Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 19 May 2023
Sec. Social Neuroscience

Performance in emotion recognition and theory of mind tasks in social anxiety and generalized anxiety disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Sandra Baez
Sandra Baez1*María Alejandra TangarifeMaría Alejandra Tangarife1Gabriela Davila-MejiaGabriela Davila-Mejia1Martha Trujillo-GüizaMartha Trujillo-Güiza2Diego A. ForeroDiego A. Forero3
  • 1Universidad de los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 2Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 3School of Health and Sport Sciences, Fundación Universitaria del Área Andina, Bogotá, Colombia

Social cognition impairments may be associated with poor functional outcomes, symptoms, and disability in social anxiety disorder (SAD) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). This meta-analysis aims to determine if emotion recognition and theory of mind (ToM) are impaired in SAD or GAD compared to healthy controls. A systematic review was conducted in electronic databases (PubMed, PsycNet, and Web of Science) to retrieve studies assessing emotion recognition and/or ToM in patients with SAD or GAD, compared to healthy controls, up to March 2022. Meta-analyses using random-effects models were conducted. We identified 21 eligible studies: 13 reported emotion recognition and 10 ToM outcomes, with 585 SAD patients, 178 GAD patients, and 753 controls. Compared to controls, patients with SAD exhibited impairments in emotion recognition (SMD = −0.32, CI = −0.47 – −0.16, z = −3.97, p < 0.0001) and ToM (SMD = −0.44, CI = −0.83 –0.04, z = −2.18, p < 0.01). Results for GAD were inconclusive due to the limited number of studies meeting the inclusion criteria (two for each domain). Relevant demographic and clinical variables (age, sex, education level, and anxiety scores) were not significantly correlated with emotion recognition or ToM impairments in SAD and GAD. Further studies employing ecological measures with larger and homogenous samples are needed to better delineate the factors influencing social cognition outcomes in both SAD and GAD.

1. Introduction

Anxiety disorders are considered ones of the most disabling psychiatric disorders, ranking among the top 25 leading causes of burden of disease worldwide (1). People with anxiety disorders show remarkable functional impairments (2, 3) and decreased quality of life (4, 5). Social cognition impairments are a significant and common feature associated with poor functional outcomes in anxiety disorders (69) and are believed to contribute to both symptoms and disability (7, 8). Emotion recognition and theory of mind (ToM) are two social cognition domains critical to successful social and interpersonal functioning. These two domains are especially relevant for anxiety disorders characterized by social impairment, such as generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and social anxiety disorder (SAD, also known as social phobia).

GAD is a chronic and disabling disorder characterized by excessive, uncontrollable worry, and anticipatory anxiety, which often results in severe cognitive, occupational, and social dysfunction (3, 5). Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterized by a persistent, excessive fear, and avoidance of social and performance situations. Both disorders are associated with severe occupational dysfunction and marked social and interpersonal impairments (4, 6). Given the severity and chronicity of social and interpersonal impairments associated with GAD and SAD, it is relevant to understand the underlying social-cognitive mechanisms. However, to date, these mechanisms are not well understood due to the heterogeneity of symptoms in anxiety disorders and the heterogeneity in studies’ methodologies.

Emotion recognition and ToM are two social cognition domains crucial to successful interpersonal interactions, which share conceptual and neuroanatomical overlaps (10), and have suggested to be impaired in GAD and SAD (1114). However, results regarding these two domains in patients with GAD or SAD are inconclusive and meta-analytic evidence on these social cognition processes is scarce.

Some studies in the literature addressing emotion recognition in GAD or SAD demonstrate impairments (1114), while others find no significant differences between patients and healthy controls (6, 1521). The same is true for ToM results, with studies showing lower accuracies in patients than in healthy controls (20, 2225), no significant differences between groups (6, 26), or even overmentalizing in patients with SAD (27, 28). Besides the uneven results regarding emotion recognition and ToM abilities in patients with GAD or SAD, meta-analytic evidence on these social cognition processes which may underlie interpersonal impairments in these two anxiety disorders is scarce.

Only one meta-analysis (7) has addressed the social cognition performance of adults with anxiety disorders, including both GAD and SAD. The results showed that patients with SAD or GAD exhibited attributional biases, and that other social cognition domains (including emotion recognition and ToM) were not affected. Notably, this meta-analysis included only two studies on emotion recognition in GAD and identified a gap in knowledge for ToM. Thus, there is no recent meta-analytic evidence on performance on emotion recognition and ToM tasks in adults with SAD or GAD, and no meta-analysis has examined ToM abilities in adults with GAD.

Considering these antecedents, the purposes of this study are (1) to sum up and update what is known from the existing literature about emotion recognition and ToM abilities of adults with GAD and SAD, and (2) to determine, through a meta-analysis, whether these abilities are impaired in GAD and SAD adults, compared to healthy controls. We also tested whether emotion recognition and ToM performance in GAD and SAD are associated with relevant variables such as the severity of anxiety, sex, age, and years of formal education.

2. Methods

The presentation of this systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA 2020) guidelines (29). We did not register our protocol in online databases. However, review methods such as the databases used, search terms, and inclusion and exclusion criteria, were established a priori and there were not deviations from them.

2.1. Systematic review

We conducted a systematic literature review to describe and examine the characteristics of studies assessing emotion recognition and ToM in adults with GAD or SAD, compared with healthy controls.

2.2. Search and study selection

We searched three databases (PubMed, PsycNet, and Web of Science) (30, 31) to identify eligible studies up to February 2022 (last updated on 17 March 2022). The combination of keywords employed for the search and selection process is shown in Figures 1, 2. We conducted two independent searches, one for GAD and another for SAD. Searches were limited to English-language publications and human participants. Titles and abstracts were independently screened by two investigators (MAT and GD), and disagreements were resolved by discussion and requesting a third author’s opinion whenever needed (SB). Full texts of articles were retrieved and read by two authors (MAT and GD) and those meeting all the eligibility criteria were included.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) flowchart displaying study screening and selection process for studies in generalized anxiety disorder.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flowchart displaying study screening and selection process for studies in social anxiety disorder.

Eligibility criteria for study inclusion were as follows: (1) the study must include a group of patients diagnosed with GAD or SAD, according to the DSM-IV or DSM-V criteria, (2) the study included a healthy control group without a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, (3) the samples were adults (above 18 years old), (4) study used data from a performance-based measure of emotion recognition or ToM, (5) article published in English, (6) studies involving human participants, and (7) published in peer-reviewed journals. There was no restriction on the country of origin and the year of publication.

Exclusion criteria were: (1) single case study, (2) non-clinical outcome study (i.e., studies validating social cognition measures), (3) participants without a clinical diagnosis of GAD or SAD, (4) treatments or interventions with no measurement of social cognition at baseline, (5) population-based studies or clinical trials, (6) review, meta-analysis, theoretical, or opinion articles, (7) studies that did not provide adequate data to calculate mean and standard deviations of emotion recognition or ToM tasks (authors were contacted to obtain the data where this was not provided in the manuscript), (8) study without a healthy control group, (9) study reporting questionnaires or self-report measures of social cognition, (10) study with animal models, and (11) study with children or adolescent samples. We excluded studies reporting self-report questionnaires of social cognition because they do not provide emotion recognition or ToM accuracy scores, which constituted our primary outcome measures.

2.3. Data extraction

Data of interest were extracted independently by two authors (MAT, GD), and checked by a third one (SB). The following variables were extracted from eligible studies: demographic data of the participants, clinical scales (anxiety and depression measures), and emotion recognition and ToM outcome measures. When available, participant characteristics were also extracted, including the number of participants in each group, age at assessment, sex, and years of formal education. In most cases, data extracted included means and standard deviations or group means of the outcome measures of social cognition. Tables 1, 2 summarize the different emotion recognition and ToM tasks used. The overall accuracy scores in these tests were considered the primary outcome measures. If results at multiple time points were reported, the baseline data were extracted. We contacted the corresponding authors to obtain required data that was not reported. Studies were excluded if required data was not obtained after at least two attempts.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the studies included for emotion recognition tests.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the studies included for TOM tests.

2.4. Statistical analyses

Meta-analyses were conducted using OpenMeta [Analyst] software (32). First, we conducted meta-analyses (one for each domain) between the anxiety disorder groups (for both SAD and GAD patients) and healthy controls (HC) using random-effects models (DerSimonian-Lard estimate; significance at p < 0.05). The effect estimate was adjusted to standardized mean differences, depending on the magnitude of variation across studies (33). Heterogeneity was analyzed using the I2 statistics and the Cochrane’s Q (34). The I2 metric is independent of the number of studies and can be compared across meta-analyses with different numbers of studies and metrics (34, 35). For I2, a low heterogeneity corresponds to values between 0% and 25%; medium between 25% and 50%, and values greater than 50% indicate considerable heterogeneity (36). Cochrane’s Q is a non-parametric test that verifies if the differences between patients and controls are consistent for all evaluated studies. Because Cochrane’s Q is sensitive to the number of studies included, it may be underpowered for samples <20 (34, 37).

Second, we performed additional analyses for both domains excluding studies reporting GAD samples to determine their effect on the overall results on emotion recognition and ToM performances. It is important to note that some studies used more than one test of ToM, fulfilling inclusion criteria. We only included one result in the analyses to prevent reporting inconsistencies.

Third, for ToM analyses, given that the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (RMET) and the Movie Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC) were the most used measures across studies, we compared the performance of GAD and SAD patients vs. healthy controls in these tasks. Finally, we repeated these analyses for SAD patients vs. healthy controls.

2.4.1. Meta-regression analyses

We conducted meta-regression analyses (38) to explore the relationship between relevant demographic and clinical variables with the primary emotion recognition and ToM outcomes. We included the following covariates: sex (represented as the percentage of females), the average age, the average years of education, and the rescaled anxiety scores. Since outcome measures varied in anxiety scales across studies, to aid interpretability, we rescaled all outcomes on a 0 to 100 scale, with the minimum scores represented as 0 and the highest as 100. This rescaling did not change the results; it was purely to allow for greater interpretability. Indeed, rescaling clinical outcomes on a 0 to 100 scale to aid interpretability is a standard procedure previously used in meta-analyses (39, 40) and clinical studies using scales with different ranges (41, 42).

2.5. Risk of bias assessment

To assess the risk of bias of the studies included in the meta-analysis, we used the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) (43). The NOS evaluates the quality of non-randomized observational studies and has been utilized in several published systematic reviews and meta-analyses (44). This scale implemented a star system in which each study can receive up to a maximum of nine stars if all criteria have been satisfied in three categories: selection, comparability, and exposure or outcome. Higher scores indicate better quality. We used a cut-off value of ≥7 to define low risk of bias (45, 46).

2.6. Publication bias

We used funnel plots (47) and Egger’s test (48) to assess publication biases.

3. Results

3.1. Study selection

The initial search of databases for GAD yielded 97 studies. After the removal of duplicates (n = 44), 53 studies were screened. We excluded 45 studies, and eight met all inclusion criteria. Four of them were included in the systematic review and meta-analysis, the remaining were excluded because they did not report complete statistical data, and this was not provided by the corresponding authors. The PRISMA flow diagram in Figure 1 provides an overview of the study selection process for GAD.

For SAD, the initial search of databases yielded 1,030 studies. After the removal of duplicates (n = 729), 302 studies were screened. We excluded 274 studies, and 26 met all inclusion criteria. Twenty-one of them were included in the systematic review and meta-analysis, the remaining were excluded due to incomplete statistical data (n = 3) or lack of an ordinal measure of emotion recognition or ToM (n = 5). Incomplete statistical data was not provided by the corresponding authors. The PRISMA flow diagram in Figure 2 provides an overview of the study selection process for SAD.

Finally, 21 studies (GAD = 4 and SAD = 17) were included in this meta-analysis.

3.2. Study characteristics

The characteristics of included studies are shown in Tables 1, 2. As can be seen from these tables, 11 studies measured emotion recognition, 10 assessed ToM, and 2 included measures of both domains. For both domains, emotion recognition and ToM, most studies assessed patients with SAD. The final sample consisted of 178 patients diagnosed with GAD (mean age = 32.3 years, SD = 9.28, 79.2% female, education level = 14.4 years, SD = 2.7), 585 with SAD (mean age = 28.3 years, SD = 7.2, 51.1% female, education level = 14.3 years, SD = 2.5), and 753 healthy controls (mean age = 27.7 years, SD = 6.1, 57.5% female, education level = 14.5 years, SD = 2.0).

It is worth noting that, for the studies assessing emotion recognition in SAD patients, almost half of them (45.5%) included individuals with comorbid psychiatric disorders. The most common comorbidities were major depressive disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, panic disorder, and obsessive–compulsive disorder. The two studies assessing emotion recognition abilities in GAD did not include patients with comorbid disorders.

For studies assessing ToM in GAD or SAD, most of them (60%) included patients with comorbid psychiatric diagnoses. The most common comorbidity was major depressive disorder.

Regarding the emotion recognition and ToM outcome measures, it is relevant to highlight that different measurements were used by authors when examining the same construct (as shown in Tables 1, 2). On emotion recognition, all included studies assessed basic emotion recognition. Most of them included measures of recognition of static facial stimuli, fewer studies included dynamic stimuli such as video clips, and one of them included the assessment of emotions in prosody. On ToM, the reviewed studies used the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (RMET), the Movie Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC), or the Faux Pas Test (FPT). The most used ToM measure was the RMET (80% of the studies).

3.3. Meta-analytic results

3.3.1. Emotion recognition

The meta-analysis including studies assessing emotion recognition in both patients diagnosed with GAD or SAD (k = 13) revealed that patients showed worse performance than healthy controls (SMD = −0.32, CI = −0.47 – 0.17, z = −4.17, p < 0.0001) (Figure 3A). No significant evidence for heterogeneity was found (Q = 10.22, df = 13, p = 0.60, I2 = 0%). It is worth mentioning that, despite the significant results, only two studies comparing SAD patients and healthy controls (13, 20) reported significantly worse performance in patients’ overall scores. Considering that the study by Oh et al. (13) was the one with the largest sample of patients and controls, and the higher weight in the results, we conducted a sensitivity analysis with the leave-one-out method. Results of this analysis showed that no single study was responsible for the pooled result of the meta-analysis (See Supplementary Figure 1).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall emotion recognition differences between (A) SAD and GAD patients and healthy controls (B) only SAD patients and healthy controls.

When we compared only patients with SAD and healthy controls (k = 11), we also found significant differences between groups (SMD = −0.32, CI = −0.47 – −0.16, z = −3.97, p < 0.0001) (Figure 3B), and no significant evidence for heterogeneity (Q = 10.05, df = 10, p = 0.43, I2 = 1%). We did not compare patients with GAD and healthy controls since only two studies met inclusion criteria. These two studies (16, 17) did not report significant differences between patients and controls in overall emotion recognition performance.

3.3.2. ToM

The meta-analysis including studies assessing ToM in patients diagnosed with GAD or SAD together (k = 10) showed significant differences between groups, with patients showing lower scores than healthy controls (SMD = −0.38, CI = −0.71 – 0.005, z = −2.280, p < 0.01) (Figure 4A). Significant and substantial heterogeneity was observed (Q = 46.12, df = 9, p < 0.001, I2 = 81%). It is worth noting that only two studies (6, 26) showed no significant differences between GAD or SAD patients and healthy controls in ToM abilities using the RMET. Seven studies (20, 2225, 28, 49) reported significantly lower performance in patients, three of them used the RMET, three used the RMET and the MASC, and one, the RMET and the FPT. Only one study, using the FPT, found that SAD patients exhibited higher accuracy than the control group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall theory of mind differences between (A) SAD and GAD patients and healthy controls (B) only SAD patients and healthy controls.

When we compared only patients with SAD and healthy controls (k = 8), we also found significant differences between groups (SMD = −0.44, CI = −0.83 – 0.04, z = −2.18, p < 0.01) (Figure 4B), and substantial heterogeneity (Q = 38.27, df = 7, p < 0.01, I2 = 81.7%). We did not compare patients with GAD and healthy controls, given only two studies met inclusion criteria. These studies showed mixed results. One of them (26) found no significant differences between GAD patients and healthy controls in the RMET or the MASC overall performances. The other one (22) reported significant lower performance in GAD patients in the RMET total score.

Given that the RMET and the MASC were the most used measures of ToM across included studies, we also compared the performance of GAD and SAD patients vs. healthy controls in these tasks (Figure 5). For the RMET (k = 8), results revealed that patients performed significantly worse than controls (SMD = −0.426, CI = −0.68 – −0.16, z = −3.20, p < 0.01). We still found significant heterogeneity, but less than the one observed when comparing studies with different ToM tasks (Q = 18.52, df = 7, p < 0.01, I2 = 62.2%) (Figure 5A). For the MASC (k = 4), we did not find significant differences between patients and controls (SMD = −0.38, CI = −0.90 –0.13, z = −1.47, p = 0.14), and there was significant and substantial heterogeneity (Q = 15.40, df = 3, p < 0.01, I2 = 81%) (Figure 5B).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall theory of mind differences between SAD and GAD patients and healthy controls in (A) the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (RMET), and (B) the Movie Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC).

When we analyzed the performance of SAD patients compared to healthy controls (k = 6), we also found that patients performed significantly worse in the RMET (SMD = −0.52, CI = −0.74 – −0.30, z = −4.55, p < 0.01), and no significant evidence for heterogeneity was found (Q = 8.97, df = 5, p = 0.26, I2 = 23.2%) (Figure 6A). For the MASC (k = 3), we also found significantly worse performance in SAD patients than controls (SMD = −0.59, CI = −1.07 – −0.11, z = −2.39, p = 0.02). However, considerable heterogeneity was found (Q = 5.07, df = 2, p = 0.08, I2 = 61%) (Figure 6B).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall theory of mind differences between only SAD patients and healthy controls in (A) the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (RMET), and (B) the Movie Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC).

We did not compare the performance of patients with GAD and healthy controls in the RMET and the MASC due to the low number of studies meeting inclusion criteria.

3.4. Meta-regression analyses

3.4.1. Emotion recognition

We conducted meta-regression analyses to explore the relationship between relevant variables and emotion recognition performance in GAD and SAD patients. Results showed that emotion recognition abilities were not significantly influenced by age (k = 13, z = 0.58, p = 0.55, R2 = 0.00), sex (k = 13, z = 0.35, p = 0.72, R2 = 0.00), years of education (k = 9, z = −1.09, p = 0.27, R2 = 0.00), or severity of anxiety (k = 10, z = −0.06, p = 0.94, R2 = 0.00).

3.4.2. ToM

We also performed meta-regression analyses to explore the relationship of relevant variables with ToM performance in GAD and SAD patients. Results showed that ToM performance was not significantly dependent on age (k = 10, z = −0.65, p = 0.51, R2 = 0.00), sex (k = 10, z = 1.04, p = 0.29, R2 = 1.36), years of education (k = 5, z = −1.15, p = 0.24, R2 = 0.00), or severity of anxiety (k = 8, z = 0.53, p = 0.59, R2 = 0.00).

3.5. Risk of bias assessment

The quality assessment results are shown in Supplementary Table 1. The mean score was 7.80 (SD = 0.82), 20 of the 21 included studies were awarded ≥7 stars and considered to be of low risk of bias.

3.6. Publication bias

We used funnel plots and Egger’s test to assess publication bias. For emotion recognition, funnel plot was symmetric and Egger’s test non-significant (intercept = −0.17; p = 0.69), suggesting no publication bias for this set of studies (see Supplementary Figure 2). For ToM, visual inspection of the funnel plot showed some asymmetry suggesting the presence of small studies effect (see Supplementary Figure 3). However, the Egger’s test was not significant (intercept = 0.75, p = 0.16), indicating insufficient evidence for publication bias.

4. Discussion

This is the first meta-analysis of emotion recognition and ToM abilities in adults with anxiety disorders, including both GAD and SAD. Results of the 21 studies included in this meta-analysis showed that compared to healthy controls, patients with SAD exhibited impairments in emotion recognition and ToM. Results for GAD were not conclusive and should be interpreted with caution, given that these social cognition domains have been less studied in these patients, with only four studies fulfilling the inclusion criteria (two for each domain). In addition, meta-regression analyses indicated that relevant demographic and clinical variables (age, sex, education level, and anxiety scores) were not associated with emotion recognition or ToM impairments of GAD and SAD patients.

4.1. Emotion recognition

The meta-analysis of studies assessing emotion recognition in patients diagnosed with GAD or SAD showed that patients exhibited significantly worse performance than healthy controls. It is worth mentioning that, despite the significance of this meta-analysis, only two studies comparing SAD patients and healthy controls (13, 20) reported significantly worse performance in patients’ overall scores. Considering that the study from Oh et al. (13) was the one with the largest sample of patients, and the highest weight in the results, we conducted a sensitivity analysis with the leave-one-out method. The results were very similar to the original ones, indicating that the study by Oh et al. (13) neither other single study was responsible for the pooled result of this meta-analysis. Thus, although the study by Oh et al. (13) was the one with the most precise estimates, the significant effects shown in the current meta-analysis seem to be guided by the fact that all studies, except for the one by Lundh and Öst (18), showed a lower mean performance in patients compared to controls. We also conducted the analysis for studies assessing patients with SAD only, and the results remained similar to the original ones.

It is striking to notice the diversity of tests used to assess the recognition of emotions in the included studies. Some studies used static visual facial stimuli; in some studies, the intensity of emotions varied; in other articles, the facial expressions showed the same emotion intensity; and some used moving images for the assessment. In addition, methods of presenting photographs varied concerning the total number of photographs, the time presented, and so on. Although these differences may have affected the results and data interpretation, these methodological differences did not seem to represent a source of heterogeneity.

It is noteworthy that, from the 13 included studies, only two assessed patients with GAD (16, 17). These two studies reported no significant differences between patients and healthy controls in overall emotion recognition scores, suggesting that this domain seems to be preserved in patients with GAD. However, it is essential to highlight that the same authors have conducted some studies including small sample sizes (15 and 21, respectively) of patients without psychiatric comorbidities and used a task in which participants were instructed to match the target facial expression to one of two faces. These factors could have an impact the results. Thus, research on emotion recognition abilities in patients with GAD is still lacking and should be further investigated in future studies with larger samples and more ecologically valid and demanding tasks.

Our results are not consistent with those of the only previous meta-analysis assessing emotion recognition impairments in adults with anxiety disorders (7). Some methodological factors may partially explain these differences. First, only one study (50) was included in both meta-analyses. Although we contacted the respective authors, we could not obtain the data for the remaining six studies analyzed by Plana et al. (7). Second, our work includes more recent evidence since only four of the 21 included studies were published before 2014. Thus, the samples analyzed here and in the previous meta-analysis notably differ, which probably explains the uneven results.

Although most of the studies included in the current meta-analysis did not provide the data to analyze positive and negative emotions recognition independently, it is remarkable that two studies did not found significant differences between SAD patients and healthy controls in overall scores (11, 14), but reported significant impairments in particular emotions. Specifically, Maoz et al. (2016) reported that SAD participants judged a significantly higher proportion of faces as angry, and Tseng et al. (14) found that SAD participants exhibited significantly lower accuracy in recognizing facial and prosodic emotions of fear. These results are consistent with those of a previous review (51) showing that facial emotion processing in SAD is influenced by a negative bias, that is, affected individuals tend to manifest peculiar processing of pictures with negative facial displays of emotion. Thus, the emotional valence of facial stimuli is one factor to be considered in SAD assessment. The lack of available data to further explore the influence of this factor, highlights the importance of addressing the recognition of particular emotions in future meta-analyses.

We also explored the relationship between emotion recognition in GAD and SAD patients and age, sex, years of education, and anxiety levels. Meta-regression results showed that emotion recognition impairments did not depend on any of these variables. As data regarding years of education was not reported by several studies, result on this meta-regression should be considered with caution. Similarly, data on anxiety levels was not included in all studies and there were differences regarding the instruments used to assess this variable. However, one of the reviewed studies (52) reported a significant association between symptom severity of social anxiety and functional connectivity between brain regions involved in perception of fearful faces (amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex). Thus, the potential relationship between anxiety severity and emotion recognition impairment should be further investigated in patients with GAD and SAD.

4.2. Theory of mind

This is the first meta-analysis addressing ToM in adults diagnosed with GAD or SAD. The analysis comparing both groups, GAD and SAD, and healthy controls revealed that patients showed significantly lower performance in ToM tasks. These results were similar when we analyzed only the studies assessing SAD patients. However, in both cases, significant and substantial heterogeneity was observed.

Considering that the diversity of tests used to assess ToM may be one of the possible sources of heterogeneity, we conducted the analyses independently for studies that used the RMET or the MASC. When we compared both groups of patients and controls, we found that patients performed significantly worse than controls in the RMET, but there were no significant differences in the MASC total scores. For both analyses, we still found significant heterogeneity, but less than the one observed when comparing studies using different ToM tasks. This suggests significant heterogeneity seems to be associated with both the diversity of ToM tasks and the differences between SAD and GAD samples. Interestingly, when we analyzed only the SAD group compared to healthy controls, we found that patients showed significantly lower performance in both tasks, the RMET and the MASC. For studies using the RMET, there was low and non-significant heterogeneity. However, for studies using the MASC, heterogeneity was considerable. These differences in heterogeneity in the results of studies using RMET and MASC may be explained by the low number of studies employing the second measure (k = 3) and the heterogeneity in the samples assessed in these three studies. Two of them included patients with SAD and comorbid psychiatric conditions (53, 54) and the other one (55) assessed patients with SAD without comorbidities but with a very small the sample size (n = 12). Considering these limitations of previous studies, further investigation is needed on ToM abilities assessing more representative and homogenous samples of SAD patients.

Overall, our results suggest that ToM impairments are present in patients with SAD, regardless of the tasks employed in the assessment of this domain (RMET or MASC). Thus, ToM deficits in SAD seem to include more basic decoding abilities to detect emotion, such as the ones measured by the RMET (e.g., visual emotion detection using only the eyes), and the more complex affective and cognitive abilities to understand others’ emotions, intentions, and behaviors from multiple channels of data (i.e., video unfolding with auditory, visual, and interactive facial and body movement), such as the ones assessed by the MASC. Given that most of the studies included in the current meta-analysis did not provide the data to analyze affective and cognitive ToM abilities independently, differences in these domains should be addressed in future studies.

For GAD patients, results are not conclusive. One study using the RMET showed impairments (22), and the other one, using the RMET and the MASC, failed to find significant differences between patients and healthy controls in any task (26). Further studies are needed to determine whether the ToM abilities are impaired in patients with GAD.

Meta-regression results showed that age, years of education, or anxiety levels were not significantly correlated with ToM performance in GAD and SAD patients. However, the influence of education and anxiety severity to ToM should be considered with caution, given that these data were not provided by all included studies. In particular, the specific relationship between symptom severity and performance in the RMET was assessed by one of the included studies showing no significant associations in patients with GAD (56). Further studies are needed to understand the influence of demographic and severity of symptoms on ToM abilities in patents with SAD and GAD.

4.3. Limitations

Some of the most significant methodological limitations of research in this area are the limited number of studies and the small sample sizes available for entry into the meta-analysis, especially for GAD. Also, many of the studies fulfilling the inclusion criteria did not report complete statistical data. While efforts were made, these studies were not included despite multiple attempts to contact their authors. There was also substantial variability in measurement tools used to assess emotion recognition and ToM. In particular, for emotion recognition, different studies adopted different instruments to assess the same construct. Although all of them assessed basic emotions recognition and the majority used facial stimuli, the type of stimuli and the methods of presenting stimuli varied among them. These differences may have affected the current results.

In addition, one factor that might influence the current results is the frequent presence of comorbidities between the different disorders and in the same patient. Almost half of the studies’ samples included in this meta-analysis comprised patients with comorbid psychiatric disorders (1315, 20, 2224, 26, 52). Although lifetime comorbidity in patients with anxiety disorders occurs in more than 80% (57), variations in the composition of the samples across studies result in a relevant disadvantage for between studies comparisons, highlighting the need for future research assessing social cognition in more homogeneous samples GAD and SAD patients. Although it is expected that the presence of comorbid disorders would be associated with deeper impairments in emotion recognition and ToM, this has not been studied in GAD or SAD patients. Future research should further explore the impact of comorbid disorders on social cognition abilities of GAD and SAD patients.

Additionally, we investigated some variables (age, sex, education level, and severity of anxiety) that may affect emotion recognition or ToM performances (5860). However, other factors (such as the presence of comorbidities, prior substance abuse, and executive functioning) were not examined. Given that most studies did not report data on these variables, future research should address the relationship between said factors and emotion recognition and ToM abilities of patients with SAD and GAD. Finally, only studies published in English were included in this review, which may result in limited generalizability of results.

4.4. Implications and future directions

Our meta-analytic findings revealed that patients with SAD exhibited impairments in basic emotions recognition and ToM. Accordingly, individuals with SAD are unable to identify basic emotions or make inferences about the thoughts of others, thereby misunderstanding social situations in everyday life. Consistently, research on cognitive biases suggests that individuals with SAD may lack an accurate view of how they are perceived by others, especially in social situations when they allocate attentional resources to monitoring their own actions as well as external threat (24). In addition, it has been suggested (14) that SAD patients take longer than healthy participants to recognize emotions across modalities which may imply a longer cognitive elaboration process and facilitate their avoidance responses. As the underlying mechanisms for emotion recognition and ToM impairments in SAD are unknown, further investigation is needed. Also, future meta-analytic studies should test whether patients with SAD are more likely to attribute more intense emotions and greater meaning to what others were thinking and feeling.

It is well established that impaired emotion recognition and ToM may contribute to significant everyday social difficulties, including reduced social competence, social isolation, and poorer social integration (61). In particular, this meta-analytic evidence suggests that emotion recognition and ToM may underlie interpersonal impairments observed in SAD patients. Despite this, it is important to recognize the limitations of employed social cognitive tests, including issues concerning ecological validity (6264). Future studies should assess emotion recognition, ToM, and other social cognition domains in patients with SAD using more ecological tests. Also, our findings underscore the importance of routine clinical screening for social cognition in patients with SAD, as well as a critical need to develop evidence-based treatments for social cognitive impairment in this population.

Regarding GAD, results are not conclusive. Due to the small number of studies involving measures of emotion recognition and ToM in GAD patients, meta-analysis of these social cognitive domains was not possible. However, some previous studies (22, 65) and the only previous meta-analysis (7) found social cognition impairments in patients with GAD. These social cognition impairments may be associated with interpersonal problems in everyday life. These problems may be also linked with the excessive and uncontrollable worry which is the hallmark symptom of GAD (66). For example, people with GAD seem to be more likely than controls to either under- or over-estimate their impact and hostile behaviors on others (66). Considering the limited evidence, further research is urgently needed on social cognition in patients with GAD.

5. Conclusion

The findings of the current study synthesize the body of literature on emotion recognition and ToM in patients with SAD or GAD. Overall, our meta-analytic findings reveal that both of these domains are impaired in patients with SAD. Results for GAD are not conclusive due to the small number of studies involving measures of emotion recognition and ToM. Further studies employing ecological measures with larger and homogenous samples are needed to better delineate related factors influencing social cognition outcomes in patients with SAD and GAD. Such efforts will be beneficial for informing the design and implementation of evidence-based treatments for social cognitive impairment in these patients.

Author contributions

SB, MT, and DF designed the study and supervised study conduct. GD and MAT performed the title and abstract review. SB, GD, and MAT reviewed the full texts and agreed on incusion/exclusion. GD and MAT contacted authors for missing data. MAT analyzed the data under supervision of SB and DF. SB drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Ministerio de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación (Minciencias, Colombia) (grant # 915, 2019, and 123384466952).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1192683/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Vos, T, Lim, SS, Abbafati, C, Abbas, KM, Abbasi, M, Abbasifard, M, et al. Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet. (2020) 396:1204–22. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30925-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Saris, IMJ, Aghajani, M, van der Werff, SJA, van der Wee, NJA, and Penninx, BWJH. Social functioning in patients with depressive and anxiety disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2017) 136:352–61. doi: 10.1111/acps.12774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Taillieu, TL, TO, A, Turner, S, Cheung, K, Fortier, J, Zamorski, M, et al. Risk factors, clinical presentations, and functional impairments for generalized anxiety disorder in military personnel and the general population in Canada. Can J Psychiatr. (2018) 63:610–9. doi: 10.1177/0706743717752878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Dryman, MT, Gardner, S, Weeks, JW, and Heimberg, RG. Social anxiety disorder and quality of life: how fears of negative and positive evaluation relate to specific domains of life satisfaction. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 38:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wang, J, Chen, Y, Tan, C, and Zhao, X. Family functioning, social support, and quality of life for patients with anxiety disorder. Int J Soc Psychiatry. (2016) 62:5–11. doi: 10.1177/0020764015584649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pepper, KL, Demetriou, EA, Park, SH, Song, YC, Hickie, IB, Cacciotti-Saija, C, et al. Autism, early psychosis, and social anxiety disorder: understanding the role of social cognition and its relationship to disability in young adults with disorders characterized by social impairments. Transl Psychiatry. (2018) 8:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0282-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Plana, I, Lavoie, MA, Battaglia, M, and Achim, AM. A meta-analysis and scoping review of social cognition performance in social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder and other anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2014) 28:169–77. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Stein, MB, and Kean, YM. Disability and quality of life in social phobia: epidemiologic findings. AJP. (2000) 157:1606–13. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Tekin, U, Erermiş, HS, Satar, A, Aydın, AN, Köse, S, and Bildik, T. Social cognition in first episode adolescent depression and its correlation with clinical features and quality of life. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:140–53. doi: 10.1177/1359104520973254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Mitchell, RLC, and Phillips, LH. The overlapping relationship between emotion perception and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 70:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Maoz, K, Eldar, S, Stoddard, J, Pine, DS, Leibenluft, E, and Bar-Haim, Y. Angry-happy interpretations of ambiguous faces in social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 241:122–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mathai, AT, Rai, S, and Behere, RV. Emotional threat perception and its association with Neurocognition in social anxiety disorder. Indian J Psychol Med. (2021) 44:544–51. doi: 10.1177/02537176211046472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Oh, KS, Lee, WH, Kim, S, Shin, DW, Shin, YC, and Lim, SW. Impaired facial expression recognition in patients with social anxiety disorder: a case-control study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2018) 23:218–28. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2018.1462695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tseng, HH, Huang, YL, Chen, JT, Liang, KY, Lin, CC, and Chen, SH. Facial and prosodic emotion recognition in social anxiety disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2017) 22:331–45. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2017.1330190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bayraktutan, M, Oğuzhanoğlu, NK, and Uğurlu, TT. Sympathetic skin response in social anxiety disorder and its relationship with empathy skills. Alexithymia Noro Psikiyatr Ars. (2019) 57:18–22. doi: 10.29399/npa.24719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fonzo, GA, Ramsawh, HJ, Flagan, TM, Sullivan, SG, Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder is associated with attenuation of limbic activation to threat-related facial emotions. J Affect Disord. (2014) 169:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fonzo, GA, Ramsawh, HJ, Flagan, TM, Sullivan, SG, Letamendi, A, Simmons, AN, et al. Common and disorder-specific neural responses to emotional faces in generalised anxiety, social anxiety and panic disorders. Br J Psychiatry. (2015) 206:206–15. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.149880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lundh, LG, and Öst, LG. Face recognition in patients with social phobia. Scand J Behav Ther. (1996) 25:139–48. doi: 10.1080/16506079609456019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sladky, R, Höflich, A, Atanelov, J, Kraus, C, Baldinger, P, Moser, E, et al. Increased neural habituation in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder revealed by fMRI. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e50050. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tetik, D, Gica, S, Bestepe, EE, Buyukavsar, A, and Gulec, H. Emotional information processing and assessment of cognitive functions in social anxiety disorder: an event-related potential study. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2022) 53:104–13. doi: 10.1177/1550059420981506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yoon, S, Kim, HS, Kim, JI, Lee, S, and Lee, SH. Reading simple and complex facial expressions in patients with major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2016) 70:151–8. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Aydın, O, Balıkçı, K, Çökmüş, FP, and Ünal, AP. The evaluation of metacognitive beliefs and emotion recognition in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: effects on symptoms and comparison with healthy control. Nord J Psychiatry. (2019) 73:293–301. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2019.1623317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Buhlmann, U, Wacker, R, and Dziobek, I. Inferring other people’s states of mind: comparison across social anxiety, body dysmorphic, and obsessive-compulsive disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2015) 34:107–13. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hezel, DM, and McNally, RJ. Theory of mind impairments in social anxiety disorder. Behav Ther. (2014) 45:530–40. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Küçükparlak, İ, Karaş, H, Kaşer, M, and Yildirim, EA. The relationship of theory of mind and attachment characteristics with disease severity in social anxiety disorder. Noro Psikiyatr Ars. (2021) 58:63–7. doi: 10.29399/npa.27169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zainal, NH, and Newman, MG. Worry amplifies theory-of-mind reasoning for negatively valenced social stimuli in generalized anxiety disorder. J Affect Disord. (2018) 227:824–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hendriks, N, McEnteggart, C, Barnes-Holmes, Y, De Mey, HRA, Witteman, CLM, Janssen, GTL, et al. Assessing deictic relational responding in psychosis using the implicit relational assessment procedure. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. (2020) 20:287–299.

Google Scholar

28. Washburn, D, Wilson, G, Roes, M, Rnic, K, and Harkness, KL. Theory of mind in social anxiety disorder, depression, and comorbid conditions. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 37:71–7. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021):n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lin, X, Zhang, X, Liu, Q, Zhao, P, Zhong, J, Pan, P, et al. Social cognition in multiple sclerosis and its subtypes: A meta-analysis. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2021) 52:102973. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2021.102973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yamada, Y, Inagawa, T, Sueyoshi, K, Sugawara, N, Ueda, N, Omachi, Y, et al. Social cognition deficits as a target of early intervention for psychoses: A systematic review. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:333. Available from:. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Wallace, B, Trikalinos, T, Lau, J, Trow, P, and Schmid, C. Closing the gap between methodologists and end-users: R as a computational Back-end. J Stat Softw. (2011) 49:49. doi: 10.18637/jss.v049.i05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bakbergenuly, I, Hoaglin, DC, and Kulinskaya, E. Estimation in meta-analyses of mean difference and standardized mean difference. Stat Med. (2020) 39:171–91. doi: 10.1002/sim.8422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ruppar, T. Meta-analysis: how to quantify and explain heterogeneity? Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. (2020) 19:646–52. doi: 10.1177/1474515120944014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Higgins, J, Thompson, S, Deeks, J, and Altman, D. Statistical heterogeneity in systematic reviews of clinical trials: a critical appraisal of guidelines and practice. J Health Serv Res Policy. (2002) 7:51–61. doi: 10.1258/1355819021927674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Higgins, JPT, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, and Altman, DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Higgins, JPT, and Thompson, SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. (2002) 21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Baker, WL, Michael White, C, Cappelleri, JC, Kluger, J, and Coleman, CI, From the Health Outcomes, Policy, and Economics (HOPE) Collaborative Group. Understanding heterogeneity in meta-analysis: the role of meta-regression. Int J Clin Pract. (2009) 63:1426–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-1241.2009.02168.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. van de Schoot, R, Sijbrandij, M, Depaoli, S, Winter, SD, Olff, M, and van Loey, NE. Bayesian PTSD-trajectory analysis with informed priors based on a systematic literature search and expert elicitation. Multivar Behav Res. (2018) 53:267–91. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2017.1412293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Abdel Shaheed, C, Maher, CG, Williams, KA, Day, R, and McLachlan, AJ. Efficacy, tolerability, and dose-dependent effects of opioid analgesics for low back pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. (2016) 176:958–68. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.1251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kennedy, A, Bower, P, Reeves, D, Blakeman, T, Bowen, R, Chew-Graham, C, et al. Implementation of self management support for long term conditions in routine primary care settings: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ. (2013) 346:f2882. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f2882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Kalokerinos, EK, Murphy, SC, Koval, P, Bailen, NH, Crombez, G, Hollenstein, T, et al. Neuroticism may not reflect emotional variability. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2020) 117:9270–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1919934117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wells, G, Shea, B, O’Connell, D, Robertson, J, Peterson, J, Losos, M, et al. (2013). The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analysis. Available at: https://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp

Google Scholar

44. Lo, CKL, Mertz, D, and Loeb, M. Newcastle-Ottawa scale: comparing reviewers’ to authors’ assessments. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2014) 14:45. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Halac, E, Ciray, RO, Turan, S, Tuncturk, M, Agac, N, Elmas, FN, et al. Impaired theory of mind and emotion recognition in pediatric bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. (2021) 138:246–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. On, ZX, Ryan, NP, Konjarski, M, Catroppa, C, and Stargatt, R. Social cognition in Paediatric traumatic brain injury: A systematic review and Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. (2022) 32:127–48. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09488-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sterne, JAC, and Harbord, RM. Funnel plots in Meta-analysis. Stata J. (2004) 4:127–41. doi: 10.1177/1536867X0400400204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Egger, M, Davey Smith, G, Schneider, M, and Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Maleki, G, Zabihzadeh, A, Richman, MJ, Demetrovics, Z, and Mohammadnejad, F. Decoding and reasoning mental states in major depression and social anxiety disorder. BMC Psychiatry. (2020) 20:463. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02873-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Montagne, B, Schutters, S, Westenberg, HGM, van Honk, J, Kessels, RPC, and de Haan, EHF. Reduced sensitivity in the recognition of anger and disgust in social anxiety disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2006) 11:389–401. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Machado-de-Sousa, JP, Arrais, KC, Alves, NT, Chagas, MHN, de Meneses-Gaya, C, Crippa Ja De, S, et al. Facial affect processing in social anxiety: tasks and stimuli. J Neurosci Methods. (2010) 193:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Demenescu, LR, Kortekaas, R, Cremers, HR, Renken, RJ, van Tol, MJ, van der Wee, NJA, et al. Amygdala activation and its functional connectivity during perception of emotional faces in social phobia and panic disorder. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:1024–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Buhlmann, U, Wacker, R, and Dziobek, I. Inferring other people’s states of mind: comparison across social anxiety, body dysmorphic, and obsessive-compulsive disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2015) 34:107–13. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hezel, DM, and McNally, RJ. Theory of mind impairments in social anxiety disorder. Behav Ther. (2014) 45:530–40. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Washburn, D, Wilson, G, Roes, M, Rnic, K, and Harkness, KL. Theory of mind in social anxiety disorder, depression, and comorbid conditions. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 37:71–7. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Aydın, O, Balıkçı, K, Çökmüş, FP, and Ünal, AP. The evaluation of metacognitive beliefs and emotion recognition in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: effects on symptoms and comparison with healthy control. Nord J Psychiatry. (2019) 73:293–301. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2019.1623317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Klein Hofmeijer-Sevink, M, Batelaan, NM, van Megen, HJGM, Penninx, BW, Cath, DC, van den Hout, MA, et al. Clinical relevance of comorbidity in anxiety disorders: A report from the Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA). J Affect Disord. (2012) 137:106–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Orgeta, V, and Phillips, LH. Effects of age and emotional intensity on the recognition of facial emotion. Exp Aging Res. (2007) 34:63–79. doi: 10.1080/03610730701762047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Pezzuti, L, Longobardi, E, Milletti, K, and Ovidi, A. A study about the theory of mind in primary and secondary aging. Life Span and Disability. (2011) 14:31–44.

Google Scholar

60. Wingenbach, TSH, Ashwin, C, and Brosnan, M. Sex differences in facial emotion recognition across varying expression intensity levels from videos. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0190634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cacioppo, JT. Social neuroscience: understanding the pieces fosters understanding the whole and vice versa. Am Psychol. (2002) 57:819–31. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.11.819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Baez, S, García, AM, and Ibanez, A. The social context network model in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2017) 30:379–96. doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Baez, S, and Ibanez, A. The effects of context processing on social cognition impairments in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Front Neurosci. (2014) 8:8. Available from:. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hermans, K, Achterhof, R, Myin-Germeys, I, Kasanova, Z, Kirtley, O, and Schneider, M. Chapter ten - improving ecological validity in research on social cognition In: KE Lewandowski and AA Moustafa, editors. Social cognition in psychosis [internet]. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (2019). 249–68.

Google Scholar

65. Palm, ME, Elliott, R, McKie, S, Deakin, JFW, and Anderson, IM. Attenuated responses to emotional expressions in women with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychol Med. (2011) 41:1009–18. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Erickson, TM, and Newman, MG. Interpersonal and emotional processes in generalized anxiety disorder analogues during social interaction tasks. Behav Ther. (2007) 38:364–77. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, emotion recognition, theory of mind, social cognition

Citation: Baez S, Tangarife MA, Davila-Mejia G, Trujillo-Güiza M and Forero DA (2023) Performance in emotion recognition and theory of mind tasks in social anxiety and generalized anxiety disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1192683. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1192683

Received: 23 March 2023; Accepted: 02 May 2023;
Published: 19 May 2023.

Edited by:

Marialaura Di Tella, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Eid G. Abo Hamza, Tanta University, Egypt
Sixiang Liang, Capital Medical University, China

Copyright © 2023 Baez, Tangarife, Davila-Mejia, Trujillo-Güiza and Forero. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sandra Baez, sj.baez@uniandes.edu.co

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.