Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 June 2022
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding the Influences of Sex and Gender Differences in Mental Disorders View all 8 articles

Where Sex Meets Gender: How Sex and Gender Come Together to Cause Sex Differences in Mental Illness

  • 1Department of Psychology, National Centre for Psychotraumatology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
  • 2Department of Pharmacology, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Sex differences are prevalent in multiple mental disorders. Internalizing disorders are more commonly diagnosed in women, whereas externalizing and neurodevelopmental disorders are more often diagnosed in men. Significant sex/gender differences are reported in prevalence, symptom profile, age of onset, comorbidities, functional impairment, prognosis, as well as in responses to various treatments. In this conceptual article, we discuss theories and empirical studies of sex- and gender-related influences in mental health, by focusing on three examples: autism spectrum disorder (ASD), acknowledged as a disorder whose roots are mainly biological; eating disorders, whose origins are considered to be mainly psychosocial, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), an environmentally caused disorder with both psychosocial and biological underpinnings. We examine the ways in which sex differences emerge, from conception through adulthood. We also examine how gender dichotomies in exposures, expectations, role assumptions, and cultural traditions impact the expression of our three selected mental illnesses. We are especially interested in how sex-based influences and gender-based influences interact with one another to affect mental illness. We suggest that sex and gender are multi-faceted and complex phenomena that result in variations, not only between men and women, but also within each sex and gender through alterations in genes, hormone levels, self-perceptions, trauma experiences, and interpersonal relationships. Finally, we propose a conceptual diatheses-stress model, depicting how sex and gender come together to result in multiple sex/gender differences across mental disorders. In our model, we categorize diatheses into several categories: biological, intrapersonal, interpersonal, and environmental. These diatheses interact with exposure to stressors, ranging from relatively minor to traumatic, which allows for the sometimes bidirectional influences of acute and long-term stress responses. Sex and gender are discussed at every level of the model, thereby providing a framework for understanding and predicting sex/gender differences in expression, prevalence and treatment response of mental disorders. We encourage more research into this important field of study.

Introduction

This hypothesis and theory paper is based on observations of a substantial effect of sex and gender on the expression of many mental disorders. We consider “sex” and “gender” to be two different, though overlapping, concepts. Both refer to the designation of “male/masculine” or “female/feminine,” but research has traditionally addressed the two terms differently. Individual disciplines have used different methodologies, and sometimes different terminologies to study sex and gender. Sex is generally defined as the biological distinction between males and females based on sex chromosomes XX or XY, established at conception. The male/female distinction is identified visually, even prior to birth, by the structure of external genitalia, and is later confirmed by its consistency with internal reproductive organs.

In contrast to sex, gender signifies one's relationship to the subjective perception of oneself as male or female. Gender is grounded in biological sex and strongly influenced by genotype and the prenatal hormonal environment, yet it is also subject to the perception and expectations of others and affected by parental rearing practices, by environmental exposures, by the development of sexual preferences, by cultural pressures, and by the process of identity formation. Research on the influence of sex has used biological tools, and has often been based on animal models. Research on gender has used anthropological or sociological terminology and methodology, with observations often viewed through a feminist lens. Though often treated as separate constructs, the two (sex and gender) are inseparable and neither can be fully understood in the absence of the other.

The worldwide prevalence of some mental disorders is reported as significantly more prevalent in girls and women whereas other disorders are more often diagnosed in boys and men. As a general rule of thumb, from puberty onward, internalizing disorders (those during which negative emotions are subjectively experienced) are more commonly seen in women (13). This group of disorders includes, among others, disordered eating and disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (1, 47), which are characterized by subjective feelings of low self-esteem and insecurity, a tendency to self-blame, and a habit of inhibiting the outward expression of anger (8). In contrast, externalizing disorders, where distress is outwardly expressed through impulsive or aggressive behavior, are more commonly reported in men (8). Externalizing disorders are characterized by behaviors rather than by emotions, and by impaired ability to communicate feelings (1, 4, 6, 7, 9). Another general distinction with respect to prevalence, is that disorders that first manifest in childhood are usually more prevalent in boys. This is true for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (10, 11). The rates of mental disorders more commonly seen in women rise after puberty and come to surpass those of men. In the elderly, male/female differences in prevalence tend to wane (11). For some DSM-5 disorders, the extent of prevalence differences by sex/gender is substantial (please see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. F:M sex differences across mental disorders.

Whereas, most research on sex/gender differences in mental disorders has focused on prevalence and symptom severity, there also exist differences in the nature of symptoms, in age of onset, in frequency of episode recurrence, in chronicity, comorbidities, precursors, triggers, functional impairment, and treatment response (24, 25). In some cases, sex/gender differences are found in the psychiatric comorbidities associated with a predominant mental disorder -e.g., aggressive behavior and substance use disorder in men, contrasting with eating disorders and depression in women (2630). The purpose of this hypothesis and theory paper is to apply new interpretations of recent data to the role of sex and gender in mental illness. Our aim is to highlight how sex and gender coalesce to result in differences between men's and women's mental health, the focus of this special Frontiers issue. Our three selected diagnoses each represent different poles of the sex/gender continuum, with ASD being more commonly diagnosed in boys, PTSD presenting in both sexes, and eating disorders seen far more frequently in women. Another important distinction among the three is that ASD begins early in life; eating disorders usually start at adolescence, and PTSD in adulthood. Finally, these three disorders were selected because they are differently influenced by diatheses (predispositions) and stressors (triggers): ASD more by biological diatheses, eating disorders more by societal diatheses, and PTSD requiring, by definition, a traumatic stressor. These three disorders thus fit into different niches of the new theoretical framework that we will present at the end of this paper.

Autism Spectrum Disorders

ASD is ~4 times more prevalent in boys than in girls (16, 31). The source of this marked difference remains unclear; its basis is likely to be multifactorial but is poorly understood. There is no doubt about the biological etiology of ASD—these disorders are highly heritable. But reasons for the sex ratio in prevalence is a mystery because few of the 100+ thus far identified risk genes reside on sex chromosomes. Baron-Cohen has proposed the “extreme male brain theory” of ASD, attributing sex differences to testosterone levels (32). This is plausible because multiple lines of evidence suggest dysregulated steroidogenesis (33, 34), but testosterone levels are unlikely to fully explain the male/female discrepancy. Rather than searching for the causes of heightened risk in males, one might look, instead, for female protective factors. Girls who are diagnosed with ASD carry significantly more genetic load than boys (35), suggesting adaptive mechanisms such as the ability to effectively mask ASD symptoms (36). If such mechanisms exist, they remain to be discovered. There are also several clinical differences between boys and girls with ASD: more externalizing behavior (e.g., aggressive outbursts, social impairment, hyperactivity, restricted behaviors and interests) in boys and more internalizing symptoms in girls (e.g., depression, anxiety, emotional symptoms) (37). Since diagnostic criteria are based on the much more prevalent male presentation, it is likely that girls, who present somewhat differently, are underdiagnosed (38).

Eating Disorders

If one omits neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD, the male/female prevalence ratio is higher in eating disorders than in any other forms of mental illness. Anorexia nervosa affects 0.9–4% of women and roughly 0.3% of men (39). This major difference in prevalence rate is usually attributed not to biology but to contrasts in the gender roles assumed by women and men in adolescence and to the peer pressures to which they are exposed (40). Multiple interacting factors, however, probably play a part. Genetic and epigenetic causes, gonadal hormone levels, and constituents of the immune system have all been considered as potentially contributory to the prevalence ratio. In other words, socialization into gender-specific roles and societal expectations may be exaggerating innate proclivities (41). Because thinness in many cultures equates to female beauty, and beauty serves as a proxy for youth, health and reproductive fitness, eating disorders may, from an evolutionary perspective, reflect an ancient mating strategy—enhancing beauty to attract men who will ensure long term survival of offspring (42). In modern times, this strategy is made increasingly complex because media and the fashion industry define beauty standards.

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

In most prevalence studies, approximately twice as many women as men are diagnosed with PTSD, and this ratio is consistent across diagnostic systems, measurement methods, ethnicities, cultural backgrounds, and study populations (43). The trauma preceding this disorder is considered to be an exposure to one or more extremely threatening events, such as sexual, physical, or severe emotional abuse, a death in the family, or a serious accident, natural or manmade disaster (44). Symptoms of PTSD are more severe, chronic, and recurrent in women than they are in men (4547). Associated symptoms/behaviors also differ. Women with PTSD in addition to the core symptoms report dissociative phenomena, somatization, disordered eating, low self-esteem, guilt, and shame. Men, in contrast, present with problems relating to aggression, thrill-seeking, risk-taking, and impulse-control (7). Diagnosable comorbidities induced by trauma also differentiate men and women -e.g., depressive and anxiety disorders accompany PTSD in women while substance use disorders are prevalent in men (48).

We will now explore in greater detail some of the theories that have been proposed to explain sex and gender differences in mental illnesses, using our three selected disorders as examples. The emphasis in each of the theories is different, but they all acknowledge that they are, in themselves, incomplete, and that complementary perspectives are needed for a full explanation of the many factors that result in sex/gender differences.

Sex-Based Theories

Sex Chromosomes

At conception, most females possess two X chromosomes and most males have one X and one Y. Some male-specific genes are located on the Y chromosome, the most important one being the SRY or testis-determining gene. Once testes develop, the process of subsequent differentiation between the sexes begins (49). Male testes and female ovaries secrete different levels of hormones (estrogens, androgens, progestins, and anti-Müllerian hormone) that subsequently influence most body organs, including the brain. Because females have two X chromosomes, one in each body cell is automatically and randomly inactivated so that males and females end up with an approximately equal total of gene products (50). But ~25% of genes on the inactivated X chromosome escape full inactivation. This process, in addition to epigenetic effects on genes on all chromosomes (50, 51), results in sex/gender differences in the rates of many medical conditions. Proposed mechanisms include the unequal level of gene products expressed by sex biased genes. These are genes that are present in both sexes but that express different quantities of protein products in men and women due to the impact of either epigenetic factors or sex hormone levels (52).

Genes are important even in conditions such as eating disorders, which result from mainly social causes. Twin-based heritability for anorexia nervosa ranges from 48 to 74%, for bulimia nervosa from 55 to 62%, and for binge-eating disorder from 39 to 45% (53). A recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) of ~17,000 anorexia nervosa cases and 55,000 controls yielded 8 significant genetic loci that may contribute to etiology (54). Susceptibility genes, it should be noted, may lie dormant for long periods and only become expressed under specific dietary or perhaps hormonal conditions (55). Sex chromosomes have only recently begun to be included in GWAS screens. In addition, many of the databases used for transcriptome analysis are skewed toward data that has been generated predominantly in males, which means that more genes relevant to sex differences in medical conditions still await discovery (56).

Brain Structure

Sex biased genes are present in the central nervous system in great number and may be partially responsible for the neuroanatomical sex differences that have been reported in the human brain. Brain structure develops not only through the action of genes, but also in response to environmental influence so that postnatal experience, which usually differs significantly in girls and boys, affects the size of specialized brain regions. In spite of many reported sex/gender differences in both animal and human brains, these are subject to considerable heterogeneity, even within regions, so that the human brain is not male or female, but best described as a mosaic of relative masculinity and femininity (57). Nonetheless, the question remains as to whether early life programming by testosterone in male fetuses leads to enduring sex differences in brain and behavior (33). Girls who experience elevated androgens in utero tend to display prototypic male behaviors to a variable degree that does not necessarily correlate with structural changes in the genitalia (58). For instance, persons who are born completely insensitive to androgen can be chromosomally XY but phenotypically and psychosexually female (59). This is important because it means that in humans, as in experimental animals, the lack of androgens, rather than the presence of ovarian steroids, is what principally determines femininity.

The developmental trajectory of the brain differs by region and by sex such that the pace of maturation of large areas of the brain, such as the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus, differs in boys and girls (60, 61). As a result, experiences of trauma or injury, though occurring at comparable ages, may manifest very differently in the two sexes/genders (62). In general, the pace of maturation and development of the nervous system proceeds at a faster pace in girls (63) and, at puberty, comes under the influence of estrogens, which exert protective effects on brain systems via reduction in neuro-inflammation, promotion of synaptic plasticity, and regulation of neurotransmitter signals (64). Growing scientific attention is also being given to sex-based differences in brain circuitry (65), which probably affect response to environmental insult. This means that, although differences in the brain are commonly considered the product of biological sex, gender-based influences may also play a strong, if not equal, role (66).

The Central Nervous System and HPA-Axis

The central nervous system (CNS) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis are both central to the body's response to stress. The role of the CNS is to respond quickly and efficiently to perceived stressors and the role of the HPA axis is to maintain that response for as long as needed, and to return the body to homeostasis once the threat has subsided (67, 68). The HPA axis connects the CNS to the endocrine system while also impacting the sympathetic-adrenal medullary (SAM) axis, the immune system, and the cardiovascular system, as well as directly modulating affective and cognitive processes in the brain (69, 70). A dysregulated HPA axis is central to the trauma and stress response. Dysregulation can result in either an inadequate response to threat and or an excessive stress response that persists long after the threat has passed; both are potentially harmful (71). As a result, the HPA axis is hypothesized to strongly influence the development of stress- and trauma-related disorders such as PTSD. Furthermore, a dysregulated HPA axis can help to explain why people suffering from PTSD respond to traumatic triggers either with hyper-arousal (increased heart rate, respiration, and blood pressure), often resulting in a startle response and a feeling of panic, or with hypo-arousal (decreased heart rate, immobility), a frequent precursor to dissociation. Interestingly, the former response appears more common in men and the latter in women (71), for unknown reasons.

The development and functioning of the HPA axis are influenced by one's genome, the prenatal environment, and by life experiences. HPA functioning is especially sensitive to repeated early life trauma (72). As a result, there are multiple ways in which both sex and gender can affect the HPA axis. Both adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and free cortisol levels have been reported to increase twice as much in men as in women in response to a psychosocial stress task, though results may depend on the specific stressor studied (69). Some studies have suggested that women's monthly hormonal fluctuations sensitize the HPA-axis (73, 74). This may be one reason why women, more than men, risk developing stress-and-trauma-related disorders. In ASD, on the other hand, multiple lines of evidence suggest that dysregulated steroidogenesis during early development contributes to male preponderance (33, 34). With respect to eating disorders, an activated HPA axis has been closely associated with these conditions although the activation may be secondary, an effect of starvation (75).

Somatic Factors

Somatic factors outside the brain, such as immune processes, are capable of influencing the brain and resulting in psychiatric symptoms. In most animals, male sex is associated with a relatively lowered immune response and a relatively higher susceptibility to infection (76). The explanation may lie in the many immune-related genes that are encoded on the X chromosome. Although, as explained earlier, one of the two copies in females is silenced, immune-related genes may escape inactivation in some body cells. As well, immune cells such as T cells and B cells show varying sex-related rates of age-associated decline, with immunosenescence starting earlier in males than in females by ~6 years (76). Furthermore, female hormones play an important protective role in immunity. Estradiol, for instance, has been convincingly shown to reduce the levels of inflammatory cytokines (77). This accords with preclinical animal research, which finds both greater levels of inflammatory signaling molecules and activated immune cells in healthy males than in healthy females (78). From an evolutionary perspective, immune sex differences serve an important purpose because the female immune system needs to suppress an antibody response to male sperm during conception and to male fetuses during pregnancy (79).

Sex differences in the immune system affect the brain, and are seen most notably in the functions of astroglia and microglia, both brain cells that reportedly play a role in psychiatric disease (80). It is currently believed that a combination of genetic sex, plus the organizational and activational effects of gonadal hormones, plus levels of stress hormones contributes significantly to brain immune function (81).

Inflammation experienced either in utero or very early in life is a known risk factor for ASD (8183), and transcriptome analysis of fetal human brain reveals high levels of gene expression consistent with increased baseline immune reactivity in PTSD (46, 47), which may be transmitted to offspring in a sex-specific manner through varying levels of maternal proinflammatory cytokines over the course of pregnancy (84). Animal research suggests that maternal immune activation or early life inflammation exert negative impact on the brains and subsequent behavior of males, but not females (85).

There are connections between the immune system, the brain, and the gut (86). The composition of intestinal microorganisms appears to affect brain functioning and sex/gender reportedly plays a part in that composition, although study results are inconsistent (87, 88). Men and women tend to eat different foods, take different medications, show different susceptibility to weight gain, and their colonic transit times, on average, differ. These are all factors that affect gut bacteria and potentially influence the expression of many psychiatric disorders (8991), but this area of research is still in its infancy.

Gonadal Hormones

Gonadal hormones play important roles in differentiating the manifestations of mental disorders in men and women (9294). Besides conferring protection against the effects of injury, inflammation, ischemia and apoptosis, estrogens also promote neuronal growth and enhance neuro-reparative processes such as remyelination. They also serve important regulatory functions in relation to neurotransmitter systems (94). There is evidence that gonadal hormones play a critical role in aiding the epigenetic expression of eating disorder genes (9597). Gonadal hormones also contribute to our understanding of ASD (98) and PTSD (99).

Though findings are confounded by differences in measurement methods and ascertainment times, time periods associated with female reproduction, such as pregnancy, the postpartum, and menopause are often associated with the emergence or aggravation of psychiatric symptoms (100). For example, women exposed to traumatic events during the early follicular phase of their menstrual cycle show comparatively increased risk of developing PTSD (100). Naturally occurring hormonal fluctuations in women can also affect response to treatment with psychiatric medications (101). The efficacy of certain psychotropic medications appears to decrease during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle (102) and the risk of drop-outs from trauma-focused cognitive behavior therapy varies according to the time of the month in which it is initiated (7).

An important limitation to note when discussing the influences of gonadal hormones on symptom development is that much of the data is based on animal studies and requires further study in humans (101).

Gender-Based Theories

Parental Expectations

Whereas, sex differentiation is initiated at conception, the influences of gender start to take effect after birth. Parental expectations of offspring, however, precede birth. Parents choose nursery decor, infant clothing, distinctive toys and given names to match cultural gender norms. For example, in many cultures, girls' names allude to beauty and grace while boys' names suggest strength and sturdiness (103). Differentiation along similar lines is widely accepted, as shown by results of a recent study from China. College students, when asked to rate forenames, characterized feminine names as reflecting warmth and masculine names as representing competence (104).

Gender norms are defined as the expectation that boys and girls, men and women, think, feel, talk, and generally behave in gendered ways. The content and rigidity of gender norms and their endorsement by parents and peers vary across cultures and sub-cultures, but the general rule is that boys are expected to be strong and to control their emotions (except anger, which is permitted) whereas girls throughout the world are expected to be amiable, relatively docile, and nurturing (8). Gender traits are often grouped together and referred to as femininity vs. masculinity.

Gender Socialization

Children learn to act and to think according to gender norms, with parents considered to be the primary channel through which these norms are transmitted (105). Parents interact differently with girl and boy children, encouraging more physical exploration and toughness in boys and more emotional communication, tenderness, and focus on appearance in girls (106). This has been seen as contributing to the high prevalence of eating disorders in girls and women. Parental expectations are reinforced by interpersonal interactions and by the media and can lead to the development, in girls, of vulnerability to body image concerns. Though this is a generality, gender norms frequently encourage girls and women to aim for an idealized body shape and boys and men to nourish a drive for muscularity, accomplishment and power (107, 108).

In general, children are rewarded for what the world around them judges to be desirable behavior and criticized for behaviors that are idiosyncratic (109). In terms of gender socialization, this means that society, including one's parents, teachers, and peers, have low tolerance for personality traits and characteristics traditionally assigned to the opposite gender. Over time, such socialization processes strongly impact the way children see themselves. This happens through mechanisms such as the Rosenthal (Pygmalion) effect (110). Phenomena such as the frequently impaired communicative abilities in boys and underdeveloped mathematical abilities in girls have been linked to this effect.

Stress arises when the environment poses threats to the gender norms that one's culture values. For this reason, men and women are to an extent stressed by different kinds of threats. Women, reportedly, are most threatened by relationship losses whereas threats to status and financial stability are especially stressful for men (111).

Gender Roles

Children internalize the gender norms to which they are exposed and, over time, impose these stereotypes on themselves, a phenomenon referred to as self-socialization (112). Children especially take on gender roles modeled by same-sex adults and peers whom they admire and this affects their actions, appearance, hobbies, dreams, educational pursuits, careers, intimate relationships, and eventual parenting styles (113). Structural theories suggest that different gender roles lead to exposure to different kinds of threats and stressors, each calling for different coping strategies, and resulting in gender differences in adaptability and mental health (114). One such theory, role restraint theory, states that, when men and women are allowed to take on similar roles and face similar stressors, differences in their responses disappear (115). Socialization theories address not only exposures but also the development of sex-specific vulnerabilities to stressors and sex-specific coping strategies, e.g., women tending to confide and share, men tending to deny and seek distraction (116).

Gender Identity

Though the term “gender identity” is sometimes used to refer to self-perceived gender, it is best defined as a person's deeply felt internal and personal experience of their own gender (117). This includes how one sees oneself, the possibilities and restraints attributable to one's gender, a sense of one's body, societal role, and sexual preferences. It also includes non-binary gender identification (118). Gender identity is most lastingly developed during adolescence, with body image, sexual orientation, and range of potential gender roles becoming central themes (119). A perceived incongruence between society's gender norms and one's gender identity can become a source of emotional problems and felt constraints. For example, masculine gender-role stress, defined as stress resulting from the struggle to live up to masculine ideals, has been associated with PTSD (1, 120). A striking example would be a male veteran who suffers from PTSD after a sexual assault and struggles with the perceived discrepancy between military masculine ideals and the role of victimhood and someone struggling with mental illness (120).

Diagnostic Bias

There are multiple sources of gender bias both in mental health research and in the clinical setting. Women have traditionally not been widely recruited for biologically-based mental health research studies because their hormonal fluctuations complicate the task of appropriate matching and because of the concern that an experimental intervention may potentially harm the fetus should an unexpected pregnancy occur. This means that clinical guidance is often still based on research involving mainly men (7). In contrast, many psychotherapeutic trials are conducted primarily on women, simply because more women than men seek help for mental health problems. Furthermore, the vast majority of PTSD research on men is conducted on military personnel or veterans and may not be generalizable to civilian men, let alone to women.

Even in a biologically based disorder, such as ASD, there is evidence of diagnostic bias. This is likely due to the fact that the earliest cases of these disorders were observed in boys, so that the diagnostic criteria were established based on characteristic male symptoms. As stated earlier, it is perhaps because of this that ASD remains often undetected in girls, especially when symptoms are mild (37, 121). Sources of data have substantial clinical impact on how diagnostic criteria are formulated, on screening tools, on diagnostic and clinical guidelines, and on the efficacy of the many different ways in which mental illnesses are treated. Most standardized tests, diagnostic questionnaires, and diagnostic criteria themselves are based on the assumption that men and women experience symptoms in the same way and that a specific question is equally well-suited for eliciting relevant symptoms in women and men, whereas this may not be the case (122). In contrast, in the absence of standardized measures, clinicians may preferentially ask questions of one sex/gender and not of the other. For example, women (but not men) are commonly asked during assessments about exposure to sexual trauma and perinatal loss, though men, too, are exposed to such experiences and their sequelae. The same is true for questions about spousal abuse. Women, on the other hand, are unlikely to be asked about ever engaging in acts of aggression. Similar to the Rosenthal effect mentioned previously, when men and women are repeatedly asked questions about specific experiences and/or symptoms, in time, they will become more likely to report such symptoms whereas, when these areas are left unprobed, they stay unreported, especially so if they are perceived as incongruent with gender norms (115). Thus, clinicians may be more likely to overlook the presence of certain disorders in one sex compared to the other.

Furthermore, using dichotomous categories to assess sex and gender forces both male and female study participants into highly heterogenous groups, with no allowance for intra-group variability. It may be time to rethink how we assess sex and gender and introduce continuous measures, allowing for a wider range of gender-normative variability.

The Coming Together of Sex and Gender

As can be gathered from the literature we have cited, it is essentially impossible to state with certainty which differences between male/female expression of illness are primarily based in biology and which are products of nurture, learning, or environment. For most mental health syndromes, there is evidence for the impact of both sex and gender, through the different factors summarized in Tables 2, 3. Some distinctions between the two influences may, however, be hypothesized. The fact, for instance, that sex/gender differences are found across cultures favors sex-related explanations, whereas cross-cultural variations in the prevalence of differences suggest the influence of gender. In most cases, including the three disorders we focus on here, though sex/gender differences are found universally, their strength varies somewhat across regional and cultural groups, indicating that both sex (innate vulnerability) and gender (specific exposures and expectations) play a part (123, 124). Because most published research has been conducted in Western cultures, relatively little is known of the extent to which results from studies on gender-based factors, such as gender identity and gender-role stress, generalize across cultures. For example, whereas the negative effects of masculine gender-role stress are relatively well-established within Western culture, it is possible that feminine gender-role stress may be clinically significant in parts of the world where women's roles are more restricted. Indeed, many culture-bound syndromes (125127) can be considered as expressions of women's difficulties with their assigned gender roles.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of sex-based theories.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of gender-based theories.

In most cases, when looking for etiological strands leading to mental illnesses, it is impossible to disentangle the many interacting influences of sex vs. gender. For example. though sex/gender differences in “masculine” and “feminine” traits are rooted in biological sex, socialization processes prod girls to adopt traits assigned to femininity and boys to model traits generally assigned to masculinity (112). Furthermore, the sudden increase of sex/gender differences around the time of puberty in many mental disorders is often interpreted as support for the influence of gonadal hormones, but to what degree hormones directly contribute to this finding is unclear. Of probably equal importance is the increased exposure in adolescence to societal pressures to conform to gender-role expectations (72). Evidence for the impact of both sex and gender is present in almost all such attributions.

It is also important to note that sex- and gender-based influences often modulate each other. For example, prenatal gonadal hormones affect gender identity and sexuality, which, along with secondary sex characteristics and hormones, lead to different socialization experiences, which, in their turn, impact the body's gonadal hormone production (128, 129). As such, sex and gender appear to influence each other in multiple bidirectional ways. In general, both brain functions and HPA-axis reactivity are affected by genetic predisposition, biological development, and environmental exposures/expectations (72).

In the following section, we build on the observations presented above to suggest how sex and gender based influences coalesce.

Discussion

We have cited literature demonstrating multiple potential explanations for men's and women's different vulnerabilities to specific psychiatric disorders. On the basis of such explanations, several theoretical models have been constructed, of which mediation models form one subgroup. Mediation models are built on the premise of risk factors and protective factors. For example, in one such model (130), most of the variance of sex/gender in PTSD was attributed to pre-existing symptoms and traits, peri-traumatic reactions, and subsequent negative thoughts, on one hand, and absence of social support on the other (130). Such models, however, generally fail to explain why, as a group, men and women differ in the all-important risk and protective factors. We, thus, run the risk of tautological arguments (e.g., women have higher symptom levels after a traumatic event because they had higher symptom levels prior to the event). One way of arriving at more accurate models is to analyze the roots of sex- and gender-related mediators, be they risk or good fortune. For example, whereas the presence of substantial social support may distinguish women from men, we still need to understand why men and women elicit different levels of social support, use their supports for different purposes, and appraise their helpfulness in different ways. As stated by Kistner, naming mediators is not sufficient (11). Instead, we need to better understand how sex/gender differences in both distal and proximal risk factors arise.

In what follows, we examine how exactly sex and gender come to be associated with specific sex differences in mental disorders by proposing a model that combines multiple sex and gender based influences on mental health.

A Diathesis-Stress Model for Understanding Sex and Gender Differences in Mental Disorders

Diathesis-stress models have been used across disciplines to integrate multiple baseline vulnerabilities or predispositions, so-called diatheses, with situational stressors (triggers) (131). According to these models, whether or not a specific person develops a disorder depends upon the interaction between pre-existing vulnerabilities and the extent, number, timing, and severity of the stressors they experience.

We propose a diathesis-stress model for sex and gender differences across mental disorders (though we illustrate the model via our three selected disorders—ASD, eating disorders, and PTSD). As indicated in Figure 1 through the use of color gradations, the emphasis on diathesis vs stress varies according to disorder. A diagnostic category thought to originate mostly from diatheses is shown in a darker blue than one mostly caused by exposure to stressors and traumatic events. For example, ASD shows substantial heritability and results mainly from biological causes (132). Moreover, it emerges very early in life before many environmental stresses have had a chance to accumulate. However, in utero stressors, are also part of the genesis of ASD (133), thus a diathesis-stress model is appropriate. Furthermore, disruptions in everyday life and comorbidities elicit symptoms and, thus, are central to the expression and quality of life in ASD (134). In contrast, PTSD requires exposure to a traumatic event and cannot, by definition, develop in the absence of such an event. However, whereas highly resilient individuals may only develop PTSD after a particularly toxic traumatic event, extensive underlying diatheses can cause others to succumb in response to relatively minor stresses. Finally, though most investigators consider social pressures to be the main factors causing eating disorders, there is evidence for substantial individual predispositions (135), thus placing eating disorders in between ASD and PTSD. Please see Table 4.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A diathesis stress model of sex and gender differences in mental disorders. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; S, schizophrenia; ED, eating disorders; D, depression; A, anxiety; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder. The differences in shading represents a shift in the diathesis vs. stress balance for the different disoders.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Examples of how the diathesis-stress model may account for sex/gender differences in ASD, eating disorders, and PTSD.

Few models have addressed the potential effects of multiple diatheses in combination with an accumulation of situational stressors. Such a model (131), however, is truer to life given that the etiology of psychiatric disorders is multifactorial and complex. One complexity is the usual categorization of sex and gender either as a diathesis or as a risk factor (136), but this is an oversimplification because sex/gender exerts its combined effects through a relatively large number of mediating factors (136).

Hyde and colleagues (137) constructed an integrated model of the emergence of sex/gender differences in depression in which they hypothesize that affective, biological, and cognitive factors converge to result in a vulnerability that is only expressed when a severe stressor triggers symptoms, the so-called ABC model (137). Factors a, b, and c interact with each other and with sex/gender to form the diathesis. The presence of a particularly strong risk factor within one of these domains may be sufficient for symptoms to emerge. Equally possible is for relatively weak risk factors across two or three domains to combine and, together, trigger depression. Although Hyde et al.'s model focuses on depression, a similar model can be constructed for other disorders, including those that first emerge in childhood (e.g., ASD) and those that most commonly emerge in adolescence (eating disorders) or adulthood (PTSD).

We suggest that a diathesis-stress model can be created to account for the complex interaction between sex and gender and their combined involvement in the development and expression of multiple mental disorders. Whereas, some aspects of the model will be specific to certain disorders, many elements will be relevant across diagnoses. We will now introduce some of the key elements of such a model.

Diatheses

In accordance with a model presented by Parker and Brotchie (138), designed to account for sex/gender differences in depression, Christiansen (1) first suggested that the diathesis component of the model represents an inherent preparedness toward responding to a stressor with excessive fear and anxiety, which, when a person is exposed to trauma, significantly increases the risk of developing symptoms (1, 138). This inherent preparedness is, on average, more marked in women than in men and, thus, represents a biological sex difference in PTSD vulnerability (139). There is evidence that women, as a group, have a greater likelihood than men to be genetically predisposed toward fear (140). In line with other models (130, 131), we acknowledge that diatheses may vary in their degree of specificity, with some being disorder-specific (e.g., early childhood mealtime conflicts as a specific risk factor for eating disorder in adolescence), while others are shared among multiple disorders (e.g., negative affectivity or childhood trauma) (141, 142). Elwood and colleagues (136) suggest a possible hierarchy of diatheses that are more or less essential for the development of a specific disorder. Their model supports our earlier statement, that a single high-level diathesis may be sufficient for a person to develop a disorder when exposed to stress, whereas a lower-level (less specific) diathesis can only lead to symptoms when combined with other pre-existing vulnerabilities.

McKeever and Huff (131), in their diathesis-stress model of PTSD, suggest two types of diatheses: ecological and biological, which influence each other. Moreover, either one can elicit specific stressors from the environment. Ecological diatheses consist of risk factors linked to an individual's self (e.g., developmental history, coping mechanisms, social support). The distinction between ecological and biological diatheses is particularly relevant in a model of sex/gender differences, as gender would probably be considered an ecological diathesis and sex a biological diathesis, depending on the disorder. The distinction is further relevant to a model spanning multiple disorders, with some being biologically influenced (e.g., ASD) and others (e.g., PTSD) being more ecologically based. Our model distinguishes among different ecological diatheses by separating out intrapersonal factors, interpersonal/relational factors, and societal factors. Our model also differentiates between sex and gender because we believe that biological diatheses will mostly (but not exclusively) rely on sex, whereas societal diatheses will mostly rely on gender, and intra- and interpersonal factors will likely rely equally on both. A non-conclusive list of diatheses related to sex/gender differences in mental disorders can be found in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Examples of diatheses relevant to sex differences.

Finally, whereas diathesis-stress models generally focus specifically on personal vulnerabilities to disorders, we think of the presence or absence of external resources also being diatheses. An example would be the presence of social capital serving as protective factor against symptom development (143).

Biological Diatheses

Biological vulnerabilities in any one disorder are often specific to that disorder, but some are more general. One example is the involvement of the same neurotransmitter in multiple disorders, which is why selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), for instance, are effective in several different categories of mental disorder. Biological vulnerabilities relevant to sex/gender differences are likely to include genes on X and Y chromosomes (but also on autosomes), impairments of brain structure and function, insufficient or excessive levels of gonadal and other hormones, disturbances of HPA axis reactivity, immune system, and/or microbiome irregularities, as addressed earlier. Biological diatheses are often present at birth and strongly impacted by sex. However, gender also affects biological factors through experience, particularly through the influence of socialization and gender-specific stressors that lead, for instance, to the activation of the HPA axis or to epigenetic arousal of dormant genes.

Intrapersonal Diatheses

Intrapersonal diatheses include affective and cognitive vulnerabilities that, along with pre-existing symptoms and disorders, serve as predispositions to new disorders. Affective factors consist of temperament, negative affectivity, sometimes referred to as neuroticism, and mood. Most research agrees that women score higher than men on measures such as sadness, grief, negative affectivity, and anxiety proneness, whereas men tend to score higher on measures of aggression, anger, and impulsivity. Common cognitive vulnerabilities include rumination, maladaptive coping styles, poor self-perceived coping efficacy, negative appraisals, and negative schemas. Cognitive schemas are best understood as internal working models of how individuals view themselves, other people, and the world. Understanding gender norms, gender roles and gender-related schemas, and one's expectations of oneself and others fall into this category.

In our model, coping strategies and appraisals are considered post-stressor factors in that they are often situationally specific and associated with a particular stressor and a person's response to that stressor, as well as with subsequent symptoms. However, there are overall general coping styles and attributional styles that remain relatively stable over a person's life, and, as such, should be considered diatheses. For instance, women have repeatedly been found to ruminate more than men, to appraise situations as more threatening/severe than men do, and to score higher on maladaptive aspects of emotion-focused coping (144). These are enduring traits that serve as diatheses for all internalizing disorders. Furthermore, they exacerbate existing disorders, including neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD, by undermining levels of functioning and quality of life, making it more difficult for already vulnerable individuals to cope with their symptoms. By contrast, enduring tendencies toward anger, aggression, and lack of impulse control serve as diatheses for externalizing disorders. Finally, our model also places internalized gender roles, masculine and feminine traits, gender identification, and sexual preferences into the category of intrapersonal vulnerabilities.

Interpersonal Diatheses

Interpersonal aspects of our model include sex/gender differences related to insecure attachment, poor or negative social support (145), and victim blaming (146). Attachment is usually considered an intrapersonal factor, but we have included it in this category, as the quality of early attachments exerts a strong influence on subsequent relationships. Though women, as a group, are not less securely attached than men, they do score higher on the attachment anxiety dimension of attachment, which has been linked to later symptom development (147). With respect to social support, women have consistently been found to be recipients of both positive and negative social support, more so than men. Though positive support serves to buffer the impact of stressors, the effects of negative support (hurtful social interactions) may carry greater weight and undermine mental health.

Recurrent patterns of intimate interpersonal interactions with friends, family members, and lovers (e.g., abusive relationships, response to sexual intimacy, enjoyment of sex, relationship gender roles) fall into the category of interpersonal diatheses, whereas we categorize one off stressful interactions (e.g., fights, assaults) as stressors.

Societal Diatheses

The last group of diatheses relating to sex/gender differences in mental disorders are social and environmental: socio-economic status, caste, residential area, educational level, work status, degree of independence, marginalization, politics and other societal factors that set individuals apart. Social hierarchies affect men's and women's access to financial resources, support, protection, financial aid, medical and psychological treatment, police assistance, and legal justice and matter greatly in the aftermath of a traumatic exposure. They are strongly influenced by gender. Factors such as financial dependence, poor access to healthcare, and exposure to domestic abuse put women at a much higher risk than men for developing trauma-related disorders, even though men, as a group, are more exposed to industrial accidents, criminal victimization, and the trauma of war (7).

Gender-based socialization, gendered violence, misogyny (and, to a much lesser extent, misandry), homophobia, and transphobia, also belong in the social diathesis category, whereas the internalized version and enactment of such phenomena fit best under intrapersonal diatheses. Though we designated gender roles as intrapersonal (because they arguably result from internalized gender norms), we have called gender-role stress a social diathesis as it results from a mismatch between internalized gender identity and society's gender-based expectations.

We note in our model that sex and gender has the power to modulate societal diatheses, for example through the ability of secondary sexual characteristics, dress, hair styles, the application of cosmetics or scents, and plastic surgery to elicit and change the behavior of others, leading to changes in one's social status. Traits associated with masculinity, such as assertiveness and aggressiveness, are also able to alter established social hierarchies.

Stressors

The stress component of our model is composed of a range of events, from severe to minor. A non-exclusive list of such events shown previously to contribute to sex/gender differences in mental disorders is shown in Table 6. Aversive childhood events such as childhood sexual abuse are associated with multiple mental disorders later in life. But even a relatively minor break in routine upsets a child with ASD and can trigger a cascade of symptoms. As proposed elsewhere (1), the stress component of this model is much more likely to be affected by gender than by sex.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Examples of stressors relevant to sex differences.

Men, on average, experience a higher number of traumatic stressors than women. These is particularly the case for non-sexual torture, physical violence, severe accidents, gang violence, and witnessing severe injuries and death of others during both war and peacetime (46, 48, 144). Women, on the other hand, experience obstetrical trauma, sexual violence, and betrayal trauma, including intimate partner violence. With respect to everyday stressors, female gender role norms result in women being more embedded than men in the social networks of family and friends. As a result, women are exposed to more non-romantic interpersonal conflicts, losses, and indirect stressors [i.e., negative events that happen to others, such as the divorce or illness and death of close friends (148)]. Though slightly dated, one study of married heterosexual American couples found that wives reported more everyday stressors than husbands, especially in terms of household stressors, family demands, demands from others, and arguments with children (148). In turn, husbands reported more work stressors, stressors related to transportation, and arguments with non-family others. These differences may be accurate but may also result from reporting bias, as men and women differ, both in term of what stresses them, and how willingly they report such stressors. According to a classical paper, unmarried women live longer than married women, whereas married men live longer than single men (149). This may no longer apply because the division of labor between wife and husband has changed markedly over the years (at least in the West), but married women still take on additional caretaker tasks in families, as well as being more affected than men by partnership and family conflict, whereas men mostly benefit from the positive social support they receive through marriage (150).

Finally, sex/gender differences in the types of traumatic and non-traumatic events that men and women are exposed to may influence the type of symptoms they develop. For example, perhaps as a result of being more commonly exposed to betrayal trauma, women may be more likely develop self-doubts that lowers their self-esteem (151). Another example referred to earlier: whereas sexual assault often causes multiple problems with intimacy, sexuality, and self-worth in both men and women, male victims may, in addition, subsequently struggle with what they perceive to be a threat to their masculinity (120).

In accordance with other diathesis-stress models (e.g., 120), only immediate stressors are represented in the stressor component of our model. Once the stress has either subsided or become chronic, it may become part of the diathesis going forward, influencing the response to subsequent stressors. For example, a specific incident of racism, sexism, or homophobia may initially serve as a stressor and subsequently contribute to societal (e.g., perceived systemic racism or institutional sexism), intrapersonal (e.g., rumination), interpersonal (e.g., problems with intimacy), and biological diatheses (e.g., a more reactive HPA axis). The reverse is also true. Diatheses contribute not only to symptomatology but also to the types of stressors to which men and women are differentially exposed. Furthermore, an acute episode of severe mental illness may itself serve as a stressor, thus perpetuating the cycle of stress and distress. In this way, the diathesis and the stressor components of our model are impacted by each another.

Peri- and Post-stressors

Though peri- and post-stressors do not fit neatly into either the diathesis or the stressor part of the model, they play a very important role in the development of many psychiatric disorders. In the case of PTSD, two meta-analyses have found that pre-traumatic risk factors account for only a small percentage of variance in PTSD development compared to peritraumatic and posttraumatic risk factors (152). Thus, a full diathesis-stress model can fully explain sex/gender differences in PTSD only to the extent that it can find a way to include effects of risk factors occurring at the time of and following a trauma. Similarly, for other disorders, the manner in which an individual appraises, responds to, and copes with a given stressor is often central to severity and chronicity. Although very relevant to chronicity, few diathesis-stress models have explicitly included peri and post-stressor factors (153).

It could be argued that peri-stressor factors should be part of the stress component of the model, as it is not the event in itself, but rather the full experience of it, e.g., perception, appraisal, and response, that is central to symptom development. However, this solution does not accommodate post-stressor factors that are vital to the development, and especially the maintenance, of symptoms.

It is important to note that peri-and post-stressor risk factors are strongly linked to pre-stressor risk factors. How a person responds to an event is, in large part, dependent on personality, affectivity, neurobiology, and past experience. Thus, we argue here that, since peri- and post-stressor factors are simultaneously influenced by diatheses and by the type and severity of the stressor, while also contributing to the overall experience and effect of the stressor, they deserve separate representation in the model. We have linked them to the diathesis-stressor interaction by referring to them as “stress reactions,” which includes both acute and long-term responses. Acute responses are appraisals of the stressor plus the biological and psychological responses that occur during and shortly after stress exposure. More long-term responses are continued appraisal, situational coping, social support, and the reactions of others. While these stress reactions contribute to both the type and the severity of symptoms, they are also, in turn, affected by the symptoms that emerge. For example, an individual may appraise early symptom development as either a natural reaction to a traumatic event or, at the opposite extreme, as a sign of losing one's mind. This may, in turn, either decrease or increase the number and severity of symptoms, as well as affect re-appraisal of the traumatic event. Though this interaction is bidirectional during the early aftermath of the stressor, we have chosen in our illustration of the model to include only arrows going from stress reactions to disorders/symptoms in Figure 1, in order to acknowledge that, over time, this association goes mostly in one direction.

Stress reactions are particularly relevant to consider in a diathesis-stress model of sex/gender differences as men and women tend to differ widely in both their acute and their long-term reactions to stress. As previously discussed, sex/gender differences have been demonstrated in the immediate physiological and psychological stress response, e.g., in hypo- vs. hyperarousal, perception of social support, coping ability, and treatment-seeking. It is our belief that sex/gender differences in these early responses are central to the emergence of clinically relevant symptoms and to their maintenance.

Conclusions

Figure 1 is our diathesis stress model that focuses on the role of sex and gender in psychiatric disorders. We argue that sex/gender disparities in (a) biological, intrapersonal, interpersonal, and societal diatheses, (b) everyday stressors, major life events, and traumatic events, and (c) the physiological and psychological response to such stressors, when combined together, result in sex/gender differences in the epidemiology, clinical expression, and treatment response of most, if not all, mental disorders. Thus, though the model is exemplified here with ASD, eating disorders, and PTSD, it is our belief that it may equally be used with other disorders, including, but not limited to, schizophrenia, anxiety disorders, and depression, as shown in Figure 1. It is a major strength of this model that, in addition to highlighting differences between men and women, it also takes account of the many intra-sex variations (hormone levels, sexual identity, minority status) that are sometimes used in attempts to dismiss the influence of sex/gender.

The distinctions between different groups of diatheses is admittedly somewhat artificial because they are almost always intertwined. The concept of specificity within our model is compatible with the idea of multiple pathways, consisting of different combinations of diatheses and stressors, resulting in different categories of disorders, different symptom constellations, and different comorbidities (152). As an example, the high female/male prevalence ratio of trauma-related disorders, such as PTSD anxiety disorders, depression, somatization, and dissociative disorders, may to a large extent be accounted for by shared diatheses from each of the four domains: a more reactive HPA-axis, more negative affectivity, greater exposure to negative social support, and socialization processes that reinforce learned helplessness. It is important to emphasize that learned helplessness is, indeed, learned and not a function of biological sex. Preclinical research shows that female rodents are less likely than their male counterparts to show learned helplessness (154). Women may be vulnerable because of the types of traumatic events (e.g., rape) to which they are exposed (155). In contrast, men, for reasons not yet understood, are particularly vulnerable to in utero and early life adversity affecting brain maturation (156).

The combination of the relative specificity of diatheses and the complex interplay of sex and gender in our model may help to explain why certain disorders tend to aggregate together (internalizing vs. externalizing disorders) and to become, to some extent, sexually dimorphic in prevalence. We believe that this adds to the clinical relevance of our model.

Specificity in terms of high- vs. low-level diatheses for a disorder opens the possibility for the establishment of a hierarchy of diatheses, which may, in the future, help with the creation of reliable vulnerability models. This would serve the cause of prevention in persons at high risk of developing specific disorders. For example, such research could enable clinicians to target interventions at high-risk disaster survivors or military veterans to help prevent the development of PTSD.

Finally, it should be noted that the diathesis-stress model may itself be moderated by sex. The tendency for women to suffer more from trauma- and stress-related disorders and for men to suffer more from neurodevelopmental disorders suggests that the overall weight of stress on one hand and diathesis on the other, may shift according to sex. If it is true that males are particularly vulnerable to pre-natal or very early life stressors, whereas females show special vulnerability at times of hormonal flux, this may also help in structuring prevention programs. Another moderating effect on psychiatric symptoms may turn out to be coping style. Strategies that use emotional processing and emotional expression to manage adversity may engender protective social support, but may do so more effectively in one sex than the other. Cognitive therapies may gain in effectiveness if they take sex into account.

There are many ways in which both sex and gender affect mental health. It is our belief that, by systematically including analyses of sex and/or gender in research, and conducting moderation analyses of sex/gender when interpreting study results, we can improve our understanding of differences between men and women's expression of disorder, and make sense of previously inconsistent study results. Our recommendation is to include sex and gender in the design phase of clinical trials in order to ensure sufficient power to conduct meaningful analyses. However, even in the absence of a priori inclusion of sex/gender, consideration of how results may potentially be affected by sex/gender would represent an improvement in trial methodology.

Improving the meaningfulness of psychiatric research goes beyond detecting group differences between men and women. When differences are found, it is important to search for underlying causes and to search for heterogeneity within the same sex.

This paper does not constitute a systematic search of the vast literature on all aspects of sex/gender effects on mental illnesses. Instead, it proposes a model that we hope readers will critique and expand on. One day, we will have a model that more completely accounts for the accumulating knowledge in this area of research. Our hope is that researchers across disciplines will test the theories presented and examine the intersection of sex, gender, and mental illness, thus continuing to contribute new insights that will advance the prevention of and recovery from mental illnesses.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

DC, MM, and MS contributed to the article and approved of the final version. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Christiansen DM. Examining sex and gender differences in anxiety disorders. In: Durbano F, editor. A Fresh Look at Anxiety Disorders. Rijeka, Croatia: In Tech (2015). p. 17–49. doi: 10.5772/60662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Parker G, Brotchie H. Gender differences in depression. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2010) 22:429–36. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2010.492391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Schulz W, Muschalla B. What predicts internal and external mental disorders in adolescent boys and girls? Results from a 10-year longitudinal study. Eur J Develop Psychol. (2021) 19:234–50. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2021.189004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Otten D, Tibubos AN, Schomerus G, Brähler E, Binder H, Kruse J, et al. Similarities and differences of mental health in women and men: a systematic review of findings in three large German cohorts. Front Pub Health. (2021) 9:553071. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.553071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mackinaw-Koons B, Vasey MW. Considering sex differences in anxiety and its disorders across the life span: a construct-validation approach. Appl Prevent Psychol. (2000) 9:191–209. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(05)80004-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Eaton NR, Keyes KM, Krueger RF, Balsis S, Skodol AE, Markon KE, et al. An invariant dimensional liability model of gender differences in mental disorder: evidence from a national sample. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121:282–8. doi: 10.1037/a0024780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Christiansen DM. Trauma and gender in primary care. In: Legato MJ, editor. Principles of Gender-Specific Medicine. 4th ed. In press.

Google Scholar

8. Bekker MHJ, van Assen MALM. Autonomy-connectedness mediates sex differences in symptoms of psychopathology. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0181626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Schulte Holthausen B, Habel U. Sex differences in personality disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2018) 20:107. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0975-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Klein LC, Corwin EJ. Seeing the unexpected: how sex differences in stress responses may provide a new perspective on the manifestation of psychiatric disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2002) 4:441–8. doi: 10.1007/s11920-002-0072-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kistner JA. Sex differences in child and adolescent psychopathology: an introduction to the special section. J Clinical Child Adolesc Psychol. (2009) 38:453–9. doi: 10.1080/15374410902976387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hasin DS, Sarvet AL, Meyers JL, Saha TD, Ruan WJ, Stohl M, et al. Epidemiology of adult DSM-5 major depressive disorder and its specifiers in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:336–46. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kessler RC, Hudson JI, Shahly V, Kiejna A, Cardoso G. Bulimia nervosa and binge-eating disorder. In: Stein DJ, Scott KM, de Jonge P, Kessler RC, editors. Mental Disorders Around the World: Facts and Figures From the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2018). p. 263–85. doi: 10.1017/9781316336168.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Goldstein RB, Smith SM, Chou SP, Saha TD, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. The epidemiology of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2016) 51:1137–48. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1208-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ruscio AM, Hallion LS, Lim CCW, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Al-Hamzawi A, Alonso J, et al. Cross-sectional comparison of the epidemiology of DSM-5 generalized anxiety disorder across the globe. JAMA Psychiatry. (2017) 74:465–75. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, DiRienzo M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years. autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States 2016. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2020) 69:1–12. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Long J, Huang G, Liang W, Liang B, Chen Q, Xie J, et al. The prevalence of schizophrenia in mainland China: evidence from epidemiological surveys. Acta Psychiatrica Scand. (2014) 130:244–56. doi: 10.1111/acps.12296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Blanco C, Compton WM, Saha TD, Goldstein BI, Ruan WJ, Huang B, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 bipolar I disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions - III. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 84:3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, Ghandour RM, Holbrook JR, Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ. Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among U.S. children and adolescents 2016. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2018) 47:199–212. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry. (2015) 72:757–66. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Grant BF, Saha TD, Ruan WJ, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Jung J, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 drug use disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73:39–47. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Saha TD, Smith SM, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. The epidemiology of antisocial behavioral syndromes in adulthood: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. J Clin Psychiatry. (2017) 78:90–8. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m10358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Roest AM, de Vries YA, Lim CCW, Wittchen HU, Stein DJ, Adamowski T, et al. A comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV agoraphobia in the World Mental Health Surveys. Depress Anxiety. (2019) 36:499–5. doi: 10.1002/da.22885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mayne BT, Bianco-Miotto T, Buckberry S, Breen J, Clifton V, Shoubridge C, et al. Large scale gene expression meta-analysis reveals tissue-specific, sex-biased gene expression in humans. Front Genetics. (2016) 7:183. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2016.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smigielski L, Jagannath V, Rössler W, Walitza S, Grünblatt E. Epigenetic mechanisms in schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders: a systematic review of empirical human findings. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:1718–48. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0601-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Schultze-Lutter F, Schimmelmann BG, Fluckiger R, Michel C. Effects of age and sex on clinical high-risk for psychosis in the community. World J Psychiatry. (2020) 10:101–24. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v10.i5.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rosen M, Haidl TK, Ruhrmann S, Vogeley K, Schultze-Lutter F. Sex differences in symptomatology of psychosis-risk patients and in prediction of psychosis. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2020) 23:339–49. doi: 10.1007/s00737-019-01000-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Comacchio C, Lasalvia A, Bonetto C, Cristofalo D, Miglietta E, Petterlini S, et al. Gender and 5-years course of psychosis patients: focus on clinical and social variables. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2020) 23:63–70. doi: 10.1007/s00737-019-0945-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Seeman MV. Eating disorders and psychosis: seven hypotheses. World J Psychiatry. (2014) 4:112–9. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cullen AE, Holmes S, Pollak TA, Blackman G, Joyce DW, Kempton MJ, et al. Associations between non-neurological autoimmune disorders and psychosis: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. (2019) 85:35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ferri SL, Abel T, Brodkin ES. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2018) 20:9. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0874-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. (2002) 6:248–54. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Baron-Cohen S, Tsompanidis A, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard DM, Abdallah M, et al. Foetal oestrogens and autism. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:2970–8. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0454-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard DM, Abdallah MW, Melgaard L, et al. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism. Mol Psychiatry. (2015) 20:369–76. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gockley J, Willsey AJ, Dong S, Dougherty JD, Constantino JN, Sanders SJ. The female protective effect in autism spectrum disorder is not mediated by a single genetic locus. Mol Autism. (2015) 6:25. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0014-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Dean M, Harwood R, Kasari C. The art of camouflage: gender differences in the social behaviors of girls and boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2017) 21:678–89. doi: 10.1177/1362361316671845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Werling DM, Geschwind DH. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. (2013) 26:146–53. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rea HM, Øien RA, Shic F, Webb SJ, Ratto AB. Sex differences on the ADOS-2. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022). doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05566-3. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Paolacci S, Kiani AK, Manara E, Beccari T, Ceccarini MR, Stuppia L, et al. Genetic contributions to the etiology of anorexia nervosa: new perspectives in molecular diagnosis and treatment. Mol Genet Genom Med. (2020) 8:e1244–e. doi: 10.1002/mgg3.1244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hartung C, Lefler E. Sex and gender in psychopathology: DSM-5 and beyond. Psychol Bull. (2019) 145:390–409. doi: 10.1037/bul0000183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Polivy J, Herman CP. Causes of eating disorders. Ann Rev Psychol. (2002) 53:187213. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

42. Buss DM, Schmitt DP. Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychol Rev. (1993) 100:204–32. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

43. World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 2019. Available online at: https://icd.who.int/ (accessed May 25, 2022).

Google Scholar

44. David B, Payne L, Choate M, Barlow DH, Allen LB, Choate ML. Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders. Behav Ther. (2004) 35:205–30. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80036-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Breslau N, Kessler RC, Chilcoat HD, Schultz LR, Davis GC, Andreski P. Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1998) 55:626–32. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1995) 52:1048–60. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hapke U, Schumann A, Rumpf H-J, John U, Meyer C. Post-traumatic stress disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2006) 256:29906. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0654-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tolin DF, Foa EB. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychol Trauma. (2008) S:37–85. doi: 10.1037/1942-9681.S.1.37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ellegren H, Parsch J. The evolution of sex-biased genes and sex-biased gene expression. Nat Rev Genet. (2007) 8:689–98. doi: 10.1038/nrg2167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cerase A, Pintacuda G, Tattermusch A, Avner P. Xist localization and function: new insights from multiple levels. Genome Biol. (2015) 16:166. doi: 10.1186/s13059-015-0733-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Maschietto M, Bastos LC, Tahira AC, Bastos EP, Euclydes VL, Brentani A, et al. Sex differences in DNA methylation of the cord blood are related to sex-bias psychiatric diseases. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:44547. doi: 10.1038/srep44547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

52. Xia Y, Dai R, Wang K, Jiao C, Zhang C, Xu Y, et al. Sex-differential DNA methylation and associated regulation networks in human brain implicated in the sex-biased risks of psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:1. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0416-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Yilmaz Z, Hardaway JA, Bulik CM. Genetics and epigenetics of eating disorders. Adv Genomics Genet. (2015) 5:131–50. doi: 10.2147/AGG.S55776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Watson HJ, Yilmaz Z, Thornton LM, Hübel C, Coleman JRI, Gaspar HA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat Genet. (2019) 51:1207–14. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0439-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hübel C, Marzi SJ, Breen G, Bulik CM. Epigenetics in eating disorders: a systematic review. Mol Psychiatry. (2019) 24:901–15. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0254-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Yehuda R, Hoge CW, McFarlane AC, Vermetten E, Lanius RA, Nievergelt CM, et al. Post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2015) 1:15057. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Joel D, Berman Z, Tavor I, Wexler N, Gaber O, Stein Y, et al. Sex beyond the genitalia: the human brain mosaic. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:15468–73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509654112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hines M. Gender development and the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2011) 34:69–88. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Hines M, Ahmed SF, Hughes IA. Psychological outcomes and gender-related development in complete androgen insensitivity syndrome. Arch Sex Behav. (2003) 32:93–101. doi: 10.1023/a:1022492106974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Raznahan A, Lee Y, Stidd R, Long R, Greenstein D, Clasen L, et al. Longitudinally mapping the influence of sex and androgen signaling on the dynamics of human cortical maturation in adolescence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2010) 107:16988–93. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006025107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron. (2010) 107:16988–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. White JD, Kaffman A. The moderating effects of sex on consequences of childhood maltreatment: from clinical studies to animal models. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:1082. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. DiPietro JA, Voegtline KM. The gestational foundation of sex differences in development and vulnerability. Neurosci. (2017) 342:4–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Green PS, Simpkins JW. Neuroprotective effects of estrogens: potential mechanisms of action. Int J Develop Neurosci. (2000) 18:347–58. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5748(00)00017-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. McCarthy MM, Arnold AP, Ball GF, Blaustein JD, DeVries GJ. Sex differences in the brain: the not so inconvenient truth. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:2241–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5372-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Tooley UA, Bassett DS, Mackey AP. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2021) 22:372–84. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00457-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. McEwen BS, Milner TA. Understanding the broad influence of sex hormones and sex differences in the brain. J Neurosci Res. (2017) 95:24–39. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Tiwari A, Gonzalez A. Biological alterations affecting risk of adult psychopathology following childhood trauma: a review of sex differences. Clin Psychol Rev. (2018) 66:69–79. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biol Psychol. (2005) 69:113–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Reynolds RM, Hii HL, Pennell CE, McKeague IW, Kloet ERd, Lye S, et al. Analysis of baseline hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in late adolescence reveals gender specific sensitivity of the stress axis. Psychoneuroendocrinol. (2012) 38:1271–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Stam R. PTSD and stress sensitisation: a tale of brain and body. part 1: human studies. Neurosci Biobehavior Rev. (2007) 31:530–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Christiansen DM. Mediation and moderation effects of sex and gender in PTSD: PhD dissertation. Aarhus: Aarhus University, Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences (2017).

Google Scholar

73. Abo S, Smith D, Stadt M, Layton A. Modelling female physiology from head to Toe: impact of sex hormones, menstrual cycle, and pregnancy. J Theor Biol. (2022) 540:11 doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2022.111074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Meewisse ML, Reitsma JB, de Vries GJ, Gersons BP, Olff M. Cortisol and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2007) 191:387–92. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.024877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Khalil RB, Souaiby L, Fares N. The importance of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis as a therapeutic target in anorexia nervosa. Physiol Behav. (2017) 171:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.12.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Takahashi T, Iwasaki A. Sex differences in immune responses. Science. (2021) 371:347. doi: 10.1126/science.abe7199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Carlessi AS, Borba LA, Zugno AI, Quevedo J, Réus GZ. Gut microbiota–brain axis in depression: the role of neuroinflammation. Eur J Neurosci. (2021) 53:222–5. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. McCarthy MM. Sex differences in neuroimmunity as an inherent risk factor. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:38–44. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0138-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Huppertz, B. The feto–maternal interface: setting the stage for potential immune interactions. Semin Immunopathol. (2007) 29:83–94. doi: 10.1007/s00281-007-0070-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Nakagawa Y, Chiba K. Role of microglial m1/m2 polarization in relapse and remission of psychiatric disorders and diseases. Pharmaceuticals (Basel). (2014) 7:1028–48. doi: 10.3390/ph7121028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Breach MR, Lenz KM. Sex differences in neurodevelopmental disorders: a key role for the immune system. In: Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer (2022). doi: 10.1007/7854_2022_308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. McCarthy MM, Wright CL. Convergence of sex differences and the neuroimmune system in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 81:402–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Patterson PH. Immune involvement in schizophrenia and autism: etiology, pathology and animal models. Behav Brain Res. (2009) 204:313–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Goldstein JM, Cohen JE, Mareckova K, Holsen L, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gilman SE, et al. Impact of prenatal maternal cytokine exposure on sex differences in brain circuitry regulating stress in offspring 45 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2021) 118:e2014464118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2014464118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Hoffman JF, Wright CL, McCarthy MM. A critical period in Purkinje cell development is mediated by local estradiol synthesis, disrupted by inflammation and has enduring consequences only for males. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:10039–49. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1262-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Rizzetto L, Fava F, Tuohy KM, Selmi C. Connecting the immune system, systemic chronic inflammation and the gut microbiome: the role of sex. J Autoimmun. (2018) 92:12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jaut.2018.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Kim YS, Unno T, Kim B-Y, Park M-S. Sex differences in gut microbiota. World J Mens Health. (2020) 38:48–60. doi: 10.5534/wjmh.190009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

88. Margolis KG, Cryan JF, Mayer EA. The microbiota-gut-brain axis: from motility to mood. Gastroenterology. (2021) 160:1486–501. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2020.10.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Butler MJ, Perrini AA, Eckel LA. The role of the gut microbiome, immunity, and neuroinflammation in the pathophysiology of eating disorders. Nutrients. (2021) 13:500. doi: 10.3390/nu13020500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Hemmings SMJ, Malan-Müller S, van den Heuvel LL, Demmitt BA, Stanislawski MA, Smith DG, et al. The microbiome in posttraumatic stress disorder and trauma-exposed controls: an exploratory study. Psychosom Med. (2017) 79:936–46. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Vuong HE, Hsiao EY. Emerging roles for the gut microbiome in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 81:411–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Azcoitia I, Barreto GE, Garcia-Segura LM. Molecular mechanisms and cellular events involved in the neuroprotective actions of estradiol. analysis of sex differences. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2019) 55:100787. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2019.100787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2006) 7:477–84. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Grumbach MM. To an understanding of the biology of sex and gender differences: “an idea whose time has come. J Men's Health Gender. (2004) 1:12–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jmhg.2004.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Hardin SL, Thornton LM, Munn-Chernoff MA, Baker JH. Premenstrual symptoms as a marker of ovarian hormone sensitivity in eating disorders. Int J Eating Disord. (2020) 53:296–301. doi: 10.1002/eat.23213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Mikhail ME, Culbert KM, Sisk CL, Klump KL. Gonadal hormone contributions to individual differences in eating disorder risk. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2019) 32:484–90. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Steegers-Theunissen RPM, Wiegel RE, Jansen PW, Laven JSE, Sinclair KD. Polycystic ovary syndrome: a brain disorder characterized by eating problems originating during puberty and adolescence. Int J Mol Sci. (2020) 21:8211. doi: 10.3390/ijms21218211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Wilson HA, Creighton C, Scharfman H, Choleris E, MacLusky NJ. Endocrine insights into the pathophysiology of autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscientist. (2021) 27:650–67. doi: 10.1177/1073858420952046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Gogos A, Ney LJ, Seymour N, Van Rheenen TE, Felmingham KL. Sex differences in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorder: are gonadal hormones the link? Br J Pharmacol. (2019) 176:4119–35. doi: 10.1111/bph.14584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Nillni YI, Pineles SL, Patton SC, Rouse MH, Sawyer AT, Rasmusson AM. Menstrual cycle effects on psychological symptoms in women with PTSD. J Trauma Stress. (2015) 28:1–7. doi: 10.1002/jts.21984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Gillies GE, McArthur S. Estrogen actions in the brain and the basis for differential action in men and women: a case for sex-specific medicines. Pharmacol Rev. (2005) 14:609–16. doi: 10.1124/pr.109.002071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Thase ME, Entsuah R, Cantillon M, Kornstein SG. Relative antidepressant efficacy of venlafaxine and SSRIs: sex-age interactions. J Womens Health. (2005) 14:609–16. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2005.14.609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Zwebner Y, Sellier AL, Rosenfeld N, Goldenberg J, Mayo R. We look like our names: the manifestation of name stereotypes in facial appearance. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2017) 112:527–54. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Zuo B, Liu C, Wen F, Tan X, Xie Z. The impact of gender orientation of names on individuals' evaluation of impressions and interpersonal interaction. Acta Psychol Sinica. (2021) 53:387–99. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Keel PK, Forney KJ. Psychosocial risk factors for eating disorders. Int J Eating Disord. (2013) 46:433–9. doi: 10.1002/eat.22094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Mesman J, Groeneveld MG. Gendered parenting in early childhood: subtle but unmistakable if you know where to look. Child Develop Persp. (2018) 12:22–7. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Murray SB, Rieger E, Karlov L, Touyz SW. Masculinity and femininity in the divergence of male body image concerns. J Eating Disord. (2013) 1:11. doi: 10.1186/2050-2974-1-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Yean C, Benau E, Dakanalis A, Hormes J, Perone J, Timko A. The relationship of sex and sexual orientation to self-esteem, body shape satisfaction, and eating disorder symptomatology. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:887. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Bussey K, Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychol Rev. (1999) 106:676–713. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Rosenthal R. On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: the experimenter's hypothesis as unintended determinant of experimental results. Am Sci. (1963) 51:268–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

111. Vandello JA, Bosson JK. Hard won and easily lost: a review and synthesis of theory and research on precarious manhood. Psychol Men Masculinity. (2012) 14: doi: 10.1037/a0029826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Halim MLD, Walsh AS, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Zosuls KM, Ruble DN. The roles of self-socialization and parent socialization in toddlers' gender-typed appearance. Arch Sex Behav. (2018) 47:2277–85. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1263-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Solbes-Canales I, Valverde-Montesino S, Herranz-Hernández P. Socialization of gender stereotypes related to attributes and professions among young Spanish school-aged children. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:609. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Ptacek JT, Smith RE, Zanas J. Gender, appraisal, and coping: a longitudinal analysis. J Personality. (1992) 60:747–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00272.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Rosario M, Shinn M, Mørch H, Huckabee CB. Gender differences in coping and social supports: testing socialization and role constraint theories. J Community Psychol. (1988) 16:55–69. doi: 10.1002/1520-6629(198801)16:1<55::AID-JCOP2290160108>3.0.CO;2-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Sousa C, Kuschel K, Brito A, Gonçalves G. Work-oriented men and women: similar levels of work-family conflict and guilt yet different coping strategies. Psychol Thought. (2018) 11:195–211. doi: 10.23668/psycharchives.1885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. The Yogyakarta principles on the application of international human rights law in relation to sexual orientation gender identity. (2006). Available online at: https://yogyakartaprinciples.org/ (accessed May 25, 2022).

118. Chew D, Tollit MA, Poulakis Z, Zwickl S, Cheung AS, Pang KC. Youths with a nonbinary gender identity: a review of their sociodemographic and clinical profile. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. (2020) 4:322–30. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30403-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Mousavi MS, Shahriari M, Salehi M, Kohan S. Gender identity development in the shadow of socialization: a grounded theory approach. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2019) 22:245–51. doi: 10.1007/s00737-018-0888-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Christiansen DM, Berke ET. Gender- and sex-based contributors to sex differences in PTSD. Curr Psychiatr Rep. (2020) 224:19. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-1140-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2015) 54:11–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Reynolds CR. Need we measure anxiety differently for males and females? J Person Ass. (1998) 70:212–21. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa7002_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Norris FH, Perilla JL, Ibanez GE, Murphy AD. Sex differences in symptoms of posttraumatic stress: does culture play a role. J Trauma Stress. (2001) 14:7–28. doi: 10.1023/A:1007851413867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Christiansen DM, Elklit A. Sex Differences in PTSD. In: Lazinica A, Ovuga E, editors. InTech - Open Access Publisher (2012). doi: 10.5772/28363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Coreil J, Barnes-Josiah DL, Augustin A, Cayemittes M. Arrested pregnancy syndrome in Haiti: findings from a national survey. Med Anthropol Q. (1996) 10:424–36. doi: 10.1525/maq.1996.10.3.02a00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Farmer P. Bad blood, spoiled milk: bodily fluids as moral barometers in rural Haiti. Am Ethnologist. (1988) 15:62–83. doi: 10.1525/ae.1988.15.1.02a00050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Weaver LJ. Tension among women in North India: an idiom of distress and a cultural syndrome. Cult Med Psychiatry. (2017) 41:35–55. doi: 10.1007/s11013-016-9516-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Davis M, Emory E. Sex differences in neonatal stress reactivity. Child Develop. (1995) 66:14–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Christiansen DM. Sex and gender differences in trauma victims presenting for treatment. In: Legato MJ, editor. Principles of Gender-Specific Medicine: Academic Press (2017). p. 497–511. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-803506-1.00043-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Christiansen DM, Hansen M. Accounting for sex differences in PTSD: a multivariable mediation model. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2015) 6:26068. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.26068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. McKeever VM, Huff ME. A diathesis-stress model of posttraumatic stress disorder: ecological, biological, and residual stress pathways. Rev Gen Psychol. (2003) 7:237–50. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.7.3.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Werling DM, Parikshak NN, Geschwind DH. Gene expression in human brain implicates sexually dimorphic pathways in autism spectrum disorders. Nat Commun. (2016) 7:10717. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

133. Bölte S, Girdler S, Marschik PB. The contribution of environmental exposure to the etiology of autism spectrum disorder. Cell Mol Life Sci. (2019) 76:1275–97. doi: 10.1007/s00018-018-2988-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Fuld S. Autism spectrum disorder: the impact of stressful and traumatic life events and implications for clinical practice. Clin Soc Work J. (2018) 46:210–9. doi: 10.1007/s10615-018-0649-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Tasca GA. Attachment and eating disorders: a research update. Curr Opin Psychol. (2019) 25:59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Elwood LS, Hahn KS, Olatunji BO, Williams NL. Cognitive vulnerabilities to the development of PTSD: a review of four vulnerabilities and the proposal of an integrative vulnerability model. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:87–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Hyde JS, Mezulis AH, Abramson LY. The ABCs of depression: integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychol Rev. (2008) 115:291–313. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Parker GB, Brotchie HL. From diathesis to dimorphism: the biology of gender differences in depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2004) 192:210–6. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000116464.60500.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Day HLL, Stevenson CW. The neurobiological basis of sex differences in learned fear and its inhibition. Eur J Neurosci. (2019) 52:2466–86. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. McLean CP, Anderson ER. Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Barlow DH. Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:1247–63. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Kotler LA, Cohen P, Davies M, Pine DS, Walsh BT. Longitudinal relationships between childhood, adolescent, and adult eating disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2001) 40:1434–40. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200112000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. McKenzie K, Whitley R, Weich S. Social capital and mental health. Br J Psychiatry. (2018) 181:280–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.4.280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Christiansen DM, Olff M, Elklit A. Parents bereaved by infant death: sex differences and moderation in PTSD, attachment, coping and social support. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2014) 36:655–61. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2014.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Lincoln KD. Social support, negative social interactions, and psychological well-being. Soc Serv Rev. (2000) 74:231–52. doi: 10.1086/514478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Johnson VE, Nadal KL, Sissoko G, King R. “It's not in your head”: Gaslighting, ‘splaining, victim blaming, and other harmful reactions to microaggressions. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2021). 16:1024–36. doi: 10.1177/17456916211011963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Goldberg LR, Freyd JJ. Self-reports of potentially traumatic experiences in an adult community sample: gender differences and test-retest stabilities of the items in a brief betrayal trauma survey. J Trauma Dissoc. (2006) 7:39–63. doi: 10.1300/J229v07n03_04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Almeida DM, Kessler RC. Everyday stressors and gender differences in daily distress. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1998) 75:670–80. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Gove WR. Sex, marital status, and mortality. Am J Sociol. (1973) 79:45–67. doi: 10.1086/225505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Kamp Dush CM, Yavorsky JE, Schoppe-Sullivan SJ. What are men doing while women perform extra unpaid labor? Leisure and specialization at the transitions to parenthood. Sex Roles. (2018) 78:715–30. doi: 10.1007/s11199-017-0841-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Tang SSS, Freyd JJ. Betrayal trauma and gender differences in posttraumatic stress. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. (2012) 4:469–78. doi: 10.1037/a0025765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Valentine JD. Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. J Consul Clin Psychol. (2000) 68:748–66. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Kidd SA, Howison M, Pilling M, Ross LE, McKenzie K. Severe mental illness in LGBT populations: a scoping review. Psychiatr Serv. (2016) 67:779–83. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201500209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Dalla C, Edgecomb C, Whetstone AS, Shors TJ. Females do not express learned helplessness like males do. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2008) 33:1559–69. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Nothling J, Abrahams N, Jewkes R, Mhlongo A, Lombard C, Hemmings SMJ, et al. Risk and protective factors affecting the symptom trajectory of posttraumatic stress disorder post-rape. J Affect Disord. (2022) 309:151–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.04.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Poggi Davis E, Pfaff D. Sexually dimorphic responses to early adversity: implications for affective problems and autism spectrum disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 49:11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sex differences, gender differences, mental health, diathesis-stress model, posttraumatic stress disorder, autism spectrum disorders, eating disorders

Citation: Christiansen DM, McCarthy MM and Seeman MV (2022) Where Sex Meets Gender: How Sex and Gender Come Together to Cause Sex Differences in Mental Illness. Front. Psychiatry 13:856436. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.856436

Received: 17 January 2022; Accepted: 02 June 2022;
Published: 28 June 2022.

Edited by:

Richard Adams, Kent State University, United States

Reviewed by:

William Davies, Cardiff University, United Kingdom
Kathleen Brady, MUSC Health, United States

Copyright © 2022 Christiansen, McCarthy and Seeman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dorte M. Christiansen, dochristiansen@health.sdu.dk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.