Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 July 2021
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Empathy and Perspective Taking: Understanding the Mechanisms of Normal and Abnormal Experiences and Abilities View all 10 articles

Cognitive Empathy in Subtypes of Antisocial Individuals

  • 1Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

Cognitive empathy allows individuals to recognize and infer how others think and feel in social situations and provides a foundation for the formation and maintenance of mutually constructive relationships. It may seem intuitive to assume that individuals who engage in antisocial behavior, who disregard the rights of others, might have problems with cognitive empathy. However, careful examination of the literature suggests that any dysfunction in cognitive empathy associated with antisociality varies by subtype of antisocial individual and is specific to subcomponents of cognitive empathy. In this review, we (1) briefly define subtypes of antisocial individuals (“psychopathic” vs. “antisocial-only”), (2) summarize specific components of cognitive empathy; (3) review existing literature examining cognitive empathy through questionnaires, behavioral tasks, and neuroimaging within different antisocial subtypes; and (4) discuss the limitations of the current research and potential future directions. Individuals in the psychopathic subtype fail to implicitly engage in cognitive empathy, and potentially lack insight into this issue reflected in no self-reported problems with cognitive empathy, but show an ability to engage in cognitive empathy when explicitly required. Individuals in the antisocial-only subtype appear able to engage in cognitive empathy, showing no differences on questionnaire or behavioral tasks that tap explicit cognitive empathy, but may display subtle difficulties accurately inferring (affective theory of mind) the emotions of others. We end the review by noting areas for future research, including the need to: (1) document the patterns of equifinality that exist across levels of analysis for these antisocial subtypes; (2) examine the temporality of empathy and antisociality development; (3) carefully consider and label subcomponents of cognitive empathy in research on antisocial behavior; and (4) investigate the intersection among environmental experiences, cognitive empathy, and antisocial behavior.

Successful social interaction requires the ability to represent what other people are thinking and feeling. This ability, often referred to as cognitive empathy, helps individuals predict and interpret others' behaviors, develop meaningful social relationships, communicate effectively, and engage in appropriate moral reasoning (1, 2). Cognitive empathy is critical in everyday social interactions, and a variety of psychiatric disorders, including autism, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia (35) are characterized by difficulties with cognitive empathy. However, psychiatric disorders associated with antisocial behaviors, which are actions that violate social norms (e.g., lying, intimidation, inflicting physical harm), show mixed effects with regard to cognitive empathy dysfunctions.

It seems intuitive to think that the actions of those who continually violate the rights of others are, in part, a reflection of the person's difficulty in representing and understanding what others might be thinking or feeling (6, 7). However, careful examination of the empirical work on cognitive empathy abilities in antisocial individuals indicates that the relationship between cognitive empathy and antisociality is far more complex than this intuitive account. The primary goal of this paper is to review research on cognitive empathy in subtypes of antisocial individuals. To this end, we (1) briefly describe two subtypes of individuals who engage in chronic and damaging antisocial behavior, (2) summarize the specific components of cognitive empathy that will be examined in this paper; (3) review existing literature examining cognitive empathy within different antisocial subtypes; and (4) discuss the limitations of the current research and potential future directions.

Subtypes of Antisocial Individuals: the Psychopathic Vs. Antisocial-Only Subtype

Individuals chronically engaging in antisocial behaviors are at risk for a variety of adverse life outcomes, such as suicide, school dropout, unemployment, psychopathology, substance abuse, and incarceration (8, 9). Moreover, estimates of the financial impact of antisocial behavior (e.g., the cost of law enforcement, incarceration, property damage, loss of wages, healthcare, etc.) on society exceed $2 trillion annually in the United States alone (10). Research demonstrates that there are two clinically meaningful subtypes of individuals engaging in high levels of antisocial behavior (see Figure 1) (1114).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Clinical assessment tools and phenotypes for “psychopathic” subtype vs “antisocial-only” subtype. Information represents common tools and tendencies across subtypes of antisocial individuals.

The first subtype, which we term the “psychopathic” subtype, are individuals infamous for their prolific antisocial behavior and their ability to be interpersonally manipulative and charming. They engage in elaborate cons, callously assault others, impulsively look for adventures, and chronically commit antisocial acts in order to obtain their goals (e.g., money, power, thrills). Psychopathic individuals commit two to three times more violent and non-violent crimes than non-psychopathic individuals, recidivate at a much higher rate, and are responsible for a disproportionate share of the estimated annual costs associated with crime in the United States (10). In his seminal writings, Cleckley states that the individual with psychopathy “. cannot be depended upon to show the ordinary responsiveness to special consideration or kindness or trust. No matter how well he is treated… he shows no consistent reaction of appreciation except superficial and transparent protestations. Such gestures are exhibited most frequently when he feels they will facilitate some personal aim” [(15), p. 354]. The individual with psychopathy, therefore, uses their ability to connect interpersonally and emotionally at a surface level in order to arrange their relationships and social transactions in ways that will benefit them, usually at the expense of others.

For adults, in both clinical and research settings, the gold standard assessment of psychopathy is Hare's Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Revised [PCL-R (16)], an interview-based measure of the interpersonal (charm, gradisotiy), affective (shallow affect, lack of empathy, lack of remorse), impulsive (poor behavioral control, irresponsibility), and antisocial (engagement in criminal activity, aggression) subcomponent characteristics of this disorder. The PCL-R rates individuals on 20 different items that cut across these four characteristics on a scale from 0 to 2 for each item. In the United States, individuals with a score of 30 or above are diagnosed with psychopathy. Approximately 15–25% of incarcerated adult offenders, and 1% of the general population, meet a diagnosis of psychopathy (1618). Other than formal diagnostic measures, some researchers utilize self-report questionnaires, such as the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (19) or the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (20) to assess psychopathy. Though there is evidence that individuals in the psychopathic subtype engage in impression management/dissimulation (21), self-report questionnaires in a research setting are valid and reliable metrics of psychopathy and correlate well with diagnostic measures (e.g., PCL-R) in community and incarcerated samples.

Moreover, there is a growing body of research demonstrating that the interpersonal, affective, and behavioral characteristics of psychopathy emerge during childhood and often persist throughout development (2224). Callous-unemotional (CU) traits are a specifier of conduct disorder (CD) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) called “limited prosocial emotions,” and include callous use of others, a lack of remorse or guilt, and an absence of empathy. Researchers theorize that, in youth, the presence of CU traits, grandiose narcissism and impulsive-antisocial traits, increase risk of developing psychopathy (14, 2530). On average, CU traits are present in 9–25% of youth offenders (2527). In addition to conceptualizing CU traits as a qualifier of a unique subgroup of youth who also show conduct problems, some researchers examine CU traits by themselves, without consideration for conduct disorder/problems. CU traits, themselves, are predictive of antisocial behavior, academic underachievement, and interpersonal problems in some youth (31, 32). Measuring CU traits without consideration of conduct disorder/problems, effectively captures the interpersonal-affective characteristics, such as shallow affect, callousness, and a lack of empathy, of the psychopathic subtype. In addition to the diagnostic criteria provided in the DSM-5, CU traits can be assessed using self-report questionnaires or other (e.g., teacher, parent)-report questionnaires [e.g., Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (33)].

The second subtype, the “antisocial-only” subtype, is defined by their chronic impulsive, irresponsible, reactively aggressive, and antisocial behavior. Unlike, the “psychopathic” subtype, these individuals are not characterized by grandiose charm and a callous, lack of empathy. Rather, individuals in this subtype are typically assessed using diagnostic criteria that reflect various antisocial acts only. Adults in this subtype can be identified diagnostically by assessing for antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) using the criteria put forth by the DSM-5. ASPD is related to repeated social norm violations, impulsivity, irresponsibility, and aggression that began in childhood to persist into adulthood (34). In order to receive a diagnosis of ASPD, individuals must meet criteria for CD prior to the age of 16 (which can be diagnosed retrospectively). In the DSM-5, youth with CD are characterized by a pattern of behaviors that violate the rights of others or societal norms in several ways (e.g., aggression to people or animals, destruction of property, theft, rule violations, etc.). In terms of prevalence, estimates suggest that between 50 and 66% of male prisoners meet criteria for ASPD (35, 36). Finally, some researchers, particularly using young samples, examine cumulative scores of conduct problems that cut across rule-breaking and aggressive behavior.

Both subtypes of individuals are known to act on impulse, display aggression, and engage in antisocial behaviors. One distinguishing aspect of the behavior of the “psychopathic” subtype is the presence of traits that reflect superficial interpersonal connections and blunted affect that impede their ability to form and maintain, meaningful, long-term relationships. On the one hand, the “psychopathic” individual draws you in with charm and manipulation, but also engages in hostile, impulsive and irresponsible behavior with an uncanny selfish drive. On the other hand, the “antisocial-only” individual engages in hostile, impulsive, and irresponsible behavior with a tinge of reactivity and brute force. Thus, despite many similarities in the actions of these individuals, a growing body of research suggests that relatively distinct socio-affective processes characterize these subtypes of individuals (1114, 3741). Accordingly, a closer examination of socio-affective processing could tell us why a particular individual continues to engage in these behaviors despite the persistence of social and legal problems. In this review, we focus on cognitive empathy as a set of socio-affective processes purportedly implicated in antisocial behavior1.

Brief Review on the Measurement of Cognitive Empathy

Cognitive empathy is involved in assessing another agent's emotions, beliefs, goals, or intentions within a given situational context. It comprises of several subcomponent processes, such as perspective-taking and attributing feelings and thoughts to self and others (42, 43). More specifically, some researchers separate the ability to recognize another agent's feelings or thoughts (perspective-taking) from forming an inference about the feelings and thoughts of the other agent (sometimes called cognitive empathy, Theory of Mind (ToM), or “mentalizing”). Further, researchers often distinguish affective perspective-taking/ToM and cognitive perspective-taking/ToM. Affective perspective-taking refers to the capacity to recognize the emotional state of another agent, whereas cognitive perspective-taking reflects that ability to infer the thoughts of another agent. For example, affective perspective-taking would be when a person is able to label that, while they are happy getting invited to a party, their friend is sad about not getting invited to the party. Cognitive perspective-taking would be when a person recognizes that a co-worker does not know about the change in protocol announced at a staff meeting because the co-worker did not attend the staff meeting.

These cognitive empathy capabilities can be measured through questionnaires or experimental tasks. Several different questionnaires exist for assessing cognitive empathy. One of the most widely used questionnaires is the Interpersonal Reactivity Index [IRI (44)]. A subscale of this measure taps perspective-taking (e.g., “I try to look at everybody's side of a disagreement before I make a decision.”; “Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I were in their place.”). While questionnaire-based measures might provide broadband assessments of cognitive empathy, there is some question about the precision with which questionnaire measures, such as the IRI, specifically assess cognitive empathetic processes. For example, the perspective-taking subscale of the IRI includes some questions that are more cognitive in nature and some that reflect emotions, making it difficult to completely disentangle cognitive and affective perspective-taking. Therefore, questionnaire-based measures broadly evaluate some aspects of cognitive empathy, however, the specific subcomponent process is less clear.

Additionally, cognitive empathy can be evaluated using experimental tasks. During cognitive empathy tasks, participants are presented with scenarios or scenes, and are asked to use and integrate information about the situational context of a scene and/or the agent's actions to evaluate the agent's feelings or thoughts (e.g., “Character A just told Character B s/he could not have a piece of candy; how does Character B feel?”).

Cognitive empathy can be assessed explicitly or implicitly. Tasks explicitly evaluating cognitive empathy typically expose participants to a scenario (either by having them read a vignette, view a cartoon image or photograph, or watch a film clip). For affective perspective-taking/ToM tasks, the instructions would ask participants about different characters' feelings [e.g., “Pick which of four words best describes what the person in the photo is feeling.” (45)]. Though there is an emotion recognition component to many of these tasks, the specific question being asked in these tasks relates to representing/understanding or inferring other's emotion (not necessarily resonating with or responding to the emotions, which would fit more with the conceptualization affective/emotional empathy not covered in this review). For a cognitive ToM task, similar stimuli could be used to ask participants about the characters' beliefs, goals, or intentions [e.g., using a Sally-Anne-type false belief task (46, 47)].

In contrast, tasks implicitly evaluating components of cognitive empathy assess the degree to which an individual automatically (e.g., without instruction, unintentionally, unconsciously) assesses another agent's feelings, beliefs, goals, or intentions (48), sometimes even during an unrelated task [e.g., see (49)]. For example, using a Sally-Anne false belief task, researchers can examine the extent to which a participant infers, or anticipates, Sally's behavior by monitoring eye movements to assess the location of the moved ball. In another type of task tapping perspective-taking, researchers can evaluate the extent to which self-perspective-taking, such as determining the number of dots in a room, is influenced by the perspective of a task irrelevant agent, such as determining the number of dots from the perspective of an avatar.

Additionally, during all types of cognitive empathy tasks, affective or cognitive judgments can vary in their level of complexity, depending upon the number of “minds” (i.e., different individuals/agents) the participant needs to represent and track. For example, a first-order judgment is when an individual evaluates another agent's thoughts or feelings, only requiring that the individual represent one other agent's feelings or thoughts (e.g., evaluate if Character A likes Object X). A second-order judgment, however, is when an individual judges what another agent thinks about a third agent's thoughts or feelings, requiring the individual to simultaneously represent two other agent's feelings or thoughts (e.g., evaluate if Character A thinks Character B likes object X).

At a neurobiological level, cognitive empathy relies on the dynamic integration of information between a variety of cortical structures (50). Specifically, the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and right temporoparietal junction are implicated in an individual's ability to judge another agent's feelings, beliefs, goals, or intentions (5153). These regions appear to be common areas across subcomponent processes of cognitive empathy. Additionally, affective perspective-taking/ToM tends to elicit additional neural activation in the orbitofrontal cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and superior temporal gyrus. Some research suggests that the amygdala acts as a detector when there are demands placed on affective perspective-taking/ToM through the presence of emotional or social stimuli (54). Cognitive perspective-taking/ToM may uniquely activate dorso-medial/lateral prefrontal regions (55).

Cognitive empathy allows individuals to recognize, understand, and predict how other agents will respond in social situations. These social cognitive processes provide a foundation for the formation and maintenance of social relationships that are mutually constructive. Researchers, clinicians, and lay people, alike, often note that those who engage in antisocial behavior lack cognitive empathy. But, what does the research actually tell us about the association between different subtypes of antisocial individuals and subcomponents of cognitive empathy?

Cognitive Empathy in the Psychopathic Subtype

Across several studies, questionnaire-based evaluations of cognitive empathy reveal that higher levels of psychopathic/CU traits relates to lower levels of cognitive empathy (5660). However, a closer examination of the research suggests that a more mixed pattern emerges depending on the informant (i.e., youth themselves vs. parent vs. teacher) and how these traits are modeled. These factors are especially important for youth samples. For example, when the individual in question was the informant, there was no relationship to small negative effects in the relationship between expressions of the psychopathic subtype and cognitive empathy, whereas the strongest negative relationships between this subtype and cognitive empathy were present when the questionnaires were completed by other informants, such as parents and teachers (61). Additionally, when CU traits were measured by themselves, reductions in questionnaire-measured cognitive empathy were apparent [e.g., (62, 63)]. By contrast, when CU traits were examined in the context of CD (e.g., CD+CU), there typically were no differences reported in cognitive empathy [e.g., (64)]. Thus, in terms of questionnaire-based assessments of cognitive empathy, the presence of deficits in the psychopathic subtype might be most observed by other informants or in those who have interpersonal-affective deficits but not necessarily conduct problems.

Research using behavioral tasks shows a divergence between cognitive and affective subcomponents of cognitive empathy. Across studies, neither youth with CU nor adults with psychopathy showed neural differences or behavioral deficits in cognitive ToM, suggesting intact cognitive ToM in psychopathy (6579). By contrast, the evidence regarding the relationship between affective perspective-taking/ToM and psychopathy is more mixed.

To date, some studies reported that individuals with psychopathy were able to successfully assess another agent's affective state during affective perspective-taking/ToM tasks (70, 71, 73, 74, 76, 79), suggesting that individuals in the psychopathic subtype did not display deficits in affective perspective-taking/ToM. Conversely, other studies reported psychopathy-related behavioral abnormalities during affective perspective-taking/ToM tasks (64, 77, 78, 80, 81). For example, Sharp and Vanwoerden (78) demonstrated that, after viewing a 15-min long video clip depicting a dinner party (the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition task), adolescents high on CU were significantly worse than adolescents low on CU at evaluating what the characters in the film were feeling. Additionally, Shamay-Tsoory et al. (77) showed that after viewing a static cartoon image, adults with psychopathy were able to successfully make simple, first-order affective evaluations (e.g., Character A loves X object), but exhibited difficulty completing more complex, second-order affective evaluations (e.g., Character A loves the same object that Character B loves).

At first glance, these two studies appear to contradict the studies suggesting that individuals in psychopathic subtype show intact affective perspective-taking/ToM. However, it is possible that these apparently contradictory findings were actually the result of differences in task complexity. For example, Sharp and Vanwoerden (78) used a video of a dinner party as their task stimulus, requiring participants to process and track various pieces of information over the 15-min duration of the video. By contrast, other studies used relatively simple, static cartoon images, requiring participants to process and track, at most, three frames of information [(73, 76, 77, 79); see Roberts et al. (75) for a similar effect in cognitive ToM]. Similarly, Shamay-Tsoory et al. (77) reported that psychopathy-related difficulties in affective ToM were limited to complex, second-order judgments, which were not examined in any of the other studies. Collectively, these findings suggest that individuals in the psychopathic subtype exhibit difficulty with affective perspective-taking/ToM, but only when evaluating affective information that is embedded in a particularly complex stimulus (e.g., a movie), or when the judgment itself is highly complex or multilayered (e.g., second-order affective evaluations). This pattern of results suggests that when presented with more complex stimuli or scenarios, either the complexity of the scenario, the complexity of the affective judgments, and/or the amount of information required to process and track, impairs psychopathic individuals' ability to successfully evaluate or predict other agents' affective state.

Neural examinations of affective ToM in the psychopathic subtype yield similarly mixed results. On the one hand, several studies report that youth with CU traits or adults with psychopathy do not show substantial deficits during affective perspective-taking/ToM tasks (73, 80, 82). On the other hand, both Sebastian et al. (76) and Sommer et al. (79) reported that while individuals with psychopathy were able to successfully perform an affective ToM task (i.e., psychopathic individuals showed no behavioral differences compared to controls), they exhibited distinct neural abnormalities while performing the task. Sebastian et al. (76) specifically found that adolescents with CD who were high on CU (CD+CU) showed blunted amygdala responses during an affective ToM task that required participants to view and evaluate a static cartoon image. However, in their analysis, Sebastian et al. (76) examined amygdala reactivity across entire trials (i.e., during the initial presentation of the image and the judgment). This type of analysis made it difficult to determine what precise component of the trial was driving the blunted amygdala reactivity in adolescents with CD+CU. It is possible that the CD+CU-related blunting of the amygdala response was driven by neural differences when these youth initially saw (and affectively responded to) the cartoon images, rather than any CD+CU-related neural abnormalities in affective ToM (judgment).

Sommer et al. (79) reported that, during an affective ToM task, adults with psychopathy showed blunted responses in cortical regions associated with action observation and execution [i.e., the bilateral supramarginal gyri and superior frontal gyrus; (83)] and heightened responses in cortical regions generally associated with socio-affective processing, such as the orbitofrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, and medial prefrontal cortex (5153). This finding suggests that while adults with psychopathy were able to engage in affective ToM, they required more socio-affective neural resources to do so (79). While speculative, this need for additional neural resources to complete relatively simple (i.e., first-order) affective ToM judgments could potentially explain psychopathic individuals' apparent difficulties with more complex (i.e., second-order) affective evaluations (77). More specifically, psychopathic individuals may be able to engage enough neurocognitive resources to compensate for psychopathy-related difficulties in affective ToM during relatively simple, first-order, affective ToM evaluations. However, the additional neural resources needed to compensate for affective ToM deficits during more complex, second-order, affective evaluations may exceed the available neurocognitive resources for psychopathic individuals.

To this point, the studies reviewed exclusively examine tasks that explicitly instruct participants to engage in cognitive empathy, whether it is cognitive perspective-taking/ToM or affective perspective-taking/ToM. These studies do not assess whether individuals in the psychopathic subtype spontaneously engage in empathy (i.e., they have not assessed whether these individuals implicitly evaluate other agents' feelings, beliefs, goals, or intentions, in the absence of explicit instruction to do so).

A recent study by Drayton et al. (84) helped address this gap in the literature by examining the impact of psychopathy on an implicit measure of cognitive perspective-taking in an incarcerated sample. In this study, Drayton et al. (84) had inmates complete a cognitive perspective-taking task (49). During this task, participants were presented with static scenes depicting a gender- and race-matched avatar in a room with varying numbers of dots on the walls. The dots appeared in front of the avatar (i.e., the avatar had complete information), behind the avatar (i.e., the avatar had no information), or both (i.e., the avatar had partial information); however, the participant always saw all of the dots on every trial (i.e., the participant always had complete information). On some trials, participants were asked to evaluate how many dots the avatar could see (other-trials), and on some trials, participants were asked to evaluate how many dots they personally could see (self-trials). The other-trials provided a measure of explicit perspective-taking: could the participant take the avatar's perspective? The self-trials provided a measure of implicit perspective-taking: was the participant's perspective affected by the avatar's perspective? Research using this paradigm in the general population shows that when the avatar's perspective is different than the participant's perspective, participants are slower at reporting their own perspective (self-trials), indicating that individuals spontaneously take the avatar's perspective even if it is goal-irrelevant. Consistent with previous research on the psychopathic subtype, incarcerated individuals higher on psychopathy were able to engage in explicit perspective-taking and performed similarly to incarcerated individuals lower on psychopathy on the other-trials. However, incarcerated individuals higher on psychopathy compared to incarcerated individuals lower on psychopathy displayed significantly less interference on the self-trials (i.e., their reaction time was not affected by the perspective of the avatar). These findings suggest that psychopathic individuals do not implicitly evaluate others' mental states [i.e., they do not implicitly engage cognitive perspective-taking (84)], but can do so explicitly [see (75) for evidence of explicit abilities in CD+CU youth].

Another study examining pain perception in psychopathy suggests a similar pattern of psychopathy-related impairment in implicit affective ToM. Meffert et al. (85) used fMRI to examine neural responses to hand pain in three different conditions: passive viewing of a clip of a hand being hurt (i.e., implicit affective ToM), imagining what the person in the clip might be experiencing (i.e., explicit affective ToM), and physically experiencing the actual scenarios depicted in the clips. Meffert et al. (85) reported that, when adults with psychopathy passively viewed the pain clips, they did not exhibit significant neural overlap with their actual experience of pain (relative to controls), which the authors interpreted as evidence that adults with psychopathy did not implicitly engage in affective ToM. In contrast, however, Meffert et al. found that individuals with psychopathy showed similar overlap in neural responses to controls when instructed to imagine what the person was feeling (i.e., explicit affective ToM) and when physically experiencing the pain. These two findings suggest that adults with psychopathy are able to engage in affective ToM, but do not do so implicitly (i.e., without instruction).

While the purely neural nature of these findings makes this interpretation somewhat speculative, these findings and interpretations are consistent with both prior research demonstrating psychopathy-related neural abnormalities in pain perception in others (86), and other findings indicating that individuals with the psychopathic subtype do not implicitly engage in cognitive perspective-taking (75, 84). Thus, the current literature examining cognitive empathy in the psychopathic subtype provides strong evidence that individuals in this subtype largely are able to engage in cognitive empathy when instructed to do so, but do not do so implicitly. This is an important distinction because it helps in explaining why individuals in the psychopathic subtype can so easily manipulate others' thoughts and feelings when conning them (as the act of conning someone explicitly requires empathy), yet have difficulty with more everyday social interactions, which may require more implicit empathy. While social interactions in the real-world are inherently more complex than experimental tasks that have a participant watch a dinner party or view an avatar, the deliberate instruction during tasks, or explicit goal-focus in the real-world, may alleviate some of the processing burden that undermines empathetic functioning in individuals within the psychopathic subtype.

Overall, research indicates that individuals in the psychopathic subtype may not have a complete deficit in cognitive empathy (see Figure 2). When individuals in psychopathic subtype are asked to report on their own empathy or complete simple, cognitive empathy, tasks, empathy appears intact. However, when other observers are asked to report on the behavior of CU youth, or psychopathic individuals are asked to engage cognitive empathy in more complex situations, deficits are more apparent. Moreover, a recurrent finding across various aspects of cognitive empathy in psychopathy is that, even if individuals within this subtype can normatively engage different empathetic processes (in specific circumstances), they tend to only do so when instructed. The failure to implicitly attend to, and process, others' emotions or mental states, combined perhaps with a lack of self-awareness about this tendency, may explain how these individuals are able to callously harm others during goal-pursuit, but also able to charm, con, and manipulate others when necessary.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Summary of cognitive empathy findings by subtype of antisocial individual. CU, callous-unemotional; CD, conduct disorder; ToM, theory of mind; --, indicates no research to date.

Cognitive Empathy in the Antisocial-Only Subtype

For the antisocial-only subtype, questionnaire-based evaluations of cognitive empathy suggested that these processes are intact (70, 87). Similarly, when assessed behaviorally, several studies demonstrated that individuals in the antisocial-only subtype, across all developmental stages, exhibit intact cognitive empathy (68, 75, 76, 8890). Though, admittedly examination of cognitive empathy in the antisocial-only subtype has been less systematic than research in the psychopathic subtype. For example, no studies have examined implicit cognitive empathy in antisocial-only individuals. Despite the overall pattern of intact cognitive empathy in the antisocial-only subtype, some research indicates that the specific demands of the tasks reveal nuanced dysfunction in subcomponent processes of cognitive empathy.

Across youth and adult samples, individuals in the antisocial-only subtype (CD; ASPD) display dysfunction when there is a specific demand on affective perspective-taking/ToM (64, 87, 91, 92). For example, Dolan and Fullam (66) reported that, while individuals with ASPD were able to successfully complete traditional false belief tasks and identify subtle violations of social norms (e.g., identify when someone accidentally said something that should not have been said; i.e., social faux pas), they exhibited difficulties with affective ToM within the context of these subtle norm violations. More specifically, adults with ASPD displayed difficulties assessing characters' affective states/perspective after the characters experienced a subtle norm violation. In another study, Newbury-Helps et al. (91) administered several cognitive empathy tasks in a sample of justice-involved individuals. Individuals with ASPD displayed a particularly pronounced deficit in affective ToM during the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition task, scoring in a range that reflected difficulty with memory, general comprehension, and abstraction.

Examination of neural differences in cognitive empathy for individuals in the antisocial-only subtype has been limited. Sebastian et al. (76) reported that, during an affective ToM fMRI paradigm assessing second-order judgments, CD symptomology (controlling for CU traits) in adolescents was unrelated to behavioral task performance. However, CD symptomology was associated with increased amygdala reactivity across the entire trial to affective vs. cognitive ToM scenarios after controlling for CU traits (76). The effect of increased amygdala activation in this study could be the result of neural abnormalities in affective ToM, or simply the product of increased amygdala reactivity when initially seeing (and affectively responding to) the affectively valenced scenes. Regardless of the specific interpretation, however, at a neural level there may be evidence that antisocial-only individuals, especially compared to individuals in the psychopathic subtype, over-react to affective information (see (93, 94) for similar effects in inferring the pain of others [i.e., first-order judgment]).

Generally, research examining cognitive empathy in the antisocial-only subtype shows that these individuals exhibit intact cognitive empathy when measured through questionnaires and behavioral tasks that tap explicit empathic functioning (see Figure 2). Antisocial-only individuals appear to attend to, recognize, and make inferences about social cues. However, individuals in the antisocial-only subtype may display some difficulty inferring the emotions of others. Though research has been limited in this subtype, it is possible that evidence of some affective ToM dysfunctions reflects issues with executive functions, such as abstract reasoning, and imprecision in detecting and regulating affective capacities (37, 76, 9597). Antisocial-only individuals tend to display deficits in executive functions, such as flexibility and abstract reasoning (98). These executive functions are necessary for a full range of empathetic functioning, including picking up on subtle affective cues. Moreover, problems with executive functioning, combined with dysfunction in affective processing that reflects over-and-under-responding in various situations (99), can undermine regulated responding to affective information. Thus, impairments in inferring the emotions of others when the signals are subtle and difficulty remembering or comprehending the emotions of others may result in the unpredictable, perhaps impulsive, interpersonal interactions characteristic of these individuals. Moreover, possible over-reactivity to salient affective information may generate an explosive, poorly regulated, reaction from antisocial-only individuals in these interpersonal contexts.

Considerations for Future Research and Conclusions

There is both clinical and empirical support for cognitive empathy disruptions in psychopathic and antisocial-only subtypes of individuals. However, a close examination of the available data suggests that the specific manner of dysfunction varies between subtypes of individuals and subcomponents of cognitive empathy. Individuals within the psychopathic subtype appear to be viewed by others as deficient in cognitive empathy (based on questionnaires), but show adequate performance on cognitive empathy tasks, particularly when explicitly asked to engage empathy. However, in cognitive and affective perspective-taking/ToM tasks, these individuals appear not to engage these processes automatically, requiring instructions to direct their attention to relevant socio-affective information in order to respond normatively. Antisocial-only individuals reliably report intact cognitive empathy and are able to perform reasonably well on behavioral tasks that tap explicit processes, but may struggle to fully comprehend or process affective signals, particularly if subtle. Overall, differences in cognitive empathetic functioning differentiate these two subtypes of individuals and may relate to their differential phenotypic expressions.

The specific nature of the problems with cognitive empathy in psychopathic and antisocial-only individuals further highlight the equifinality of antisocial behavior. Despite both subtypes of individuals exhibiting chronic violations of social norms and a disregard for others, the processes underlying their behavior appear distinct. Moreover, the pattern of dysfunction in cognitive empathy for these subtypes of individuals follows a larger literature on cognitive-affective functioning in psychopathic and antisocial-only individuals. On the whole, cognitive empathy dysfunction in the psychopathic subtype, particularly their ability to explicitly engage cognitive empathy but their deficient propensity to implicitly do so, may echo the broader cognitive-affective deficits these individuals have in attending to and integrating multiple streams of information (38, 100). Similarly, individuals in the antisocial-only subtype do not appear to engage in antisocial behaviors because of a fundamental deficit in cognitive empathy. In fact, the situations when dysfunction in cognitive empathy are apparent (e.g., inaccurate affective perspective-taking following subtle violations, over-reacting neurally during affective ToM) may reflect cognitive-affective dysfunctions related to deficits in executive functioning and poor affective regulatory capacities that just happen to arise during cognitive empathy tasks, which place demands on these functions (11, 37). Noting the consistency in dysfunction across levels of analysis is important for future work that may explore the specific processes that underlie complex social cognitive dysfunction in antisocial individuals.

The identification of subtype-specific core cognitive-affective dysfunctions that cut across levels of analysis raises an important question of whether the cognitive-affective dysfunctions lead to the psychopathic or antisocial-only expressions or are just related to these expressions. Very few longitudinal studies examining the development of antisociality and cognitive empathy have been conducted. In one study, displays of concern for others, which encompasses a range of affective and cognitive indicators of empathy, at age 14 to 36 months, did not predict ASPD at age 23 years. However, observed disregard for others, which represents responding to other's distress with anger or hostility, predicted the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy, and ASPD (101). In another study, cognitive ToM at 4.5 years old did not predict CU traits at 10 years old (102). Cognitive ToM did predict impulsive behavior at 10 years old, but this relationship was better accounted for by exhibition of externalizing behaviors (conduct problems, hyperactivity) at age 5. Thus, very preliminary evidence suggests that cognitive empathy does not predict antisociality, and, that affective sensitivities may be more likely as candidate processes that pre-date antisociality (101, 102). Far more research examining subcomponents of empathy that span cognitive and affective domains is needed.

The type of measure selected to tap cognitive empathy within each antisocial subtype also reveals interesting divergences. Notably, within and across antisocial subtypes, there were inconsistencies depending on whether questionnaires or tasks were used, and even depending on the specific task being used. For the differences between questionnaires and task performance, these inconsistencies may reflect the fact that questionnaire-based measures often fail to precisely capture a specific process of cognitive empathy, whereas many tasks are more specifically designed to tap a subcomponent of cognitive empathy. Thus, researchers must accurately label and discuss the measures being used in their particular study. Moreover, specific biases in certain tasks may lead to deficit performance but may not actually reflect a deficit in cognitive empathy per se. For example, the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition uses a white middle-class dinner party as a key stimulus. Cognitive empathy is sensitive to in-group and out-group effects (103, 104), as such, participants who are not white and middle-class may have difficulty identifying with the characters. Thus, any performance deficits on this task may not be because of a failure to represent the characters' thoughts and feelings, but rather an unfamiliarity or disconnect with the experiences presented in these clips due to larger sociocultural differences. Therefore, researchers should consider ways to match stimuli and participant characteristics [see (84) for example], and to develop more culturally sensitive measures of cognitive empathy.

Another aspect of cognitive empathy that requires further exploration is the distinction between explicit and implicit cognitive empathy. The handful of studies in the psychopathic subtype highlight the value in distinguishing between implicit and explicit engagement of cognitive empathy, underscoring that individuals in the psychopathic subtype lack the propensity to implicitly engage cognitive empathy but not the ability to explicitly engage cognitive empathy (75, 84, 85). The distinction between implicit and explicit empathy also may be reflected in the questionnaires dissociations observed in youth with CU traits. It is possible that youth endorse cognitive empathy on a questionnaire (i.e., show an explicit ability to recognize the appropriate response), but do not engage with it naturally or implicitly in the day-to-day life witnessed by others. Research within the antisocial-only subtype has not compared implicit vs. explicit tendencies in cognitive empathy. Disentangling whether someone has an ability to explicitly engage cognitive empathy vs. lacks a propensity to implicitly to do so has important clinical implications. The presence of an ability to explicitly engage cognitive empathy, but the absence of an implicit propensity, suggests that compensatory strategies that allow antisocial individuals to circumvent their cognitive-affective deficits (e.g., difficulty processing and tracking complex stimuli) may be beneficial for increasing prosocial behavior. For example, by instructing individuals with psychopathy or CU traits to focus on key social information (e.g., facial affect, contextual cues about the situation), these individuals may be able to more deliberately integrate this information. While empathy itself may not be normalized, the behavior of those with psychopathy or CU traits has the potential to reflect the use of important social information by making the focus on that information more deliberate.

Beyond specific processes supporting cognitive empathy in antisocial subtypes, little research in this domain accounts for the contribution of environmental risk factors that are related to both the quality of cognitive empathy functioning and subtype of antisociality. For example, early childhood deprivation, maltreatment, and poverty occur at high rates among individuals who chronically engage in antisocial behavior (105, 106). Outside of research on antisociality, early childhood maltreatment and other environmental factors, such as concentrated disadvantage, are known to negatively impact empathetic functioning and development (107109). For example, children who are maltreated experience substantial deficits and delays in ToM (107, 109). Accordingly, it is possible that some of the deficits associated with antisocial subtypes are promoted by certain environmental experiences. However, research examining the intersection of antisociality, early environment, and cognitive empathy is limited, making this possibility hard to evaluate, but an exciting endeavor for future research.

The relationship between cognitive empathy and antisociality is complex. Lay beliefs that antisocial individuals must engage in antisocial behavior because they are incapable of cognitive empathy are not supported by extant literature. Rather, dysfunction in cognitive empathy appears dependent on subtype of individuals and subcomponent process of cognitive empathy. Advancing our understanding of the links between cognitive empathy disruptions and antisocial subtypes is crucial to providing unique insight into the development and maintenance of the chronic, disruptive, and costly behaviors exhibited by these individuals.

Author Contributions

SAC and AB-S wrote the first draft of the manuscript. ST wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^This review is a part of a special topic examining “Cognitive Empathy and Perspective Taking: Understanding the Mechanisms of Normal and Abnormal Experiences and Abilities.” Thus, we focus our discussion on cognitive empathy, as opposed to affective empathy, which refers to the ability to emotionally resonate with or experientially share another person's internal affective state.

References

1. Apperly I. Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of “Theory of Mind”. New York, NY: Psychology Press (2010).

Google Scholar

2. Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Lombardo M. Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2013).

Google Scholar

3. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. (1985) 21:37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Corcoran R, Mercer G, Frith CD. Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (1995) 17:5–13. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kerr N, Dunbar RI, Bentall RP. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar affective disorder. J Affect Disord. (2003) 73:253–9. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00008-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. FeldmanHall O, Son J-Y, Heffner J. Norms and the flexibility of moral action. Pers Neurosci. (2018) 1:e15. doi: 10.1017/pen.2018.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Patil I. Trait psychopathy and utilitarian moral judgement: the mediating role of action aversion. J Cogn Psychol. (2015) 27:349–66. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1004334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Caspi A, Wright BRE, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Early failure in the labor market: childhood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition to adulthood. Am Sociol Rev. (1998) 63:424–51. doi: 10.2307/2657557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2005) 46:837–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kiehl KA, Hoffman MB. The criminal psychopath: history, neuroscience, treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics. (2011) 51:355–97.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Brennan GM, Hyde LW, Baskin-Sommers AR. Antisocial pathways associated with substance use disorders: characterizing etiological underpinnings and implications for treatment. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2017) 13:124–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Drislane LE, Patrick CJ. Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. In: Shally-Jensen M, editors. Mental Health Care Issues in America: An Encyclopedia. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO (2013). p. 599–607.

Google Scholar

13. Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Hariri AR. Understanding youth antisocial behavior using neuroscience through a developmental psychopathology lens: review, integration, and directions for research. Dev Rev. (2013) 33:168–223. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2013.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Viding E, Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJ. Antisocial behaviour in children with and without callous-unemotional traits. J R Soc Med. (2012) 105:195–200. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cleckley H. The Mask of Sanity. St. Louis: Mosby (1941).

Google Scholar

16. Hare RD. Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist, 2nd ed. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Systems (2003).

Google Scholar

17. Hare RD. Psychopathy: a clinical and forensic overview. Psychiatric Clin North Am. (2006) 29:709–24. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hare RD, Neumann CS. Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2008) 4:217–46. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Paulhus DL, Neumann CS, Hare RD. Manual for the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Systems (2017).

Google Scholar

20. Lilienfeld SO, Andrews BP. Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. J Pers Assess. (1996) 66:488–524. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sellbom M, Lilienfeld S, Fowler K, McCrary K. The self-report assessment of psychopathy: challenges, pitfalls, and promises. In: Patrick C, editor. Handbook of Psychopathy. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2018). pp. 211–58.

Google Scholar

22. Blair J, Peschardt KS, Budhani S, Mitchell DGV, Pine DS. The development of psychopathy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:262–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01596.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Gao Y, Glenn AL, Schug RA, Yang Y, Raine A. The neurobiology of psychopathy: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Can J Psychiatry. (2009) 54:813–23. doi: 10.1177/070674370905401204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Raine A, Lee L, Yang Y, Colletti P. Neurodevelopmental marker for limbic maldevelopment in antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry. (2010) 197:186–92. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.078485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. (2014) 140:1–57. doi: 10.1037/a0033076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2008) 49:359–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Frogner L, Gibson CL, Andershed AK, Andershed H. Childhood psychopathic personality and callous–unemotional traits in the prediction of conduct problems. Am J Orthopsychiatry. (2016) 88:211–25. doi: 10.1037/ort0000205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Viding E, Fontaine NMG, Oliver BR, Plomin R. Negative parental discipline, conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differences study. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 195:414–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.061192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Viding E, McCrory E. Developmental risk for psychopathy. In: Thapar A, Pine D, Leckman JF, Scott S, Snowling MJ, Taylor EA, editors. Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 6. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd (2015). pp. 966–80.

Google Scholar

30. Viding E, McCrory EJ. Understanding the development of psychopathy: progress and challenges. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:566–77. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kahn RE, Byrd AL, Pardini DA. Callous-unemotional traits robustly predict future criminal offending in young men. Law Hum Behav. (2013) 37:87–97. doi: 10.1037/b0000003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Pardini DA, Byrd AL. Perceptions of aggressive conflicts and others' distress in children with callous-unemotional traits:‘I'll show you who's boss, even if you suffer and I get in trouble'. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2012) 53:283–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02487.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Essau CA, Sasagawa S, Frick PJ. Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment. (2006) 13:454–69. doi: 10.1177/1073191106287354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publication (2013).

Google Scholar

35. Fazel S, Danesh J. Serious mental disorder in 23 000 prisoners: a systematic review of 62 surveys. Lancet. (2002) 359:545–50. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07740-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Moran P. The epidemiology of antisocial personality disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (1999) 34:231–42. doi: 10.1007/s001270050138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Baskin-Sommers A, Newman JP. Differentiating the cognition-emotion interactions that characterize psychopathy versus externalizing. In: Robinson M, Watkins E, Harmon-Jones E, editors. Cognition and Emotion. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2013). pp. 501–20.

Google Scholar

38. Estrada S, Tillem S, Stuppy-Sullivan A, Baskin-Sommers AR. Specifying the connection between reward processing and antisocial psychopathology across development: review, integration, future directions. In: Gruber J, editors. Oxford Handbook of Positive Emotion and Psychopathology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2018). p. 312–32.

Google Scholar

39. Lobbestael J, Cima M, Arntz A. The relationship between adult reactive and proactive aggression, hostile interpretation bias, and antisocial personality disorder. J Pers Disord. (2013) 27:53–66. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.1.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. In: Beaver K, Walsh A, editors. Biosocial Theories of Crime. New York, NY: Routledge (2017). pp. 69–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

41. Schönenberg M, Jusyte A. Investigation of the hostile attribution bias toward ambiguous facial cues in antisocial violent offenders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2014) 264:61–9. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013-0440-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Shamay-Tsoory SG. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist. (2011) 17:18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Zaki J, Ochsner KN. The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat Neurosci. (2012) 15:675–80. doi: 10.1038/nn.3085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Cat Sel Doc Psychol. (1980) 10:85.

Google Scholar

45. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1997) 38:813–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Leslie AM, Friedman O, German TP. Core mechanisms in “theory of mind”. Trends Cogn Sci. (2004) 8:528–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J. Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:3054–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Schneider D, Slaughter VP, Dux PE. Current evidence for automatic Theory of Mind processing in adults. Cognition. (2017) 162:27–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Samson D, Apperly IA, Braithwaite JJ, Andrews BJ, Bodley Scott SE. Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. (2010) 36:1255. doi: 10.1037/a0018729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Schurz M, Radua J, Tholen MG, Maliske L, Margulies DS, Mars RB, et al. Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: a neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychol Bull. (2021) 147:293. doi: 10.1037/bul0000303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Jack AI, Dawson AJ, Begany KL, Leckie RL, Barry KP, Ciccia AH, et al. fMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains. Neuroimage. (2013) 66:385–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Spreng RN, Grady CL. Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. J Cogn Neurosci. (2010) 22:1112–23. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. (2009) 21:489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Schmitgen MM, Walter H, Drost S, Rückl S, Schnell K. Stimulus-dependent amygdala involvement in affective theory of mind generation. Neuroimage. (2016) 129:450–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Healey ML, Grossman M. Cognitive and affective perspective-taking: evidence for shared and dissociable anatomical substrates. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:491. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Brouns BH, de Wied MA, Keijsers L, Branje S, van Goozen SH, Meeus WH. Concurrent and prospective effects of psychopathic traits on affective and cognitive empathy in a community sample of late adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2013) 54:969–76. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Frost AD, Vassallo S, Bunn P, Hunter K, et al. Learning to ‘talk the talk': the relationship of psychopathic traits to deficits in empathy across childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2009) 50:599–606. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02058.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Díaz-Galván K, Ostrosky-Shejet F, Romero-Rebollar C. Cognitive and affective empathy: the role in violent behavior and psychopathy. Rev Méd Hosp Gen México. (2015) 78:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.hgmx.2015.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Georgiou G, Kimonis ER, Fanti KA. What do others feel? Cognitive empathy deficits explain the association between callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems among preschool children. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2019) 16:633–53. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2018.1478810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Robinson EV, Rogers R. Empathy faking in psychopathic offenders: the vulnerability of empathy measures. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2015) 37:545–52. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9479-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Waller R, Wagner NJ, Barstead MG, Subar A, Petersen JL, Hyde JS, et al. A meta-analysis of the associations between callous-unemotional traits and empathy, prosociality, and guilt. Clin Psychol Rev. (2020) 75:101809. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Golmaryami FN, Marsee MA. The moderating role of anxiety in the associations of callous-unemotional traits with self-report and laboratory measures of affective and cognitive empathy. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2017) 45:583–96. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0179-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lethbridge EM, Richardson P, Reidy L, Taroyan NA. Exploring the relationship between callous-unemotional traits, empathy processing and affective valence in a general population. Eur J Psychol. (2017) 13:162. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v13i1.1179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Warden D. Cognitive and affective perspective-taking in conduct-disordered children high and low on callous-unemotional traits. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2008) 2:1–11. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-2-16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Blair J, Sellars C, Strickland I, Clark F, Williams A, Smith M, et al. Theory of mind in the psychopath. J Forensic Psychiatry. (1996) 7:15–25. doi: 10.1080/09585189608409914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Dolan M, Fullam R. Theory of mind and mentalizing ability in antisocial personality disorders with and without psychopathy. Psychol Med. (2004) 34:1093–102. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Domes G, Hollerbach P, Vohs K, Mokros A, Habermeyer E. Emotional empathy and psychopathy in offenders: an experimental study. J Pers Disord. (2013) 27:67–84. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.1.67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Jones AP, Happe FG, Gilbert F, Burnett S, Viding E. Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2010) 51:1188–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Lockwood PL, Bird G, Bridge M, Viding E. Dissecting empathy: high levels of psychopathic and autistic traits are characterized by difficulties in different social information processing domains. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:760. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Milone A, Cerniglia L, Cristofani C, Inguaggiato E, Levantini V, Masi G, et al. Empathy in youths with conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits. Neural Plast. (2019) 2019:9638973. doi: 10.1155/2019/9638973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Nentjes L, Bernstein D, Arntz A, van Breukelen G, Slaats M. Examining the influence of psychopathy, hostility biases, and automatic processing on criminal offenders' Theory of Mind. Int J Law Psychiatry. (2015) 38:92–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2015.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Nentjes L, Bernstein DP, Arntz A, Slaats ME, Hannemann T. Theory of mind, social desirability, and unlikely symptom reporting in offenders with and without psychopathy. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2015) 203:596–603. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. O'Nions E, Sebastian CL, McCrory E, Chantiluke K, Happe F, Viding E. Neural bases of Theory of Mind in children with autism spectrum disorders and children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Dev Sci. (2014) 17:786–96. doi: 10.1111/desc.12167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Richell RA, Mitchell DG, Newman C, Leonard A, Baron-Cohen S, Blair RJR. Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the ‘language of the eyes'? Neuropsychologia. (2003) 41:523–6. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00175-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Roberts R, McCrory E, Bird G, Sharp M, Roberts L, Viding E. Thinking about others' minds: mental state inference in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2020) 48:1279–90. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00664-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sebastian CL, McCrory EJP, Cecil CAM, Lockwood PL, De Brito SA, Fontaine NMG, et al. Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2012) 69:814–22. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Harari H, Aharon-Peretz J, Levkovitz Y. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex. (2010) 46:668–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Sharp C, Vanwoerden S. Social cognition: empirical contribution: the developmental building blocks of psychopathic traits: revisiting the role of theory of mind. J Pers Disord. (2014) 28:78–95. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2014.28.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Sommer M, Sodian B, Dohnel K, Schwerdtner J, Meinhardt J, Hajak G. In psychopathic patients emotion attribution modulates activity in outcome-related brain areas. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 182:88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Deming P, Dargis M, Haas BW, Brook M, Decety J, Harenski C, et al. Psychopathy is associated with fear-specific reductions in neural activity during affective perspective-taking. Neuroimage. (2020) 223:117342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Lui JH, Barry CT, Sacco DF. Callous-unemotional traits and empathy deficits: mediating effects of affective perspective-taking and facial emotion recognition. Cogn Emot. (2016) 30:1049–62. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1047327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Gao Y, Rogers JC, Pauli R, Clanton R, Baker R, Birch P, et al. Neural correlates of theory of mind in typically-developing youth: influence of sex, age and callous-unemotional traits. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52261-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Van Overwalle F, Baetens K. Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage. (2009) 48:564–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Drayton LA, Santos LR, Baskin-Sommers AR. Psychopaths fail to automatically take the perspective of others. PNAS. (2018) 115:3302–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721903115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Meffert H, Gazzola V, den Boer JA, Bartels AA, Keysers C. Reduced spontaneous but relatively normal deliberate vicarious representations in psychopathy. Brain. (2013) 136:2550–62. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Decety J, Skelly LR, Kiehl KA. Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:638–45. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Schwenck C, Schmitt D, Sievers S, Romanos M, Warnke A, Schneider W. Cognitive and emotional empathy in children with ADHD and conduct disorder. Zeitschr Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. (2011) 39:265–76. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Arango Tobón OE, Rosa O-L, Restrepo Tamayo V, Puerta Lopera IC. Empathic skills and theory of mind in female adolescents with conduct disorder. Braz J Psychiatry. (2018) 40:78–82. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2016-2092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Martin-Key N, Brown T, Fairchild G. Empathic accuracy in male adolescents with conduct disorder and higher versus lower levels of callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2017) 45:1385–97. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0243-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Woodworth M, Waschbusch D. Emotional processing in children with conduct problems and callous/unemotional traits. Child Care Health Dev. (2008) 34:234–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00792.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Newbury-Helps J, Feigenbaum J, Fonagy P. Offenders with antisocial personality disorder display more impairments in mentalizing. J Pers Disord. (2017) 31:232–55. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2016_30_246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Sharp C. Theory of mind and conduct problems in children: deficits in reading the “emotions of the eyes”. Cogn Emot. (2008) 22:1149–58. doi: 10.1080/02699930701667586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Decety J, Michalska KJ, Akitsuki Y, Lahey BB. Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Biol Psychol. (2009) 80:203–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Lockwood PL, Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Hyde ZH, Gu X, De Brito SA, et al. Association of callous traits with reduced neural response to others' pain in children with conduct problems. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:901–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Baskin-Sommers AR, Newman JP. Psychopathic and externalizing offenders display dissociable dysfunctions when responding to facial affect. Pers Disord Theory Res Treat. (2014) 5:369–79. doi: 10.1037/per0000077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Ha C, Sharp C, Goodyer I. The role of child and parental mentalizing for the development of conduct problems over time. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 20:291–300. doi: 10.1007/s00787-011-0174-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Hyde LW, Byrd AL, Votruba-Drzal E, Hariri AR, Manuck SB. Amygdala reactivity and negative emotionality: divergent correlates of antisocial personality and psychopathy traits in a community sample. J Abnorm Psychol. (2014) 123:214–24. doi: 10.1037/a0035467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev. (2000) 20:113–36. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00096-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Fairchild G, Hawes DJ, Frick PJ, Copeland WE, Odgers CL, Franke B, et al. Conduct disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2019) 5:43. doi: 10.1038/s41572-019-0095-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Brazil IA, van Dongen JDM, Maes JHR, Mars RB, Baskin-Sommers A. Classification and treatment of antisocial individuals: from behavior to biocognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 91:259–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Rhee SH, Woodward K, Corley RP, du Pont A, Friedman NP, Hewitt JK, et al. The association between toddlerhood empathy deficits and antisocial personality disorder symptoms and psychopathy in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. (2021) 33:173–83. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Centifanti LCM, Meins E, Fernyhough C. Callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity: distinct longitudinal relations with mind-mindedness and understanding of others. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:84–92. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Cikara M, Bruneau E, Van Bavel JJ, Saxe R. Their pain gives us pleasure: how intergroup dynamics shape empathic failures and counter-empathic responses. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2014) 55:110–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Eres R, Molenberghs P. The influence of group membership on the neural correlates involved in empathy. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:176. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Braga T, Gonçalves LC, Basto-Pereira M, Maia A. Unraveling the link between maltreatment and juvenile antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Aggress Viol Behav. (2017) 33:37–50. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2017.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Thibodeau EL. The effects of child maltreatment on early signs of antisocial behavior: genetic moderation by tryptophan hydroxylase, serotonin transporter, and monoamine oxidase A genes. Dev Psychopathol. (2012) 24:907–28. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Maughan A, Toth SL, Bruce J. False belief understanding in maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol. (2003) 15:1067–91. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Drukker M, Kaplan C, Feron F, van Os J. Children's health-related quality of life, neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation and social capital. A contextual analysis. Soc Sci Med. (2003) 57:825–41. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00453-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Pears KC, Fisher PA. Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: evidence of deficits. Dev Psychopathol. (2005) 17:47–65. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive empathy, antisocial, psychopathy, callous-unemotional, theory of mind, perspective-taking

Citation: Chang SA, Tillem S, Benson-Williams C and Baskin-Sommers A (2021) Cognitive Empathy in Subtypes of Antisocial Individuals. Front. Psychiatry 12:677975. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.677975

Received: 08 March 2021; Accepted: 04 June 2021;
Published: 05 July 2021.

Edited by:

Renate L. E. P. Reniers, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Sarah Louise Mason, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
Katherine Rice Warnell, Texas State University, United States
Claudia Civai, London South Bank University, United Kingdom
Federica Meconi, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Chang, Tillem, Benson-Williams and Baskin-Sommers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Arielle Baskin-Sommers, arielle.baskin-sommers@yale.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.