Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 01 September 2021
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic From Childbearing to Childrearing: Parental Mental Health and Infant Development View all 16 articles

Parental Sensitivity and Responsiveness as Mediators Between Postpartum Mental Health and Bonding in Mothers and Fathers

  • Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Croatia, Zagreb, Croatia

Background: There is a lack of studies that examine the complex relationship between parental mental health, parental sensitivity and responsiveness, and parent-infant bonding. This study aimed to test whether parental sensitivity and responsiveness were mediators between postpartum mental health (depression, anxiety, and stress) and parent-infant bonding in mothers and fathers.

Method: Mothers (n = 427) and fathers (n = 170) of infants aged up to 1-year-old participated in an online study. The parents completed questionnaires on depression (Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, EPDS), anxiety and stress (Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale, DASS-21). Parent-infant bonding was measured by Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) that has three components: Impaired bonding (PBQ1), Anxiety about care and parental distress (PBQ2), and Lack of enjoyment and affection with infant (PB3Q). Parental sensitivity was measured as the number of correct recognitions of infant facial expressions (City Infant Faces Database, CIFD). Responsiveness was measured as a self-report with two subscales of responsiveness and non-responsiveness (Maternal Infant Responsiveness Instrument, MIRI).

Results: The path analysis showed that the model had a good fit to the data. Parental sex was a significant moderator, indicating different paths in mothers and fathers. In mothers, responsiveness and non-responsiveness were significant mediators between depression symptoms and three dimensions of bonding. In fathers, only non-responsiveness was a significant mediator between anxiety and PBQ3. Although recognizing infant facial expressions directly affected PBQ3 in mothers (but not in fathers), it was not a significant mediator between mental health and bonding.

Conclusion: Higher levels of parental mental health problems (depression and anxiety) were associated with lower levels of parental responsiveness, which is, in turn, related to poor parent-infant bonding. Prevention and intervention programs should be offered for both mothers and fathers, focusing on postpartum mental health promotion and enhancing responsiveness in infant care.

Introduction

Maternal sensitivity and responsiveness have been identified as crucial for secure infant attachment. Maternal sensitivity seems to be a stable maternal characteristic during infancy (1). It refers to the maternal ability to perceive the infant cues and signals, interpret them correctly, and respond to them timely and adequately (24). These iterative processes in mother-infant interactions are essential for infant development as infants learn that their actions affect the environment, especially the secure figure, which gives them a sense of efficacy. Consequently, an infant who feels secure will explore the environment more, which will increase their socio-emotional and cognitive competencies (2).

Shin et al. (5) pointed out that the conception of maternal sensitivity has changed over time. Based on their conceptual analysis, four aspects of maternal sensitivity have been pointed out. These refer to maternal sensitivity as (i) dynamic process, (ii) including reciprocal exchanges between mother and infant, (iii) contingent with infant's previous behavior, and (iv) including appropriate responses based on specific infant cues. Maternal responsiveness is one aspect of sensitivity (5) and refers to maternal prompt and frequent responses to the infant's cues about physical and emotional states (3).

Shin et al. (5) provided a conceptual structure of maternal sensitivity describing its antecedents, affecting factors and consequences. Antecedents are described as maternal identity or identification of self as a mother, and of course, the infant's needs and cues upon which mother will express her sensitivity. One of the consequences of maternal sensitivity is the development of secure mother-infant relationships and higher quality of infant-to-mother attachment. Indeed, there is a bulk of literature showing the association between maternal sensitivity and secure attachment in infancy (1, 6), early childhood (7), and young adulthood (6).

Bonding is sometimes erroneously used as a synonym with attachment (810). However, the former can be defined as the maternal feelings and thoughts about the infant (9, 11), while attachment refers to the relationship between the child and the parent and makes the child feel secured (12). Furthermore, the methods of measures differ between the two, with questionnaires to measure bonding (13) and the observational method of Strange Situation Task (14) as a gold standard to measure attachment. As a strong mother-infant relationship, bonding is considered crucial for postpartum development (15). A recent literature review has also shown that maternal sensitivity is sometimes used as a synonym for bonding (16). However, although these are different constructs, there is a lack of studies looking at maternal sensitivity and responsiveness in relation to mother-infant bonding.

As affecting factors on maternal sensitivity, Shin et al. (5) distinguished some positive, such as social support and high self-esteem, and negative factors, such as maternal depression, stress, and anxiety. Maternal mental health in the postpartum period can be seriously challenged, with one in three primiparous women having symptoms of depression, anxiety or stress (17). A recent meta-analysis revealed that around 17% of healthy women report postpartum depression (18) and 8–10% report one or more anxiety disorders (19, 20). Also, comorbidity between postpartum depression and anxiety has been established (2123).

Poor parental mental health is one of the main risk factors for disrupted parent-infant interactions and may negatively affect bonding and attachment (24). Postpartum depression symptoms diminish the quality of bonding (2528). However, there are inconsistencies in respect to anxiety and bonding. Namely, some studies showed that anxiety is associated with poor bonding (29), while other studies showed that this relationship is fully mediated by depression. Moreover, once depression was controlled for, anxiety was not associated with poor bonding anymore (28, 30). Nevertheless, one study showed that anxiety was associated with improved mother-infant bonding (31), which the authors attributed to the increased maternal sensitivity. Although, the other study with mothers with social phobia did not show the difference in sensitivity compared to healthy controls (32). A meta-analysis showed that maternal depression symptoms were associated with diminished sensitivity during the first postpartum year (33). However, inconsistencies concerning postpartum depression and responsiveness are evident, as well. It was shown that postpartum depressive symptoms were associated with lower levels of maternal responsiveness (34, 35), or no association was found (36). However, the latter was established in a small sample of mothers with preterm infants.

There is a lack of studies that examines the complex relationship between maternal mental health, maternal sensitivity and responsiveness, and mother-infant bonding. Furthermore, in previous studies, self-report measures of sensitivity and responsiveness or observation during mother-infant interaction have been applied. Although observation is preferred over the self-report measured, the former can also be jeopardized due to personal bias of observer, the difficulty of coding, and change of behavior in the presence of the observer. On the other hand, objective measures of sensitivity, such as facial expression recognition, has been rarely applied. Nevertheless, several new databases of infant facial expression photographs have been developed (37, 38), which could be used as an objective measure of maternal sensitivity. This kind of measures was proven to be sensitive for maternal mental health, as it was shown that, e.g., mothers with postpartum depression tended to rate negative infant faces more negatively (39). Also, previous studies have mainly addressed the quality of bonding as unidimensional, although measured with the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) (40), which measures different aspects of bonding difficulties. Thus, the role of parental mental health and sensitivity should be examined concerning varying dimensions of bonding.

Finally, previous research on perinatal mental health problems or parental sensitivity has mainly focused on women, thus unjustifiably neglecting fathers (41, 42). Paternal role in the family functioning has substantially changed over the last several decades, with fathers becoming more involved and engaged nowadays (43). Although mothers are rated as more responsive to their preschool children needs than fathers (44), maternal and paternal sensitive parenting have comparable effects on children's cognitive ability (45). Furthermore, it was found that parental sensitivity was a full mediator between parenting stress and child cognitive abilities and prosocial behavior in both mothers and fathers (46). Also, for parental sensitivity, the vital is the parental ability to mentalize a child's thoughts, emotions, and needs that drive their behavior, the so-called reflective functioning, is essential (47). It was shown that the paternal reflective functioning was associated with their toddler's distress, even after accounting for maternal reflective functioning, and it also buffered the adverse effects of low income (41), thus implying the importance of the father's sensitivity for child development. Furthermore, there is a growing body of literature on paternal mental health in recent years, with a meta-analysis showing that around 8% of fathers have postpartum depression (48). In a recent large study of fathers, several depression profiles emerged with substantial stability from pregnancy to 2 months postpartum, although the depression levels decreased in the perinatal period (49). Another meta-analysis showed similar maternal and paternal depression effects on parenting behavior where depressed parents express less positive and more negative behaviors toward their children (50). Also, postpartum mental health difficulties in fathers extend to anxiety and stress (51), with paternal stress mediating the effect of anxiety on father-infant bonding (51). Parent-infant bonding is similar in mothers and fathers, although fathers report less fear and higher emotional involvement with the newborn in the first days after delivery (52). However, studies that would encompass mental health, sensitivity and responsiveness, and parent-infant bonding in fathers, are still scarce, as well as the studies in mother.

Therefore, this study aimed to examine the mediational role of parental sensitivity and responsiveness in a relationship between parental mental health and parent-infant bonding in both mothers and fathers. Also, we wanted to investigate different aspects of mental health, including depression, anxiety, and stress, as the conceptual analysis (5) pointed out as the affecting factors on maternal sensitivity. Furthermore, we wanted to provide different measures of parental sensitivity, including both objective measures of infant facial expression recognition and a self-report measure of responsiveness concerning various aspects of parent-infant bonding. The hypothesis was that parental sensitivity and responsiveness would mediate the relationship between mental health and parent-infant bonding in mothers and fathers.

Materials and Methods

Sample

A sample of mothers (n = 427) and fathers (n = 170) participated in the study. The inclusion criterium was having an infant of 1–12 months. The sample was predominantly married or cohabiting, highly educated, average to above-average self-reported socioeconomic status and lived in a city (Table 1). Approximately 60% of the sample had the first child.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic data for the sample of mothers (n = 427) and fathers (n = 170).

The sample of mothers and fathers did not differ in marital status, education level, socioeconomic status, number of children, and psychiatric heredity (Table 1). However, mothers were on average 2.5 years younger than fathers [Mmothers = 30.80, Mfathers = 33.19, t(595) = 4.94, p = 0.000] and less mothers than fathers were from the city larger than 100,000 citizens [mothers: 46.4%, fathers 57.6%, χ(2)2 = 7.57, p = 0.0228]. Also, more mothers reported psychiatric treatment during lifetime [mothers: 6.1%, fathers 1.8%, χ(1)2 = 4.03, p = 0.0447].

Instruments

Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale [EPDS; (53)] is a self-report measure of depression symptoms after childbirth. It consists of 10 items with four options different for each item, rated from 0 to 3, out of which seven items are reversely scored. The total possible score ranges from 0 to 30. A higher score indicates a higher level of depression symptoms, and the recent individual patient data meta-analysis established 11 as a cut-off score (54). The EPDS was previously translated and validated in the Croatian perinatal population with a one-factor structure and Cronbach α = 0.86 (55). In the current study, McDonald's ω coefficient was 0.86, respectively.

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale [DASS-21; (56)] is a self-report measure with three subscales for depression, anxiety and stress symptoms presented during the previous week. Each item was rated on a four-point scale (0—Did not apply to me at all to 3—Applied to me very much or most of the time). The scale was translated to Croatian (57). In the current study, a short version with 21 items (7 per subscale) was used, where the final score for each subscale is multiplied by 2 to be comparable to the full scale with a possible range from 0 to 42 where a higher score indicates a higher level of symptoms (56). The anxiety and stress subscales were used in the current study, with McDonald's ω coefficient of 0.84 and 0.88, respectively.

Maternal Infant Responsiveness Instrument [MIRI; (36, 58)] is a self-report measure of maternal responsiveness to the infant cues and perception of infant's response. The MIRI consists of 22 items rated on a 5-point scale (1—strongly disagree to 5—strongly agree), where a higher score indicates higher responsiveness. Six items are reversely scored. In the original study, a total score was calculated as a unidimensional construct, and the Cronbach's was α = 0.87–0.89 (36, 58). The MIRI was previously also administered in fathers, with = 0.88 (59). In the current study, to be comparable to both mothers and fathers, we excluded three items referring to the feeding items (e.g., I believe I know when my baby wants me to feed him/her). The CFA showed poor fit with the one-factor model [χ(152)2 = 1624.12, p < 0.001; χ2/df = 10.69, RMSEA = 0.127, SRMR = 0.100, CFI = 0.803]. Therefore, exploratory factor analysis was performed where the scree plot indicated two factors: positively framed items loaded onto one factor (Responsiveness) and reversely coded items loaded on the second (Non-responsiveness). The CFA was re-run testing the two model with better fit [χ(151)2 = 857.05, p < 0.001; χ2/df = 5.68, RMSEA = 0.088, SRMR = 0.064, CFI = 0.905] and showed non-significant correlation between the subscales (r = −0.03, p = 0.4850). The score on the Responsiveness (13 items) and Non-responsiveness subscale (6 items) could range from 13 to 65 and 6 to 30, respectively. Items on the Non-responsiveness scale remained reversely coded. Hence, a higher score on this subscale indicates a higher Non-Responsiveness (exemplary item: I believe my baby wants me to touch her/him too often). The McDonald's ω coefficient of internal consistency + was 0.96 for the Responsiveness and 0.77 for the Non-responsiveness.

City Infant Face Database [CIFD; (38)] is a set of 154 black-and-white photographs of infant emotional expressions. Photos were collected from infants varied in sex, age (1–12 months), and cultural background where infants express different emotional states, from positive (smiling and laughing), neutral to negative (sad, angry, scared etc.). In this study, we used a previous selection of 139 photographs validated in the sample of Croatian mothers, fathers, and students (60). In the current study, each participant rated the infant expression on 20 randomly chosen photographs (negative, neutral, or positive). The correct answer was scored with 1 point, so the total possible score ranged from 0 to 20.

Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire [PBQ; (40)] is a self-report measure of difficulties in the maternal-infant relationship and has been validated in the sample of mothers with different forms of maternal-infant disorders. The PBQ has 25 items rated on a 6-point scale (0—never to 5—always), with several reversely scored items, where a higher score indicates more disturbed bonding. Four subscales measure General Factor (12 items), Rejection and pathological anger (7 items), Anxiety about infant (4 items), and Incipient abuse (2 items) (40, 61). The Cronbach's α of the four factors ranged from 0.35 to 0.75 and was 0.78 for the total scale (40). The PBQ was validated in a large sample of Croatian mothers and fathers, where modified 20-item scale showed the excellent fit of both three-factor and one-factor structure: Impaired bonding (10 items, α = 0.94), Anxiety about care and maternal distress (6 items, α = 0.81), Lack of enjoyment and affection with infant (4 items, α = 0.77) (62). In the current study, the same three-factor structure was followed. McDonald's ω coefficient of internal consistency was 0.93, 0.94, 0.81, and 0.77 for the total scale, Impaired bonding, Anxiety about care, and Lack of enjoyment, respectively.

The sociodemographic questionnaire comprised question on age, marital status, level of education, employment status (before maternity leave for mothers), perceived socioeconomic level, and place of living. Furthermore, psychiatric history was examined. Participants could report a previous episode of depression or changed mood (no; yes, shorter than 2 weeks; yes, longer than 2 weeks), receiving psychiatric treatment (yes, no), and psychiatric heredity in the family (yes, no). A final set of questions referred to the pregnancy and the infant regarding the number of children, having twins from the last pregnancy, the infant age, and sex.

Procedures

The study was conducted following Helsinki 1964 Declaration. The Ethical Committee of the Catholic University of Croatia granted the ethical approval for the research. This cross-sectional study was conducted online via Google Forms with separate links for mothers and fathers. It was advertised on social networks (Facebook groups for parents) and shared through personal communication. The data were collected from May 2018 to May 2019. Each participant read the informed consent and by clicking the “Next” button gave their consent to participate in the study. It took ~20 min to fill in all the questionnaires.

Statistical Analysis

Samples of mothers and fathers were compared in sociodemographic and psychological variables using the t-test and χ2-test (with Yates' correction when necessary) with SPSS Statistics 21.0 for Windows and GraphPad Prism version 9.0 for χ2-test. Correlations between the studied variables were examined using the Pearson r correlation coefficient. The factor structure of the examined constructs was examined by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) by MPlus 8.2 software or exploratory factor analysis (Principal Axis Factoring) when necessary.

All variables were normally distributed with skewness and kurtosis index (Table 2) within the suggested 3 and 10, respectively (63), except for the parent-infant bonding. Data on Impaired bonding (PBQ1) and Lack of enjoyment and affection with infant (PBQ3) exceeded both skewness index above 3 and kurtosis index above 20, which indicate serious non-normality (63).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive data for psychological variables with comparison between mothers (n = 427) and fathers (n = 170).

Path analysis of the associations between parental mental health (depression and anxiety), parental sensitivity, and bonding (three aspects) was performed in MPlus 8.2. The maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors—the MLR estimator—was used as this procedure takes into account non-normality induced bias in the standard errors (64, 65). The goodness of fit was evaluated by several indices χ2-test, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), and Comparative Fit Index (CFI). Acceptable model fit is indicated when RMSEA and SRMR values are below 0.08, and CFI values are above 0.90 (66), while a very good fit is displayed when the RMSEA is below 0.06, SRMR is below 0.08, and CFI values are above 0.95 (67). Reliability of measures was calculated as the internal consistency via McDonald ω coefficient, as a better alternative to Cronbach α (68) using the OMEGA macro for SPSS (69). Sample size calculation was performed as per the general rule of thumb to have at least 50 participants per variable in the path analysis and to have a medium sample size of 100–200 per group (70). Given that nine variables were examined, at least 450 participants were necessary, which was exceeded with 597 participants, out of which 170 were fathers.

Results

Descriptive Data

Descriptive data for all psychological variables is presented in Table 2. A somewhat reduced range was obtained for depression and anxiety in both mothers and fathers. However, 20.8% of mothers and 14.7% of fathers reported depression symptoms above the proposed cut-off of 11 on the EPDS (54). A full range of observed data was obtained for bonding scores, and the almost whole possible range was obtained for responsiveness and facial expression recognition. The scores were compared between mothers and fathers, showing that mothers reported higher depression symptoms, stress, responsiveness, and a lower level of non-responsiveness. On the other hand, fathers expressed more inferior bonding in the Lack of enjoyment and affection with the infant. However, all effects were small (Table 2).

Associations Between Examined Variable

Very similar patterns of associations were established for mothers and fathers (Table 3). Higher levels of depression symptoms were associated with higher anxiety and stress levels in both samples with modest correlations. Also, in both mothers and fathers, higher parental mental health difficulties (depression, anxiety, and stress) were related to poor bonding but with small correlations. Further, higher levels of mental health difficulties were associated with lower responsiveness and higher non-responsiveness. Facial expression recognition was not related to parental mental health or responsiveness. However, it had a slight negative correlation with non-responsiveness, indicating that poor facial expression recognition was associated with higher non-responsiveness. Also, poor facial expression recognition was related to a Lack of enjoyment and affection with the infant, in mothers only, but with a small correlation.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson's correlation coefficients between psychological variables in mothers (n = 427, above diagonal) and fathers (n = 170, below diagonal).

Parental Sensitivity and Responsiveness as Mediators

The model of parental sensitivity as a mediator between parental mental health and bonding was tested. Depression symptoms, anxiety, and stress were entered as predictors; responsiveness, non-responsiveness, and facial expression recognition were entered as mediators; and three aspects of bonding were entered as the outcome. All possible direct and indirect effects were defined in the model. The model was saturated with excellent fit to the data [χ(6)2 = 14.47, p = 0.0248; χ2/df = 2.41, RMSEA = 0.069, SRMR = 0.021, CFI = 0.989].

The parental sex was examined as the moderator in the model. This was tested with the nested model with specified parameters set to be equal between mothers and the fathers [χ(48)2 = 117.77, p < 0.0001; χ2/df = 2.45, RMSEA = 0.070, SRMR = 0.062, CFI = 0.906]. This model was significantly different from the initial model [Satorra-Bentler Scaled χ2 difference was SBS- χ(42)2 = 103.26, p < 0.0001; CD = 1.1766], indicating that the parental sex was a significant moderator. Thus, different paths were established in mothers and fathers (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model paths predicting parent-infant bonding from parental mental health via parental sensitivity and responsiveness in mothers and fathers. Only significant paths are presented. All coefficients are standardized. *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01.

In mothers, responsiveness was a significant mediator between postpartum depression symptoms and all three bonding dimensions (Table 4). Namely, higher levels of depression symptoms were associated with lower levels of responsiveness, which was, in turn, associated with Impaired bonding, Anxiety about care and maternal distress, and Lack of enjoyment and affection with the infant. Furthermore, non-responsiveness was a significant mediator between maternal depression and one aspect of bonding. More specifically, higher levels of depression were associated with higher levels of non-responsiveness, which was, in turn, related to poor bonding concerning Anxiety about care and maternal distress.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Model estimates of multigroup path analysis: Depression symptoms and anxiety on bonding via responsiveness and facial expression recognition (N = 603).

In fathers, the only significant indirect path was for non-responsiveness. Higher levels of anxiety were associated with higher levels of non-responsiveness, which was, in turn, related to poor father-infant bonding concerning Lack of enjoyment.

Finally, facial expression recognition did not mediate mental health and bonding in mothers or fathers. Nevertheless, it did directly affect bonding in mothers so that mothers who were less accurate at recognition reported higher levels of Lack of enjoyment and affection with baby. Also, even though anxiety and stress did not correlate with infant facial expression recognition in mothers or fathers, in the model, these direct effects were significant, indicating possible suppressor effect (71, 72). Direct effects from parental mental health on all three dimensions of bonding were established (Figure 1). However, it is interesting to note that anxiety did not directly affect bonding concerning the Anxiety about care. Also, responsiveness had a direct effect on all dimensions of bonding, both in mothers and fathers.

Discussion

There was a lack of studies looking into the role of maternal sensitivity for mother-to-infant bonding in the literature, and even more, there was a neglect of fathers. Therefore, this study aimed to examine parental sensitivity as a mediator in the relationship between parental mental health and parent-infant bonding in both mothers and fathers. The model had a good fit to the data, and parental responsiveness was a significant mediator between postpartum mental health and bonding quality. However, different paths were established for mothers and fathers, which will be discussed further.

First, parental sensitivity in the current study was measured by a self-report measure of responsiveness as one aspect of sensitivity (5) and an objective measure of infant facial expression recognition. Also, before going further, it should be noted that the Maternal Infant Responsiveness Instrument was previously used as a unidimensional measure (3436, 58, 59) without questioning its factor structure. However, the initial psychometric evaluation in the current study showed a poor fit of the unidimensional model to the data. This secondary finding highlights the need for psychometric testing of instruments at each administration. Namely, psychometric properties are not fixed characteristics of the instrument, as they also reflect the sample characteristics and administration circumstances (73). The two-factor structure of the MIRI had a better fit and resulted in subscales of responsiveness and non-responsiveness. These two were mutually uncorrelated, indicating that the non-responsiveness subscale is not a mere negative pole of responsiveness. Moreover, non-responsiveness taps different responsiveness aspects, reflecting fear of taking care of the infant and appraisals of the infant as being too demanding. Furthermore, it was interesting that these two subscales had a unique role in the relationship between mental health and bonding in mothers and fathers.

In mothers, responsiveness was a significant mediator between depression symptoms and bonding. Higher levels of depression symptoms were associated with lower levels of responsiveness, which was, in turn, related to poor bonding on all three dimensions, i.e., Impaired bonding, Anxiety about the care, and Lack of enjoyment with the infant. In fathers, responsiveness was not a significant mediator between mental health and bonding. However, non-responsiveness was a significant mediator both for mothers and fathers. Despite specific differences in the patterns of mediational pathways, we can summarize that both for mothers and fathers, (non)responsiveness has an important role in the shape of parent-infant bonding.

These findings are somewhat difficult to relate to previous research on bonding, as these constructs have not been examined all together in a mediational model, especially not in fathers. However, previous studies demonstrated an adverse effect of maternal depression symptoms on maternal sensitivity (33) and maternal responsiveness (34). On the other hand, it is not easy to compare findings on parental sensitivity and parent-infant bonding, as previous studies have mainly investigated maternal sensitivity observationally with infant-mother attachment (1, 6, 7). The same goes for examining the relationship between responsiveness and mother-infant bonding. However, Tester-Jones et al. (35) did investigate depression symptoms, maternal responsiveness, and bonding, but they did not relate these constructs in the same model but on the bivariate level. They did show that maternal depression was associated with lower levels of responsiveness and bonding, and these relationships were mediated by infant temperament.

On the other hand, another study did not show an association between maternal depressive symptoms and responsiveness but found a more dominant role of stress for responsiveness (36). However, the latter finding comes from a small sample of mothers with preterm babies who have specific childbirth and postpartum experience. It is known that mothers with preterm delivery are at higher risk of posttraumatic stress disorder following birth (74), which is, in turn, associated with impaired bonding in mothers (75).

The ability to recognize infant facial expressions was previously suggested to reflect maternal sensitivity (38). However, the mediational role of infant facial expression recognition was not established in the current study, either for mothers or fathers. It was expected that depression symptoms would be associated with facial expression recognition, but this was not evident. This finding was unexpected as previous studies showed that depressed mothers were less likely to identify happy infant faces (76) and rated negative infant faces more negatively (39). Different attentional processing of positive and negative infant emotions associated with depression symptoms was evident even during pregnancy (77, 78). A similar effect of depression was demonstrated in fathers, as well. A recent study showed that depressed fathers recognized happy faces with more difficulty but negative faces more easily, which, in turn, affected negatively on the father-infant interaction (79). On the other hand, some studies did not show attentional bias toward negative infant faces in mothers with affective disorders (80). So, the infant facial expressions recognition remains to be demonstrated as a measure of maternal sensitivity to infant's cues and its role in predicting parent-infant bonding.

The findings of this study have several implications for clinical practice. First, the study highlights the need for screening for a wide range of mental health difficulties. In addition to depression symptoms that most screening attempts are focused on (81), anxiety and stress also contributed to parental sensitivity and parent-infant bonding. Also, the screening should be applied to both mothers and fathers (82, 83). Because of the contributing effect of the partner depression (84), both parents can get into a vicious circle of depression, where a parent has a higher probability of developing depression symptoms if their partner also shows depression symptoms. Also, fathers should be provided with the same opportunities in the (prenatal) classes as mothers have to learn about newborn care, parenting sensitivity, and parent-infant bonding. Bonding between fathers and infants is a process that develops over the first year of the infant's life, as shown in the meta-synthesis of paternal experiences (43). The process progresses by getting to know the infant and having physical contact and interaction with the infant, which is especially rewarding for fathers. Therefore, courses for paternal engagement and enhancement in bonding should encourage fathers to take care of infants, play with them, or simply hold them. As they may feel the lack of knowledge and skills in infant care, they should be taught about this in (prenatal) classes and supported by their spouses, as fathers found their partners' support very encouraging (43). Particular focus should be on fathers whose infants are breastfed, as they may feel excluded and may need some additional time to catch up with their infant. Also, concerning the parental role in fathers, future studies should shift more from mere involvement, i.e., quantity, to the father-child relationship quality (42). Furthermore, sensitive parenting should be promoted to ensure a safe environment that is supportive and stimulating for the child development. Parents should comfort the child and provide a secure base for their exploration and autonomy (41, 42).

Several limitations of the study should be discussed. First, this sample of mothers and fathers was a non-clinical sample. Therefore, other possible conclusions could be withdrawn if the sample included parents with clinical depression, anxiety, or a bonding disorder. Nevertheless, at least one part of the parents from the sample struggled with depression symptoms, as one in five mothers and one in six fathers reported clinically significant depression symptoms. Furthermore, the sample was recruited online via social network groups for parents, so one can argue that this sample is self-restricted. Indeed, the sample was urban, highly educated; almost all parents were married or cohabiting, with the majority reporting average to above-average socioeconomic status. As they have decided to participate in this study, they were probably interested in content about parenting and more engaged in their parental role. The sample of fathers was smaller than the sample of mothers; therefore, future studies would benefit from including the larger samples of fathers in order to replicate these findings. Also, the cross-sectional design was applied so one can speculate that different directions of associations could work as well. For instance, Brockington et al. (40) highlighted that depression in mothers could be caused or exaggerated by bonding problems. Although the model has a solid theoretical background, it was not previously tested for bonding, and future studies should confirm the model in longitudinal studies. Maternal interpersonal sensitivity measured during pregnancy was a stronger predictor of the mother-infant interaction quality than perinatal depressiveness (85), so it would be beneficial to measure maternal sensitivity even during pregnancy.

In this study, the role of anxiety for responsiveness and bonding was found only in fathers. However, it should be noted that a general measure of anxiety (DASS-21) was used in this study, which mainly covers somatic symptoms. Recent research has shown that anxiety specific for the postpartum period has a predictive value for bonding over general measures (86). Future studies could benefit from applying specific measures of anxiety that grasp the parental perinatal experience with more focus. Also, it should be noted that bonding was measured up to 12 months of the infant's age (with a mean at 6 months). However, the PBQ was designed for use in the early postpartum period (40, 61), and it has been mostly used and validated within the first 3 months after childbirth (8790). Nevertheless, some other studies applied the PBQ within the first postpartum year [e.g., (91, 92)]. Still, the factor structure and reliability across the first year postpartum should be examined in future studies. Furthermore, the infant facial recognition task included the recognition of unknown infant faces. As postpartum mothers have specific dopaminergic reward-related brain network activation when viewing their infants compared to unknown infant's faces (93), future studies should preferably include expressions of their infant.

Finally, it should be noted that the examined set of variables explained up to 32% of the parent-infant bonding variance. It means that two-thirds of the variance remains unexplained, and future studies should include other variables into the model. A recent cross-sectional study showed the interrelation of maternal mental health and bonding with perceived infant temperament (94). Infant temperament has been shown to affect the parent-infant bonding in a prospective study in mothers and father (95, 96). It also mediates the relationship between maternal depression and responsiveness (35) and might have a more substantial effect on infant-mother attachment than maternal sensitivity (97).

Also, previous studies have established the association between breastfeeding and maternal sensitivity. Longer breastfeeding was associated with higher maternal sensitive responsiveness levels during infancy (98) and even increased maternal sensitivity in middle childhood (99). Nevertheless, in the current study, different infant feeding methods were not considered as we wanted to test the same model in both mothers and fathers. The study's strength is including both parents, and further studies should focus on fathers in more depth. Also, future studies would benefit from pairing mothers and fathers so the dyadic relationships within the couple can be examined. A dyadic analysis on first-time parents revealed that postpartum depression levels are affected by own anxiety and parenting stress and partners' depression in both mothers and fathers (84). A recent study showed that mother-infant bonding contributes to father-infant bonding (51), and dyadic relationships of parental mental health and bonding should be further examined. Finally, some more stable characteristics, such as life satisfaction and self-esteem, seem to be more important predictors of maternal responsiveness (58), so the range of examined variables could be expanded.

To conclude, the current study showed that responsiveness has an important mediational role in the relationship between parental mental health and parent-infant bonding, both for mothers and fathers. This finding fits into Shin et al.'s (5) conceptual analysis of maternal sensitivity affected by maternal mental health. The model can be extended to apply not only for attachment as an infant-to-mother relationship but also to bonding as a mother-to-infant relationship and for fathers. However, theoretical and empirical work is needed to provide a solid theoretical basis for future studies on parent-infant bonding. It could have a crucial impact on developing interventions for parents and infants to alleviate mental health problems and their reflection on the bonding issues. A promising early intervention for reinforcing maternal sensitivity, especially in women with psychosocial vulnerability, has been tested recently (100). Future studies should continue developing such programs to help parents enjoy this transition to parenthood and provide safe and warm family relations for the growth of the child.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of the Catholic University of Croatia. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SNR devised the main conceptual idea, supervised the project, performed the analysis, and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was financed by the project of the Catholic University of Croatia “Parental mental health and contextual factors as determinants of parent-infant bonding”, under Grant HKS-2018-4.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Maja Brekalo and Marijana Matijaš for help with data collection, and special thanks to Maja Brekalo for insightful discussions.

References

1. Bigelow AE, MacLean K, Proctor J, Myatt T, Gillis R, Power M. Maternal sensitivity throughout infancy: continuity and relation to attachment security. Infant Behav Dev. (2010) 33:50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bornstein MH, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Maternal responsiveness and infant mental abilities: specific predictive relations. Infant Behav Dev. (1997) 20:283–96. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90001-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. De Wolff M, van Ijzendoorn M. Sensitivityand attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. (1997) 68:571–91.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

4. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (1978).

Google Scholar

5. Shin H, Park YJ, Ryu H, Seomun GA. Maternal sensitivity: a concept analysis. J Adv Nurs. (2008) 64:304–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04814.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Schoenmaker C, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Linting M, van der Voort A, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. From maternal sensitivity in infancy to adult attachment representations: a longitudinal adoption study with secure base scripts. Attach Hum Dev. (2015) 17:241–56. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Posada G, Trumbell J, Noblega M, Plata S, Peña P, Carbonell OA, et al. Maternal sensitivity and child secure base use in early childhood: studies in different cultural contexts. Child Dev. (2016) 87:297–311. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Benoit D. Infant-parent attachment: definition, types, antecedents, measurement and outcome. Paediatr Child Health. (2004) 9:541–5. doi: 10.1093/pch/9.8.541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bicking Kinsey C, Hupcey JE. State of the science of maternal-infant bonding: a principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery. (2013) 29:1314–20. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2012.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Redshaw M, Martin C. Babies, “bonding” and ideas about parental “attachment.” J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2013) 31:219–21. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2013.830383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dubber S, Reck C, Müller M, Gawlik S. Postpartum bonding: the role of perinatal depression, anxiety and maternal–fetal bonding during pregnancy. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2015) 18:187–95. doi: 10.1007/s00737-014-0445-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. Am J Orthopsychiatry. (1982) 52:664–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wittkowski A, Vatter S, Muhinyi A, Garrett C, Henderson M. Measuring bonding or attachment in the parent-infant-relationship: a systematic review of parent-report assessment measures, their psychometric properties and clinical utility. Clin Psychol Rev. (2020) 82:101906. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ainsworth MD, Bell SM. Attachment, exploration and separation: illustrated by the behaviour of one year olds in a strange situation. Child Dev. (1970) 41:49–67.

15. Brockington I. Motherhood and Mental Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1996).

Google Scholar

16. Abuhammad S, Johnson T. Potential impact of breastfeeding and maternal sensitivity during the first year of life: an integrative review of the literature. Int Journla Pediatr. (2018) 6:8655–67. doi: 10.22038/ijp.2018.33637.2975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Miller RL, Pallant JF, Negri LM. Anxiety and stress in the postpartum: is there more to postnatal distress than depression. BMC Psychiatry. (2006) 6:12. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-6-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Shorey S, Chee CYI, Ng ED, Chan YH, Tam WWS, Chong YS. Prevalence and incidence of postpartum depression among healthy mothers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. (2018) 104:235–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dennis CL, Falah-Hassani K, Shiri R. Prevalence of antenatal and postnatal anxiety: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2017) 210:315–23. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.116.187179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Goodman JH, Watson GR, Stubbs B. Anxiety disorders in postpartum women: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 203:292–331. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Correia; LL, Linhares MBM. Maternal anxiety in the pre- and postnatal period: a literature review. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem. (2007) 15:677–83. doi: 10.1590/s0104-11692007000400024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nakić Radoš S, Tadinac M, Herman R. Anxiety during pregnancy and postpartum: course, predictors and comorbidity with postpartum depression. Acta Clin Croat. (2018) 57:39–51. doi: 10.20471/acc.2018.57.01.05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Putnam K, Robertson-Blackmore E, Sharkey K, Payne J, Bergink V, Munk-Olsen T, et al. Heterogeneity of postpartum depression: a latent class analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2015) 2:59–67. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00055-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Field T. Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting, and safety practices: a review. Infant Behav Dev. (2010) 33:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kinsey CB, Baptiste-Roberts K, Zhu J, Kjerulff KH. Birth-related, psychosocial, and emotional correlates of positive maternal-infant bonding in a cohort of first-time mothers. Midwifery. (2014) 30:e188–94. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2014.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kerstis B, Aarts C, Tillman C, Persson H, Engström G, Edlund B, et al. Association between parental depressive symptoms and impaired bonding with the infant. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2016) 19:87–94. doi: 10.1007/s00737-015-0522-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Nakić Radoš S, Matijaš M, Andelinović M, Cartolovni A, Ayers S. The role of posttraumatic stress and depression symptoms in mother-infant bonding. J Affect Disord. (2020) 268:134–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Tolja R, Nakić Radoš S, Andelinović M. The role of maternal mental health, infant temperament, and couple's relationship quality for mother-infant bonding. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2020) 38:395–407. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2020.1733503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nicol-harper R, Harvey AG, Stein A. Interactions between mothers and infants: impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behav Dev. (2007) 30:161–7. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Tietz A, Zietlow AL, Reck C. Maternal bonding in mothers with postpartum anxiety disorder: the crucial role of subclinical depressive symptoms and maternal avoidance behaviour. Arch Womens Ment Heal. (2014) 17:433–42. doi: 10.1007/s00737-014-0423-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Edhborg M, Nasreen HE, Nahar Kabir Z. Impact of postpartum depressive and anxiety symptoms on mothers' emotional tie to their infants 2–3 months postpartum: a populationbased study from rural Bangladesh. Arch Womens Ment Heal. (2011) 14:307–16. doi: 10.1007/s00737-011-0221-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Murray L, Cooper P, Creswell C, Schofield E, Sack C. The effects of maternal social phobia on mother-infant interactions and infant social responsiveness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2007) 48:45–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01657.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bernard K, Nissim G, Vaccaro S, Harris JL, Lindhiem O. Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: a meta-analysis. Attach Hum Dev. (2018) 20:578–99. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1430839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Miller ML, O'Hara MW. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms, intrusive thoughts and depressive symptoms: a longitudinal study examining relation to maternal responsiveness. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2020) 38:226–42. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2019.1652255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Tester-Jones M, O'Mahen H, Watkins E, Karl A. The impact of maternal characteristics, infant temperament and contextual factors on maternal responsiveness to infant. Infant Behav Dev. (2015) 40:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Amankwaa LC, Pickler RH, Boonmee J. Maternal responsiveness in mothers of preterm infants. Newborn Infant Nurs Rev. (2007) 7:25–30. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2006.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Maack JK, Bohne A, Nordahl D, Livsdatter L, Lindahl ÅAW, Øvervoll M, Wang CEA, et al. The tromso infant faces database (TIF): development, validation and application to assess parenting experience on clarity and intensity ratings. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Webb R, Ayers S, Endress A. The City Infant Faces Database: a validated set of infant facial expressions. Behav Res Methods. (2018) 50:151–9. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0859-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Stein A, Arteche A, Lehtonen A, Craske M, Harvey A, Counsell N, et al. Interpretation of infant facial expression in the context of maternal postnatal depression. Infant Behav Dev. (2010) 33:273–8. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Brockington IF, Oates J, George S, Turner D, Vostanis P, Sullivan M, et al. A screening questionnaire for mother-infant bonding disorders. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2001) 3:133–40. doi: 10.1007/s007370170010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Buttitta K V, Smiley PA, Kerr ML, Rasmussen HF, Querdasi FR, Borelli JL. In a father's mind: paternal reflective functioning, sensitive parenting, and protection against socioeconomic risk. Attach Hum Dev. (2019) 21:445–66. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2019.1582596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cabrera NJ. Father involvement, father-child relationship, and attachment in the early years. Attach Hum Dev. (2020) 22:134–8. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2019.1589070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Shorey S, Ang L. Experiences, needs, and perceptions of paternal involvement during the first year after their infants' birth: a meta-synthesis. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0210388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wilson S, Durbin CE. Mother-child and father-child dyadic interaction: parental and child bids and responsiveness to each other during early childhood. Merrill Palmer Q. (2013) 59:249. doi: 10.13110/MERRPALMQUAR1982.59.3.0249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mills-Koonce RW, Willoughby MT, Zvara B, Barnett M, Gustafsson H, Cox MJ. Mothers' and fathers' sensitivity and children's cognitive development in low-income, rural families. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2015) 38:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ward KP, Lee SJ. Mothers' and fathers' parenting stress, responsiveness, and child wellbeing among low-income families. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2020) 116:105218. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Slade A, Grienenberger J, Bernbach E, Levy D, Locker A. Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: a preliminary study. Attach Hum Dev. (2005) 7:283–98. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Cameron EE, Sedov ID, Tomfohr-Madsen LM. Prevalence of paternal depression in pregnancy and the postpartum: an updated meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 206:189–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Garthus-Niegel S, Staudt A, Kinser P, Haga SM, Drozd F, Baumann S. Predictors and changes in paternal perinatal depression profiles—insights from the DREAM study. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:563761. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.563761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wilson S, Durbin CE. Effects of paternal depression on fathers' parenting behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:167–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bieleninik Ł, Lutkiewicz K, Jurek P, Bidzan M. Paternal postpartum bonding and its predictors in the early postpartum period: cross-sectional study in a polish cohort. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:628650. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.628650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Figueiredo B, Costa R, Pacheco A, Pais A. Mother-to-infant and father-to-infant initial emotional involvement. Early Child Dev Care. (2007) 177:521–32. doi: 10.1080/03004430600577562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. (1987) 150:782–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Levis B, Negeri Z, Sun Y, Benedetti A, Thombs BD. Accuracy of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) for screening to detect major depression among pregnant and postpartum women: systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data. BMJ. (2020) 371:m4022. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m4022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Nakić Radoš S, Tadinac M, Herman R. Validation study of the croatian version of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). Suvremena Psihol. (2013) 16:203–18. http://suvremena.nakladaslap.com/public/pdf/16-2-4.pdf

Google Scholar

56. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:335–43.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

57. Reić Ercegovac I, Penezić Z. Skala depresivnosti, anksioznosti i stresa [Depression, anxiety stress scale]. In: Proroković A, Adorić VC, Penezić Z, Tucak Junaković I, editors. Zbirka psihologijskih skala i upitnika, svezak 6 [Collection of psychological scales questionnaires]. Vol. 6. Zadar: University of Zadar. p. 15–22 (2012).

Google Scholar

58. Drake EE, Humenick SS, Amankwaa L, Younger J, Roux G. Predictors of maternal responsiveness. J Nurs Scholarsh. (2007) 39:119–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.2007.00156.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Seah CKF, Morawska A. When mum is stressed, is dad just as stressed? Predictors of paternal stress in the first six months of having a baby. Infant Ment Health J. (2016) 37:45–55. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Nakić Radoš S, Matijaš M, Andelinović M, Webb R, Ayers S. Validation of the City Infant Faces Database in student and parent samples. (2021).

61. Brockington IF, Fraser C, Wilson D. The postpartum bonding questionnaire: a validation. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2006) 9:233–42. doi: 10.1007/s00737-006-0132-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Andelinović M, Nakić Radoš S, Matijaš M. Validation of postpartum bonding questionnaire in a sample of Croatian mothers and fathers. In: Pačić-Turk L, KneŽević M, editors. 2nd International Scientific Conference Brain Mind: Promoting Individual Community Well-Being. Book of Abstract. Zagreb: Catholic University of Croatia p. 153. Available online at: https://www.bib.irb.hr/1039400 (accessed August 6, 2021).

63. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 3rd ed. London: Guilford Press (2011).

Google Scholar

64. Finney SJ, DiStefano C. Non-normal categorical data in structural equation modeling. In: Hancock GR, Mueller RD, editors. Structural Equation Modeling: A Second Course. Greenwich, Connecticut: Information Age Publishing. p. 269–314 (2006).

Google Scholar

65. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide. 8th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (1998-2017).

Google Scholar

66. Little TD. Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2013).

Google Scholar

67. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model A Multidiscip J. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Trizano-Hermosilla I, Alvarado JM. Best alternatives to Cronbach's alpha reliability in realistic conditions: congeneric and asymmetrical measurements. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:769. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hayes AF, Coutts JJ. Use omega rather than cronbach's alpha for estimating reliability. Commun Methods Meas. (2020) 14:1–24. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press (2005).

Google Scholar

71. Mackinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevent Sci. (2000) 1:173–81. doi: 10.1023/A:1026595011371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Thompson FT, Levine DU. Examples of easily explainable suppressor variables in multiple regression research. Mult Linear Regres Viewpoints. (1997) 24:11–13.

Google Scholar

73. Streiner DL, Kottner J. Recommendations for reporting the results of studies of instrument and scale development and testing. J Adv Nurs. (2014) 70:1970–79. doi: 10.1111/jan.12402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Dikmen Yildiz P, Ayers S, Phillips L. The prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder in pregnancy and after birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2017) 208:634–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Stuijfzand S, Garthus-Niegel S, Horsch A. Parental birth-related PTSD symptoms and bonding in the early postpartum period: a prospective population-based cohort study. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:570727. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.570727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Arteche A, Joormann J, Harvey A, Craske M, Gotlib IH, Lehtonen A, et al. The effects of postnatal maternal depression and anxiety on the processing of infant faces. J Affect Disord. (2011) 133:197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Pearson RM, Cooper RM, Penton-Voak IS, Lightman SL, Evans J. Depressive symptoms in early pregnancy disrupt attentional processing of infant emotion. Psychol Med. (2010) 40:621–31. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Tang W, Bao C, Xu L, Zhu J, Feng W, Zhang W, et al. Depressive symptoms in late pregnancy disrupt attentional processing of negative–positive emotion: an eye-movement study. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:780. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Koch S, De Pascalis L, Vivian F, Meurer Renner A, Murray L, Arteche A. Effects of male postpartum depression on father–infant interaction: the mediating role of face processing. Infant Ment Health J. (2019) 40:263–76. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Webb R, Ayers S. Postnatal mental health and mothers' processing of infant emotion: an eye-tracking study. Anxiety Stress Coping. (2019) 32:484–97. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2019.1620215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Lieb K, Reinstein S, Xie X, Bernstein PS, Karkowsky CE. Adding perinatal anxiety screening to depression screening: is it worth it? Am J Obstet Gynecol MFM. (2020) 2:100099. doi: 10.1016/j.ajogmf.2020.100099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Asper MM, Hallén N, Lindberg L, Månsdotter A, Carlberg M, Wells MB. Screening fathers for postpartum depression can be cost-effective: an example from Sweden. J Affect Disord. (2018) 241:154–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Walsh TB, Davis RN, Garfield C. A call to action: screening fathers for perinatal depression. Pediatrics. (2020) 145:e20191193. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-1193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Vismara L, Rollè L, Agostini F, Sechi C, Fenaroli V, Molgora S, et al. Perinatal parenting stress, anxiety, and depression outcomes in first-time mothers and fathers: a 3- to 6-months postpartum follow-up study. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:938. doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2016.00938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Raine K, Cockshaw W, Boyce P, Thorpe K. Antenatal interpersonal sensitivity is more strongly associated than perinatal depressive symptoms with postnatal mother-infant interaction quality. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2016) 19:917–25. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0640-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Fallon V, Silverio SA, Grovenor JC, Bennett KM, Harrold JA, Fallon V, et al. Postpartum-specific anxiety and maternal bonding : further evidence to support the use of childbearing specific mood tools. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2021) 39:114–24. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2019.1680960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Reck C, Klier CM, Pabst K, Stehle E, Steffenelli U, Struben K, et al. The German version of the postpartum bonding instrument: psychometric properties and association with postpartum depression. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2006) 9:265–71. doi: 10.1007/s00737-006-0144-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Mazúchová L, Kelčíková S, Maskalová E, Malinovská N, Grendár M. Mother-infant bonding and its associated factors during postpartum period. Kontakt. (2021) 23:126–32. doi: 10.32725/kont.2021.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Suetsugu Y, Honjo S, Ikeda M, Kamibeppu K. The Japanese version of the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire: examination of the reliability, validity, and scale structure. J Psychosom Res. (2015) 79:55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Busonera A, Cataudella S, Lampis J, Tommasi M, Zavattini GC. Psychometric properties of the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire and correlates of mother-infant bonding impairment in Italian new mothers. Midwifery. (2017) 55:15–22. doi: 10.1016/J.MIDW.2017.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Wittkowski A, Williams J, Wieck A. An examination of the psychometric properties and factor structure of the Post-partum Bonding Questionnaire in a clinical inpatient sample. Br J Clin Psychol. (2010) 49:163–72. doi: 10.1348/014466509X445589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Hairston IS, Handelzalts JE, Lehman-Inbar T, Kovo M. Mother-infant bonding is not associated with feeding type: a community study sample. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. (2019) 19:125. doi: 10.1186/S12884-019-2264-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Strathearn L, Li J, Fonagy P, Montague PR. What's in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics. (2008) 122:40–51. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Davies SM, Silverio SA, Christiansen P, Fallon V. Maternal-infant bonding and perceptions of infant temperament: the mediating role of maternal mental health. J Affect Disord. (2021) 282:1323–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Parfitt Y, Pike A, Ayers S. Infant developmental outcomes: a family systems perspective. Infant Child Dev. (2014) 23:353–73. doi: 10.1002/icd.1830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Takács L, Smolík F, Kazmierczak M, Putnam SP. Early infant temperament shapes the nature of mother-infant bonding in the first postpartum year. Infant Behav Dev. (2020) 58:101428. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Coffman S, Levitt MJ, Guacci-Franco N. Infant-mother attachment: relationships to maternal responsiveness and infant temperament. J Pediatr Nurs. (1995) 10:9–18. doi: 10.1016/S0882-5963(05)80094-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Tharner A, Luijk MPCM, Raat H, IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Moll HA, et al. Breastfeeding and its relation to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2012) 33:396–404. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318257fac3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Weaver JM, Schofield TJ, Papp LM. Breastfeeding duration predicts greater maternal sensitivity over the next decade. Dev Psychol. (2018) 54:220–27. doi: 10.1037/dev0000425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Aarestrup AK, Skovgaard Væver M, Petersen J, Røhder K, Schiøtz M. An early intervention to promote maternal sensitivity in the perinatal period for women with psychosocial vulnerabilities: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychol. (2020) 8:41. doi: 10.1186/s40359-020-00407-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: postnatal depression, anxiety, stress, responsiveness, fathers, mother-infant bonding, maternal sensitivity

Citation: Nakić Radoš S (2021) Parental Sensitivity and Responsiveness as Mediators Between Postpartum Mental Health and Bonding in Mothers and Fathers. Front. Psychiatry 12:723418. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.723418

Received: 10 June 2021; Accepted: 12 August 2021;
Published: 01 September 2021.

Edited by:

Antoine Bechara, University of Southern California, United States

Reviewed by:

Laura Vismara, University of Cagliari, Italy
Karolina Kossakowska, University of Łódź, Poland

Copyright © 2021 Nakić Radoš. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sandra Nakić Radoš, snrados@unicath.hr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.