Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 August 2023
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploring Prevention Strategies and Treatment in Addictive Disorders View all 9 articles

Methylphenidate as a treatment option for substance use disorder: a transdiagnostic perspective

  • Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

A transition in viewing mental disorders from conditions defined as a set of unique characteristics to one of the quantitative variations on a collection of dimensions allows overlap between disorders. The overlap can be utilized to extend to treatment approaches. Here, we consider the overlap between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorder to probe the suitability to use methylphenidate as a treatment for substance use disorder. Both disorders are characterized by maladaptive goal-directed behavior, impaired cognitive control, hyperactive phasic dopaminergic neurotransmission in the striatum, prefrontal hypoactivation, and reduced frontal cortex gray matter volume/density. In addition, methylphenidate has been shown to improve cognitive control and normalize associated brain activation in substance use disorder patients and clinical trials have found methylphenidate to improve clinical outcomes. Despite the theoretical basis and promising, but preliminary, outcomes, many questions remain unanswered. Most prominent is whether all patients who are addicted to different substances may equally profit from methylphenidate treatment.

1. Introduction

Substance use disorder (SUD) is currently one of the most prominent mental disorders worldwide (1). According to the “World Drug Report” from 2019 by the United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime, around 36 million people suffer from SUD, which may be an underestimation, given that estimates for Europe reached 15 million cases of alcohol dependence alone in 2011 (2). The large scale of SUD occurrence bears an enormous burden to many individuals and society as a whole (3). For example, the current opioid epidemic leads to a rapidly increasing number of overdose deaths due to opioid misuse (4), and out of all brain disorders, alcohol use disorder is estimated to induce the third highest number of years of life lost (2). These numbers highlight the need for successful treatment of patients with SUD.

Existing treatments of SUD often fail to prevent relapse, and progress has been modest over the last 20 years. Only 17–35% of the individuals treated for alcohol use disorder (including pharmacotherapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, and group sessions) stayed abstinent for at least 1 year as reported in 2001 (5), while in 2016 a conservative estimate of 35% was obtained of SUD patients who can be considered in long-term remission (6). In addition, only a minority of SUD patients receive treatment. Development of effective prevention and cost-efficient novel treatment programs for SUD are, therefore, of significant importance, and alternative approaches should be explored.

New treatment approaches may stem from novel approaches to diagnose psychopathology. Specifically, the traditional categorical approach to psychopathology attempts to identify treatments based on maladaptive behavior and symptoms that characterize a disorder in a narrow sense by intentionally excluding overlap with other disorders. More concretely, a diagnosis for a given disorder is less likely when symptoms can be explained by another disorder. Therefore, the approach largely ignores behavioral and neural deficits shared by different disorders (7), thus potentially overlooking effective treatments. In contrast, contemporary approaches to diagnoses of mental disorders utilize various dimensions of symptoms within and across disorders. For example, deficits in executive functions may be characteristic of multiple mental disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and SUD [but also, among others, schizophrenia, autism, and Alzheimer's disease (1)]. This view is becoming more prevalent as it is acknowledged in the current version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM-5; (1)]. Nonetheless, the DSM-5 can still be considered to be in a transitional period, as it does not fully endorse the view. Others take the dimensional approach further. For instance, the Research Domain Criteria project [RDoC; (8)] classifies disorders as quantitative variations on dimensions of a set of constructs (e.g., cognitive control) within domains (e.g., cognitive systems). These constructs are defined by elements that include behavioral performance, self-report, and biological mechanisms (c.f. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc/constructs/rdoc-matrix). As these constructs can cut across mental disorders, we consider it reasonable that treatments follow suit by exploring treatments for seemingly different disorders that are similar in, at least some, underlying cognitive constructs and associated biological mechanisms. These shared deficits within the constructs can provide important opportunities for treatment (9, 10). Therefore, the potential of a pharmacological treatment to be used in SUD, but which is currently used for a disorder that shows similarities in the construct of cognitive control, should be explored.

Treatments for ADHD may be useful in SUD as both disorders are characterized by poor performance within the construct of cognitive control. The more specific aim of this study was to evaluate the potential of pharmacological ADHD treatment for successful use in SUD. At the core of the idea to utilize pharmacological treatment for ADHD in SUD lies the cognitive control deficit represented by inhibitory control failures as the most prominent (11, 12), which is shared between the two conditions (13) and the underlying dopaminergic dysfunction. In general, inhibitory control is the ability to suppress a prepotent or habitual response when necessary (14). When inhibitory control is lacking, actions are immediately and impulsively executed, often at the expense of long-term consequences, and with the disregard for long-term goals. These actions are inappropriate to the situation and are difficult to terminate once started. On a surface level, poor inhibitory control in SUD is demonstrated by displaying addiction behavior (e.g., consuming drugs), while rationally being aware that it is better not to do so given the long-term consequences (1). Similarly, in ADHD, poor impulse control is expressed by the inability to remain physically immobile and by frequent engagement in distractions (1). On the task-performance level, individuals with ADHD or SUD both perform poorly on laboratory measures of inhibitory control, such as the stop-signal task (SST) (11, 15). Lower response regulatory abilities have also been coined a risk factor for substance abuse (16). On a neurotransmitter level, dopamine (DA) receptor families play a prominent role in cognitive and inhibitory control (17), particularly deficient D2-like receptors are observed in individuals with high impulsivity (18). In addition, both ADHD and SUD patients show abnormal dopaminergic neurotransmission (1820). Therefore, this transdiagnostic impairment in both ADHD and SUD suggests inhibitory control to be a suitable construct to investigate further as a target for pharmacological treatment (13). The potentially shared underlying neurobiological correlates should be explored to validate the pharmacological treatment. However, it should be noted that other disorders may share the neurobiological atypicalities as well as ADHD and SUD. Those disorders may not be suitable (e.g., schizophrenia) to be treated with typical ADHD medication. Therefore, the rationale presented below should be considered within the boundaries defined by the suitability of disorders to be treated with stimulant medication.

Existing pharmacotherapy for SUD does not appear to effectively target cognitive control (21). Nonetheless, neuropharmacological therapy is the most prominent treatment to target cognitive control deficits and deficient DA transmission in ADHD and is considered effective. DA-based ADHD interventions have a success rate of around 70% (1). While various stimulant drugs exist to treat ADHD, methylphenidate (MPH) is the most widely used and therefore, given the suggested overlap, can be considered a prime candidate for treating SUD. MPH blocks dopamine (DAT) and noradrenaline (NAT) transporters (22) and thereby increases extracellular DA and noradrenaline (NA) levels. MPH improves response inhibition task performance and reduces impulsive behavior in children with ADHD (23). MPH also has been shown to improve inhibitory control abilities in cocaine-dependent patients (24, 25), although this may be due to normalizing catecholamine levels by MPH in these particular patients. In addition, MPH has been considered safe with low addiction liability due to the slower onset and elimination of its effects (26). These findings suggest that MPH may successfully increase inhibitory control in SUD as it does in ADHD.

The purpose of this perspectives paper was to evaluate the potential for methylphenidate to be a suitable treatment option in SUD. By comparing ADHD and SUD on a (a) theoretical level, (b) behavioral/clinical level, and (c) neurobiological level, seemingly similar deficits are explored to assess the stimulant treatment's potential. Second, some direct evidence for the efficacy and effects of MPH in SUD treatment and criticism are reviewed, and finally, a guide is proposed for future research into knowledge gaps, ultimately to establish new treatments for SUD.

2. Evaluation of the hypothesis

2.1. ADHD and SUD overlap in theoretical models

Both theoretical models of ADHD and SUD attribute a central role to reduced or biased cognitive control of behavior. Currently, one hypothesis for the mechanism underlying ADHD characteristic dysfunction is that low tonic catecholamine (DA, NE) activation leads to phasic hyper-responses causing distractibility, impulsivity, and disorganized thoughts (19, 27). ADHD is also associated with low prefrontal cortex (PFC) functioning, which causes reduced control over both external and internal stimuli, and reduced control of behavior. The PFC controls stimuli by dynamically inhibiting sensory cortices and subcortical structures. In ADHD, both structure and function of PFC are affected, reducing the ability to regulate information, which results in distractibility and impulsivity (28). This notion of core deficits in ADHD is supported by observations that the PFC, caudate, and cerebellum as a network are most prominently affected in ADHD (29, 30).

SUD is currently best explained by extended hypotheses that emphasize the increased value of potential rewards (31, 32) and increased habitual control of behavior (33, 34). The incentive salience/sensitization hypothesis states that stimuli that predict drug consumption cause an intense “wanting” of the drug, which is associated with a hyper-responsive mesolimbic DA system (31, 32). This response amplifies the motivational drive to obtain the predicted drug at the expense of long-term consequences and can, therefore, be considered biased goal-directed behavior. A second hypothesis that attempts to explain addiction behavior stresses reduced control over behavior, rendering goal-directed behavior habitual (33, 34). This transition is marked by a shift in brain activation from ventral striatum, indicating reward-based goal-directed behavior, to dorsal striatum, indicating fast, non-controlled behavior. While these hypotheses appear to differ in the way goal-directed behavior is affected (biased in incentive salience and reduced in goal-directed to habitual shift), both share a notion of a reduction in the weight of the behavioral choice to refrain from drug taking and thereby a relative lack of cognitive control directed at achieving long-term goals.

Taken together, both conditions are theorized to implicate insufficient or biased behavioral control governed by phasic catecholamine responses.

2.2. ADHD and SUD shared behavioral deficits

Impaired cognitive control as a broad term is often operationalized as various measurements of impulsivity. For example, the BIS-11 is an established self-report measurement of impulsivity (35). The scale consists of different subscales capturing aspects of inattention, spontaneous actions, lack of forethought, non-planning, and inhibition (36). More objective measures of impulsivity are provided by perseveration paradigms [e.g., Kertesz et al. (37)], antisaccade paradigms (38), conflicting or contralateral motor response tasks [e.g., Bellato et al. (39), Watson et al. (40)], go/no-go tasks [e.g., Trommer et al. (41)], and the stop-signal paradigm (42). All paradigms aim to assess the ability to inhibit prepotent responses. Antisaccade paradigms, conflicting and contralateral motor response tasks, go/no-go, and perseveration tasks all require the participant to withhold or change a response to a stimulus, whether the response is a saccade, eye-gaze, or a simple motor response like a button press. Perseveration paradigms are characterized by the need to change a response from a previously given response. Antisaccade paradigms require participants to make goal-directed saccades in the opposite direction to a presented stimulus, which elicits a reflexive saccade that needs to be suppressed. Similarly, a conflicting or contralateral motor response task requires the participant to make a spatial non-congruent response to a visual or tactile stimulus, e.g., in opposite direction to an indicated location. The go/no-go paradigm entails responding or withholding a cued response depending on the identity of the presented stimulus. Similarly, in the stop-signal task participants need to respond as fast as possible every time they see a target stimulus within a sequence of other insignificant stimuli (Go trials). However, when the target stimulus is presented together with a secondary cue, participants must refrain from responding (15). The difference between this paradigm and the former is that the secondary cue is presented after the imperative cue and therefore the stop-signal response time (SSRT) can be calculated, which enables quantification of the time needed to inhibit a response (43).

ADHD-diagnosed individuals show higher BIS-11 scores compared with matched controls (44), suggesting increased subjective impulsivity. Objectively assessed impulsivity is also increased in ADHD adults (45) as well as children (46, 47), particularly as assessed with the stop-signal task (46, 48). Up to 1,000 ms, slower inhibition of their responses compared with healthy controls has been observed (49, 50), while recent studies find smaller but significant differences between these groups (51). However, some studies report no group differences (52), which may be explained by the suggested ability of adults with ADHD to compensate for their deficits for a short amount of time (53). ADHD patients have shown impaired performance on antisaccade tasks compared with healthy controls, although the results are not unequivocal (54, 55). They also show lower scores on tests of perseveration [e.g., Houghton et al. (56), Fischer et al. (57)], conflicting motor response [e.g., Mahone et al. (58)], and on go/no-go paradigms (59).

Similar to ADHD individuals, SUD patients have shown high scores on the BIS-11 questionnaire indicating enhanced subjective impulsivity levels (60, 61). Various measures of brain structure and functions associated with inhibition failure have been shown to predict binge drinking during adolescence (62). Poor response inhibition abilities have also been shown to predict adolescent drug and alcohol use (63). More specifically concerning the SST, drug-dependent individuals show impaired performance on the SST. For example, individuals with cocaine use disorder have a lower probability of successfully inhibiting a response and do this more slowly compared with healthy controls (11). Furthermore, longer SSRTs can predict the degree of future alcohol consumption and progression toward dependence in heavy drinkers (60). Similarly, increased electrophysiological activation during SST performance predicts smoking cessation duration (64) and smoking behavior (65).

SUD patients have been found to display rigid response behavior by showing increased perseveration in a probabilistic reversal learning paradigm compared with healthy controls (66, 67). In addition, these differences could be reversed using the dopaminergic drugs pramipexole (66) and amisulpride (67). A correlation between perseveration and duration of cocaine use has also been observed using an instrumental learning paradigm (68). However, perseveration did not differ from healthy controls in this particular study (68). In addition, increased perseveration in SUD patients has not been observed consistently across addictions (69). While cocaine users did show perseveration, chronic amphetamine and opioid users did not (70).

Nicotine-dependent individuals have been shown to perform worse compared with healthy controls on go/no-go paradigms (61), which is correlated with how much a person smokes per day (71). The poor performance on go/no-go paradigms has also been associated with relapse vulnerability (72). In addition, satiated smokers show impaired performance monitoring potentially contributing to the continuation of their smoking habits (73).

Adolescents at risk for developing an addiction show reduced antisaccadic performance (74) or a lack of performance improvement during adolescence (75), but which could be enhanced using incentives (76, 77). Performance on antisaccadic eye movements has been found to correlate with smoking status (71). Please see Table 1 for a qualitative summary of the behavioral observations.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Qualitative summary of differences between patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls, and between patients with substance use disorder (SUD) and healthy controls on subjective and objective measures of impulsivity.

To the best of our knowledge, there is one study comparing ADHD and SUD directly on objective measures of inhibitory control. Gerhardt et al. (78) observed more commission errors in ADHD subjects compared with alcohol use disorder subjects in a comprehensive paradigm assessing various cognitive aspects of impulsivity. However, they did not observe any behavioral differences on six other measures. Further direct comparisons that result in differences between the groups may argue against a common deficit. However, as presented above, both populations can be successfully discriminated from healthy controls using the BIS-11 (44, 60). In addition, similar cognitive control performance patterns already provide some objective evidence for similar inhibitory control deficits. Similar neural abnormalities might be responsible for the observed inhibition impairments and may strengthen this position.

2.3. ADHD and SUD shared neurobiological characteristics

Altered neurobiological metrics in both ADHD and SUD that form an overlap in brain areas that govern inhibitory control of behavior can be considered evidence for the shared deficits. Shared differences compared with neurotypical and healthy controls in neurotransmission, brain activation, and brain matter volume/density may exist. The aim of the following section is not to provide an exhaustive review of the neurobiological alterations in ADHD and SUD, but identification of overlap relevant to inhibitory control. Given that MPH targets catecholamine neurotransmission, shared catecholamine deficits should have the most weight in the evaluation.

2.3.1. Dopamine

DA plays a major role in the regulation of behavior, and small changes in DA levels impair cognitive control (29). Abnormal DA neurotransmission is one of the most important factors leading to behavioral dysfunction in ADHD (79, 80). Within the PFC–striatal–PFC loop, the “tonic–phasic DA hypothesis” of ADHD offers an explanation for characteristic ADHD symptoms (27, 81, 82). The model describes how a striatal imbalance between D1-like and D2-like receptor activation in patients with ADHD (79) affects the gating of PFC-striatal signals. The reduced gating ability leads to hyperresponsiveness of the individual. According to the model, normal striatal gating function is established by tonic extracellular DA concentrations that activate D2-like autoreceptors. This tonic DA-induced D2 receptor activation subsequently reduces phasic DA responses by a reduction in DA synthesis and release (81). In ADHD-affected individuals, low tonic striatal DA activity leads to decreased inhibition and subsequent increased phasic (burst of high level) DA signaling via striatal postsynaptic D1-like receptor activity (83, 84). Corresponding non-optimal receptor activation balance (19, 84) and low tonic/high phasic activation patterns (19) observed in individuals with ADHD support this view.

Increased DAT activity might underlie the observed D1-like/D2-like receptor activation imbalance (19). Elevated DAT levels accelerate DA reuptake and therefore reduce tonic D2-like receptor activation disinhibiting phasic activity. Multiple studies report heightened DAT levels in individuals with ADHD, which might be the result of inadequate neurodevelopment (23, 85, 86). These findings must be interpreted with caution because other studies do not confirm elevated DAT levels in ADHD patients (87). The inconsistent findings might originate from different inclusion criteria, methods, or screening techniques. Nonetheless, the most successful treatment for ADHD, MPH, blocks the DAT (88). DAT occupancy by MPH is positively correlated with reduced self-reported impulsivity (89). Therefore, whatever the neural deficit in ADHD, targeting DAT appears successful in ADHD treatment.

Taken together, while it is still unclear whether poor PFC functioning is the cause of low tonic DA activation in the striatum, and what role DAT activity plays in this, or whether increased phasic striatal output causes poor PFC functioning, it is clear that this PFC DA circuitry plays a prominent role in ADHD-related behavioral deficits (80). In favor of this view, stimulant-induced enhancement of catecholamine function in the PFC of ADHD individuals is associated with behavioral improvements and can be reversed by noradrenergic α2 and DA D1 receptor antagonists (80).

Alterations in dopaminergic functioning have been observed in alcohol, cocaine, opioid, cannabis, and tobacco-addicted individuals (24, 90, 91) and have been well-studied using PET and SPECT (92). Drugs elicit high DA surges in the mesolimbic reward system (93). Tonic DA activity is suggested to be reduced to compensate for these excessive DA responses [(94), p. 104]. Cannabis users have shown reduced DA synthesis (95) and reduced striatal DA response following a stimulant challenge (96, 97), which might explain the lost interest for natural rewards and compulsive drug-seeking in SUD patients. Conversely, increased DA signaling has been observed subsequent to the presentation of addiction-related stimuli (98, 99) and D2/D3 antagonism reduced cue-induced responding and reward-obtaining impulsivity (100). In addition, reduced D2-like receptor availability has been observed (18, 91, 101), which correlates with age of cannabis use onset (97) and current use history (102). In addition, lower D2 receptor availability in the dorsal striatum in methamphetamine users predicted relapse (103). Lower D2-like receptor levels might underlie the observed hypofrontality and control impairments in SUD patients (104). A study showed that blocking D2-like receptors decreases prefrontal activity, especially in the IFG and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), compared with participants receiving a placebo (104). The attenuated brain activation correlated with performance impairments in an SST. These findings confirm the regulatory function of DA in prefrontal inhibitory control mechanisms.

Concerning DAT availability, the many studies performed report inconsistent findings in SUD patients. Researchers argue for unchanged (105), increased (106), or decreased DAT densities (107, 108) in this population. Reduced DAT availability might be a reversible neuroadaptive response to the attenuated tonic DA activity observed in drug-dependent individuals (107, 109). When tonic DA levels are low, DAT may be downregulated to accommodate sufficient DA activity despite the lower levels. Contrariwise, increased DAT levels might be a failed attempt to compensate for extremely high DA levels following drug binges (23). It is of great clinical relevance to clarify DAT's role in addiction as many drugs, such as MPH, target these molecular complexes (93).

In conclusion, both ADHD and SUD are characterized by a hyperresponsive mesocortical DA system exerting increased phasic responding upon relevant (e.g., addiction associated) stimulation, which may be associated with reduced D2-like receptor function, low tonic DA, and altered DAT activity. However, findings concerning DAT levels are inconsistent for both conditions. For ADHD, the evidence appears to lean toward increased DAT levels whereas for SUD evidence points equally in both directions. Importantly, the mechanisms underlying the disorders differ, such that in SUD there may be an overall reduction in DA functioning leading to low tonic DA activation and low phasic activation in response to natural rewards, while only displaying high phasic activation to addiction-related stimuli. Conversely, in ADHD the low tonic activation is hypothesized to lead to high phasic activation toward a large number of different stimuli.

2.3.2. Brain activation

2.3.2.1. Mesolimbic system

The mesolimbic system plays a pivotal role in reward-based learning and incentive salience (110) and includes, most importantly, DA neurons in the ventral tegmental area and projections toward the nucleus accumbens. Exaggerated mesolimbic activity has been observed in animal models in which rats exhibit ADHD symptoms (111). However, only a few studies propose a hyperactive mesolimbic system in human individuals with ADHD, for example, when monetary rewards are presented to participants (112). Therefore, further research needs to clarify whether these functional deviations exist in human ADHD populations.

Mesolimbic neuroadaptations in SUD patients have been observed more frequently. Particularly relevant is the hyperactive mesolimbic reward system when addicted individuals are presented with drug-related cues [(113115), see Leyton and Vezina (116); Berridge and Robinson (31) for nuanced views]. For example, alcohol-dependent drinkers show greater striatal activation in response to alcohol-related cues compared with social drinkers [(117), but see Vollstadt-Klein et al. (118) for conflicting results]. Following addiction-relevant cues, cocaine users (119), cannabis users (120, 121), alcohol-dependent patients (122124), smokers [(125127), but see Vollstadt-Klein et al. (128)], and cannabis users and heavy alcohol drinkers (129) all show increased frontostriatal activation (most often including nucleus accumbens) compared with neutral cues. The striatal response has also been associated with alcohol use problems (130) and, among other factors, the amount of alcohol used (131). This may reflect increased signaling of potentially high reward value and subsequent high motivation to obtain the substance of abuse (31). The high motivational drive may not be appropriately governed by frontal cortex circuits and ultimately leads to behavior strongly biased toward obtaining the substance.

Altogether, both ADHD and SUD individuals may show maladaptive mesolimbic processes. The high phasic DA-dependent striatal responses to environmental cues signaling potential rewards may lead to impulsive behavior. Ultimately, these maladaptive neuronal characteristics impair ADHD and SUD patients in their inhibitory control abilities and goal-directed behavior.

2.3.2.2. Cognitive control-related areas

Cognitive control is mainly mediated by an interaction between a frontoparietal network (14) and subcortical structures (132) in which DA plays a prominent role (133). Numerous dopaminergic connections between the subcortical areas and the PFC allow the inhibition of prepotent impulses (134). For example, frontal cortical “top-down” mechanisms inhibit subcortical “bottom-up” impulses via reciprocal connections (135), and the basal ganglia (e.g., striatum, subthalamic nucleus) can facilitate or inhibit frontal processes and therefore modulate behavior (136). Lesions in frontal areas can increase impulsive and disinhibited behavior (137), which is shown by several studies suggesting an association between frontal lobe functioning and performance on a response inhibition task (136, 138). Therefore, impulsive–compulsive disorders may be associated with frontal lobe functioning, which may occur in both ADHD and SUD (109). As a number of studies report hypofrontality in both populations (136, 139), the extent of hypofrontality in both disorders may suggest a common brain-function deficit.

Functional imaging robustly supports prefrontal hypoactivity in individuals with ADHD. ADHD patients show attenuated activity in the ACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), during cognitive task performance (140142) and significant hypoactivity in inferior prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices (OFC), striatum, thalamus, and parietal cortices (141). Decreased PFC activity is particularly evident in the performance of tasks that require sustained attention or inhibition of inappropriate movement (143). In addition, in an electrophysiological study, ADHD patients showed reduced activity in the superior frontal gyrus, which modulates self-control during the performance of a cognitive task, compared with controls (51). In support, transcranial direct current stimulation of the DLPFC can improve inhibitory control and reduces impulsivity in ADHD patients (140). That said, not all studies confirm these activation differences. For example, Dillo et al. (53) reported no prefrontal hypoactivity, but increased recruitment of attentional parietal areas. However, these findings may reflect less-efficient processing or compensatory strategies (52, 144) indicating non-optimal functioning.

From a brain network perspective, many of the brain areas that have been found hypoactive are part of the executive control network [ECN, (145)] consisting of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), ACC, pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), DLPFC, anterior insula, and posterior parietal cortex (146148). The ECN is hypoactive in individuals with ADHD during cognitive tasks (149). In addition, the PFC is less extensively connected with subcortical regions (139), which may lead to insufficient 'top-down' regulation of the default mode network [DMN, (150)] and the dorsal attention network (145). This was confirmed by observed disrupted functional connectivity between prefrontal control areas and regions of the DMN (151). In addition, task engagement should decrease DMN activation, but in children with ADHD, the network is activated during an inhibitory control task (52, 152). The disrupted interplay between the ECN and DMN might explain deficient control abilities in individuals with ADHD.

Frontal hypoactivation is also observed in SUD [(136), see Klugah-Brown et al. (153) for a meta-analysis]. For example, cocaine-dependent participants can be discriminated from healthy controls based on attenuated frontal activity during an SST (154). Especially areas of the ECN are affected, including, but not limited to, the IFG, ACC, and DLPFC (155, 156). Methamphetamine users showed reduced activation in brain areas associated with cognitive control (right IFG, supplementary motor area/ACC, and anterior insula) and performed worse than controls on a Stroop task (157). In further support, exciting the hypoactive DLPFC in SUD patients can improve decision-making and decreases craving (155). Furthermore, metabolism in the ACC and OFC is attenuated in individuals with SUD (155, 158), potentially contributing to reduced sensitivity to negative consequences of to-be-performed behavior (155, 159). However, these findings may reflect a neuronal vulnerability as deficient inhibitory control processes have been observed in non-consuming biological siblings as well as drug-dependent individuals (160), and white matter abnormalities are shared by first-degree biological relatives of SUD patients, who have no history of drug use (136).

One study directly compared brain activation of ADHD and alcohol use disorder patients as elicited by a comprehensive response inhibition task (78). Authors report more activation of a frontoparietal network, cortical and subcortical motor areas, and occipital areas in alcohol use disorder patients compared with ADHD patients. Taken together, while some activation differences seem to exist between SUD and ADHD patients, in comparison with healthy controls the hypoactivation of the ECN, and in particular the DLPFC, ACC, OFC, and inferior frontal gyrus, and the increase in cognitive control following DLPFC stimulation are shared between ADHD patients and SUD patients.

2.3.3. Gray matter

In addition to reduced activation, several imaging studies have shown that the DLPFC is smaller in patients with ADHD compared with controls (151, 161164). Despite that whole brain reduction in thickness, volume, folding, and surface area have been observed (165), suggesting non-specific brain deficits, various measures converge on PFC deformation (28). Variations in dopamine D4 receptors (166, 167) are associated with thinning of PFC in ADHD (168) and reduction in DLPFC neuron density (169). In addition, PFC maturation has been observed to be slower in ADHD (167). Nonetheless, smaller caudate/putamen volume appears most prominent across various meta-analyses (28). These results support the earlier discussed potentially maladaptive prominent PFC–striatal–PFC network characteristics in ADHD patients.

In line with the idea of reduced cognitive control in SUD are the volumetric differences with healthy controls. A recent meta-analysis of 60 voxel-based morphometry studies shows reduced volume of the ACC, thalamus, and insula and increased putamen volume (170). This pattern of results is possibly reflective of decreased cognitive control and increased putamen (i.e., habits) governance of behavior. In addition, a large-scale analysis of gray matter volume in SUD patients has shown brainwide reduction in cortical thickness. While results were mostly driven by alcohol-dependent patients, thinning of OFC, inferior parietal, insula, and middle temporal cortices is shared across addictions to various substances (171). OFC plays an important role in value assignment to future rewards (172), damage to which leads to poor decision-making (173) and may help explain substance-biased behavior.

In summary, evidence for gray matter abnormalities in ADHD most strongly points to reduced DLPFC, while SUD appears best characterized by abnormalities of the OFC. Nonetheless, both structures are part of an executive control system governing reward-motivated behavior (174). ADHD and SUD appear to be differentiated concerning putamen volume, where smaller volume is observed in ADHD and larger in SUD. Please see Figure 1 for a qualitative overview of structural and functional differences between ADHD patients and healthy controls and between SUD patients and healthy controls.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Qualitative overview of differences between patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls, and between patients with substance use disorder (SUD) and healthy controls in brain activation during performance of cognitive control tasks and measurements of local brain volume. ↓, reduced activation/volume; ↓↑, inconsistent findings; -, no observed difference.

2.4. Current evidence for MPH efficacy in SUD

Studies aimed at temporarily reversing neurobiological and behavioral deficits in SUD patients largely show that MPH is able to normalize brain function and task performance associated with three key deficits. First, concerning impulsivity, MPH has been shown to decrease stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) in cocaine users which correlated positively with middle FC activation and negatively with ventral medial PFC (25). A complementary analysis of the same data showed that MPH also restored [otherwise impaired (175)] activation in the ventral medial PFC before making commission errors on the stop-signal task (176). Similar findings of normalizing effects of MPH in cocaine users were observed for a cue reactivity task and ACC activation (24, 177, 178). Second, MPH improved cognitive control as shown by increased performance in both cocaine SUD patients and healthy controls on the Stroop task and selectively increasing DLPFC activation in SUD patients (179). Finally, hyperresponsiveness to drug-related cues was reduced by MPH in cocaine users (180).

Direct evidence for the efficacy of stimulants as treatment for SUD has been reviewed a number of times in the recent past. From these reviews emerges a view that MPH is the most, and perhaps the only (181), effective stimulant treatment [however see Dursteler et al. (182) for a nuanced view]. Two studies have shown higher abstinence rates from methamphetamine as a primary outcome after subchronic MPH administration compared with placebo [10–22 weeks; (183185), but see Miles, Sheridan et al. (184)], as reviewed in Soares and Pereira (186). Ling, Chang et al. (183) observed approximately 15% positive urine drug screens after 14 weeks of MPH treatment vs. ~34% for placebo. Moreso, in another but similar trial, the number of positive urine drug screens was also lower (~20%) compared with placebo (~35%) after 10 weeks of MPH treatment (185) or showed a reduced probability of being positive (187). In addition, MPH showed lower scores on measures of depression and craving (185), withdrawal symptoms, and addiction severity as secondary outcomes (186). Next to these studies, two other studies have shown favorable treatment outcomes of MPH in a sustained release formulation [(188, 189), reviewed in Lee et al. (190)]. After 10 weeks of MPH treatment, 46% of the urine tests were positive for the presence of amphetamine compared with 79% after placebo treatment (188). Finally, adding MPH to an existing behavioral therapy was superior in reducing craving and addiction severity, increasing mental health and number of negative urine tests than either treatments alone (191). Although MPH treatment remains to be refined (e.g., establishing dose–response curves in various SUD patient groups, with comorbidities, and with various addiction severities), these findings strongly suggest the potential value of MPH treatment. Please see Table 2 for an overview.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Qualitative overview of studies assessing the efficacy of methylphenidate treatment for substance use disorder.

The neural mechanism from which these improvements may result remains unclear. Nonetheless, some data suggest that MPH reduces abnormally strong ventral to dorsal striatal functional connectivity in cocaine SUD patients, where addiction severity was associated with lower connectivity (197). These findings are in line with the prominent theory describing a ventral to dorsal striatal shift in the governance of behavior (33, 34). Another mechanism may be that MPH intervenes in assigning reward value to a drug experience. Evers et al. (198) observed that increased tonic dopamine activity by MPH reduced phasic ventral striatal response upon receiving reward. MPH may therefore suppress reward value and diminish reward-based learning in SUD patients, which is in line with an MPH-induced reduction in functional connectivity between the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum (199) as a neural substrate for drug liking (200). Improved behavioral control is suggested by MPH-induced altered functional connectivity between the nucleus accumbens medial PFC (199), an area associated with reflective cognition (201). In addition, MPH reversed an acute stress-induced reduction in brain activation associated with goal-directed behavior (202). Taken together, these data suggest MPH to increase behavioral control at times of choosing to perform a particular behavior associated with consequences including drug-related reward, an ability that may be key in maintaining abstinence.

2.5. Criticism

Despite the promising results, some criticism exists that needs to be considered. For example, cocaine users report increased drug wanting after MPH when prompted by relevant situations (203). MPH has also been shown to enhance the reinforcing effects of amphetamines in mice (204), and increase smoking behavior in neurotypical (205) and ADHD patients (206). Such effects would be counterproductive in a treatment setting. It should be noted that participants in both latter studies did not intend to quit smoking.

MPH-based treatment may also not be effective in some other addiction populations (186). For example, pathological gamblers have been shown to increase their motivation to gamble (207) and show increased DA release in dorsal striatal structures following amphetamine administration (208). The latter may contrast methamphetamine users in whom low levels of dorsal striatal DA release predict relapse (103) and in which MPH has shown to be effective the most. Another potential subgroup of SUD patients is formed by ADHD patients. There is a high comorbidity between ADHD and SUD (209, 210) for which various explanations exist. For example, individuals with undiagnosed ADHD may self-medicate with stimulants in an attempt to alleviate the symptoms (211). ADHD individuals also may be inherently vulnerable to SUD due to impulsive behavior and neurobiological characteristics (210). Subsequently, the proportion of ADHD patients in the SUD population is relatively large. The evidence for the efficacy of MPH to treat SUD in this particular population is limited, as many of these patients are already treated with MPH while SUD is still present [c.f. Wilens and Morrison (211)]. Crunelle et al. (89) detected that the limited success of MPH in cocaine-using ADHD patients compared with an ADHD-only group did not correlate with lower DAT occupancy. In addition, the effect of MPH is compromised in ADHD patients with comorbid cocaine dependence (196). On the other hand, MPH treatment of ADHD children has been shown to reduce the risk of substance (ab)use during adolescence (212). Therefore, individuals presenting with comorbidity at a later age may be predominantly more treatment resilient. It should be considered that the population with such comorbidity may require a different treatment approach.

Results of studies assessing the efficacy of MPH in the SUD population are not unequivocal (186), with some large, well-designed studies showing no difference between MPH treatment and placebo [e.g., Noroozi et al. (195)]. In addition, Dursteler et al. (182) report negative findings concerning the efficacy of MPH as a replacement medication in patients with cocaine use disorder specifically in five randomized controlled trials (187, 192194, 196). However, these studies did not form a homogenous group. Differences between the studies exist as some studies included patients with ADHD and others did not. Other differences were the use of concomitant medication, duration of treatment, and dose of MPH. All the above criticisms could be considered a starting point for determining boundary conditions in which MPH may be effective; e.g., in which populations and with which doses may MPH be most effective?

Another potential limitation to the use of MPH as treatment is its abuse potential and associated health risk. Abuse potential may be suggested by observed behavioral cross-sensitization with amphetamine (213), increased drug-seeking behavior (204), and conditioned place preference (214) in rodents. The abuse potential may also be signaled by the abundant use among the student population (215), which may be associated with the risk of cardiac disease (216). Moreover, MPH has been ranked 12th on a list of substances causing physical harm (217). However, most studies in humans have not observed reinforcing effects of MPH using clinical oral doses [for an overview see Kapur (218)]. In addition, conditioned place preference for orally administered MPH was only observed for higher doses or when administered immediately before testing (214). The slow onset of effects of orally administered MPH [in contrast to intranasal administration (219)] and its slow clearance have also been associated with reduced abuse potential compared with that of other stimulants (220). In conclusion, misuse of MPH is observed and is associated with health risks. However, potential clinical use to treat SUD may be safe in clinical oral doses and when closely monitored to guard against abuse.

One of the pillars of the current argument is that hypofrontality is characteristic of impaired inhibitory control and is shared by ADHD and SUD. However, hypofrontality is not exclusive to ADHD and SUD but also occurs in schizophrenia (221), bipolar disorder (222), and major depressive disorder (223). Therefore, the fact that ADHD and SUD share this characteristic is insufficient to treat both with the same pharmacological agent, as this is clearly undesirable in, for example, schizophrenia. In addition, the functional interplay between frontal and striatal areas underlies working memory, attentional function, and task-switching performance (224) as well as response inhibition, which means that hypofrontality alone cannot be considered evidence of impaired inhibitory control specifically. Therefore, shared hypofrontality in isolation should not be considered conclusive evidence of similarities between the disorders in inhibitory control deficits. Instead, it should be considered in conjunction with the overlap in theoretical models, and behavioral and, most importantly, DA deficits.

3. Discussion

The primary objective of this paper was to explore shared behavioral and neurobiological atypicalities between ADHD and SUD to evaluate the potential usefulness of MPH treatment for SUD. Overlap between disorders can be considered based on their independent explanatory hypotheses stating impaired or biased cognitive control. Empirically, both show inhibitory behavior deficits as subjectively and objectively measured using the BIS-11, and SST, antisaccade, perseveration, conflicting motor response, and go/no-go paradigms. Functionally, they both show hyperactivation of the mesolimbic pathways, albeit for ADHD only in animal models. Atypical neurotransmission is shared by both disorders characterized by low tonic DA signaling and higher phasic response given appropriate stimuli. Finally, behavioral control networks, including frontal gyri, ACC, OFC, and DLPFC (53, 155), and frontal gray matter is prominently compromised in both disorders, which is in line with the behavioral deficits. These observations support the application of DA-based stimulants as a treatment for SUD, and current evidence identifies MPH as the main candidate among other stimulants.

Despite the shared characteristics, there are differences between ADHD and SUD patients. For example, a prominent ADHD symptom is inattentiveness, which is not commonly observed in SUD individuals (1). In addition, SUD individuals' impulsive behavior appears to be restricted to responding to drug-related cues, whereas ADHD patients behave disinhibited regardless of the context (60). For example, ADHD individuals show increased reward-circuit response during a monetary incentive task (112), while SUD patients often show a reduced response [e.g., Luijten et al. (225)]. ADHD patients commonly use compensatory strategies involving parietal attention areas to mask their cognitive deficits, while SUD patients do not show these additional activity patterns (53). Until now, only individuals with ADHD show unusual activity patterns in the DMN and the OFC is dysfunctional primarily in SUD patients (101, 151). Structural differences between these populations are also present with putamen having been shown to be enlarged (170) in SUD, but decreased in ADHD (28). Important for the present purpose of evaluating MPH suitability as a treatment for SUD, DA-related characteristics in ADHD appear to be mostly represented by high levels of DAT (85, 86), while results for SUD are inconsistent. As MPH blocks the DAT, similar characteristics may be desired. However, both conditions share low tonic DA and high phasic DA (79, 226) that may be reversed using MPH regardless of the DAT differences.

Future studies should be aimed at clarifying apparent discrepancies, behavioral relevance of neurobiological and neurofunctional atypicalities, and the effects of MPH on these measures of performance. For example, on a behavioral level studies may directly compare ADHD and SUD individuals on BIS-11 scores and SST performance. It may be established whether both populations differ to an equal extent from neurotypical, healthy controls. Performance on the SST may also be subject to boundary conditions. For example, it should be established whether SUD individuals only show higher SSRT when presented with addiction-relevant cues or whether they are impaired across a variety of task conditions. Such knowledge can aid in designing situation-specific treatments.

In addition to behavior, further direct comparisons between the populations should also be made concerning brain activation [e.g., Gerhardt et al. (78)]. The key questions that need to be answered are (1) whether ADHD and SUD individuals show similar altered responses of brain networks governing behavioral control, and if so, (2) are hypofunctional frontal brain areas equally relevant in both disorders for behavioral control specifically? (3) What is the contribution of other executive function deficits to the maintenance of the disorders? Multiple executive functions are impaired in both groups, e.g., reward processing, and further studies should establish similarities between the disorders and the effects of stimulant treatment of these functions (49). These functions can be assessed using various well-established performance tasks addressing different aspects of behavioral control like SST assessing motor control (42), gambling tasks assessing reward learning (227), and devaluation tasks assessing goal-directed behavior (228). Concerning the latter, and in parallel to the theory that addiction is characterized by a transition from goal-directed toward habitual behavior, a direct comparison would be very valuable to determine whether ADHD individuals show a similar shift in activation of brain areas governing goal-directed/habitual behavior as is often observed in SUD individuals [e.g., Sjoerds et al. (229)]. In addition, future studies may investigate whether factors that are known to elicit habitual behavior [e.g., stress, Schwabe and Wolf (230)] are equally effective in these populations in affecting brain activation and associated goal-directed behavior.

Further key questions concern explaining the inconsistent observations of DAT levels in SUD. For example, it may be argued that if DAT levels are a consequence of drug use, different drugs may affect DAT levels to a different extent. Drugs exerting their effects through strong activation of catecholaminergic neurotransmission (e.g., amphetamine, cocaine) may induce downregulation of DAT, while drugs like cannabis and heroin may do this to a lesser extent. Within that context, most evidence for the efficacy of MPH in SUD comes from studies in stimulant use disorder patients. It appears reasonable that SUD involving catecholaminergic drugs (e.g., cocaine, amphetamine) may benefit most from MPH treatment. It remains an empirical question whether all addictive disorders are equally suitable to be treated with MPH. It is assumed that all addictions share underlying neurobiological alterations, and based on that notion, it can be hypothesized that MPH is potentially effective in all forms of addiction. However, such extrapolation should be made carefully and awaits empirical confirmation, as most studies showing efficacy in SUD only concern stimulant use disorder patients.

If subpopulations within SUD can be identified, treatments may be tailored to these groups such that MPH treatment may be suitable for one but not the other group or that different dosages may be needed. As well as SUD subgroups, ADHD subgroups (inattentive, impulsive, combined) should be considered. It may be predicted that the impulsive type shares most behavioral and neurobiological characteristics compared with the inattentive type. However, the efficacy of MPH in potential subgroups remains an empirical question that future studies should answer.

Another characterization that may define a suitable sub-population of patients may be neuroimaging measures of the dopamine system. For example, cocaine-dependent patients that show high D2/3 receptor binding and dopamine release following MPH choose a monetary incentive over cocaine more often compared with patients showing less D2/3 binding and dopamine release (231). Such methods may even be hypothesized to assess sensitivity to treatment effects on an individual level. It has been argued that individual tonic dopamine levels are associated with the clinical effectiveness of treatments (21). Such variability in tonic dopamine levels can further be utilized to define individual treatment needs. For example, monetary rewards can be an effective reward for cocaine (231, 232) and smoking (233) abstinence. It is an outstanding hypothesis that the level of dopamine responding to MPH or D2/3 receptor binding can define the height of the monetary incentive, such that lower dopamine system level responding requires larger rewards. In conclusion, more detailed information concerning subgroups, individual differences, and other boundary conditions is needed to determine the suitability of MPH in treating SUD.

The current paper is limited in its scope in evaluating the potential use of MPH as treatment for SUD. Nonetheless, alternative approaches to treating SUD should be considered. Thus far, part of the rationale presented in the current paper to treat SUD patients with MPH is based on a current theoretical explanation of addiction (31, 32). Addiction-related cues elicit a DA response in the ventral striatum, which is associated with an intense craving (“wanting”) of the drug. MPH is theorized to be able to reduce the phasic DA response and therefore the craving. An alternative approach to the function of DA and the mesolimbic system is one in which the system provides a learning signal whenever a reward is larger than predicted [reward-prediction error; (234)]. This signal aims to strengthen the association between the stimulus, response, and outcome (235). In rodents (236) and humans (237, 238), high doses of nicotine have been observed to enhance this signal, which may consequently continue the learning of and strengthen the associations between smoking cigarettes and obtaining reward. An effective treatment may be the dampening of the DA signal whenever a cigarette is smoked to reduce the positive reward-prediction error. MPH has been shown to reduce ventral striatal activation in response to receiving a reward (198). In addition, psilocybin (a hallucinogenic substance found in “magic mushrooms”) may also reduce phasic DA neurotransmission in the mesolimbic pathway. Psilocybin is a 5-HT2A agonist, which predominantly is expressed in the mesolimbic pathways. There is consensus that 5-HT2A activation inhibits DA release (239). Therefore, it can be hypothesized that psilocybin administration leads to a reduced positive reward-prediction error and the association between substance taking and reward. Future, studies should test these hypotheses derived from this rationale.

Concerning alternative approaches, non-invasive brain stimulation or neurofeedback increases prefrontal activity, reduces impulsivity, and enhances cognitive functions (14, 88, 140) and may therefore be considered a potential treatment. In addition, for drug-dependent individuals it is important to train psychosocial skills alongside pharmacological treatment to ensure abstinence. Learning new coping skills, developing a new support system, and challenging expectations about drugs are important factors that enhance self-regulation abilities (156).

Taken together, this perspectives paper provides evidence toward a dimensional approach of psychopathology and serves as an illustration for the promise of developing transdiagnostic treatment programs. More specifically, it contributes to the development of novel pharmacological treatment approaches that may be based on treatments for disorders that are similar in underlying etiology (9). The transdiagnostic symptoms of disinhibition and impulsivity that are characteristic of both SUD and ADHD may have overlapping underlying etiology, namely, abnormal tonic/high phasic DA transmission that leads to a strong drive to perform a given action. This behavior may be associated with prefrontal hypoactivity and brain structural deficits. The DA transmission deficit can be treated with MPH, which has been proven successful in ADHD and may be suitable for use in SUD. The key question is whether the maladaptive behaviors in ADHD that can be treated with MPH are indeed resulting from the DA atypicalities that are shared by both conditions. Also, it is clear that ADHD and SUD are not the same, and it should be studied whether the neurobiological differences underlie other aspects of the respective phenotypes.

Author contributions

PR and LF contributed to establish the first draft of the manuscript and contributed to developing the rationale. LF summarized the existing studies. PR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. NM, JR, and PS contributed to the manuscript in its current version. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM 5. Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Association. (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Wittchen HU, Jacobi F, Rehm J, Gustavsson A, Svensson M, Jonsson B, et al. The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe (2010). Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2011) 21:655–79. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. American Psychiatric Association. The American Psychiatric Association Practice Guideline for the Pharmacological Treatment of Patients With Alcohol use Disorder. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. (2018).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

4. Rudd RA, Seth P, David F, Scholl L. Increases in drug and opioid-involved overdose deaths - United States, 2010–2015. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. (2016) 65:1445–52. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm655051e1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Miller WR, Walters ST, Bennett ME. How effective is alcoholism treatment in the United States? J Stud Alcohol. (2001) 62:211–20. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Fleury MJ, Djouini A, Huynh C, Tremblay J, Ferland F, Menard JM, et al. Remission from substance use disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2016) 168:293–306. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.08.625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Widiger TA. A dimensional model of psychopathology. Psychopathology. (2005) 38:211–4. doi: 10.1159/000086094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry. (2014) 13:28–35. doi: 10.1002/wps.20087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Robbins TW, Gillan CM, Smith DG, De Wit S, Ersche KD. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wilens TE, Fusillo S. When ADHD and substance use disorders intersect: relationship and treatment implications. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2007) 9:408–14. doi: 10.1007/s11920-007-0053-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Li CS, Luo X, Yan P, Bergquist K, Sinh AR. Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2009) 33:740–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00891.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD–a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behav Brain Res. (2002) 130:29–36. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00432-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sallard E, Mouthon M, De Pretto M, Spierer L. Modulation of inhibitory control by prefrontal anodal tDCS: a crossover double-blind sham-controlled fMRI study. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0194936. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Livesey EJ, Livesey DJ. Validation of a Bayesian adaptive estimation technique in the stop-signal task. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0165525. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:926–36. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Eagle DM, Wong JC, Allan ME, Mar AC, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles for dopamine D1 and D2 receptor subtypes in the dorsomedial striatum but not the nucleus accumbens core during behavioral inhibition in the stop-signal task in rats. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:7349–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6182-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Groman SM, Jentsch JD. Cognitive control and the dopamine D(2)-like receptor: a dimensional understanding of addiction. Depress Anxiety. (2012) 29:295–306. doi: 10.1002/da.20897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Badgaiyan RD, Sinha S, Sajjad M, Wack DS. Attenuated tonic and enhanced phasic release of dopamine in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0137326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Faraone SV. The pharmacology of amphetamine and methylphenidate: Relevance to the neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 87:255–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Butler K, Le Foll B. Impact of substance use disorder pharmacotherapy on executive function: a narrative review. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:98. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Montgomery AJ, Asselin MC, Farde L, Grasby PM. Measurement of methylphenidate-induced change in extrastriatal dopamine concentration using [11C]FLB 457 PET. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. (2007) 27:369–77. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Wigal T, Kollins SH, Newcorn JH, Telang F, et al. Long-term stimulant treatment affects brain dopamine transporter level in patients with attention deficit hyperactive disorder. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e63023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Goldstein RZ, Woicik PA, Maloney T, Tomasi D, Alia-Klein N, Shan J, et al. Oral methylphenidate normalizes cingulate activity in cocaine addiction during a salient cognitive task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2010) 107:16667–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011455107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Li CS, Morgan PT, Matuskey D, Abdelghany O, Luo X, Chang JL, et al. Biological markers of the effects of intravenous methylphenidate on improving inhibitory control in cocaine-dependent patients. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2010) 107:14455–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002467107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Swanson JM. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications. Mol Psychiatry. (2004) 9:557–69. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sharma A, Couture J. A review of the pathophysiology, etiology, and treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Ann Pharmacother. (2014) 48:209–25. doi: 10.1177/1060028013510699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cortese S. The neurobiology and genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): what every clinician should know. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. (2012) 16:422–33. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2012.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Arnsten AF, Pliszka SR. Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical function: relevance to treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and related disorders. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2011) 99:211–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Churchwell JC, Kesner RP. Hippocampal-prefrontal dynamics in spatial working memory: interactions and independent parallel processing. Behav Brain Res. (2011) 225:389–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am Psychol. (2016) 71:670–9. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (1993) 18:247–91. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:1481–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Drug addiction: updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu Rev Psychol. (2016) 67:23–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fields S, Edens JF, Smith ST, Rulseh A, Donnellan MB, Ruiz MA, et al. Examining the psychometric properties of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Brief Form in justice-involved samples. Psychol Assess. (2015) 27:1211–8. doi: 10.1037/a0039109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Ireland JL, Archer J. Impulsivity among adult prisoners: A confirmatory factor analysis study of the Barratt Impulsivity Scale. Pers Individ Dif. (2008) 45:286–92. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kertesz A, Nicholson I, Cancelliere A, Kassa K, Black SE. Motor impersistence: a right-hemisphere syndrome. Neurology. (1985) 35:662–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.35.5.662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hallett PE. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res. (1978) 18:1279–96. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90218-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Bellato A, Arora I, Kochhar P, Hollis C, Groom MJ. Indices of heart rate variability and performance during a response-conflict task are differently associated with ADHD and autism. J Atten Disord. (2022) 26:434–46. doi: 10.1177/1087054720972793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Watson RT, Miller BD, Heilman KM. Nonsensory neglect. Ann Neurol. (1978) 3:505–8. doi: 10.1002/ana.410030609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Trommer BL, Hoeppner JA, Lorber R, Armstrong KJ. The go-no-go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol. (1988) 24:610–4. doi: 10.1002/ana.410240504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. (1984) 10:276–91. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Carter JD, Farrow M, Silberstein RB, Stough C, Tucker A, Pipingas A. Assessing inhibitory control: a revised approach to the stop signal task. J Atten Disord. (2003) 6:153–61. doi: 10.1177/108705470300600402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Nandagopal JJ, Fleck DE, Adler CM, Mills NP, Strakowski SM, Delbello MP. Impulsivity in adolescents with bipolar disorder and/or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and healthy controls as measured by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2011) 21:465–8. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Onandia-Hinchado I, Pardo-Palenzuela N, Diaz-Orueta U. Cognitive characterization of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by domains: a systematic review. J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2021) 128:893–937. doi: 10.1007/s00702-021-02302-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, Van Engeland H. A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control? J Abnorm Psychol. (2005) 114:216–22. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Pievsky MA, Mcgrath RE. The neurocognitive profile of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of meta-analyses. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2018) 33:143–57. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Kofler MJ. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2007) 35:745–58. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9131-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Schachar R, Tannock R, Marriott M, Logan G. Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1995) 23:411–37. doi: 10.1007/BF01447206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Schachar R, Mota VL, Logan GD, Tannock R, Klim P. Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2000) 28:227–35. doi: 10.1023/A:1005140103162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chmielewski W, Bluschke A, Bodmer B, Wolff N, Roessner V, Beste C. Evidence for an altered architecture and a hierarchical modulation of inhibitory control processes in ADHD. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 36:100623. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hellrung H, Grunling C, Overmeyer S, Ligges M, Weiler HT, Mentzel HJ, et al. Inhibitory control in adolescent boys with attention-deficit-/hyperactivity disorder: a fMRI study. Neuroimage. (2001) 13:S319–S319. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91662-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Dillo W, Goke A, Prox-Vagedes V, Szycik GR, Roy M, Donnerstag F, et al. Neuronal correlates of ADHD in adults with evidence for compensation strategies–a functional MRI study with a Go/No-Go paradigm. Ger Med Sci. (2010) 8:09. doi: 10.3205/000098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Everling S, Fischer B. The antisaccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies. Neuropsychologia. (1998) 36:885–99. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00020-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Klein CH, Raschke A, Brandenbusch A. Development of pro- and antisaccades in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. Psychophysiology. (2003) 40:17–28. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Houghton S, Douglas G, West J, Whiting K, Wall M, Langsford S, et al. Differential patterns of executive function in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder according to gender and subtype. J Child Neurol. (1999) 14:801–5. doi: 10.1177/088307389901401206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Dev Neuropsychol. (2005) 27:107–33. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2701_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Mahone EM, Powell SK, Loftis CW, Goldberg MC, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Motor persistence and inhibition in autism and ADHD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2006) 12:622–31. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kolodny T, Mevorach C, Stern P, Ankaoua M, Dankner Y, Tsafrir S, et al. Are attention and cognitive control altered by fMRI scanner environment? Evidence from Go/No-go tasks in ADHD. Brain Imaging Behav. (2022) 16:1003–13. doi: 10.1007/s11682-021-00557-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rubio G, Jimenez M, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Martinez I, Avila C, Ferre F, et al. The role of behavioral impulsivity in the development of alcohol dependence: a 4-year follow-up study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2008) 32:1681–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00746.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Yan WS, Chen RT, Liu MM, Zheng DH. Monetary reward discounting, inhibitory control, and trait impulsivity in young adults with internet gaming disorder and nicotine dependence. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:628933. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.628933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Whelan R, Watts R, Orr CA, Althoff RR, Artiges E, Banaschewski T, et al. Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature. (2014) 512:185. doi: 10.1038/nature13402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, et al. Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2006) 45:468–75. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Lespine L-F, Rueda-Delgado L, Vahey N, Ruddy K, Kiiski H, Enz N, et al. Changes in inhibition-related brain function and psychological flexibility during smoking abstinence predict longer time to relapse. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. (2022). doi: 10.31234/osf.io/472p5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Jollans L, Whelan R. Predicting adolescent smoking using fMRI a possible predisposing role for inhibitory control and reward processing in addictive behaviours. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2017) 27:S1079–80. doi: 10.1016/S0924-977X(17)31878-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Abbott S, Craig KJ, Muller U, Suckling J, et al. Response perseveration in stimulant dependence is associated with striatal dysfunction and can be ameliorated by a D(2/3) receptor agonist. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:754–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kanen JW, Ersche KD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Cardinal RN. Computational modelling reveals contrasting effects on reinforcement learning and cognitive flexibility in stimulant use disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: remediating effects of dopaminergic D2/3 receptor agents. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2019) 236:2337–58. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05325-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lim TV, Cardinal RN, Savulich G, Jones PS, Moustafa AA, Robbins TW, et al. Impairments in reinforcement learning do not explain enhanced habit formation in cocaine use disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2019) 236:2359–71. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05330-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Bagci B, Dusmez S, Zorlu N, Bahtiyar G, Isikli S, Bayrakci A, et al. Computational analysis of probabilistic reversal learning deficits in male subjects with alcohol use disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:960238. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.960238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Chronic cocaine but not chronic amphetamine use is associated with perseverative responding in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2008) 197:421–31. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1051-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Spinella M. Correlations between orbitofrontal dysfunction and tobacco smoking. Addict Biol. (2002) 7:381–4. doi: 10.1080/1355621021000005964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Froeliger B, Mcconnell PA, Bell S, Sweitzer M, Kozink RV, Eichberg C, et al. Association between baseline corticothalamic-mediated inhibitory control and smoking relapse vulnerability. JAMA Psychiat. (2017) 74:379–86. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Butler K, Rusted J, Gard P, Jackson A. Performance monitoring in nicotine dependence: Considering integration of recent reinforcement history. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2017) 156:63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2017.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Mcnamee RL, Dunfee KL, Luna B, Clark DB, Eddy WF, Tarter RE. Brain activation, response inhibition, and increased risk for substance use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2008) 32:405–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00604.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Wilson S, Malone SM, Venables NC, Mcgue M, Iacono WG. Multimodal indicators of risk for and consequences of substance use disorders: executive functions and trait disconstraint assessed from preadolescence into early adulthood. Int J Psychophysiol. (2021) 163:47–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Chung T, Geier C, Luna B, Pajtek S, Terwilliger R, Thatcher D, et al. Enhancing response inhibition by incentive: comparison of adolescents with and without substance use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2011) 115:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Tervo-Clemmens B, Quach A, Luna B, Foran W, Chung T, De Bellis MD, et al. Neural correlates of rewarded response inhibition in youth at risk for problematic alcohol use. Front Behav Neurosci. (2017) 11:205. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Gerhardt S, Luderer M, Bumb JM, Sobanski E, Moggi F, Kiefer F, et al. Stop What you're doing!-An fMRI study on comparisons of neural subprocesses of response inhibition in ADHD and alcohol use disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:691930. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.691930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Wu J, Xiao H, Sun H, Zou L, Zhu LQ. Role of dopamine receptors in ADHD: a systematic meta-analysis. Mol Neurobiol. (2012) 45:605–20. doi: 10.1007/s12035-012-8278-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Prince J. Catecholamine dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an update. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2008) 28:S39–45. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e318174f92a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Grace AA. The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation: its relevance for understanding how stimulant abuse can alter basal ganglia function. Drug Alcohol Depend. (1995) 37:111–29. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)01066-T

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Levy F, Swanson JM. Timing, space and ADHD: the dopamine theory revisited. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2001) 35:504–11. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00923.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Heijtz RD, Kolb B, Forssberg H. Motor inhibitory role of dopamine D1 receptors: implications for ADHD. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:155–60. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Levy F, Dadds MR. Stimulant side effects: prefrontal/basal ganglia circuit control at dopamine D1/D2 receptors. Australas Psychiatry. (2014) 22:179–82. doi: 10.1177/1039856213517948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Madras BK, Miller GM, Fischman AJ. The dopamine transporter: relevance to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Res. (2002) 130:57–63. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00439-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Madras BK, Dougherty DD, Bonab AA, Livni E, et al. Further evidence of dopamine transporter dysregulation in ADHD: a controlled PET imaging study using altropane. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 62:1059–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Newcorn J, Fowler JS, Telang F, Solanto MV, et al. Brain dopamine transporter levels in treatment and drug naive adults with ADHD. Neuroimage. (2007) 34:1182–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Bluschke A, Friedrich J, Schreiter ML, Roessner V, Beste C. A comparative study on the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying effects of methylphenidate and neurofeedback on inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clin. (2018) 20:1191–203. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Crunelle CL, Van Den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Van Emmerik-Van Oortmerssen K, Dom G, Schoevers RA, et al. Low dopamine transporter occupancy by methylphenidate as a possible reason for reduced treatment effectiveness in ADHD patients with cocaine dependence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2013) 23:1714–23. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Chen BT, Hopf FW, Bonci A. Synaptic plasticity in the mesolimbic system: therapeutic implications for substance abuse. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2010) 1187:129–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Nutt DJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Erritzoe D, Stokes PR. The dopamine theory of addiction: 40 years of highs and lows. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:305–12. doi: 10.1038/nrn3939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Trick L, Butler K, Chukwueke C, Di Ciano P, Ibrahim C, Rubin-Kahana DS, et al. Abnormalities of Neurotransmission in Drug Addiction. In:Dierckx RA, Otte A, DE Vries EFJ, Van Waarde A, Sommer IE, , editor. PET and SPECT in Psychiatry. Cham: Springer. (2021). doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-57231-0_21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Solinas M Belujon P Fernagut PO Jaber M Thiriet Dopamine N and and addiction: what have we learned from 40 years of research. J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2019) 126:481–516. doi: 10.1007/s00702-018-1957-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Hancock SD, Mckim WA. Drug and Behavior: An Introduction to Behavioral Pharmacology. New York, Pearson education. (2018).

Google Scholar

95. Bloomfield MA, Morgan CJ, Kapur S, Curran HV, Howes OD. The link between dopamine function and apathy in cannabis users: an [18F]-DOPA PET imaging study. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2014) 231:2251–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3523-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Van De Giessen E, Weinstein JJ, Cassidy CM, Haney M, Dong Z, Ghazzaoui R, et al. Deficits in striatal dopamine release in cannabis dependence. Mol Psychiatry. (2017) 22:68–75. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.21

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Urban NB, Slifstein M, Thompson JL, Xu X, Girgis RR, Raheja S, et al. Dopamine release in chronic cannabis users: a [11c]raclopride positron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 71:677–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:6583–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. Neuroimage. (2008) 39:1266–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Weber SC, Beck-Schimmer B, Kajdi ME, Muller D, Tobler PN, Quednow BB. Dopamine D2/3- and mu-opioid receptor antagonists reduce cue-induced responding and reward impulsivity in humans. Transl Psychiatry. (2016) 6:e850. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F. Addiction: beyond dopamine reward circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2011) 108:15037–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010654108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Albrecht DS, Skosnik PD, Vollmer JM, Brumbaugh MS, Perry KM, Mock BH, et al. Striatal D(2)/D(3) receptor availability is inversely correlated with cannabis consumption in chronic marijuana users. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2013) 128:52–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Wang GJ, Smith L, Volkow ND, Telang F, Logan J, Tomasi D, et al. Decreased dopamine activity predicts relapse in methamphetamine abusers. Mol Psychiatry. (2012) 17:918–25. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Luijten M, Veltman DJ, Hester R, Smits M, Nijs IM, Pepplinkhuizen L, et al. The role of dopamine in inhibitory control in smokers and non-smokers: a pharmacological fMRI study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2013) 23:1247–56. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Hitzemannn R, Gatley SJ, et al. Cocaine uptake is decreased in the brain of detoxified cocaine abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology. (1996) 14:159–68. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00073-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Mash DC, Pablo J, Ouyang Q, Hearn WL, Izenwasser S. Dopamine transport function is elevated in cocaine users. J Neurochem. (2002) 81:292–300. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.00820.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Chang L, Alicata D, Ernst T, Volkow N. Structural metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse. Addiction. (2007) 102 Suppl 1:16–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01782.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Leroy C, Karila L, Martinot JL, Lukasiewicz M, Duchesnay E, Comtat C, et al. Striatal and extrastriatal dopamine transporter in cannabis and tobacco addiction: a high-resolution PET study. Addict Biol. (2012) 17:981–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00356.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Lyvers M. “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2000) 8:225–49. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.8.2.225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Berridge KC. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. Eur J Neurosci. (2012) 35:1124–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Viggiano D, Grammatikopoulos G, Sadile AG. A morphometric evidence for a hyperfunctioning mesolimbic system in an animal model of ADHD. Behav Brain Res. (2002) 130:181–9. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00423-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Bjork JM, Chen G, Smith AR, Hommer DW. Incentive-elicited mesolimbic activation and externalizing symptomatology in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2010) 51:827–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02201.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Chase HW, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Hogarth L. The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:785–93. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Abnormal psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill Education. (2020).

Google Scholar

115. Tang DW, Hello B, Mroziewicz M, Fellows LK, Tyndale RF, Dagher A. Genetic variation in CYP2A6 predicts neural reactivity to smoking cues as measured using fMRI. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:2136–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Leyton M, Vezina. Striatal ups and downs: their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37(9 Pt A):1999–2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Vollstadt-Klein S, Loeber S, Kirsch M, Bach P, Richter A, Buhler M, et al. Effects of cue-exposure treatment on neural cue reactivity in alcohol dependence: a randomized trial. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 69:1060–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Vollstadt-Klein S, Wichert S, Rabinstein J, Buhler M, Klein O, Ende G, et al. Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction. (2010) 105:1741–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Kaag AM, Reneman L, Homberg J, Van Den Brink W, Van Wingen GA. Enhanced amygdala-striatal functional connectivity during the processing of cocaine cues in male cocaine users with a history of childhood trauma. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9:70. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Filbey FM, Schacht JP, Myers US, Chavez RS, Hutchison KE. Marijuana craving in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2009) 106:13016–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903863106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Filbey FM, Dunlop J, Ketcherside A, Baine J, Rhinehardt T, Kuhn B, et al. fMRI study of neural sensitization to hedonic stimuli in long-term, daily cannabis users. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:3431–43. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Grusser SM, Wrase J, Klein S, Hermann D, Smolka MN, Ruf M, et al. Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2004) 175:296–302. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1828-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Heinz A, Wrase J, Kahnt T, Beck A, Bromand Z, Grusser SM, et al. Brain activation elicited by affectively positive stimuli is associated with a lower risk of relapse in detoxified alcoholic subjects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2007) 31:1138–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00406.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Wrase J, Grusser SM, Klein S, Diener C, Hermann D, Flor H, et al. Development of alcohol-associated cues and cue-induced brain activation in alcoholics. Eur Psychiatry. (2002) 17:287–91. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(02)00676-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Hong JS, Kim SM, Jung HY, Kang KD, Min KJ, Han DH. Cognitive avoidance and aversive cues related to tobacco in male smokers. Addict Behav. (2017) 73:158–64. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Rubinstein ML, Luks TL, Moscicki AB, Dryden W, Rait MA, Simpson GV. Smoking-related cue-induced brain activation in adolescent light smokers. J Adolesc Health. (2011) 48:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Wagner DD, Dal Cin S, Sargent JD, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF. Spontaneous action representation in smokers when watching movie characters smoke. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:894–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5174-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Vollstadt-Klein S, Kobiella A, Buhler M, Graf C, Fehr C, Mann K, et al. Severity of dependence modulates smokers' neuronal cue reactivity and cigarette craving elicited by tobacco advertisement. Addict Biol. (2011) 16:166–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00207.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. De Sousa Fernandes Perna EB, Theunissen EL, Kuypers KP, Evers EA, Stiers P, Toennes SW, et al. Brain reactivity to alcohol and cannabis marketing during sobriety and intoxication. Addict Biol. (2017) 22:823–32. doi: 10.1111/adb.12351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Claus ED, Ewing SW, Filbey FM, Sabbineni A, Hutchison KE. Identifying neurobiological phenotypes associated with alcohol use disorder severity. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2011) 36:2086–96. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Kaag AM, Wiers RW, De Vries TJ, Pattij T, Goudriaan AE. Striatal alcohol cue-reactivity is stronger in male than female problem drinkers. Eur J Neurosci. (2019) 50:2264–73. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Li CS, Yan P, Sinha R, Lee TW. Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task. Neuroimage. (2008) 41:1352–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Weafer J, Gorka SM, Hedeker D, Dzemidzic M, Kareken DA, Phan KL, et al. Associations Between Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Inhibitory Control and Amphetamine Reward Sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:1905–13. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Nandam LS, Hester R, Wagner J, Dean AJ, Messer C, Honeysett A, et al. Dopamine D(2) receptor modulation of human response inhibition and error awareness. J Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 25:649–56. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Kelly AM, Hester R, Murphy K, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ, Garavan H. Prefrontal-subcortical dissociations underlying inhibitory control revealed by event-related fMRI. Eur J Neurosci. (2004) 19:3105–12. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03429.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW. Fronto-striatal circuits in response-inhibition: Relevance to addiction. Brain Res. (2015) 1628:117–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Tsuchida A, Fellows LK. Are core component processes of executive function dissociable within the frontal lobes? Evidence from humans with focal prefrontal damage. Cortex. (2013) 49:1790–800. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Solanto MV, Schulz KP, Fan J, Tang CY, Newcorn JH. Event-related FMRI of inhibitory control in the predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes of ADHD. J Neuroimaging. (2009) 19:205–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2008.00289.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Qiu MG, Ye Z, Li QY, Liu GJ, Xie B, Wang J. Changes of brain structure and function in ADHD children. Brain Topogr. (2011) 24:243–52. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0168-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Allenby C, Falcone M, Bernardo L, Wileyto EP, Rostain A, Ramsay JR, et al. Transcranial direct current brain stimulation decreases impulsivity in ADHD. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:974–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:1051–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Kollins SH, Adcock RA. ADHD, altered dopamine neurotransmission, and disrupted reinforcement processes: implications for smoking and nicotine dependence. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 52:70–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Toone B, Taylor E. Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:1067–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Mulligan RC, Knopik VS, Sweet LH, Fischer M, Seidenberg M, Rao SM. Neural correlates of inhibitory control in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from the Milwaukee longitudinal sample. Psychiatry Res. (2011) 194:119–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Spreng RN, Sepulcre J, Turner GR, Stevens WD, Schacter DL. Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 25:74–86. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Breukelaar IA, Williams LM, Antees C, Grieve SM, Foster SL, Gomes L, et al. Cognitive ability is associated with changes in the functional organization of the cognitive control brain network. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:5028–38. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Chen M, Wu YJ, Wu J, Fu Y, Li S, Liu H, et al. Individual differences in inhibitory control abilities modulate the functional neuroplasticity of inhibitory control. Brain Struct Funct. (2019) 224:2357–71. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01911-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Cole MW, Schneider W. The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage. (2007) 37:343–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Faraone SV, Rohde LA. Adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: key conceptual issues. Lancet Psychiatry. (2016) 3:568–78. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30032-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Hoekzema E, Carmona S, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Richarte Fernandez V, Bosch R, Soliva JC, et al. An independent components and functional connectivity analysis of resting state fMRI data points to neural network dysregulation in adult ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:1261–72. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, Kirsch A, et al. Cingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 63:332–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Liddle EB, Hollis C, Batty MJ, Groom MJ, Totman JJ, Liotti M, et al. Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: effects of motivation and methylphenidate. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2011) 52:761–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Klugah-Brown B, Di X, Zweerings J, Mathiak K, Becker B, Biswal B. Common separable neural alterations in substance use disorders: a coordinate-based meta-analyses of functional neuroimaging studies in humans. Hum Brain Mapp. (2020) 41:4459–77. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Elton A, Young J, Smitherman S, Gross RE, Mletzko T, Kilts CD. Neural network activation during a stop-signal task discriminates cocaine-dependent from non-drug-abusing men. Addict Biol. (2014) 19:427–38. doi: 10.1111/adb.12011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Feil J, Sheppard D, Fitzgerald PB, Yucel M, Lubman DI, Bradshaw JL. Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2010) 35:248–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Volkow ND, Koob GF, Mclellan AT. Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. N Engl J Med. (2016) 374:363–71. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1511480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Nestor LJ, Ghahremani DG, Monterosso J, London ED. Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res. (2011) 194:287–95. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Anderson BM, Stevens MC, Meda SA, Jordan K, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD. Functional imaging of cognitive control during acute alcohol intoxication. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2011) 35:156–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01332.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Howard JD, Reynolds R, Smith DE, Voss JL, Schoenbaum G, Kahnt T. Targeted stimulation of human orbitofrontal networks disrupts outcome-guided behavior. Curr Biol. (2020) 30:490–8 e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, Turton AJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science. (2012) 335:601–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1214463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Castellanos FX. Anatomic magnetic resonance imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2002) 4:444–8. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2002.4.4/fxcastellanos

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Mostofsky SH, Cooper KL, Kates WR, Denckla MB, Kaufmann WE. Smaller prefrontal and premotor volumes in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2002) 52:785–94. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01412-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SC, Simmons A, et al. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study with functional MRI. Am J Psychiatry. (1999) 156:891–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.6.891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N. Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1263–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Wolosin SM, Richardson ME, Hennessey JG, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Abnormal cerebral cortex structure in children with ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:175–84. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Durston S, Fossella JA, Casey BJ, Hulshoff Pol HE, Galvan A, Schnack HG, et al. Differential effects of DRD4 and DAT1 genotype on fronto-striatal gray matter volumes in a sample of subjects with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, their unaffected siblings, and controls. Mol Psychiatry. (2005) 10:678–85. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Shaw P, Gornick M, Lerch J, Addington A, Seal J, Greenstein D, et al. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome, and cortical structure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:921–31. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2006) 63:540–9. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW, Peterson BS. Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. (2003) 362:1699–707. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14842-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Pando-Naude V, Toxto S, Fernandez-Lozano S, Parsons CE, Alcauter S, Garza-Villarreal EA. Gray and white matter morphology in substance use disorders: a neuroimaging systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl Psychiatry. (2021) 11:29. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-01128-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Mackey S, Allgaier N, Chaarani B, Spechler P, Orr C, Bunn J, et al. Mega-analysis of gray matter volume in substance dependence: general and substance-specific regional effects. Am J Psychiatry. (2019) 176:119–28. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17040415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Mackey S, Chaarani B, Kan KJ, Spechler PA, Orr C, Banaschewski T, et al. Brain regions related to impulsivity mediate the effects of early adversity on antisocial behavior. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 82:275–82. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. (2000) 123:2189–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Holroyd CB, Yeung N. Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:122–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Bednarski SR, Zhang S, Hong KI, Sinha R, Rounsaville BJ, Li CS. Deficits in default mode network activity preceding error in cocaine dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2011) 119:e51–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Matuskey D, Luo X, Zhang S, Morgan PT, Abdelghany O, Malison RT, et al. Methylphenidate remediates error-preceding activation of the default mode brain regions in cocaine-addicted individuals. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 214:116–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Goldstein RZ, Alia-Klein N, Tomasi D, Carrillo JH, Maloney T, Woicik PA, et al. Anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivations to an emotionally salient task in cocaine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2009) 106:9453–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900491106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Oral methylphenidate normalizes cingulate activity and decreases impulsivity in cocaine addiction during an emotionally salient cognitive task. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2011) 36:366–7. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Moeller SJ, Honorio J, Tomasi D, Parvaz MA, Woicik PA, Volkow ND, et al. Methylphenidate enhances executive function and optimizes prefrontal function in both health and cocaine addiction. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:643–53. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Tomasi D, Telang F, Fowler JS, Pradhan K, et al. Methylphenidate attenuates limbic brain inhibition after cocaine-cues exposure in cocaine abusers. PLoS ONE. (2010) 5:e11509. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Chan B, Freeman M, Kondo K, Ayers C, Montgomery J, Paynter R, et al. Pharmacotherapy for methamphetamine/amphetamine use disorder-a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. (2019) 114:2122–36. doi: 10.1111/add.14755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Dursteler KM, Berger EM, Strasser J, Caflisch C, Mutschler J, Herdener M, et al. Clinical potential of methylphenidate in the treatment of cocaine addiction: a review of the current evidence. Subst Abuse Rehabil. (2015) 6:61–74. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S50807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Ling W, Chang L, Hillhouse M, Ang A, Striebel J, Jenkins J, et al. Sustained-release methylphenidate in a randomized trial of treatment of methamphetamine use disorder. Addiction. (2014) 109:1489–500. doi: 10.1111/add.12608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Miles SW, Sheridan J, Russell B, Kydd R, Wheeler A, Walters C, et al. Extended-release methylphenidate for treatment of amphetamine/methamphetamine dependence: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Addiction. (2013) 108:1279–86. doi: 10.1111/add.12109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Rezaei F, Emami M, Zahed S, Morabbi MJ, Farahzadi M, Akhondzadeh S. Sustained-release methylphenidate in methamphetamine dependence treatment: a double-blind and placebo-controlled trial. Daru. (2015) 23:2. doi: 10.1186/s40199-015-0092-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Soares E, Pereira FC. Pharmacotherapeutic strategies for methamphetamine use disorder: mind the subgroups. Expert Opin Pharmacother. (2019) 20:2273–93. doi: 10.1080/14656566.2019.1681970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Levin FR, Evans SM, Brooks DJ, Garawi F. Treatment of cocaine dependent treatment seekers with adult ADHD: double-blind comparison of methylphenidate and placebo. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2007) 87:20–9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Tiihonen J, Kuoppasalmi K, Fohr J, Tuomola P, Kuikanmaki O, Vorma H, et al. A comparison of aripiprazole, methylphenidate, and placebo for amphetamine dependence. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:160–2. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.1.160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Minarik J, Gabrhelik R, Malcolm R, Pavlovska A, Miller P. Methylphenidate substitution for methamphetamine addiction and implications for future randomized clinical trials: a unique case series. J Subst Use. (2016) 21:435–8. doi: 10.3109/14659891.2015.1045047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Lee NK, Jenner L, Harney A, Cameron J. Pharmacotherapy for amphetamine dependence: A systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2018) 191:309–37. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.06.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Aryan N, Banafshe HR, Farnia V, Shakeri J, Alikhani M, Rahimi H, et al. The therapeutic effects of methylphenidate and matrix-methylphenidate on addiction severity, craving, relapse and mental health in the methamphetamine use disorder. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. (2020) 15:72. doi: 10.1186/s13011-020-00317-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Dursteler-Macfarland KM, Farronato NS, Strasser J, Boss J, Kuntze MF, Petitjean SA, et al. A randomized, controlled, pilot trial of methylphenidate and cognitive-behavioral group therapy for cocaine dependence in heroin prescription. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2013) 33:104–8. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31827bfff4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Grabowski J, Roache JD, Schmitz JM, Rhoades H, Creson D, Korszun A. Replacement medication for cocaine dependence: methylphenidate. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (1997) 17:485–8. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199712000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Levin FR, Evans SM, Brooks DJ, Kalbag AS, Garawi F, Nunes EV. Treatment of methadone-maintained patients with adult ADHD: double-blind comparison of methylphenidate, bupropion and placebo. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2006) 81:137–48. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. Noroozi A, Motevalian SA, Zarrindast MR, Alaghband-Rad J, Akhondzadeh S. Adding extended-release methylphenidate to psychological intervention for treatment of methamphetamine dependence: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Med J Islam Repub Iran. (2020) 34:137. doi: 10.47176/mjiri.34.137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Schubiner H, Saules KK, Arfken CL, Johanson CE, Schuster CR, Lockhart N, et al. Double-blind placebo-controlled trial of methylphenidate in the treatment of adult ADHD patients with comorbid cocaine dependence. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2002) 10:286–94. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.10.3.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Konova AB, Moeller SJ, Tomasi D, Volkow ND, Goldstein RZ. Effects of methylphenidate on resting-state functional connectivity of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine pathways in cocaine addiction. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:857–68. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Evers EA, Stiers P, Ramaekers JG. High reward expectancy during methylphenidate depresses the dopaminergic response to gain and loss. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:311–8. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Ramaekers JG, Evers EA, Theunissen EL, Kuypers KP, Goulas A, Stiers P. Methylphenidate reduces functional connectivity of nucleus accumbens in brain reward circuit. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2013) 229:219–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3105-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: 'liking', 'wanting', and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. (2009) 9:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:1458–63. doi: 10.1038/nn1584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Van Ruitenbeek P, Quaedflieg CW, Hernaus D, Hartogsveld B, Smeets T. Dopaminergic noradrenergic modulation of stress-induced alterations in brain activation associated with goal-directed behaviour. J Psychopharmacol. (2021) 35:1449–63. doi: 10.1177/02698811211044679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

203. Goldstein RZ, Woicik PA, Moeller SJ, Telang F, Jayne M, Wong C, et al. Liking and wanting of drug and non-drug rewards in active cocaine users: the STRAP-R questionnaire. J Psychopharmacol. (2010) 24:257–66. doi: 10.1177/0269881108096982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Calipari ES, Ferris MJ, Salahpour A, Caron MG, Jones SR. Methylphenidate amplifies the potency and reinforcing effects of amphetamines by increasing dopamine transporter expression. Nat Commun. (2013) 4:2720. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Rush CR, Higgins ST, Vansickel AR, Stoops WW, Lile JA, Glaser PE. Methylphenidate increases cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2005) 181:781–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0021-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Vansickel AR, Stoops WW, Glaser PE, Poole MM, Rush CR. Methylphenidate increases cigarette smoking in participants with ADHD. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2011) 218:381–90. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2328-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Zack M, Poulos CX. Amphetamine primes motivation to gamble and gambling-related semantic networks in problem gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2004) 29:195–207. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. Boileau I, Payer D, Chugani B, Lobo DS, Houle S, Wilson AA, et al. In vivo evidence for greater amphetamine-induced dopamine release in pathological gambling: a positron emission tomography study with [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO. Mol Psychiatry. (2014) 19:1305–13. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

209. Katzman MA, Bilkey TS, Chokka PR, Fallu A, Klassen LJ. Adult ADHD and comorbid disorders: clinical implications of a dimensional approach. BMC Psychiatry. (2017) 17:302. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1463-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

210. Martinez-Raga J, Szerman N, Knecht C, De Alvaro R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and dual disorders. Educational needs for an underdiagnosed condition. Int J Adolesc Med Health. (2013) 25:231–43. doi: 10.1515/ijamh-2013-0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

211. Wilens TE, Morrison NR. The intersection of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance abuse. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2011) 24:280–5. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328345c956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

212. Klassen LJ, Bilkey TS, Katzman MA, Chokka P. Comorbid attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorder: treatment considerations. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. (2012) 5:190–8. doi: 10.2174/1874473711205030190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

213. Yang PB, Atkins KD, Dafny N. Behavioral sensitization and cross-sensitization between methylphenidate amphetamine, and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in female SD rats. Eur J Pharmacol. (2011) 661:72–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2011.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

214. Wooters TE, Walton MT, Bardo MT. Oral methylphenidate establishes a conditioned place preference in rats. Neurosci Lett. (2011) 487:293–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

215. Sharif S, Guirguis A, Fergus S, Schifano F. The use and impact of cognitive enhancers among university students: a systematic review. Brain Sci. (2021) 11. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11030355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

216. Liu H, Feng W, Zhang D. Association of ADHD medications with the risk of cardiovascular diseases: a meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 28:1283–93. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1217-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

217. Nutt D, King LA, Saulsbury W, Blakemore C. Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. Lancet. (2007) 369:1047–53. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60464-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

218. Kapur A. Is methylphenidate beneficial and safe in pharmacological cognitive enhancement? CNS Drugs. (2020) 34:1045–62. doi: 10.1007/s40263-020-00758-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

219. Stoops WW, Glaser PE, Rush CR. Reinforcing, subject-rated, and physiological effects of intranasal methylphenidate in humans: a dose-response analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2003) 71:179–86. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00131-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

220. Kollins SH. Comparing the abuse potential of methylphenidate vs. other stimulants: a review of available evidence and relevance to the ADHD patient. J Clin Psychiatry. (2003) 64 Suppl 11:14–8.

Google Scholar

221. Hill K, Mann L, Laws KR, Stephenson CM, Nimmo-Smith I, Mckenna PJ. Hypofrontality in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2004) 110:243–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00376.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

222. Onitsuka T, Oribe N, Kanba S. Neurophysiological findings in patients with bipolar disorder. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. (2013) 62:197–206. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-7020-5307-8.00013-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

223. Onishi Y, Kikuchi S, Watanabe E, Kato S. Alterations in prefrontal cortical activity in the course of treatment for late-life depression as assessed on near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2008) 62:177–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01752.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

224. Stiers P, Goulas A. Task-specific subnetworks extend from prefrontal cortex to striatum. Cortex. (2022) 156:106–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

225. Luijten M, Schellekens AF, Kuhn S, Machielse MW, Sescousse G. Disruption of reward processing in addiction: an image-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. JAMA Psychiatry. (2017) 74:387–98. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

226. Grieder TE, George O, Tan H, George SR, Le Foll B, Laviolette SR, et al. Phasic D1 and tonic D2 dopamine receptor signaling double dissociate the motivational effects of acute nicotine and chronic nicotine withdrawal. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2012) 109:3101–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114422109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

227. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. (1994) 50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

228. Balleine B, Dickinson A. Instrumental Performance Following Reinforcer Devaluation Depends Upon Incentive Learning. Q J Exp Psychol. (1991) 43:279–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

229. Sjoerds Z, De Wit S, Van Den Brink W, Robbins TW, Beekman AT, Penninx BW, et al. Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl Psychiatry. (2013) 3:e337. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

230. Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:7191–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

231. Martinez D, Carpenter KM, Liu F, Slifstein M, Broft A, Friedman AC, et al. Imaging dopamine transmission in cocaine dependence: link between neurochemistry and response to treatment. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:634–41. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10050748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

232. Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Broft A, et al. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:622–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

233. Van Den Brand FA, Nagelhout GE, Winkens B, Chavannes NH, Van Schayck OCP. Effect of a workplace-based group training programme combined with financial incentives on smoking cessation: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Public Health. (2018) 3:e536–44. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(18)30185-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

234. Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. (2007) 30:203–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

235. Abler B, Walter H, Erk S, Kammerer H, Spitzer M. Prediction error as a linear function of reward probability is coded in human nucleus accumbens. Neuroimage. (2006) 31:790–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

236. Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1988) 85:5274–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

237. Fedota JR, Sutherland MT, Salmeron BJ, Ross TJ, Hong LE, Stein EA. Reward anticipation is differentially modulated by varenicline and nicotine in smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2015) 40:2038–46. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

238. Moran LV, Stoeckel LE, Wang K, Caine CE, Villafuerte R, Calderon V, et al. Nicotine increases activation to anticipatory valence cues in anterior insula and striatum. Nicotine Tob Res. (2018) 20:851–8. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntx217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

239. Howell LL, Cunningham KA. Serotonin 5-HT2 receptor interactions with dopamine function: implications for therapeutics in cocaine use disorder. Pharmacol Rev. (2015) 67:176–97. doi: 10.1124/pr.114.009514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: addiction, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, psychopharmacology, methylphenidate (MPH), cognitive control

Citation: van Ruitenbeek P, Franzen L, Mason NL, Stiers P and Ramaekers JG (2023) Methylphenidate as a treatment option for substance use disorder: a transdiagnostic perspective. Front. Psychiatry 14:1208120. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1208120

Received: 18 April 2023; Accepted: 14 July 2023;
Published: 03 August 2023.

Edited by:

Cristina Núñez, University of Murcia, Spain

Reviewed by:

Eric Andrew Woodcock, Wayne State University, United States
Kevin Butler, University of Sunderland, United Kingdom
Sarah Forster, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, United States

Copyright © 2023 van Ruitenbeek, Franzen, Mason, Stiers and Ramaekers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter van Ruitenbeek, p.vanruitenbeek@maastrichtuniversity.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.