with respect to the individual's age or the duration or intensity of the stimulus (22). In particular, immature (or maladaptive) defense styles are frequently used by patients suffering from mental disorders (29). Furthermore, research has revealed links between physical impairment and the use of different forms of defense styles. For example, studies found that the use of immature defense mechanisms may be associated with somatic symptom severity (30) and may contribute to impaired awareness in patients with traumatic brain injury (31). Other studies identified the role of psychological defense mechanisms in patients with cancer [see (27) for a review]. Defense mechanisms are known to be relatively stable in adulthood; however, they are well documented to be dynamic and reversible, e.g., via psychotherapy (28, 32, 33).

There are several indications of associations between mentalization and defense mechanisms. For example, both mentalization and defense mechanisms play important roles in the preservation of mental stability, whereas impairments are linked to psychological strain and mental disorder. Associations have also been reported in clinical research, for example, in patients suffering from alexithymia and borderline personality disorder. Both disorders are characterized by a lack of mentalization, and both disorders are characterized by the predominance of immature defense mechanisms (34, 35). Furthermore, both the enhancement of reflective functioning and the maturation of defense mechanisms are associated with mental stability and with progress in psychotherapy, e.g., in the treatment of personality disorders (11, 28, 36, 37). Since mentalization enables humans to reflect upon their own actions and, in particular, to reflect upon the mental processes that cause their own actions, an association of mentalization with the use of various kinds of defense mechanisms seems plausible. There are also indications that an increase in mentalization, as measured by reflective functioning, may enable individuals to scrutinize their own defense mechanisms, which can in turn increase their overall capability to mentalize (38).

However, there is hardly any detailed research on the associations between mentalization and the use of psychological defense styles. Only isolated studies have considered these possible associations, and even fewer studies have empirically investigated the possible intercorrelations. Shahar and colleagues (39) emphasized a possible link between impairments in mentalizing capacities and the use of immature defense mechanisms, such as projection. The authors stated that individuals with lower mentalization scores were restricted in their use of defense mechanisms, as they had struggles identifying their own mental states and those of others. This may be an explanation for why highly burdened individuals have a tendency to use immature defense mechanisms, as people are less likely to tap the full potential of their own mentalizing abilities in situations of high emotional burden (39). In one case study (40), the author presented a phobic patient who continuously used the inhibition of mentalization as a defense against mental threats. Finally, in a study by Fischer-Kern and colleagues (35), the correlations between the primitive defenses dimension of the Structured Interview of Personality Organization (41) and reflective functioning were calculated for a sample of N = 92 female outpatients with borderline personality disorder. The analysis did not find significant intercorrelations. However, the reflective functioning scores were very homogenous, with the means of the dimensions ranging from 2.4 (SD 1.1) to 2.9 (SD 1.5) (35).

Similar to the associations between psychological defense and mentalization, the associations between defense styles and concepts related to mentalization have hardly been studied. One study that analyzed N = 107 students and graduates detected positive intercorrelations between the use of adaptive defense styles and both emotional knowledge and overall emotional intelligence as well as a negative correlation between maladaptive defense styles and emotional knowledge (42). Furthermore, Brown and colleagues (43, 44) pointed out that mindfulness can lead to less ego-defensive responsivity under social threat. In line with their assumptions, one study comparing an intervention group (N = 438) with N = 281 controls found that a seven-day Vipassana meditation retreat, as an intervention to foster mindfulness, led to a reduction in the use of immature defense mechanisms, namely, displacement, regression, and projection (45).

Since the relationship between the ability to mentalize and psychological defense has not been studied in a structured way, and there is hardly any empirical research apart from some scattered results, a link between the two concepts can currently only be hypothesized. Furthermore, it is unclear whether changes in the capacity to mentalize are linked to changes in the use of defense mechanisms. Therefore, the present study analyzed patterns of associations between mentalization and the use of different psychological defense mechanisms. Because both variables are known to be affectable by treatment (11, 28), we investigated the potential relationship in patients with mental disorders over the course of inpatient therapy and during the posttreatment follow-up. The focus of the study was on patients' subjective experiences as measured by patient-reported outcomes (46). First, we analyzed the degree to which the investigated variables changed over the course of therapy and follow-up. Then, we sought to determine which variables predicted changes in defense mechanisms. We hypothesized that these changes would be predicted not only by reductions in debilitating mental factors, i.e., psychopathology and interpersonal problems, but also by an increase in mentalization.

Materials and Methods

Design

The study was designed as a quasiexperimental longitudinal study. We surveyed patients at the beginning of inpatient therapy for psychiatric disorders (T0) and shortly before discharge from the hospital (T1). Furthermore, a follow-up measurement was conducted approximately half a year later (T2).

Instruments

We used the 40-item German version of the Defense Style Questionnaire [DSQ-40 (47)] to analyze psychological defense mechanisms. The self-report instrument is a shortened version of the Defense Style Questionnaire presented by Andrews, Pollock, and Stewart (48), and it has widely been used and studied. The DSQ-40 has been translated into various languages and has proven to be suitable in both adult and adolescent populations (26). The instrument has three dimensions that were used in the analyses: adaptive defense, intermediate (neurotic) defense, and maladaptive defense. Cronbach's alphas range from 0.58 to 0.80. The test–retest coefficients range from 0.75 to 0.85 (47). To cluster the variables according to these main categories, we followed the recommendations of Schauenburg et al. (49), describing minor adaptions to the German version compared to the original version.

To assess the ability to mentalize, we used the global scale of the German version of the Mentalization Questionnaire [MZQ (50)]. This 15-item self-report instrument has proven to be a reliable and valid tool in the assessment of mentalization and yields results that are comparable to those generated by interview measures, such as the Adult Attachment Interview (Andreas et al., submitted). Several translated versions of the MZQ have been used in adult and adolescent populations (5052). For the original German version, Cronbach's alpha for the global scale is 0.81 and the test-retest reliability is 0.76 (50).

Psychopathology was assessed using the Global Severity Index (GSI) of the German version of the Brief Symptom Inventory 18 [BSI-18 (53)]. The instrument is the latest short version of the Symptom-Checklist 90-R. A study that included N = 2516 participants demonstrated the psychometric qualities of the German version (54). The GSI score represents the number and severity of the psychopathological symptoms assessed by the BSI-18. Cronbach's alpha for the GSI of the German version is 0.93 (54).

The German 32-item version of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems [IIP-32 (55)] was used to assess difficulties within interpersonal contact. The questionnaire asks patients to rate items concerning actions (e.g., in groups or other forms of interpersonal contact) that they “do too much” and that they find “too hard to do” (56). A study by Thomas et al. (55) demonstrated that the quality indicators of the German version of the IIP-32 are comparable to the original version of the IIP. Values for Cronbach's alpha range from 0.60 to 0.82 in a standard population and from 0.59 to 0.83 in clinical populations (55). For our analyses, we used the full scale that represents the total amount of distress experienced in interpersonal contexts.

Data Collection

The data were collected in two hospitals in Austria that offer psychiatric rehabilitation. In addition to medical and pharmacological treatment options, both hospitals use psychotherapy in one-on-one settings as well as group interventions. The therapy plans further include psychoeducation, ergotherapy, physiotherapy, and physical exercise. All patients in both hospitals were at least 18 years old. The standard duration of treatment at the hospitals ranges from three to six weeks. At the beginning of therapy, all patients are diagnosed according to the ICD-10 (57).

Participation in the study was voluntary, and all the patients were informed that neither their refusal to participate nor their later withdrawal from the study would have any consequences whatsoever, particularly regarding therapy and aftercare. The exclusion criteria were an inability to complete the study questionnaire and/or take part in diagnostic interviews (i.e., an insufficient ability to understand and/or speak German, acute manic or psychotic episodes, dementia, or other forms of cognitive impairment). The study was approved by the ethical commission in charge. All patients who did not meet the exclusion criteria were asked to take part in the study within the first 4 days of therapy. For the follow-up assessments, all the patients were contacted via telephone. If a participant could not be reached, we sent a standardized form in the mail to contact them.

Data Analysis

The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25. The analysis of missing values showed a missing rate of >5% at both the case and variable levels. In total, ~12.3% percent of the data were missing. Little's test of missing completely at random (58) was not significant (chi-squared = 308.533, df = 347, p = 0.932), indicating that the data were missing completely at random. For the replacement of missing values, we used multiple imputation to obtain a complete dataset. In accordance with the recommendations of White, Royston, and Wood (59), we calculated twelve imputations.

To check for possible differences between the subsamples, we calculated an independent samples t-test for age and chi-square tests for all the other sociodemographic variables. For the t-test, homogeneity of variances was tested using Levene's test for equality of variances (60).

Repeated-measures analyses of variance (rmANOVAs) were used to analyze the changes in the variables over the course of the therapy and post-treatment. The exploratory data analysis indicated that there were no outliers in the data. Mauchly's sphericity test was used to detect violations of sphericity. Since all violations that could be detected were at the level of ε > 0.75, the analyses were adjusted using the Huynh-Feldt procedure (61).

For the main part of the study, we used linear regression analyses to predict a decrease in maladaptive defense. The decrease was calculated by subtracting the T1 values from the T0 values to determine the changes over the course of therapy and by subtracting the T2 values from the T0 values to determine the difference between the baseline assessments and the follow-up assessments. Changes in interpersonal problems and symptom severity were calculated in the same way. Since mentalization was reverse coded, an improvement was expressed as an increase in the MZQ score. Therefore, we subtracted the T0 values from the T1 values to determine changes during therapy and the T0 values from the T2 values to determine the difference between the baseline assessments and the follow-up assessments. There were no indications of multicollinearity (maximum variance inflation factor = 1.829) or autocorrelation (Durbin-Watson statistics = 1.744 and 2.078, respectively). Controlling the scatterplot did not reveal any indications of heteroscedasticity. Shapiro-Wilk tests of studentized residuals did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.493 for T1 and p = 0.113 for T2), suggesting a normal distribution of the residuals in both analyses. Casewise diagnostics indicated one case in the first analysis and two cases in the second analysis as outliers on the y-axis. However, neither of these values had a leverage above the cutoff of 2k/n that would also indicate extreme x-values (62). In all three cases, the values for Cook's distance (63) were below the cutoff of ≥1, which would indicate a problematic influence on the analyses.

Results

Participants and Dropouts

Eighty-nine patients were willing to take part in the study. The sociodemographic parameters of the sample are displayed in Table 1. The majority of the participants (n = 61, 68.5%) had main diagnoses on the F3 spectrum according to the ICD-10 (57), followed by those with F4 diagnoses. Fifty-four participants (60.7%) had more than one diagnosis. Further information on the distribution of the diagnoses is displayed in Table 2. An analysis of the differences between the two subsamples is included below.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic variables of the clinical sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Distribution of diagnoses as absolute frequencies.

Between T0 and T1, n=3 patients (3.4%) dropped out of the study; two participants quit because they had no further interest in the study, and one participant had to be excluded from the study because of an acute psychosocial crisis. Between T1 and T2, another n = 15 patients (16.9%) dropped out of the study. The most common reason for dropout (n = 9, 10.1%) was that patients could not be reached at follow-up. Two participants had no further interest in the study, one participant quit because of an acute physical disease, one participant was deceased, one participant did not specify the reason for withdrawal from the study, and one participant had to be excluded from the study because of a labile mental status.

Differences Between the Subsamples

There were no statistically significant differences between the subsamples in age (p = 0.840), sex (p = 0.353), employment status (p = 0.056), level of education (p = 0.114), parenthood (p = 0.951), or the distribution of diagnoses (p = 0.269). The only significant difference that was found was in civil status (p = 0.035), with the patients from one hospital being more likely to report a single civil status at T0.

Changes Over the Course of Therapy and During Follow-Up

The rmANOVAs indicated that among the three dimensions of the DSQ-40 (49), neither adaptive defense nor intermediate (neurotic) defense significantly changed over time (see Table 3). However, maladaptive defense was significantly reduced. A post hoc analysis revealed that the patients reported significantly fewer maladaptive behaviors at T1 than at T0. At follow-up, the use of maladaptive defense mechanisms was reduced even further, but the difference between T1 and T2 did not reach statistical significance. All the other variables significantly improved over the course of therapy and post-treatment.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the rmANOVAs.

Since the changes over the course of inpatient therapy and post-treatment in adaptive and intermediate defense styles did not reach significance, these two variables were excluded from the subsequent analyses.

Prediction of a Decrease in Maladaptive Defense

In the final step of the analyses, we investigated whether a decrease in maladaptive defense mechanisms could be predicted by an increase in mentalization or whether the decrease would be explained only by a reduction in psychopathology and/or interpersonal problems. Therefore, two linear regression models were calculated for the duration of treatment and for the period from admission to hospital until follow-up. Because there was no significant change between T1 and T2, this analysis was not conducted. The results of the analyses are displayed in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the linear regression analyses.

As shown, we obtained two significant models. The R2 for the first model was 0.176 (adjusted R2 = 0.146), indicating moderate goodness of fit according to Cohen (64). The reduction in maladaptive defense was significantly predicted by both an increase in mentalization and a reduction in interpersonal problems.

For the second model, the R2 of 0.297 (adjusted R2 = 0.272) indicated high goodness of fit (64). The increase in mentalization was again found to be a significant predictor of a decrease in maladaptive defense. However, between T0 and T2, a reduction in psychopathology was also a significant predictor, whereas interpersonal problems did not significantly affect the data.

In both analyses, an increase in mentalization that was observed over the course of the inpatient therapy and the posttreatment period significantly predicted a reduction in maladaptive defense. On the other hand, a reduction in psychopathology as well as a reduction in interpersonal problems were not found to be persistent predictors of a reduction in maladaptive defense. Both variables predicted a decrease in maladaptive defense at one measurement timepoint only.

Discussion

The aim of the study was to analyze the associations between the ability to mentalize and psychological defense mechanisms in a clinical sample. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the role of mentalization in changes in defense mechanisms during and after inpatient treatment for mental disorders.

As expected, the participants significantly improved over the course of inpatient therapy. Values for interpersonal distress decreased with a medium effect size, whereas psychopathology decreased with a large effect (64). In addition, mentalization could be significantly targeted, and the patients' mentalization scores increased over the course of therapy. The results remained stable until follow-up, indicating that the treatment had continuing effects on the patients' mental well-being. The outcomes for these three variables are consistent with a variety of previous results and confirm that inpatient therapy promotes mental health, with the promoting effect persisting after discharge from the hospital [e.g., (11, 13, 65, 66)]. Regarding psychological defense, maladaptive defense mechanisms decreased with a medium effect size during treatment and remained stable throughout follow-up. Adaptive and intermediate defense styles, however, did not change significantly over the course of inpatient therapy and post-treatment. In the comparison of these outcomes with previous studies, it is salient that some authors have reported similar results (67, 68), whereas others have reported significant improvements in more mature defense styles via therapy (28, 69, 70). A more comprehensive evaluation of previous investigations reveals that changes in more mature defense styles are linked to treatment with a long duration, which was not implemented in the current study. Therefore, longer inpatient therapy or structured ambulatory aftercare may have led to significant improvements in the patients' intermediate and/or adaptive defense styles.

Since adaptive and intermediate defense styles could not be targeted during the therapy, we excluded these two variables in the analyses and focused on the reduction in maladaptive defense mechanisms. According to our data, a decrease in maladaptive defense was more closely associated with an increase in mentalization rather than a reduction in interpersonal distress or symptom severity. While the latter two variables significantly predicted a reduction in maladaptive defense at one measurement period only, mentalization was found to be a significant predictor both between the beginning and the end of therapy as well as between the beginning of therapy and follow-up. This finding indicates that a more reflective view on one's own and perhaps other people's mental states, which supports the enhancement of mentalization, can enable patients to overcome hindering, immature defensive behavior. This outcome contrasts with the results of Fischer-Kern et al. (35), who did not find a significant correlation between mentalization, as measured by reflective functioning, and the use of primitive defense styles. However, in light of previous research, the associations found in our investigations can still be considered plausible given that mentalization plays a key role in determining and controlling emotions (2) and that humans who use more rigid emotional regulation strategies are prone to maladaptive defense styles (71, 72).

Importantly, the reduction in maladaptive defense mechanisms did not accompany a significant increase in adaptive or intermediate defense mechanisms. In other words, even though maladaptive defense styles were less commonly used by the patients, we could not detect a more frequent use of more mature defense mechanisms. This finding suggests that mentalization can help patients adopt healthier ways to cope with stressful stimuli by overcoming debilitating defense styles but not by increasing their use of mature defense styles.

While it is important to interpret the results considering the associated limitations, some implications can be identified based on the comprehensive literature on the effect of defense mechanisms on quality of life (2224). In the treatment of patients with dominant maladaptive defense styles, the promotion of mentalization may be a practicable approach for therapy progress. This strategy seems particularly appropriate if alternative treatment options have failed to produce the intended results. In general, our results support the advice to implement interventions that target an increase in the capacity to mentalize. In addition to the well-documented advantages for psychotherapy e.g. (5, 8, 11, 13, 73, 74), our results suggest that through an increase in mentalization, patients may adapt a healthier overall defense style by reducing maladaptive elements. Since numerous studies have highlighted the importance of well-performing psychological defense in the rehabilitation and preservation of mental health (22, 24, 28, 29) as well as on humans' ability to adapt to and cope with severe medical conditions (27, 30, 31) and other forms of traumatizing life events (24, 75, 76), we consider interventions to foster mentalization as necessary in the reduction of maladaptive defense styles in psychotherapy. Regarding future research on psychological defense, our results suggest taking mentalization into account. This is particularly advisable when changes in defense styles are studied in the context of psychiatric or psychotherapy research.

Strengths and Limitations

The study combines two factors that are of particular importance in mental health, namely mentalization and psychological defense, and it is the first to empirically investigate the interaction between these factors in patients recovering from mental disorders. Further strengths are linked to the longitudinal design. First, we were able to detect and analyze changes in the main variables over the course of inpatient treatment and further after the discharge from the clinic. Second, the study design also allowed an analysis of the role of mentalization in the reduction of maladaptive defense. However, we must also acknowledge some limitations since they require a careful interpretation, especially regarding the generalizability of the results. First, the study relied on self-report measures rather than on expert ratings. This approach was chosen deliberately because we were interested in the subjective experiences of the participants. However, further studies should verify our results by augmenting self-report measures with other forms of diagnostic procedures, such as expert ratings. Another limitation concerns the treatment conditions that were used in the hospitals. Even though the study does not claim to meet the standard of a randomized controlled trial, it is important to consider that we did not use a psychotherapy intervention that specifically focused on an increase in mentalization and that the participants were not compared to a placebo or non-treatment control group. Therefore, we cannot clearly anticipate how a structured and specific treatment program, such as mentalization-based treatment (5), may have further improved the results. Future studies that compare unspecific psychotherapy vs. mentalization-based treatment and placebo intervention or an intervention with patients on a waiting list are needed. Finally, inpatient therapy did not lead to a significant reduction in intermediate defense or a significant increase in adaptive defense. Therefore, the data suggest that there is no association of intermediate defense or adaptive defense with mentalization, but we cannot exclude this possibility with certainty. Since other studies have documented the possibility for changes in more mature defense mechanisms via psychotherapy (28, 69), it is advisable to verify our results in a study with long-term psychotherapy.

Conclusion

This study is the first to empirically investigate the role of mentalization in changes in defense mechanisms over the course of rehabilitation from psychiatric disorders and during posttreatment follow-up. We detected a significant increase in mentalization and a significant reduction in maladaptive defense, psychopathology, and interpersonal problems. However, more mature defense styles did not change significantly during inpatient therapy or follow-up. Our data suggest that the reduction in maladaptive defense can be significantly predicted by an increase in mentalization both during and after inpatient therapy, whereas reductions in psychopathology and interpersonal problems appear to be less important. Mentalization appears to promote healthier ways to cope with negative stimuli, as it may reduce the prevalence of immature defense mechanisms. However, we could not find implications of the effects of mentalization on intermediate and adaptive defense mechanisms. Longer and more intense psychotherapy approaches may be necessary to foster more mature defense styles.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The study was reviewed and approved by Ethikkommission des Landes Kärnten Klinikum Klagenfurt am Wörthersee, Feschnigstraße 11 A-9020 Klagenfurt a. W., Austria. The patients provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MH, SA, MD, PM, and KB discussed the research question and derived the hypotheses. MH, SA, and PM designed the study. MH, RG, and BS directed the study and supervised the data collection in the hospitals. Data were collected by MH and PM. MH and KB analyzed the data and discussed the results with supervision from SA. The first draft of the manuscript was written by MH. All authors contributed and agreed to the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was conducted in the context of a research project on the validation of a self-rating instrument for the assessment of mentalization. This project was funded by Köhler Stiftung (Germany).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the staff of the hospitals in which the study was conducted for their assistance and support in the recruitment of participants and in data collection. Furthermore, we thank Andre Baumann and Nicolas Luh for their valuable assistance with the literature search.

References

1. Fonagy P, Allison E. What is mentalization? The concept and its foundations in developmental research. In: N Midgley, I Vrouva, editors. Minding the Child. Mentalization-Based Interventions with Children, Young People and their Families. London: Routledge (2012). p. 11–34.

Google Scholar

2. Fonagy P, Target M. The mentalization-focused approach to self pathology. J Pers Disord. (2006) 20:544–76. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2006.20.6.544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Choi-Kain LW, Gunderson JG. Mentalization: ontogeny, assessment, and application in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatr. (2008) 165:1127–35. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hayden MC, Müllauer PK, Gaugeler R, Senft B, Andreas S. Mentalization as mediator between adult attachment and interpersonal distress. Psychopathology. (2019) 52:10–7. doi: 10.1159/000496499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bateman AW, Fonagy P. Mentalization based treatment for borderline personality disorder. World Psychiatr. (2010) 9:11–15. doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00255.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Fonagy P, Steele M, Steele H, Moran GS, Higgitt AC. The capacity for understanding mental states: the reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment Health J. (1991) 12:201–18. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(199123)12:3<201::AID-IMHJ2280120307>3.0.CO;2-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Falkenström F, Solbakken OA, Möller C, Lech B, Sandell R, Holmqvist R. Reflective functioning, affect consciousness, and mindfulness: are these different functions? Psychoanal Psychol. (2014) 31:26–40. doi: 10.1037/a0034049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Katznelson H. Reflective functioning: a review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2014) 34:107–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Luyten P, Malcorps S, Fonagy P, Ensink K. Assessment of Mentalizing. In: AW Bateman, P Fonagy, editors. Handbook of Mentalizing in Mental Health Practice (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing), p. 37–62.

Google Scholar

10. Scandurra C, Dolce P, Vitelli R, Esposito G, Testa RJ, Balsam KF, Bochicchio V. Mentalizing stigma: reflective functioning as a protective factor against depression and anxiety in transgender and gender-nonconforming people. J Clin Psychol. (2020) 76:1613–30. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Hayden MC, Müllauer PK, Gaugeler R, Senft B, Andreas S. Improvements in mentalization predict improvements in interpersonal distress in patients with mental disorders. J Clin Psychol. (2018) 74:2276–86. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Berthelot N, Lemieux R, Garon-Bissonnette J, Lacharité C, Muzik M. The protective role of mentalizing: Reflective functioning as a mediator between child maltreatment, psychopathology and parental attitude in expecting parents. Child Abuse Negl. (2019) 95:104065. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Petersen B, Toft J, Christensen NB, Foldager L, Munk-jørgensen P, Windfeld M, et al. A 2-year follow-up of mentalization-oriented group therapy following day hospital treatment for patients with personality disorders. Personal Ment Health. (2010) 4:294–301. doi: 10.1002/pmh.140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fischer-Kern M, Fonagy P, Kapusta ND, Luyten P, Boss S, Naderer A, et al. Mentalizing in female inpatients with major depressive disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2013) 201:202–7. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182845c0a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Strathearn L. Borderline personality disorder, mentalization, and the neurobiology of attachment. Infant Ment Health J. (2011) 32:47–69. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Antonsen BT, Johansen MS, Rø FG, Kvarstein EH, Wilberg T. Is reflective functioning associated with clinical symptoms and long-term course in patients with personality disorders? Compr Psychiatr. (2016) 64:46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Cramer P. Defense mechanisms: 40 years of empirical research. J Pers Assess. (2015) 97:114–22. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2014.947997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Freud S. Die Abwehr-Neuropsychosen. Versuch einer psychologischen Theorie der acquirierten Hysterie, vieler Phobien und Zwangsvorstellungen und gewisser hallucinatorischer Psychosen. Neurol Zentralblatt. (1894) 13:402–9.

Google Scholar

19. Freud S. Die Verdrängung. Int Zeitschrift für Ärztliche Psychoanal. (1915) 3:129–38.

Google Scholar

20. Freud S. Hemmung, Symptom und Angst. Wien: Internationaler Psychoanalytischer Verlag (1926).

Google Scholar

21. Freud S. Die Ichspaltung im Abwehrvorgang. Int Zeitschrift für Psychoanal und Imago. (1940) 25:241–4.

Google Scholar

22. Freud A. Das Ich und die Abwehrmechanismen. Wien: Internationaler Psychoanalytischer Verlag (1936).

Google Scholar

23. Vaillant G. Lifting the Field's “Repression” of Defenses. Am J Psychiatr. (2012) 169:885–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12050703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bowins B. Psychological defense mechanisms: a new perspective. Am J Psychoanal. (2004) 64:1–26. doi: 10.1023/B:TAJP.0000017989.72521.26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Miranda B, Louzã MR. The physician's quality of life: relationship with ego defense mechanisms and object relations. Compr Psychiatry. (2015) 63:22–9. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ruuttu T, Pelkonen M, Holi M, Karlsson L, Kiviruusu O, Heilä H, et al. Psychometric properties of the defense style questionnaire (DSQ-40) in adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2006) 194:98–105. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000198141.88926.2e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Di Giuseppe M, Ciacchini R, Micheloni T, Bertolucci I, Marchi L, Conversano C. Defense mechanisms in cancer patients: a systematic review. J Psychosom Res. (2018) 115:76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Perry JC, Bond M. Change in defense mechanisms during long-term dynamic psychotherapy and five-year outcome. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:916–25. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bond M. Empirical studies of defense style: relationships with psychopathology and change. Harv Rev Psychiatr. (2004) 12:263–78. doi: 10.1080/10673220490886167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hyphantis TN, Taunay TC, Macedo DS, Soeiro-De-Souza MG, Bisol LW, Fountoulakis KN, et al. Affective temperaments and ego defense mechanisms associated with somatic symptom severity in a large sample. J Affect Disord. (2013) 150:481–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.04.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Belchev Z, Levy N, Berman I, Levinzon H, Hoofien D, Gilboa A. Psychological traits predict impaired awareness of deficits independently of neuropsychological factors in chronic traumatic brain injury. Br J Clin Psychol. (2017) 56:213–34. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Araujo K, Ryst E, Steiner H. Adolescent defense style and life stressors. Child Psychiatr Hum Dev. (1999) 30:19–28. doi: 10.1023/A:1022666908235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Diehl M, Chui H, Hay EL, Lumley MA, Grühn D, Labouvie-Vief G. Change in coping and defense mechanisms across adulthood: Longitudinal findings in a European American sample. Dev Psychol. (2014) 50:634–48. doi: 10.1037/a0033619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Šago D, Babić G. Roots of Alexithymia. Arch Psychiatry Res. (2019) 55:71–84. doi: 10.20471/may.2019.55.01.06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fischer-Kern M, Buchheim A, Hörz S, Schuster P, Doering S, Kapusta ND, et al. The relationship between personality organization, reflective functioning, and psychiatric classification in borderline personality disorder. Psychoanal Psychol. (2010) 27:395–409. doi: 10.1037/a0020862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Keefe JR, Derubeis RJ. Changing character: a narrative review of personality change in psychotherapies for personality disorder. Psychother Res. (2019) 29:752–69. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1425930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Fitzmaurice G. Defense mechanisms reported by patients with borderline personality disorder and axis II comparison subjects over 16 years of prospective follow-up: description and prediction of recovery. Am J Psychiatr. (2013) 170:111–20. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Esposito G, Karterud S, Freda MF. Mentalizing underachievement in group counseling: analyzing the relationship between members' reflective functioning and counselors' interventions. Psychol Serv. (2019). doi: 10.1037/ser0000350. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Shahar G, Porcerelli JH, Kamoo R, Epperson CN, Czarkowski KA, Magriples U, et al. Defensive projection, superimposed on simplistic object relations, erodes patient-provider relationships in high-risk pregnancy: an empirical investigation. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. (2010) 58:953–74. doi: 10.1177/0003065110392228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bodin G. Inhibiting the mentalizing capacities. Scand Psychoanal Rev. (1996) 19:180–95. doi: 10.1080/01062301.1996.10592369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Stern BL, Caligor E, Clarkin JF, Critchfield KL, Horz S, MacCornack V, et al. Structured Interview of Personality Organization. (STIPO): preliminary psychometrics in a clinical sample. J Pers Assess. (2010) 92:35–44. doi: 10.1080/00223890903379308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pellitteri J. The relationship between emotional intelligence and ego defense mechanisms. J Psychol. (2002) 136:182–94. doi: 10.1080/00223980209604149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Brown KW, Ryan RM, Creswell JD. Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychol Inq. (2007) 18:211–37. doi: 10.1080/10478400701598298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brown KW, Ryan RM, Creswell JD, Niemiec CP. Beyond me: Mindful responses to social threat. In: Wayment HA, Bauer JJ, editors. Transcending Self-Interest: Psychological Explorations of the Quiet Ego. Washington: American Psychological Association. (2008). p. 75–84. doi: 10.1037/11771-007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Emavardhana T, Tori CD. Changes in self-concept, ego defense mechanisms, and religiosity following seven-day Vipassana meditation retreats. J Sci Study Relig. (1997) 36:194. doi: 10.2307/1387552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Greenhalgh J, Long AF, Flynn R. The use of patient reported outcome measures in routine clinical practice: lack of impact or lack of theory? Soc Sci Med. (2005) 60:833–43. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Andrews G, Singh M, Bond M. The Defense style questionnaire. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1993) 181:246–56. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199304000-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Andrews G, Pollock C, Stewart G. The determination of defense style by questionnaire. Arch Gen Psychiatr. (1989) 46:455. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810050069011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Schauenburg H, Willenborg V, Sammet I, Ehrenthal JC. Self-reported defence mechanisms as an outcome measure in psychotherapy: A study on the German version of the Defence Style Questionnaire DSQ 40. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. (2007) 80:355–66. doi: 10.1348/147608306X146068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hausberg MC, Schulz H, Piegler T, Happach CG, Klöpper M, Brütt AL, et al. Is a self-rated instrument appropriate to assess mentalization in patients with mental disorders? Development and first validation of the Mentalization Questionnaire. (MZQ). Psychother Res. (2012) 22:699–709. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2012.709325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Ponti L, Stefanini MC, Gori S, Smorti M. The assessment of mentalizing ability in adolescents: the italian adaptation of the mentalization questionnaire (MZQ). TPM - Testing, Psychom Methodol Appl Psychol. (2019) 26:29–38. doi: 10.4473/TPM26.1.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Falconer CJ, Cutting P, Bethan Davies E, Hollis C, Stallard P, Moran P. Adjunctive avatar therapy for mentalization-based treatment of borderline personality disorder: a mixed-methods feasibility study. Evid Based Ment Heal. (2017) 20:123–7. doi: 10.1136/eb-2017-102761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Spitzer C, Hammer S, Löwe B, Grabe HJ, Barnow S, Rose M, et al. Die Kurzform des Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI-18): erste Befunde zu den psychometrischen Kennwerten der deutschen Version. Fortschritte der Neurol Psychiatr. (2011) 79:517–523. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1281602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Franke GH, Jaeger S, Glaesmer H, Barkmann C, Petrowski K, Braehler E. Psychometric analysis of the brief symptom inventory 18. (BSI-18) in a representative German sample. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2017) 17:14. doi: 10.1186/s12874-016-0283-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Thomas A, Brähler E, Strauß B. IIP-32: Entwicklung, Validierung und Normierung einer Kurzform des Inventars zur Erfassung interpersonaler Probleme. Diagnostica. (2011) 57:68–83. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Barkham M, Hardy GE, Startup M. The IIP-32: a short version of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Br J Clin Psychol. (1996) 35:21–35. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1996.tb01159.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization (1992).

Google Scholar

58. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc. (1988) 83:1198. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. (2011) 30:377–99. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Levene H. Robust tests for equality of variances. In: Olkin I, Ghurye SG, Hoeffding W, Madow WG, Mann HB, editors. Contributions to Probability and Statistics: Essays in Honor of Harold Hotelling. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press (1960). p. 278–92.

61. Girden E. ANOVA: Repeated Measures. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications (1992). doi: 10.4135/9781412983419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Urban D, Mayerl J. Entdeckung und Beseitigung von Modellverstößen. In: Angewandte Regressionsanalyse: Theorie, Technik und Praxis Studienskripten zur Soziologie. Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden (2018). p. 169–299. doi: 10.1007/978-3-658-01915-0_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Cook RD. Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics. (1977) 19:15. doi: 10.2307/1268249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd. ed. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (1988).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

65. Hartmann A, Zeeck A, Barrett MS. Interpersonal problems in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. (2010) 43:619–27. doi: 10.1002/eat.20747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Liebherz S, Rabung S. Do Patients' symptoms and interpersonal problems improve in psychotherapeutic hospital treatment in germany? - a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e105329. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bond M, Perry JC. Long-term changes in defense styles with psychodynamic psychotherapy for depressive, anxiety, and personality disorders. Am J Psychiatr. (2004) 161:1665–71. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Heldt E, Blaya C, Kipper L, Salum GA, Otto MW, Manfro GG. Defense mechanisms after brief cognitive-behavior group therapy for panic disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2007) 195:540–3. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318064e7c4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Knijnik DZ, Salum GA, Blanco C, Moraes C, Hauck S, Mombach CK, et al. Defense style changes with the addition of psychodynamic group therapy to clonazepam in social anxiety disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2009) 197:547–51. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181aac833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Albucher RC, Abelson JL, Nesse RM. Defense mechanism changes in successfully treated patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (1998) 155:558–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Sala MN, Testa S, Pons F, Molina P. Emotion regulation and defense mechanisms. J Individ Differ. (2015) 36:19–29. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Di Pierro R, Benzi IMA, Madeddu F. Difficulties in emotion regulation among inpatients with substance use disorders: the mediating effect of mature defenses mechanisms. Clin Neuropsychiatr. (2015) 12:83–9.

Google Scholar

73. Bateman AW, Fonagy P. 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. Am J Psychiatr. (2008) 165:631–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Bateman A. Translation: mentalizing as treatment target in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord Theory, Res Treat. (2015) 6:380–92. doi: 10.1037/per0000113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Fang S, Chung MC, Wang Y. The impact of past trauma on psychological distress: the roles of defense mechanisms and Alexithymia. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:992. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Békés V, Perry JC, Starrs CJ. Resilience in holocaust survivors: a study of defense mechanisms in holocaust narratives. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. (2017) 26:1072–89. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2017.1320344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mentalization, psychological defense mechanisms, maladaptive defense, psychopathology, interpersonal problems

Citation: Hayden MC, Müllauer PK, Beyer KJP, Gaugeler R, Senft B, Dehoust MC and Andreas S (2021) Increasing Mentalization to Reduce Maladaptive Defense in Patients With Mental Disorders. Front. Psychiatry 12:637915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.637915

Received: 04 December 2020; Accepted: 16 February 2021;
Published: 11 March 2021.

Edited by:

Mariagrazia Di Giuseppe, University of Pisa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Gabriella Martino, University of Messina, Italy
Emanuele Maria Merlo, University of Messina, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Hayden, Müllauer, Beyer, Gaugeler, Senft, Dehoust and Andreas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Markus C. Hayden, markus.hayden@aau.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.