Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 July 2022
Sec. Psychopharmacology

Propofol Versus Sevoflurane General Anaesthesia for Selective Impairment of Attention Networks After Gynaecological Surgery in Middle-Aged Women: A Randomised Controlled Trial

  • 1Department of Anaesthesiology, The First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
  • 2Department of Anaesthesiology, Fourth Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China

Purpose: Attention is an essential component of cognitive function that may be impaired after surgery with anaesthesia. Propofol intravenous anaesthesia and sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia are frequently used in gynaecological surgery. However, which type of anaesthetic has fewer cognitive effects postoperatively remains unclear. We compared the differences in attention network impairment after surgery in women receiving propofol versus sevoflurane general anaesthesia.

Patients and Methods: Eighty-three patients with gynaecological diseases who were 40–60 years of age were involved in the study. All patients underwent elective gynaecological surgery under either total intravenous anaesthesia or sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia, depending on randomisation. The efficiencies of the three attention networks were captured using the attention network test preoperatively and on the 1st and 5th postoperative days.

Results: Both groups of patients showed differences in impairments on the 1st and 5th postoperative days. Pairwise comparisons indicated that the alerting and orienting networks of patients in the propofol group were impaired to a greater extent than those of patients in the sevoflurane group on the 1st postoperative day, while the executive control network was impaired to a lesser extent. On the 5th postoperative day, the alerting networks of both groups recovered to the baseline level. Patients in the propofol group still showed impairment of the orienting network, while patients in the sevoflurane group recovered to baseline. For the executive control network, patients in the sevoflurane group still exhibited more severe impairment than those in the propofol group.

Conclusion: In middle-aged women, propofol impaired orienting and alerting networks more than sevoflurane, while sevoflurane showed more residual impairment of the executive control network.

Highlights

- We studied postoperative cognitive impairment in middle-aged women after different types of anaesthesia.

- Sevoflurane inhalation and propofol intravenous anaesthesia induced different degrees of impairment and recovery in regard to the three aspects of the attention network.

- We speculate that sevoflurane and propofol act on different regions of the brain and thus speculate that they act on the main target areas of the brain.

Introduction

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) is a common complication of the central nervous system after surgery and anaesthesia (1) that can affect patients in different age groups, with incidences ranging from 7 to 80%, especially in women (25). POCD can lead to increased complications and mortality, delayed rehabilitation, prolonged hospital stay, and poor quality of life of patients after discharge. Moreover, the 5-year mortality rate caused by POCD is estimated to be 70% (6) which places a heavy medical burden on patients and society.

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction shows various cognitive deficits, including a decline in memory, attention, and psychomotor speed (7). Attention is the general term of various psychological phenomena, which represents the ability to choose from stimuli, reactions, memories and thoughts and ignores any irrelevant information in the process (8). It is generally believed that patients with cognitive disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and schizophrenia have attention deficits (9, 10). According to cognitive neuroscience models of attention, there are three attention networks: alerting, orienting and executive control (11). These networks are responsible for different functions in attention process (12) and the efficiency of these three networks can be captured using attention network testing (ANT), which was designed by Fan et al. (13). ANT measures the activities of all three networks at the same time and evaluates their relationship based on the flanking and exogenous clue paradigm. Given the facts above, we can use ANT to study the relationship between anaesthesia and POCD by detecting the attention network.

Although the aetiology and pathogenesis of POCD are uncertain and multifactorial (7), a causative link to general anaesthesia is increasingly recognised (1, 4). At present, most patients need general anaesthesia during surgery, and many studies have shown that general anaesthesia can lead to a decline in the postoperative cognitive function of patients (2, 14). Because anaesthetic choice can be effectively managed by anaesthesiologists, it is important to determine the role of anaesthetic choice in POCD. Propofol and sevoflurane are common general anaesthetics used in clinical anaesthesia, and animal experiments have also shown that propofol and sevoflurane can cause the occurrence of POCD (15, 16). The choice of anaesthetic may affect cognitive outcome after the operation, but the results from clinical research have always been contradictory. For example, Zhang and others showed that sevoflurane inhalation anaesthesia may have a higher risk of POCD than propofol intravenous anaesthesia (17). Schoen and others showed that sevoflurane anaesthesia was associated with better short-term cognitive function after surgery than propofol anaesthesia (18). It is still unclear which type of general anaesthetic has less effect on cognitive function (19). Moreover, whether intravenous and inhaled anaesthetics are associated with different risks of attention network impairment has not been reported.

Gender differences in cognitive functioning have been consistently reported in some cognitive tasks, with varying effect sizes (20). For example, men perform better on spatial memory, while women excel at verbal memory (21). Women are more likely to show mental and nervous system diseases with cognitive impairment, such as depression and AD (21, 22).

The purpose of this study was to compare the types of cognitive changes and differences in attention network impairment among women receiving propofol intravenous anaesthesia versus sevoflurane inhalation anaesthesia and to observe the recovery trajectories of attention network impairment.

Materials and Methods

Trial Registry and Ethical Approval

This trial was approved by the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR-1900024311). Each patient provided written informed consent before participating in the study, and all procedures were conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was approved by the Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University in Hefei, Anhui, China, in July 2013 (Ethics Committee No. 20130406, Chairperson, Professor Run-ling Wang).

Participants and Exclusion Criteria

A total of 120 patients aged 40–60 with gynaecological diseases were involved in the study. All patients underwent elective gynaecological surgery (hysterectomy) with either propofol intravenous anaesthesia (PROPOFOL) or sevoflurane (SEVO) inhalational anaesthesia at the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University from July 2019 to December 2019. The exclusion criteria were as follows: an ASA status of III or IV; history of respiratory or circulatory system diseases; history of neurological and psychiatric disorders; a Mini-Mental State Examination score ≤23; inadequate postoperative analgesia with a visual analogue scale (VAS) ≥ 3; or unwillingness to comply with or difficulty in understanding the protocol or procedures at any time during the trial.

Randomisation

A total of 120 patients were randomly divided into the PROPOFOL group and the SEVO group at a 1:1 ratio by a computer programme. The patients and research staff who observed and followed up with the patients after surgery were blinded to the group assignment.

Study Timeline

Baseline characteristics, including age, education level, weight, preoperative Mini-Mental State Examination score, and underlying diseases were assessed and recorded 1 day preoperatively. Duration of surgery and Sufentanil dosage were recorded during surgery. All participants completed the ANT test 1 day preoperatively and on the 1st and 5th days postoperatively. The different types of changes in recovery trajectories related to attention networks were compared between those receiving PROPOFOL versus SEVO anaesthesia.

Anaesthesia

After arrival in the operating room, all patients underwent intravenous access without any premedication and commenced receiving 4 l/min oxygen through a face mask. Patient monitoring included heart rate, pulse oximetry, ECG, end-tidal concentrations of carbon dioxide, non-invasive blood pressure and bispectral indices (BIS).

For the PROPOFOL group, after intravenous preloading, all patients received propofol (1.5–2.5 mg/kg), sufentanil (0.3–0.5 μg/kg), and cis-atracurium besylate (0.2 mg/kg) during anaesthesia induction. A laryngeal mask airway was inserted 3–5 min after induction. To maintain the appropriate depth of anaesthesia, propofol (1.5–4 μg/ml) and remifentanil (0.1–0.4 μg/kg/min) were given by continuous intravenous infusion, and cisatracurium besylate was used intermittently.

For the SEVO group, all patients were given 8% sevoflurane [fresh gas flow (FGF) 6 l/min], sufentanil (0.3–0.5 μg/kg), and cisatracurium besylate (0.2 mg/kg) when induced, and then sevoflurane was adjusted to 3–4% (FGF 1–2 l/min) after loss of consciousness. After endotracheal intubation, sevoflurane was kept between 0.7 and 1.5 minimum alveolar concentration (MAC). Remifentanil (0.1–0.4 μg/kg/min, continuous intravenous infusion) and cisatracurium besylate were used for analgesia and muscle relaxation, respectively.

Bispectral indices was monitored and set to the target range of 40–60 in both groups by adjusting either the flow of sevoflurane or the target blood concentration of propofol.

Complications were managed according to the study protocol of our department. An intravenous bolus of 6 mg ephedrine was used for hypotension (systolic blood pressure < 90 mmHg or 20% less than baseline); an intravenous bolus of 0.3 mg atropine was used for bradycardia (heart rate < 50 beats/min).

At the end of surgery, patient-controlled intravenous analgesia was administered with flurbiprofen (1 mg/ml) and sufentanil (0.02–0.03 μg/kg/ml), which was set at a background infusion rate of 2 ml/h, and a pump set at 0.5-ml boluses with a locking time of 15 min for effective postoperative analgesia. The VAS score was used to assess postoperative pain after surgery.

ANT Procedure

We used E-Prime (version 1.1; Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, United States) to conduct the attention network test, as we did in our previous research. The test began with an instruction that stressed the importance of rapid and accurate responses and that was followed by 24 practice trials for patients to become familiar with the test. The patients viewed the stimuli on a computer, and accuracy and reaction times were calculated and recorded when they pressed two response buttons (the “←” key and the “→” key).

The ANT can be divided into the following procedures. First, the participants saw a fixation cross (“+”) in the central area that did not disappear until the trials were terminated. Then, at approximately 400–1,600 ms after the appearance of the fixation cross, the patient noticed a cue (“*”) lasting for 100 ms. After 500 ms, the participants observed a target above or below the fixation cross that remained on the screen until they gave a response or until 1,700 ms later. At the end of the trial, the fixation cross disappeared. Each trial lasted approximately 4,000 ms.

Four kinds of cue conditions existed in this test: (i) no cue, in which the participants did not receive any clue; (ii) a centre cue, in which the participants caught sight of an asterisk in the middle of the screen; (iii) double cues, in which the participants saw two asterisks concurrently above and below the fixation across; and (iv) a spatial cue, in which the participants saw an asterisk either above or below the target. The test demonstrated three types of target stimuli. The first type was “neutral”, in which a single arrow appeared above or below the fixation cross. The second type was “congruent,” in which five arrows emerged in the same direction concurrently with the middle arrow serving as the target. The third type was “incongruent,” in which five arrows were presented simultaneously above or below the fixation cross with the central target arrow in the opposite direction from the other four arrows (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of the attention network test. This schematic shows the experimental procedure, the four cue conditions, and the three types of target stimuli.

As stated above, in this experiment, there were four cues (no cue, centre cue, double cues, and spatial cue), three targets (neutral, congruent, and incongruent) and three sessions (baseline, 1 day postoperative, and 5 days postoperative). Our study mainly explored the efficiencies of three attention networks, as evaluated by the extent to which the cues affected the response times (RTs), as previously described (13, 23). Specifically, the first attention network was the alerting network, with an effect score equal to the no cue RT minus the double cues RT; the higher the score was, the stronger the alerting function. The second attention network was the orienting network, with an effect score equivalent to the centre cue RT minus the spatial cue RT; the higher the score was, the better the orienting function. The last attention network was the executive control network, in which the effect score stemmed from the incongruent target RT minus the congruent target RT; the lower the score was, the more efficient the execution function.

Statistical Analyses

Demographics, education level, Mini-Mental State Examination scores, ASA class and intraoperative data, which included the operation duration and drug dose, were compared between the two groups. The ANT was administered at baseline and on days 1 and 5 postoperatively.

All analyses were conducted using SPSS software [SPSS software (version 23.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States)]. All quantitative data were tested for normality by using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Normally distributed variables were summarised using the mean and standard deviation and compared using a t test for demographic data and repeated-measures analysis of variance (RMANOVA) for attention network efficiency. The non-parametric data were analysed with the Mann–Whitney U test. All categorical data were tested using Pearson’s chi-squared test or Fisher’s exact test. A p value < 0.05 (two sided) was considered to indicate statistical significance.

Results

A total of 120 patients were screened for the study, among whom 19 declined to participate in the ANT, 12 were lost due to early discharge, and 6 had a VAS score above 3 after analgesia. The remaining 83 patients (43 patients in the propofol intravenous anaesthesia group and 40 patients in the sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia group) participated in and completed the study. Subjects in the propofol intravenous anaesthesia and sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia groups had similar baseline characteristics, including age, education level, weight, preoperative Mini-Mental State Examination score, duration of surgery, and underlying diseases (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and baseline characteristics of patients.

Accuracy

In this study, accuracy refers to the percentage of correct responses in all attention networks, including the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. The accuracy rates were similar across the testing time points and between the PROPOFOL and SEVO groups (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Accuracy of patients (x ± s).

Efficiencies of the Three Networks

Significant differences in the effect scores of the three attentional networks were observed between the two groups and at all three time points using repeated-measures ANOVA.

In the propofol group, patients exhibited significant impairments in all three attentional networks on the 1st postoperative day. The alerting network efficiency recovered to the baseline level, while the orienting network exhibited a delayed recovery on the 5th postoperative day, and no recovery of the executive control network was observed. In the sevoflurane group, significant impairments in all three attentional networks were observed on the 1st postoperative day. Total recoveries were the same for both the alerting and orienting networks on the 5th postoperative day, and the executive control network was still impaired on the 5th postoperative day.

Pairwise comparisons indicated that compared to the alerting and orienting networks of patients in the sevoflurane group on the 1st postoperative day, those of patients in the propofol group were impaired to a greater extent, while the executive control network was impaired to a lesser extent. On the 5th postoperative day, the alerting networks of both groups recovered to the same level. Patients in the propofol group still showed impairment of the orienting network, while patients in the sevoflurane group recovered to baseline. For the executive control network, patients in the sevoflurane group still exhibited more severe impairment than those in the propofol group. (Table 3 and Figure 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Differences at baseline and the 1st and 5th OP sessions between the alerting, orienting and executive networks.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Efficiency of the three attention networks in the propofol and sevoflurane groups. *p < 0.05 versus baseline values; #p < 0.05 versus 1st postoperative day; &p < 0.05 versus the propofol group.

Discussion

Sevoflurane and propofol are frequently used general anaesthetics in clinical practice, and previous studies have shown that anaesthetic agents could be beneficial, deleterious or neutral to the central nervous system (2427), which indicates that anaesthetics may affect cognitive outcomes after surgery depending on the type of anaesthetic. To understand the effects of different anaesthetics on the three distinct attentional networks, we captured the efficiency of three attention networks in middle-aged women before and after hysterectomy surgery under sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia or propofol intravenous to explore the impact of different types of anaesthesia on cognitive performance. Significant differences were found in the three attentional networks on the 1st and 5th postoperative days. Compared with patients who received sevoflurane inhalational anaesthesia, those who received propofol intravenous anaesthesia exhibited significantly lower efficiency on the alerting network task and the orienting network task but had fewer difficulties in resolving conflict. The impairment on attention networks after propofol intravenous anaesthesia and the recovery strategies of them were consistent with our previous research (28). ANT was selected as the object of this study because of its sensitivity and applicability for determining slight changes in attention deficits (29). Although we only observed early cognitive dysfunction in this study, we found that the function of the executive control network was significantly impaired and difficult to recover after surgery and anaesthesia. However, the difference in impairment types of the attention network between propofol intravenous anaesthesia and sevoflurane inhalation anaesthesia needs further study.

Propofol intravenous anaesthesia and sevoflurane inhalation anaesthesia are both commonly used in gynaecological surgery, but it is not clear which type of anaesthetic has less influence on cognitive function. A previous study showed that the use of different types of anaesthetics had no significant effect on cognitive function (30). Anaesthetic agents might even have neuroprotective effects against various neural injuries in certain circumstances (17). In contrast, the results of animal studies indicate that conventional standard doses of anaesthetics may lead to long-term learning and memory impairment (31, 32). How general anaesthesia drugs act on the central nervous system, and its pharmacological mechanism is not completely clear, but the current mainstream view holds that the mechanisms of cognitive impairment induced by general anaesthesia may include tau hyperphosphorylation (33), caspase-3 activation (34), β-amyloid deposition (35), increased activity of γ-aminobutyric acid type a receptor in the brain (36) and changes in the central cholinergic transmission of nicotine and muscarinic receptors (37). These processes have all been proven to be directly associated with the development of AD, and a final common pathway may underlie the pathogenesis of AD and PND. The findings of this study point to the phenomenon that propofol and sevoflurane can lead to postoperative cognitive decline.

Although propofol and sevoflurane can both cause postoperative cognitive impairment, the patterns of attention network impairment are different according to our study. The possible reasons may be as follows.

Distinct cognitive and behavioural processes may be regulated by different regions of the brain, and lesions in these areas result in characteristic clinical deficits (38). Formal neuroimaging and functional imaging studies can be used to infer the specific areas of the brain impaired by anaesthesia and surgery. According to previous analyses, the alerting network is responsible for maintaining alertness and warning of stimulation (39), and it is related to the frontal and parietal cortices of the right hemisphere and involves cortical projections of the norepinephrine system (40). The orienting network is responsible for substantial information selection between sensory inputs, affecting the ability to transfer attention cues, and it is related to the superior colliculus, parts of the superior and inferior parietal lobules, frontal eye fields and the temporal parietal junction while also involving the acetylcholine system (41). The executive control network is responsible for cognitive and emotional self-regulation; it is related to frontal areas, including the anterior cingulate cortex, basal ganglia, and lateral prefrontal cortex and is modulated by dopamine (42, 43).

In recent years, with the continuous improvement and development of neuroimaging technology, we have a new understanding of the mechanism of general anaesthesia. At present, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) are the most widely used in anaesthesia research. According to the changes in local cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and local cerebral glucose metabolism rate (rCMRglu) during anaesthesia, dynamic functional imaging was performed on brain groups to capture the action targets of anaesthetics in the central nervous system. The effects of propofol and sevoflurane on rCBF and rCMRglu are obviously different. Previous studies of propofol found rCBF reductions in the frontal cortex, posterior cingulate, frontal orbital gyrus cortex and cuneiform lobe (44, 45), and studies on the cerebral metabolic rate using positron emission tomography have shown that in the propofol anaesthetised state, the order of the CMR in each brain region from lowest to highest was parietal lobe < occipital lobe < temporal lobe < frontal lobe (46). Previous imaging studies showed that sevoflurane anaesthesia caused a global reduction in rCBF, but the rCBF of the cuneiform lobe, anterior cuneiform lobe and posterior limbic system decreased most obviously (47).

By studying the different changes in the relative cerebral glucose metabolism levels induced by propofol and sevoflurane, it can be inferred that the regions suppressed more by sevoflurane than propofol are mostly distributed in parts of the insula, limbic system, thalamus, pons and cerebellum. In contrast, the regions suppressed more by propofol are mostly distributed in the neocortex of the temporal, parietal and frontal lobes (48). Our results showed that propofol anestheisa had more impairment on alerting and orienting network than sevoflurane anaesthesia while sevoflurane showed more residual impairment of the executive control network which reflected similar patterns with those neuroimaging and functional imaging researches above.

Differences in neurotransmitters may be another reason. Neurotransmitters are actively involved in various brain functions, including movement, attention, emotion, learning, and memory (40, 49). At present, there are many studies on the effects of propofol and sevoflurane on neurotransmitters. Animal experiments have shown that propofol is a GABAA receptor that mainly acts on chloride channel coupling in the brain and has an anaesthetic effect by promoting the release of the inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) (50, 51). GABAA receptors are also expressed in the locus coeruleus, and GABA neurons in the locus coeruleus have synaptic connections with NE neurons, which can decrease norepinephrine (NE) levels (52). Numerous studies have confirmed that the cholinergic system is an important target of many general anaesthetics. Wang et al. showed that propofol significantly inhibits the release of acetylcholine (53), and Hvarfber et al. showed that propofol exerts anaesthetic effects by inhibiting the reuptake of neurotransmitters such as GABA and dopamine (DA) in central synapses (54). Sevoflurane also has many biochemical actions, and it has been shown to inhibit the function of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in the postsynaptic membrane and significantly reduce the transmission of acetylcholine between synapses (55). Nishikawa et al. found that sevoflurane enhanced GABAA receptor-mediated synaptic inhibition in hippocampal interneurons (56), and Silva JH et al. found that sevoflurane increased the levels of extracellular [3H] dopamine in rat brain cortical slices (57). The effects of propofol and sevoflurane on neurotransmitters in the central nervous system are related to the neural mechanism of the attention network, which can reduce the efficiency of the attention network.

In addition to the above reasons, researches about coupling functions (5860) using EEG (Electroencephalogram) enabled us to unveil a new perspective on how the neurophysiological mechanisms are affected by general anaesthesia. For example, Stankovski et al. found that both propofol and sevofluane increased the alpha–gamma coupling (which plays a prominent role in attention processing (61)) during anaesthesia, but the strength and effect were significantly stronger for sevoflurane (60).

There are also several limitations of the present study. First, this predefined analysis was performed using data from women recruited in only one centre. The generalisability of our results can legitimately be questioned. Second, we lack research on biomarkers in cerebrospinal fluid or serum closely related to cognitive function changes, such as interleukin-6 (IL-6), tumour necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) or S100 calcium binding protein β (S100β). Third, the long-term effects of propofol intravenous anaesthesia and sevoflurane inhalation anaesthesia on cognitive function have also not been studied. Fourth, we did not use neuroimaging examination to confirm the localisation of brain regions with impaired attention networks, such as PET, fMRI and voxel-based morphometry (VBM). The current study provides impetus for further research examining the effects of various anaesthetic agents on postoperative cognitive outcomes.

Conclusion

Different general anaesthetics can cause different degrees of attention network impairment after surgery. Propofol caused more serious impairment to the patients’ alerting network and orienting network, while sevoflurane caused more serious impairment to the patients’ executive control network. The impairment of the executive control network after the two kinds of anaesthesia could not be restored to the preoperative level in a short time.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University in Hefei, Anhui, China, in July 2013. (Ethics Committee No. 20130406, Chairperson, Professor Run-ling Wang), and informed consent was obtained from all individual participants. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CC: conceptualization, methodology, software, and writing—reviewing and editing. YW: conceptualization, writing—reviewing and editing. JR and WT: validation and software. WW: formal analysis. YL: writing—reviewing and editing. GX: conceptualization, investigation, and software. WZ: funding acquisition, writing—reviewing and editing, and conceptualization. All authors contributed to the article, take responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China, No. 82001185.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Wei Ye and other colleagues from the Department of Gynaecology for their assistance with this study. This article was edited for proper English language, grammar, punctuation, spelling, and overall style by Alyson Mack at AJE (www.aje.com).

References

1. Tang N, Ou C, Liu Y, Zuo Y, Bai Y. Effect of inhalational anaesthetic on postoperative cognitive dysfunction following radical rectal resection in elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment. J Int Med Res. (2014) 42:1252–61. doi: 10.1177/0300060514549781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Moller JT, Cluitmans P, Rasmussen LS, Houx P, Rasmussen H, Canet J, et al. Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly. ISPOCD1 study: ISPOCD investigators-international study of postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Lancet. (1998) 351:857–61. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(97)07382-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Canet J, Raeder J, Rasmussen LS, Enlund M, Kuipers HM, Hanning CD, et al. Cognitive dysfunction after minor surgery in the elderly. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. (2003) 47:1204–10. doi: 10.1046/j.1399-6576.2003.00238.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rasmussen LS, Johnson T, Kuipers HM, Kristensen D, Siersma VD, Vila P, et al. Does anaesthesia cause postoperative cognitive dysfunction? A randomized study of regional versus general anaesthesia in 438 elderly patients. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. (2003) 47:260–6. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2003.00057.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rasmussen LS, Larsen K, Houx P, Skovgaard LT, Hanning CD, Moller JT, et al. The assessment of postoperative cognitive function. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. (2001) 45:275–89. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2001.045003275.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Yildizeli B, Ozyurtkan MO, Batirel HF, Kuşcu K, Bekiroǧlu N, Yüksel M. Factors associated with postoperative delirium after thoracic surgery. Ann Thorac Surg. (2005) 79:1004–9. doi: 10.1016/j.athoracsur.2004.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Pappa M, Theodosiadis N, Tsounis A, Sarafis P. Pathogenesis and treatment of postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Electron Physician. (2017) 9:3768–75. doi: 10.19082/3768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Raz A. Anatomy of attentional networks. Anat Rec B New Anat. (2004) 281:21–36. doi: 10.1002/ar.b.20035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wang K, Fan J, Dong Y, Wang CQ, Lee TM, Posner MI. Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2005) 78:235–41. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fernandez-Duque D, Black SE. Attentional networks in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology. (2006) 20:133–43. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1990) 13:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ishigami Y, Fisk JD, Wojtowicz M, Klein RM. Repeated measurement of the attention components of patients with multiple sclerosis using the attention network test-interaction (ANT-I): stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. J Neurosci Methods. (2013) 216:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. (2002) 14:340–7. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Evered LA, Silbert BS. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction and noncardiac surgery. Anesth Analg. (2018) 127:496–505.

Google Scholar

15. Wang Y, An X, Zhang X, Liu J, Wang J, Yang Z. Lithium chloride ameliorates cognition dysfunction induced by sevoflurane anaesthesia in rats. FEBS Open Bio. (2020) 10:251–8. doi: 10.1002/2211-5463.12779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Huang C, Ng OT, Ho YS, Irwin MG, Chang RC, Wong GT. Effect of continuous propofol infusion in rat on tau phosphorylation with or without temperature control. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 51:213–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zhang Y, Shan GJ, Zhang YX, Cao SJ, Zhu SN, Li HJ, et al. Propofol compared with sevoflurane general anaesthesia is associated with decreased delayed neurocognitive recovery in older adults. Br J Anaesth. (2018) 121:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.05.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Schoen J, Husemann L, Tiemeyer C, Lueloh A, Sedemund-Adib B, Berger KU, et al. Cognitive function after sevoflurane– vs. propofol-based anaesthesia for on-pump cardiac surgery: a randomized controlled trial. Br J Anaesth. (2011) 106:840-50. doi: 10.1093/bja/aer091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Miller D, Lewis SR, Pritchard MW, Schofield-Robinson OJ, Shelton CL, Alderson P, et al. Intravenous versus inhalational maintenance of anaesthesia for postoperative cognitive outcomes in elderly people undergoing noncardiac surgery. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2018) 8:CD012317. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012317.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gurvich C, Thomas N, Kulkarni J. Sex differences in cognition and ageing and the influence of sex hormones. Handb Clin Neurol. (2020) 175:103–15.

Google Scholar

21. Li R, Singh M. Sex differences in cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2014) 35:385–403. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Bangasser DA, Eck SR, Ordoñes Sanchez E. Sex differences in stress reactivity in arousal and attention systems. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:129–39. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0137-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Fang D, Jiang J, Sun X, Wang W, Dong N, Fu X, et al. Attention dysfunction of postoperative patients with glioma. World J Surg Oncol. (2014) 12:317. doi: 10.1186/1477-7819-12-317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Deng J, Lei C, Chen Y, Fang Z, Yang Q, Zhang H, et al. Neuroprotective gases–fantasy or reality for clinical use? Prog Neurobiol. (2014) 115:210–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Berger M, Nadler JW, Friedman A, McDonagh DL, Bennett ER, Cooter M, et al. The effect of propofol versus isoflurane anaesthesia on human cerebrospinal fluid markers of Alzheimer’s disease: results of a randomized trial. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 52:1299–310. doi: 10.3233/JAD-151190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Berger M, Ponnusamy V, Greene N, Cooter M, Nadler JW, Friedman A, et al. The effect of propofol vs. Isoflurane anaesthesia on postoperative changes in cerebrospinal fluid cytokine levels: results from a randomized trial. Front Immunol. (2017) 8:1528. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2017.01528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Nie Y, Li S, Yan T, Ma Y, Ni C, Wang H, et al. Propofol attenuates isoflurane-induced neurotoxicity and cognitive impairment in foetal and offspring mice. Anesth Analg. (2020) 131:1616–25. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Chen C, Xu GH, Li YH, Tang WX, Wang K. Selective impairment of attention networks during propofol anaesthesia after gynaecological surgery in middle-aged women. J Neurol Sci. (2016) 363:126–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2016.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Daniels S, Blundell M, Anyane-Yeboa K, Karayiorgou M. Networks of attention in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol. (2004) 26:611–26. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ancelin ML, de Roquefeuil G, Scali J, Bonnel F, Adam JF, Cheminal JC, et al. Long-term postoperative cognitive decline in the elderly: the effects of anaesthesia type, apolipoprotein E genotype, and clinical antecedents. J Alzheimers Dis. (2010) 22(Suppl. 3):105–13. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wan Y, Xu J, Ma D, Zeng Y, Cibelli M, Maze M. Postoperative impairment of cognitive function in rats: a possible role for cytokine-mediated inflammation in the hippocampus. Anaesthesiology. (2007) 106:436–43. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200703000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bianchi SL, Tran T, Liu C, Lin S, Li Y, Keller JM, et al. Brain and behaviour changes in 12-month-old Tg2576 and nontransgenic mice exposed to anaesthetics. Neurobiol Ageing. (2008) 29:1002–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Planel E, Bretteville A, Liu L, Virag L, Du AL, Yu WH, et al. Acceleration and persistence of neurofibrillary pathology in a mouse model of tauopathy following anaesthesia. FASEB J. (2009) 23:2595–604. doi: 10.1096/fj.08-122424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Zhang G, Dong Y, Zhang B, Ichinose F, Wu X, Culley DJ, et al. Isoflurane-induced caspase-3 activation is dependent on cytosolic calcium and can be attenuated by memantine. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:4551–60. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5694-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Xie Z, McAuliffe S, Swain CA, Ward SA, Crosby CA, Zheng H, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid aβ to tau ratio and postoperative cognitive change. Ann Surg. (2013) 258:364–9. doi: 10.1097/SLA.0b013e318298b077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Zurek AA, Yu J, Wang DS, Haffey SC, Bridgwater EM, Penna A, et al. Sustained increase in α5GABAA receptor function impairs memory after anaesthesia. J Clin Invest. (2014) 124:5437–41.

Google Scholar

37. Zhang X, Jiang X, Huang L, Tian W, Chen X, Gu X, et al. Central cholinergic system mediates working memory deficit induced by anaesthesia/surgery in adult mice. Brain Behav. (2018) 8:e00957. doi: 10.1002/brb3.957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Henri-Bhargava A, Stuss DT, Freedman M. Clinical assessment of prefrontal lobe functions. Continuum (Minneap Minn). (2018) 24:704–26.

Google Scholar

39. Fan J, Fossella J, Sommer T, Wu Y, Posner MI. Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2003) 100:7406–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0732088100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Coull JT, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM. A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia. (1996) 34:1085–95. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00029-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Thiel CM, Zilles K, Fink GR. Nicotine modulates reorienting of visuospatial attention and neural activity in human parietal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2005) 30:810–20.

Google Scholar

42. Kamigaki T. Prefrontal circuit organization for executive control. Neurosci Res. (2019) 140:23–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2018.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. (2019) 23:213–34. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Pedersen JL, Lillesø J, Hammer NA, Werner MU, Holte K, Lacouture PG, et al. Thiopental and propofol affect different regions of the brain at similar pharmacologic effects. Anesth Analg. (2004) 99:912–8. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000131971.92180.DF

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T, Plourde G, Meuret P, Bonhomme V, et al. Brain mechanisms of propofol-induced loss of consciousness in humans: a positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci. (1999) 19:5506–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-13-05506.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ, Shah NK, Wu JC, Kao YJ. Cerebral metabolism during propofol anaesthesia in humans studied with positron emission tomography. Anaesthesiology. (1995) 82:393–403;discussion27A.

Google Scholar

47. Kaisti KK, Metsähonkala L, Teräs M, Oikonen V, Aalto S, Jääskeläinen S, et al. Effects of surgical levels of propofol and sevoflurane anaesthesia on cerebral blood flow in healthy subjects studied with positron emission tomography. Anaesthesiology. (2002) 96:1358–70. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200206000-00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Jeong YB, Kim JS, Jeong SM, Park JW, Choi IC. Comparison of the effects of sevoflurane and propofol anaesthesia on regional cerebral glucose metabolism in humans using positron emission tomography. J Int Med Res. (2006) 34:374–84. doi: 10.1177/147323000603400406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Chen Y, Xu J, Chen Y. Regulation of neurotransmitters by the gut microbiota and effects on cognition in neurological disorders. Nutrients. (2021) 13:2099.

Google Scholar

50. Wang B, Lv K, Liu H, Su Y, Wang H, Wang S, et al. Contribution of the α5 GABAA receptor to the discriminative stimulus effects of propofol in rat. Neuroreport. (2018) 29:347–52. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Nitz D, Siegel JM. GABA release in the locus coeruleus as a function of sleep/wake state. Neuroscience. (1997) 78:795–801. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00549-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Tamiya R, Inoue K, Takagi H. GABA-ergic and substance P-ergic double-innervation to noradrenergic neurons in the rat locus coeruleus. Osaka City Med J. (1994) 40:1–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

53. Wang Y, Kikuchi T, Sakai M, Wu JL, Sato K, Okumura F. Age-related modifications of effects of ketamine and propofol on rat hippocampal acetylcholine release studied by in vivo brain microdialysis. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. (2000) 44:112–7. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2000.440120.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hvarfner A, Hammas B, Thörn SE, Wattwil M. The influence of propofol on vomiting induced by apomorphine. Anesth Analg. (1995) 80:967–9. doi: 10.1097/00000539-199505000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Naruo H, Onizuka S, Prince D, Takasaki M, Syed NI. Sevoflurane blocks cholinergic synaptic transmission postsynaptically but does not affect short-term potentiation. Anaesthesiology. (2005) 102:920–8. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200505000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Nishikawa K, MacIver MB. Agent-selective effects of volatile anaesthetics on GABAA receptor-mediated synaptic inhibition in hippocampal interneurons. Anaesthesiology. (2001) 94:340–7. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200102000-00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Silva JH, Gomez RS, Diniz PH, Gomez MV, Guatimosim C. The effect of sevoflurane on the release of [3H]dopamine from rat brain cortical slices. Brain Res Bull. (2007) 72:309–14. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Lee U, Ku S, Noh G, Baek S, Choi B, Mashour GA. Disruption of frontal-parietal communication by ketamine, propofol, and sevoflurane. Anesthesiology. (2013) 118:1264–75. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31829103f5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Purdon PL, Pavone KJ, Akeju O, Smith AC, Sampson AL, Lee J. The Ageing Brain: age-dependent changes in the electroencephalogram during propofol and sevoflurane general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth. (2015) 115:i46–57. doi: 10.1093/bja/aev213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Stankovski T, Petkoski S, Raeder J, Smith AF, McClintock PV, Stefanovska A. Alterations in the coupling functions between cortical and cardio-respiratory oscillations due to anaesthesia with propofol and sevoflurane. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci. (2016) 374:20150186. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2015.0186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ward LM. Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn Sci. (2003) 7:553–9.

Google Scholar

Keywords: attention network, propofol, sevoflurane, postoperative cognitive dysfunction, postoperative neurocognitive disorder

Citation: Chen C, Wang Y, Rao J, Tang W, Wu W, Li Y, Xu G and Zhong W (2022) Propofol Versus Sevoflurane General Anaesthesia for Selective Impairment of Attention Networks After Gynaecological Surgery in Middle-Aged Women: A Randomised Controlled Trial. Front. Psychiatry 13:917766. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.917766

Received: 11 April 2022; Accepted: 14 June 2022;
Published: 14 July 2022.

Edited by:

Julian Roessler, Cleveland Clinic, United States

Reviewed by:

Tomislav Stankovski, Saints Cyril and Methodius University of Skopje, North Macedonia
Jamie Sleigh, The University of Auckland, New Zealand

Copyright © 2022 Chen, Wang, Rao, Tang, Wu, Li, Xu and Zhong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Guanghong Xu, xuguanghong2004@163.com; Weiwei Zhong, zww.0511@hotmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.