Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 May 2024
Sec. Addictive Disorders

The influence of adverse childhood experiences and depression on addiction severity among methamphetamine users: exploring the role of perseveration

Cheng-Hung KoCheng-Hung Ko1Yung-Chin LuYung-Chin Lu2Chun-Hung Lee,*&#x;Chun-Hung Lee1,3*†Yu-Chi Liao,,*&#x;Yu-Chi Liao4,5,6*†
  • 1Department of Addiction and Forensic Psychiatry, Jianan Psychiatric Center, Ministry of Health and Welfare (MOHW), Tainan, Taiwan
  • 2Department of Clinical Psychology, Jianan Psychiatric Center, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Tainan, Taiwan
  • 3Department of Addiction Psychiatry, Taoyuan Psychiatric Center, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Taoyuan, Taiwan
  • 4Department of Psychology, College of Medical and Health Science, Asia University, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 5Center for Prevention and Treatment of Internet Addiction, Asia University, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 6Clinical Psychology Center, Asia University Hospital, Taichung, Taiwan

Aims: This investigation aimed to clarify the intricate relationship among depression, cognitive function, adverse childhood experiences (ACEs), and their combined influence on methamphetamine use disorder (MUD).

Methods: Utilizing a battery of psychological tests, this study ascertained the impact of ACEs on the condition of 76 people with MUD who meet the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) criteria, aged 42.17 on average. The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), Conners’ Continuous Performance-II (CPT-II), the self-report Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS), and the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) were used for these evaluations. Individuals involved in the study were categorized into two discrete cohorts, mild (ME) and severe (SE), based on the extent of their ACEs exposure. This study employed the PROCESS regression, the independent t-test andχ2 tests for the analysis.

Results: The findings revealed notable discrepancies in the psychological consequences between the two groups with different degrees of ACEs; however, no substantial differences were observed in the demographic parameters. The SE group exhibited elevated BDI-II scores, more evident indications of MUD, and a higher degree of CPT-II cognitive perseveration. The PROCESS model revealed that cognitive perseveration moderated the impact of depression on ACEs and subjective MUD severity, explaining 20.2% of the variance. The ACEs and depression predicted 28.6% of the variance in MUD symptoms. However, no statistically significant differences were detected between the two groups regarding the parameters in the IGT-2 assessment.

Conclusions: These results indicate that the interaction between cognitive and depressive factors mediates the effect of ACEs on subjective MUD severity but not on MUD symptoms. The ACEs significant impact on mental health severity perception is explained by cognitive and depressive factors. This implies that MUD treatment and rehabilitation should address cognitive dysfunction and developmental trauma.

Introduction

Drug addiction is a chronic medical condition with complex cognitive, physiological, and behavioral symptoms related to unintentional drug use (1). Illicit drug use remains a major concern in Taiwan. Over the past decade, Taiwan’s criminal justice system has spent over three billion NTD annually on addiction rehabilitation and drug prevention (2). Taiwan has implemented treatment programs to replace punitive approaches to illegal drug use (3, 4). Contrary to the successful control of opioid drug use, the use of methamphetamine in pursuit of heightened pleasure and increased sexual activity (5, 6) poses a significant challenge for individuals in Taiwan attempting to pursue methamphetamine abstinence. Methamphetamine use can lead to legal issues, high-risk sexual behavior, sexually transmitted infections, withdrawal symptoms, psychosis, depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder (7, 8). Taiwanese methamphetamine users have a higher all-cause mortality rate than the general population (9). Examining why people use non-opioids may assist in helping the users develop effective addiction-coping mechanisms, which is necessary to address these concerns.

In recent years, there has been growing emphasis on analyzing childhood-related factors to gain insights into the initiation and progression of substance abuse and dependence trajectories. Increasing interest has emerged in the significance of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) (10). The ACEs encompass a wide range, including abuse (emotional, physical, or sexual), neglect (emotional or physical), severe household dysfunction (e.g., witnessing domestic violence, household member drug use, and parental separation and incarceration), and peer, community, and collective violence (11). These early life adversities, which are prevalent across diverse social and cultural contexts (12), have been associated with detrimental health and sociobehavioral outcomes throughout an individual’s lifespan (13). The likelihood of engaging in illicit drug use and developing addiction increases according to the cumulative number of ACE categories (14, 15). The impact of ACEs on the risk of developing methamphetamine (METH)-associated psychosis was also found to be substantial, with a notable correlation demonstrating an escalation in methamphetamine-associated psychosis corresponding to an increase in the number of ACEs (16). Moreover, increased recurrent methamphetamine use was associated with severe ACEs (17).

As ACEs may predispose individuals to addiction in adulthood (18, 19), it is imperative to comprehend how these early life experiences induce addiction. Adverse Childhood Experiences have been observed to disrupt emotion regulation, resulting in an impaired ability to effectively manage distressing emotions (20) or coping mechanisms with stressful events (21) in a manner that is both healthy and adaptive. Consequently, individuals may resort to substance use as a coping strategy to alleviate and regulate these emotions (22, 23), explains the association between ACEs and Substance Use Disorder (SUD). Recent evidence suggests a symbiotic relationship between methamphetamine use and depression, where each may exacerbate the vulnerability to the other. Compared with other negative emotions, the influence of depression on drug addiction holds greater clinical significance (24). A previous systematic review and meta-analysis also revealed an association between methamphetamine use and comorbid depression (25). Individuals who use methamphetamine are more likely to experience depression compared to those who do not use the substance, indicate that methamphetamine use is a potential risk factor for depression (26). The intricate relationship between substance abuse and depression is a critical area of study within the realm of addiction research. Understanding this relationship is paramount, given the potential for depression to both precede and result from methamphetamine abuse, thereby creating a vicious cycle that complicates treatment and recovery efforts. Moreover, exposure to adversity during critical periods of development is more prone to enduring rather than temporary effects on neurodevelopment (27). Modification of brain neurodevelopment correlates with ACEs, specifically affecting regions such as the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus (28), which may lead to compromised cognitive functioning (29). Individuals with ACEs exhibit extensive and clinically significant impairments in various cognitive domains, including processing speed, executive function, perceptual reasoning, memory, and verbal comprehension (30). The impact of ACEs on cognitive functioning may extend to later adulthood (31). Cognitive functionality is a potentially valuable area for interventions targeting both clinical and nonclinical individuals who have experienced childhood adversity (32).

Cognitive factors that are explicitly targeted in evidence-based psychotherapies and are malleable, have significant relevance in clinical practice (33). Addiction has been defined as a neurological disorder with remarkable advancements in neuroscience methodologies, and neuropsychological models offer enhanced explanatory frameworks for comprehending addictive behavior (34). These integrated models encompass the Impaired Response Inhibition and Salience Attribution framework (I-RISA) (35) and the Vulnerabilities in Decision-Making model (36). These theoretical frameworks postulate that addiction is associated with poor top-down cognitive control of behavior (executive functions), ultimately impacting the critical stages of the addictive cycle (37). Dysfunctional response inhibition (35); deficits in attentional control, inhibitory control, and behavioral monitoring (38); and impaired decision-making (39) have been generally emphasized as factors that predispose individuals to addiction.

A clear examination of the connections between ACEs and cognition (e.g., inhibitory control and decision-making) in addicted patients (e.g., methamphetamine addicts) has the potential to offer vital insights into strategies for preventing or treating these interrelated risk factors that compromise health. However, despite evidence establishing a connection between ACEs and methamphetamine addiction as well as some possible indications of the underlying mechanisms linking these two factors to adverse neurobiological outcomes, few empirical studies have examined how ACEs uniquely affect neurocognitive function in methamphetamine use disorder (MUD) samples. This study seeks to delve deeper into the interconnected roles of ACEs, depression, and cognitive dysfunction—specifically perseveration, a form of repetitive and rigid thinking—in the severity of methamphetamine addiction. This study categorized individuals with addiction into two distinct groups: Mild exposure (ME) and severe exposure (SE), to identify the factors influenced by childhood adversity. By examining the cumulative impact of these factors, the research aims to uncover the underlying mechanisms that contribute to addiction severity among methamphetamine users. The correlation between ACEs and depression, and their combined effect on addiction severity, necessitates a comprehensive investigation to inform targeted interventions. In addition to investigating executive function and decision-making abilities, this study aimed to incorporate demographic variables and depression levels derived from the literature (16, 24). Given the significant clinical relevance of depression in the context of drug addiction and the potential for ACEs to exacerbate this relationship, this study’s objective is to explore the complex interplay between childhood adversity, depression, perseverative cognitive processes, and addiction severity. By categorizing individuals based on the severity of childhood adversity and examining the influence of depression and cognitive dysfunction, this research aims to establish a predictive model that elucidates the multifaceted dynamics contributing to methamphetamine addiction severity. The findings may offer valuable insights into the development of more effective addiction treatment and prevention strategies, emphasizing the need for addressing mental health disorders and adverse childhood experiences in the context of substance abuse rehabilitation.

Materials and methods

This cross-sectional study was conducted in two phases. During the initial phase, comprehensive demographic information including age, gender, education, marital status, drug use history, and details of previous suicide attempts was collected. A trained psychologist conducted structured interviews and questionnaires such as the Adverse Childhood Experiences - International Questionnaire (ACE-IQ), the Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS), and the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) were collected. During the subsequent phase, a different trained clinical psychologist administered assessments to evaluate executive function and attentional ability using the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT-II) and decision-making skills were tested using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT).

Participants

To mitigate potential confounding effects of varying substance use, this study focused primarily on individuals with a history of methamphetamine use. Utilizing G*Power (40 for our linear regression analysis, which includes three predictors—childhood adversity, cognitive function, and depression—with a medium effect size (f 2 = .16) as suggested by Cohen (41), We calculated that a sample size of 76 is sufficient to achieve a power of 0.8. A total of seventy-six methamphetamine addicts (methamphetamine as the primary drug used in the past and present, polysubstance use: 25%) classified according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition (DSM-5) criteria (the diagnostic instrument commonly employed in clinical or research conditions), as verified by an addiction major psychiatrist, were recruited from the Addiction Treatment Clinics of the Jianan Psychiatric Center between January and December 2022. All participants were outpatients, aged between 22 and 67, did not have severe psychotic disorders or other systemic diseases and completed the first phase of data collection during the initial outpatient consultation. The self-reported average duration of abstinence is approximately 76 days (Table 1, range: 7-271 days). The rate of urine-verified abstinence was also 100% at the first visit. Participants who faced difficulties completing the assessment or who could not understand the content of the questionnaires because of intellectual disabilities were excluded from the sample. The researchers informed all participants that their involvement in the study would not affect their legal status. All participants provided written informed consent before the study.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Independent T-test for SE/ME subgroups.

Measures

Demographic information

In the demographic questionnaire, age, gender, educational qualifications, employment status, marital status, drug use history, whether the individual met the diagnostic criteria for a psychiatric disorder, age of first-time methamphetamine use, duration of use (defined as the last time taken, minus the first time), and previous suicide attempts were collected.

Adverse childhood experiences - international questionnaire

The 29-item ACE-IQ (42) measures exposure to “childhood maltreatment,” “family/household dysfunction,” and “violence outside the home.” Multiple countries are validating this tool, with the trials being part of larger health surveys (11). Participants were asked to answer questions about their first 18 years of age.

Each question has dichotomous (Yes/No) or four-point Likert scale responses from “Never” to “Always” or “Many times.” The ACE-IQ instrument measures exposure to 13 categories of ACEs, which can be combined to determine ACE exposure. To be exposed, at least one ACE category item must be answered yes. Thus, the initial scoring process for each ACE category determines whether participants are “exposed” or “not exposed” to it. Summing the number of ACE categories, the participants considered “exposed” yields an ACE score from 0–13. The Chinese version of the ACEs had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83, indicating internal consistency. The “childhood maltreatment,” “family/household dysfunction,” and “violence outside the home” domain subscales had Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.74, 0.62, and 0.60, respectively. The tests and retests showed no differences in response concentration or rank variance. The ACEs had good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.90), and all three subscales had ICCs between 0.78 and 0.90 (11).

According to previous studies, individuals who have encountered four or more ACEs have a higher likelihood of various health-related outcomes than those who have not (14, 4345). Participants with ACE scores lower than four were assigned mild exposure (ME), and those with more than or equal to four were assigned severe exposure (SE) (4648).

The Conners’ continuous performance test II

This task is a visual paradigm used for the evaluation of attention and the response inhibition component of executive control. It represents a reliable and objective assessment of diagnostic procedures for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and various other neurological disorders (49).

The CPT-II paradigm has six blocks with three sub-blocks. Targeted and non-targeted stimuli (letters) were randomly presented for 250 ms with varying ISIs within the blocks. A block’s three ISI sub-blocks may be 1, 2, or 4 seconds, and their order varies (50). The CPT-II generates 13 indices, such as correct hits, omission errors, and commission errors for interpretation (50), and this study used T-score formats from computer-generated reports. T-scores above 60 indicated attention issues (49). The current CPT-II split-half reliability ranged from 0.66 to 0.95, indicating good reliability. Test-retest reliability was excellent when individual measures were aggregated into ADHD (0.89) and neurological functioning (0.92) indices (45).

Iowa Gambling Task

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) involving probabilistic learning via monetary rewards and punishments was created to assess real-world decision-making, where advantageous task performance requires participants to make choices that favor long-term benefits over immediate but potentially more significant rewards to avoid the risk of experiencing substantial losses (51). The present study employed the Iowa Gambling Task™, Version 2 (IGT™2, PAR. Inc.) as an assessment tool to evaluate decision-making abilities mediated by the prefrontal cortex of addicts. The IGT™-2 as the gain-loss structure for clinical is identical to the original IGT (52) but extends the age range by including additional normative data for children and adolescents ages 8 to 17 (53). After receiving clear instructions, participants selected cards from four decks with financial rewards and punishments. This version had 100 selections in five 20-trial blocks (500ms inter-trial interval), displaying demographically corrected T-scores. The retest of IGT blocks showed no difference, with a high Cronbach’s alpha (α=0.83) (54). The construct validity of the IGT may indicate frontal lobe dysfunction (55) or poor decision-making in substance-addicted individuals (52). These findings show modest construct validity of the IGT.

The severity of dependence scale

The SDS was developed as a tool specifically designed to assess the degree of dependence experienced by users of different types of substances (56). It includes five items on drug use anxiety and control issues (57). Items 1, 2, and 4 (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often, 3 = always, or nearly always), 3 (0 = not at all, 1 = a little, 2 = quite a lot, 3 = a great deal), and 5 (0 = not difficult, 1 = quite difficult, 2 = very difficult, 3 = impossible) were scored on a four-point scale (0 to 3). Higher total SDS scores indicate greater dependence, ranging from 0 to 15. The Chinese version of the SDS has.88 test-retest reliability and Cronbach’s alpha was.75 (58).

Number of DSM-5 Criteria of MUD: The utilization of the DSM-5 criteria for Methamphetamine Use Disorder (MUD) in our study serves as an instrumental measure for assessing the severity of this condition among participants. This assessment is conducted through structured clinical interviews meticulously designed to evaluate the presence and severity of symptoms in alignment with the DSM-5 standards. The DSM-5 criteria, which categorize the severity of the disorder into mild (2-3 criteria), moderate (4-5 criteria), and severe (6 or more criteria). Serving as a fundamental tool for the evaluation of stimulant use disorder, the DSM-5 criteria offer a comprehensive framework to systematically gauge the extent of substance use and its impact.

Beck depression inventory-II

The BDI-II (59) has 21 four-point scale items (0 to 3). The BDI-II-C has proven to be reliable by empirical studies. Clinical samples had internal consistency reliability coefficients of.94 (60), while nonclinical samples had reliability coefficients of.88 –.94 (61, 62). Regarding validity, BDI-II-C is significantly correlated with hopelessness, cognitive distortion, suicidal ideation, and health status (60, 61).

Statistical analysis

The participants’ demographic, neuropsychological, and behavioral scale data are shown as means and standard deviations. Data analysis, including descriptive, independent sample t-tests and χ2 tests, was conducted in SPSS 26.0. Model templates for PROCESS by Hayes in Process 4.1 were used to elucidate the intricate relationships among childhood adversity, cognitive function, and depression status (63). Our analytical model was designed to investigate the direct and indirect effects of ACEs on MUD severity, with particular attention to the roles of cognitive function and depression. Specifically, we posited depression as a mediator that potentially channels the influence of ACEs on MUD severity. Moreover, we explored cognitive function, operationalized through measures of perseveration obtained from the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT-II), as both a mediator and a moderator in the relationship between depression and MUD severity. Also, our analysis accounted for potential covariates, including demographic variables (e.g., age, gender, and education), to control for their effects on the relationships of interest. These covariates were selected based on their theoretical relevance and prior research indicating their potential influence on the variables of interest. Continuous variables, such as ACE scores and BDI-II scores, were mean-centered prior to analysis to facilitate the interpretation of interaction effects and to reduce multicollinearity among predictors. The PROCESS macro was employed to estimate the direct and indirect effects within our model, using bootstrap sampling (5,000 samples) to generate 95% confidence intervals for indirect effects. This approach allowed for a robust examination of the hypothesized mediation and moderation effects, providing insights into the complex dynamics underlying MUD severity. The specification of moments, such as mean and variance, was inherent in the bootstrapping procedure, which assumes that the sampling distribution of the indirect effect is adequately approximated by resampling with replacement from the observed data. Through this detailed statistical approach, our study aimed to shed light on the nuanced mechanisms by which early life adversity, through the mediating role of depression and the moderating influence of cognitive function, impacts the severity of methamphetamine use disorder.

Results

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics, neuropsychological results, behavioral measures, and correlations among these variables. This study involved 76 participants, of whom 10.53% were female. The participants had a mean age of 42.17 years and a mean IQ of 89.78. The average ACE score across participants was 4.28. The mean age at initial substance use was 30.51 years, and the average duration of sustained use was 12.59 years. In terms of behavioral measures, the mean Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS), Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) score, and the average DSM-5 symptom rating by psychiatrists were presented in Table 2. For neuropsychological tests, the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) and the Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II) indices were showed. Table 2 presents the correlations between the variables, with gender correlated with RTSD (Hit reaction time standard Error for CPT-II, r = -.298, p <.01), suggesting less consistency in correct reaction time among female participants. Age was correlated with ACEs (r = -.320, p <.01), first use age (r = .508, p <.01), and sustain use years (r = .306, p <.01), suggesting that older cases were of advanced age at the time of their initial substance use than those with younger first use years and less severe experiences of childhood adversity. The IQ was correlated with commission (r = -.350, p <.01) and detectability (r = -.307, p <.01). Methamphetamine addicts with higher levels of intelligence exhibit an enhanced capacity to discriminate stimuli in the CPT-II.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Descriptive, behavioral, neuropsychological, and correlation between variables.

Table 2 also indicates that the first use age was correlated with sustain use years (r = -.663, p <.01) and BDI-II (r = -.299, p <.01). Sustain use years were correlated with SDS (r = .272, p <.05) and BDI-II (r = .268, p <.05). Concentrating on variables encompassing childhood adversity, cognitive function, depression, and addiction severity levels, ACEs was correlated with first-use age (r = -.353, p <.01), BDI-II scores (r = .533, p <.01), DSM-5 symptoms (r = .398, p <.01), and perseveration of CPT-II (r = .275, p <.05). The SDS correlated with the BDI-II (r = .231, p <.05). The BDI-II correlated with the symptoms of the DSM-5 (r = .461, p <.01) and perseveration of the CPT-II (r = .318, p <.01). The symptoms of the DSM-5 were related to perseveration (r = .234, p <.05), which not only underscores the association between addiction and the attenuation of individual decision-making (36), depression (64), and inhibitory control capabilities (37) but also substantiates the connection between reduced inhibitory ability and the severity of depression (65). However, concerning cognitive function, this study only examined the correlation between perseveration and childhood adversity.

Table 1 presents the regrouping by ACEs cutoff (3/4). There were no significant differences in the demographic variables, including sex, age, or intelligence scores, between the SE and ME ACE groups. Similar to the behavioral scale, the subjective severity of MUD behaviors remained insignificant in the subgroup analysis. However, the SE ACE group showed higher depression scores on the BDI-II and more symptoms, as noted by psychiatrists. Regarding cognitive function in the CPT-II, there were differences between the groups in the perseveration of CPT-II. However, no significant differences were found in the remaining factors, including omission, commission, reaction time (RT), and Hit RT Std. Error (RTSD), variability, detectability, and hit RT block changes. There were also no significant differences observed between the two groups in terms of total money and T-scores on the IGT-2, indicating a significant relationship between inhibitory control performance in methamphetamine addicts and their experiences of childhood adversity, while no such relationship was found regarding their decision-making abilities. Based on previous findings, this study employed perseverance as a factor to construct its association with childhood adversity, depression, and addiction severity.

Concerning the symptoms of methamphetamine use diagnosed by psychiatrists, the proposed theoretical framework provides insights into the potential mechanisms by which childhood adversity may precipitate methamphetamine addiction. The relationship between the ACEs, BDI-II, and DSM-5 is central to our overarching research concept. The model templates for the PROCESS by Hayes in Process 4.1, the predictive validity of the model for DSM-5 symptoms, as diagnosed by psychiatrists, was only confirmed for ACEs and BDI-II scores, which explained the severity of DSM-5 symptoms in participants (see Figure 1). In this model, significant paths are denoted by solid lines, whereas the dashed line represents a non-significant path. All continuous variables were centered. The R2 of model was.270 (p <.001), showed a significant path from the independent variable (IV), ACE to the dependent variable (DV), DSM-5 symptoms was shown, b = .290, p = .009; the moderator variable, depression was showed, b=1.943, p = .001. however, the interaction between ACE and depression were not shown, b = -.408, p = .074. (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 The theoretical and PROCESS model of positing the perseveration capacity of CPT-II only moderates the relationship between depression-modulated ACE scores and SDS scores.

When considering the interconnections among ACEs, BDI-II, perseveration, and subjective severity symptoms of methamphetamine use, a theoretical model was proposed to elucidate the complex interplay between childhood adversity and ACE-associated factors (see Figure 2). Concerning the subjective severity of the symptoms of methamphetamine use symptoms (SDS), the proposed theoretical framework provides insights into the potential mechanisms by which childhood adversity may precipitate methamphetamine addiction. The relationship between the ACEs, BDI-II, and SDS is central to our overarching research concept, as indicated by the aggregate scores on the ACE scale and SDS. The model supports the moderating effect of the interaction between the moderator variables (W and M) on the relationship between the independent variable X and the dependent variable Y. The model accounted for 20.2% of the total variance (p = .026), a significant path from the independent variable (ACE) to the dependent variable (SDS) was shown, b = .459, p = .038. Furthermore, there was significant interaction between the moderators (depression and perseveration), b = 5.0185, p = .046. Thus, we found evidence of moderating effect between depression and perseveration. Which posits that the perseveration capacity of the CPT-II only moderates the relationship between depression-modulated ACE and SDS scores (see Figure 2). The model supporting our hypothesis that ACEs influence addictive behaviors through alterations in cognitive function. Based on the above, the interplay between perseveration ability and depression status mediates the subjective severity of SDS. However, this study does not support the notion that ACEs affects non-subjective addiction severity through cognitive processes.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 The theoretical and PROCESS model of DSM-5 symptoms scored by psychiatrists was only confirmed for ACE and BDI-II scores, which explained the severity of DSM-5 symptoms in participants.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, no study has examined the relationship between the characteristics of methamphetamine addicts and their childhood adversity experiences and has delved into the potential involvement of cognitive function in affecting the association between experiences of childhood adversity and MUD. Compared to the mild exposure (ME) group, the addicts of methamphetamine in the severe exposure (SE) group had a more serious diagnosis by the DSM-5, started using METH at a younger age, had a higher ratio of previous suicide attempts, and higher scores on the BDI-II. The SE group demonstrated more pronounced deficits in inhibitory control, their decision-making abilities remained unaffected. Similar to previous studies demonstrating that METH users exhibit higher ACE scores in comparison to healthy controls in Taiwan (66), this study takes an additional stride by indicating a positive connection between higher ACE scores and the severity of addiction. Other results, such as the ACE scores showed a significantly increased risk of suicidal ideation (67), a higher prevalence of depression and ACEs among methamphetamine users (68), and a positive correlation between ACEs and the duration of METH use (16), results are consistent with those of previous studies. Furthermore, adult substance use such as alcohol consumption is associated with ACEs and worse executive functions, including suppression, emotional control, and self-monitoring (69). Our data provide two novel findings regarding the effect of ACEs on MUD.

First, the use of methamphetamine has been linked to the deterioration of inhibitory control (7072) and emotional states (7375), which exacerbate ACEs. Instances categorized as more severe according to the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria were positively associated with increased ACEs. Traumatic events, such as harsh parenting, were negatively associated with inhibitory control in the regression analyses after controlling for the child’s age. Parents who engage in harsh interactions with their children may create an environment with fewer opportunities for the practice and cultivation of executive functions, including inhibitory control (76). The deterioration of inhibitory control directly predicted by ACEs (77, 78) implies a potential reduction in infants’ capacity to utilize this control mechanism to regulate their emotions (79). The attentional control mechanism was preliminarily designed to improve the behavior of infants or children. Therefore, improving attentional control mechanisms could offer either an emotional boost or an adaptable behavior, possibly influencing individuals’ social behaviors or fulfilling their intrinsic needs (80).. This observation could explain why individuals with a higher degree of childhood adversity exhibited a more pronounced severity of methamphetamine use.

Our data confirm that the prediction of subjective severity based on ACEs is subject to moderation by an individual’s inclination toward depressive tendencies. The moderating role of depression in this model differs from that identified by HE et al. (24). Furthermore, it has been indicated that the moderating impact of a propensity toward depression in this model interacts with the individual’s inhibitory control ability. This finding supports the hypothesis that inhibitory control does not directly affect the positive relationship between ACEs and methamphetamine use. Instead, it induces a modification in addictive behavior by influencing an individual’s emotional state. However, the integrative model proposed by Zelazo and Cunningham (81) posits that executive functions, including inhibitory control, interact with emotional regulation, particularly when individuals encounter situations requiring goal-directed problem-solving. When emotion modulation is secondary and resolves other issues, unregulated emotions can impair executive functioning by adding extra strain to an already burdened information processing system, which may directly impair problem-solving abilities (82). From the perspective of executive function, inhibitory control is regarded as the central mechanism of emotional regulation (83), and its development is accelerated during the preschool period (84). Childhood adversity can have a detrimental effect on the development of both inhibitory control and emotion regulation. Future research should clarify the role of emotional states (moderating or mediating effects) in the impact of childhood adversity on addictive behavior.

Second, this study did not provide evidence that childhood adversity influenced the decision-making ability of methamphetamine addicts. Experiences in childhood and adolescence elicit decision-making strategies that are adaptive to prevailing environmental circumstances and can persist into adulthood (85). Early experiences of adversity influence decision-making strategies, which may help individuals adapt to their early caregiving environment (86). In the decision-making process, participants with childhood adversity may experience reduced reward sensitivity and integrate less feedback. Individuals exposed to ACE tend to accumulate fewer rewards from the environment (87). Poor IGT performance could signify increased susceptibility to immediate rewards or diminished sensitivity to probabilistic losses (88), implying that immediate rewards associated with MUD may not significantly impact addictive behavior in individuals with varying childhood adversity experiences. Furthermore, the performance of individuals with SUD on the IGT may not serve as a sensitive indicator of their decision-making capabilities. Not all studies consistently indicate the presence of differences between the initial treatment and the end results (8991), or between using the substance and not using it (92). Considering the above, this study did not substantiate decision-making ability as a primary factor in comprehending how childhood adversity influences methamphetamine usage patterns.

Although this study provides insights into the potential mechanisms through which childhood adversity influences adult MUD, the current findings are limited by inadequate control of potential confounding variables. First, concerning causality, the relationship between ACEs and subsequent MUD remains unclear as to whether the findings derived from the observational investigations in this study truly signify causal effects. Second, concerning replicability, this study used a cohort of first-time MUD offenders under deferred prosecution in Taiwan. Consequently, the generalizability of these findings to other populations of methamphetamine users or individuals involved in different forms of illicit drug use may be restricted. Third, the severity of the DSM-5 diagnosis was directly predicted by ACEs, but subjective severity was not. The assessment of methamphetamine addiction severity in our model relied on participants’ subjective recollection; this indicator could potentially reflect social alienation or the capacity for emotional and cognitive regulation among addicts (93). Classification of addiction severity according to the DSM-5 criteria by attending psychiatrists did not fit this model. Future studies should examine the influence of various addiction-severity metrics on this pattern. The model can also be refined by including a more direct prediction of biomarkers such as peripheral IL-6 levels (94). Furthermore, cognition-related Electroencephalography may exhibit greater sensitivity to alterations in cognitive function than behavioral manifestations (95). Finally, the explanatory capacity of the theoretical model developed in this study was 20%. This modest explanatory power could be attributed to the complexity of drug addiction, which is influenced by multiple mechanisms and factors (96). Exploring how diverse childhood adversities influence MUD in adults with multiple factors is another subject for future research.

Our hypothesis draws on a broad spectrum of theoretical and empirical foundations suggesting that the effects of ACEs on addiction are mediated through more intricate mechanisms than direct causal pathways. The lack of straightforward correlations between ACEs, cognitive measures, and MUD severity does not negate the potential for ACEs to exert significant indirect effects on addiction through cognitive alterations. Indeed, the complexity of human cognition and behavioral outcomes necessitates a consideration of nuanced relationships that may not be captured through simple correlational analyses. However, we propose that cognitive alterations stemming from ACEs may manifest in specific domains relevant to addiction vulnerability, such as emotional regulation, impulse control, and stress responsiveness, rather than in global cognitive measures that were the focus of our initial analysis. Our analytical approach, incorporating moderated mediation models, allows for the examination of conditional indirect effects that may elucidate the pathways through which ACEs impact MUD severity via cognitive alterations. This approach recognizes the possibility that the relationship between ACEs and addiction severity is contingent upon the presence of mediating variables such as depression, and moderated by other factors including, but not limited to, the individual’s socio-demographic background, the severity and type of ACEs encountered, and concurrent mental health conditions. The absence of significant differences between groups based on ACE exposure in direct measures of cognitive function does not preclude the presence of subtle cognitive impairments that may influence addiction severity. It is possible that these impairments are context-dependent, emerging more prominently in situations that simulate real-life decision-making and stress responses related to substance use rather than in the structured environment of neuropsychological testing. In light of these considerations, future research should emphasize the importance of adopting a holistic view of the impact of ACEs on addiction, which accounts for the mediating role of cognitive alterations and the potential for these effects to be moderated by a range of individual and environmental factors.

In summary, while direct correlations between ACEs, cognitive measures, and MUD severity were not observed in our study, we support our hypothesis through a theoretical framework that acknowledges the complex, mediated, and moderated relationships between developmental trauma, cognitive function alterations, and addictive behaviors. This perspective aligns with current understanding in the fields of developmental psychology and addiction science, advocating for a nuanced approach to studying the long-term impacts of childhood adversity.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by IRB of Jianan Psychiatric Center, Taiwan. (IRB No: 23-009). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

C-HK: Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Y-CLu: Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. C-HL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Y-CLi: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The authors express their gratitude for the financial support provided by the National Science Council, Taiwan, under Grant Nos. 111-2410-H-468-008-MY3, 110-2314-B-695-001-MY2, and 109-2314-B-695-002. Additionally, this study received support from the Integrated Drug Addiction Treatment Center of the Jianan Psychiatric Center, sponsored by the Ministry of Health and Welfare, Taiwan.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Cami J, Farre M. Drug addiction. N Engl J Med. (2003) 349:975–86. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra023160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Executive Yuan. The next generation of drug prevention policies. (2020). Taipei, Taiwan.

Google Scholar

3. Tsai TM, Wu HC, Lai YL, Su LW. Deferred prosecution with conditions for addiction treatment in Taiwan. National Development Council (2018). Taipei, Taiwan, Available at: https://ws.ndc.gov.tw/Download.ashx?u=LzAwMS9hZG1pbmlzdHJhdG9yLzEwL3JlbGZpbGUvNTY0NC8zMDczNy85ODZmY2IyYS0xODdmLTQ2ZmItOThlYS0xM2M3NTg0NzNhYjUucGRm&n=5pyf5pyr5aCx5ZGK5a6a56i%2FLnBkZg%3D%3D&icon=.pdf.

Google Scholar

4. Yang SL, Tai SF, Wu TH. Multiple drug treatment program in Taichung prison: Toward a comprehensive drug treatment model. J Correction. (2019) 8:3–39. doi: 10.6905/JC.201903_8(1).0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Chen LY, Wu SC, Ko NY. Moderating effect of illicit drug use on the relationship between sexual behaviours and prevalence of HIV or sexually transmitted infections. Drug Alcohol Rev. (2021) 40:287–95. doi: 10.1111/dar.13170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lee YC, Liu WC, Hsieh YL, Wu CH, Wu PY, Luo YZ, et al. Non-opioid recreational drug use and a prolonged HIV outbreak among men who have sex with men in Taiwan: An incident case-control study, 2006-2015. J Formos Med Assoc. (2022) 121:237–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jfma.2021.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Lamyai W, Pono K, Indrakamhaeng D, Saengsin A, Songhong N, Khuwuthyakorn P, et al. Risks of psychosis in methamphetamine users: cross-sectional study in Thailand. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e032711. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Schecke H, Lea T, Bohn A, Kohler T, Sander D, Scherbaum N, et al. Crystal methamphetamine use in sexual settings among German men who have sex with men. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:886. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lee WC, Chang HM, Huang MC, Pan CH, Su SS, Tsai SY, et al. All-cause and suicide mortality among people with methamphetamine use disorder: a nation-wide cohort study in Taiwan. Addiction. (2021) 116:3127–38. doi: 10.1111/add.15501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fuller-Thomson E, Roane JL, Brennenstuhl S. Three types of adverse childhood experiences, and alcohol and drug dependence among adults: an investigation using population-based data. Subst Use Misuse. (2016) 51:1451–61. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2016.1181089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ho GWK, Chan ACY, Chien WT, Bressington DT, Karatzias T. Examining patterns of adversity in Chinese young adults using the Adverse Childhood Experiences-International Questionnaire (ACE-IQ). Child Abuse Negl. (2019) 88:179–88. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Br J Psychiatry. (2010) 197:378–85. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, Dependent Care, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. (2012) 129(1):e232–46. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Chapman DP, Giles WH, Anda RF. Childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: the adverse childhood experiences study. Pediatrics. (2003) 111:564–72. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.3.564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ramiro LS, Madrid BJ, Brown DW. Adverse childhood experiences (ACE) and health-risk behaviors among adults in a developing country setting. Child Abuse Negl. (2010) 34:842–55. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ding Y, Lin H, Zhou L, Yan H, He N. Adverse childhood experiences and interaction with methamphetamine use frequency in the risk of methamphetamine-associated psychosis. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 142:295–300. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bax T. The adverse childhood experiences of methamphetamine users in Aotearoa/New Zealand. Int J Criminology Sociology. (2021) 10:1430–42. doi: 10.6000/1929-4409.2021.10.164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Keyes KM, Shmulewitz D, Greenstein E, McLaughlin K, Wall M, Aharonovich E, et al. Exposure to the Lebanon War of 2006 and effects on alcohol use disorders: the moderating role of childhood maltreatment. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 134:296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Leza L, Siria S, Lopez-Goni JJ, Fernandez-Montalvo J. Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and substance use disorder (SUD): A scoping review. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2021) 221:108563. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.108563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cloitre M, Stolbach BC, Herman JL, van der Kolk B, Pynoos R, Wang J, et al. A developmental approach to complex PTSD: childhood and adult cumulative trauma as predictors of symptom complexity. J Trauma Stress. (2009) 22:399–408. doi: 10.1002/jts.20444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jonson-Reid M, Kohl PL, Drake B. Child and adult outcomes of chronic child maltreatment. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:839–45. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Chandler GE, Kalmakis KA, Murtha T. Screening adults with substance use disorder for adverse childhood experiences. J Addict Nurs. (2018) 29:172–8. doi: 10.1097/JAN.0000000000000233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ducci F, Goldman D. The genetic basis of addictive disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2012) 35:495–519. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2012.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. He J, Yan X, Wang R, Zhao J, Liu J, Zhou C, et al. Does childhood adversity lead to drug addiction in adulthood? A study of serial mediators based on resilience and depression. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:871459. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.871459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. McKetin R, Leung J, Stockings E, Huo Y, Foulds J, Lappin JM, et al. Mental health outcomes associated with of the use of amphetamines: a systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine. (2019) 16:81–97. doi: 10.1016/j.eclinm.2019.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Leung J, Mekonen T, Wang X, Arunogiri S, Degenhardt L, McKetin R. Methamphetamine exposure and depression—A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Rev. (2023) 42:1438–49. doi: 10.1111/dar.13670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Nelson CA 3rd, Gabard-Durnam LJ. Early adversity and critical periods: neurodevelopmental consequences of violating the expectable environment. Trends Neurosci. (2020) 43:133–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gheorghe DA, Li C, Gallacher J, Bauermeister S. Associations of perceived adverse lifetime experiences with brain structure in UK Biobank participants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 62:822–30. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hawkins MAW, Layman HM, Ganson KT, Tabler J, Ciciolla L, Tsotsoros CE, et al. Adverse childhood events and cognitive function among young adults: Prospective results from the national longitudinal study of adolescent to adult health. Child Abuse Negl. (2021) 115:105008. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.105008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Danese A, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Bleiberg BA, Dinardo PB, Gandelman SB, et al. The origins of cognitive deficits in victimized children: implications for neuroscientists and clinicians. Am J Psychiatry. (2017) 174:349–61. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Brown MJ, Kaur A, James T, Avalos C, Addo PNO, Crouch E, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and subjective cognitive decline in the US. J Appl Gerontol. (2022) 41:1090–100. doi: 10.1177/07334648211058718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Aafjes-van Doorn K, Kamsteeg C, Silberschatz G. Cognitive mediators of the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and adult psychopathology: A systematic review. Dev Psychopathol. (2020) 32:1017–29. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. McCarthy KL, Caputi P, Grenyer BFS. Significant change events in psychodynamic psychotherapy: Is cognition or emotion more important? Psychol Psychother. (2017) 90:377–88. doi: 10.1111/papt.12116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Volkow ND, Koob GF, McLellan AT. Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. N Engl J Med. (2016) 374:363–71. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1511480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. (2002) 159:1642–52. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Redish AD, Jensen S, Johnson A. A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behav Brain Sci. (2008) 31:415–37. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0800472X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Dominguez-Salas S, Diaz-Batanero C, Lozano-Rojas OM, Verdejo-Garcia A. Impact of general cognition and executive function deficits on addiction treatment outcomes: Systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive pathways. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:772–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Garavan H, Hester R. The role of cognitive control in cocaine dependence. Neuropsychol Rev. (2007) 17:337–45. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9034-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. George O, Koob GF. Individual differences in prefrontal cortex function and the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2010) 35:232–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Fau Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Routledge, New York, US: Academic Press (1988).

Google Scholar

42. World Health Organization. Adverse Childhood Experiences International Questionnaire (ACE-IQ). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization (2016). Available at: http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/violence/activities/adverse_childhood_experiences/en/.

Google Scholar

43. Dong M, Anda RF, Dube SR, Giles WH, Felitti VJ. The relationship of exposure to childhood sexual abuse to other forms of abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction during childhood. Child Abuse Negl. (2003) 27:625–39. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(03)00105-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Hawkins M, Ciciolla L, Colaizzi J, Keirns N, Smith C, Stout M, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and cognitive function among adults with excess adiposity. Obes Sci Pract. (2020) 6:47–56. doi: 10.1002/osp4.385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, Sethi D, Butchart A, Mikton C, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. (2017) 2:e356–e66. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bryant DJ, Coman EN, Damian AJ. Association of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and substance use disorders (SUDs) in a multi-site safety net healthcare setting. Addictive Behav Rep. (2020) 12:100293. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lensch T, Clements-Nolle K, Oman RF, Evans WP, Lu M, Yang W. Adverse childhood experiences and co-occurring psychological distress and substance abuse among juvenile offenders: the role of protective factors. Public Health. (2021) 194:42–7. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2021.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zhu J, Racine N, Devereux C, Hodgins DC, Madigan S. Associations between adverse childhood experiences and substance use: a meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl. (2023) 143, 106431. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2023.106431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Homack S, Riccio CA. Conners' Continuous performance test (2nd ed.; CCPT-II). J Atten Disord. (2006) 9:556–8. doi: 10.1177/1087054705283578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Conners CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test user’s manual.: Multi-Health Systems. (2000). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

Google Scholar

51. Brevers D, Bechara A, Cleeremans A, Noel X. Iowa Gambling Task (IGT): twenty years after - gambling disorder and IGT. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:665. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bechara A. Iowa gambling task professional manual: Psychological Assessment Resources. (2007). Lutz, Florida.

Google Scholar

53. Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task, Version 2. Professional manual: PAR (2016).

Google Scholar

54. Mueller EM, Nguyen J, Ray WJ, Borkovec TD. Future-oriented decision-making in Generalized Anxiety Disorder is evident across different versions of the Iowa Gambling Task. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2010) 41:165–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Cavedini P, Bassi T, Ubbiali A, Casolari A, Giordani S, Zorzi C, et al. Neuropsychological investigation of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. (2004) 127:259–66. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gossop M, Darke S, Griffiths P, Hando J, Powis B, Hall W, et al. The Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS): psychometric properties of the SDS in English and Australian samples of heroin, cocaine and amphetamine users. Addiction. (1995) 90:607–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1995.tb02199.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Martin G, Copeland J, Gates P, Gilmour S. The Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS) in an adolescent population of cannabis users: reliability, validity and diagnostic cut-off. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2006) 83:90–3. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chen VC, Chen H, Lin TY, Chou HH, Lai TJ, Ferri CP, et al. Severity of heroin dependence in Taiwan: reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS[Ch]). Addict Behav. (2008) 33:1590–3. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Chen HY. Manual of Beck Depression Inventory–II: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation. (2000). Taipei, Taiwan.

Google Scholar

60. Lu M-L, Che HH, Chang S, Shen WW. Reliability and validity of the chinese version of the beck depression inventory-II. Taiwanese J Psychiatry. (2002) 16:301–10.

Google Scholar

61. Byrne BM, Stewart SM, Lee PWH. Validating the beck depression inventory-II for Hong Kong community adolescents. Int J Testing. (2004) 4:199–216. doi: 10.1207/s15327574ijt0403_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Wu P-C. Measurement invariance and latent mean differences of the beck depression inventory II across gender groups. J Psychoeducational Assessment. (2010) 28:551–63. doi: 10.1177/0734282909360772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Press (2017).

Google Scholar

64. Lecomte T, Paquin K, Mueser KT, MacEwan GW, Goldner E, Thornton AE, et al. Relationships among depression, PTSD, methamphetamine abuse, and psychosis. J Dual Diagnosis. (2013) 9:115–22. doi: 10.1080/15504263.2013.778930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Palmwood EN, Krompinger JW, Simons RF. Electrophysiological indicators of inhibitory control deficits in depression. Biol Psychol. (2017) 130:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lin YF, Chen PY, Liu HC, Chen YL, Chou WH, Huang MC. Shortened leukocyte telomere length in young adults who use methamphetamine. Transl Psychiatry. (2021) 11:519. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01640-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Takahashi M, Yamaki M, Kondo A, Hattori M, Kobayashi M, Shimane T. Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences and their association with suicidal ideation and non-suicidal self-injury among incarcerated methamphetamine users in Japan. Child Abuse Negl. (2022) 131:105763. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Htike M, Thepthein B, Chucharoen P. A web-based survey on adverse childhood experience, anxiety, depression, sexual behavior affecting methamphetamine use among adolescents in Bangkok, Thailand. J Public Hlth Dev. (2017) 15:17–31. Available at: https://he01.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/AIHD-MU/article/view/92366

Google Scholar

69. Liu Z, Yang Y, Shi Z, Liu J, Wang Y. The risk of male adult alcohol dependence: The role of the adverse childhood experiences and ecological executive function. Compr Psychiatry. (2016) 68:129–33. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ivanov I, Schulz KP, London ED, Newcorn JH. Inhibitory control deficits in childhood and risk for substance use disorders: a review. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. (2008) 34:239–58. doi: 10.1080/00952990802013334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Salo R, Ursu S, Buonocore MH, Leamon MH, Carter C. Impaired prefrontal cortical function and disrupted adaptive cognitive control in methamphetamine abusers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 65:706–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Weafer J, Gorka SM, Hedeker D, Dzemidzic M, Kareken DA, Phan KL, et al. Associations between behavioral and neural correlates of inhibitory control and amphetamine reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:1905–13. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Grant BF. Comorbidity between DSM-IV drug use disorders and major depression: results of a national survey of adults. J Subst Abuse. (1995) 7:481–97. doi: 10.1016/0899-3289(95)90017-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Semple SJ, Zians J, Grant I, Patterson TL. Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2005) 29:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Stellern J, Xiao KB, Grennell E, Sanches M, Gowin JL, Sloan ME. Emotion regulation in substance use disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. (2023) 118:30–47. doi: 10.1111/add.16001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Treat AE, Sheffield Morris A, Williamson AC, Hays-Grudo J, Laurin D. Adverse childhood experiences, parenting, and child executive function. Early Child Dev Care. (2017) 189:926–37. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1353978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Carr CP, Martins CM, Stingel AM, Lemgruber VB, Juruena MF. The role of early life stress in adult psychiatric disorders: a systematic review according to childhood trauma subtypes. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2013) 201:1007–20. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Ji S, Wang H. A study of the relationship between adverse childhood experiences, life events, and executive function among college students in China. Psicol Reflex Crit. (2018) 31:28. doi: 10.1186/s41155-018-0107-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Ursache A, Blair C, Stifter C, Voegtline K, Family Life Project I. Emotional reactivity and regulation in infancy interact to predict executive functioning in early childhood. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:127–37. doi: 10.1037/a0027728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Drigas A, Karyotaki M. Executive functioning and problem solving: A bidirectional relation. Int J Eng Pedagogy (iJEP). (2019) 9:76–98. doi: 10.3991/ijep.v9i3.10186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Zelazo PD, Cunningham WA. Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. In: Gross, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford, New York (2007). p. 135–58.

Google Scholar

82. Carlson SM, Wang TS. Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cogn Dev. (2007) 22:489–510. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Gagne JR, Liew J, Nwadinobi OK. How does the broader construct of self-regulation relate to emotion regulation in young children? Dev Review. (2021) 60:100965. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2021.100965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Dev Perspectives. (2012) 6:354–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. (2005) 17:271–301. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Humphreys KL, Lee SS, Telzer EH, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Goff B, Flannery J, et al. Exploration-exploitation strategy is dependent on early experience. Dev Psychobiol. (2015) 57:313–21. doi: 10.1002/dev.21293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Lloyd-Jones DM, Allen NB, Anderson CA, Black T, Brewer LC, Foraker RE, et al. Life’s essential 8: updating and enhancing the American Heart Association’s construct of cardiovascular health: a presidential advisory from the American Heart Association. Circulation. (2022) 146:e18–43. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0000000000001078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Koffarnus MN, Kaplan BA. Clinical models of decision making in addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2018) 164:71–83. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2017.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Bowden-Jones H, McPhillips M, Rogers R, Hutton S, Joyce E. Risk-taking on tests sensitive to ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction predicts early relapse in alcohol dependency: a pilot study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2005) 17:417–20. doi: 10.1176/jnp.17.3.417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Ernst M, Grant SJ, London ED, Contoreggi CS, Kimes AS, Spurgeon L. Decision making in adolescents with behavior disorders and adults with substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:33–40. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Schmitz JM, Mooney ME, Green CE, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Swann AC, et al. Baseline neurocognitive profiles differentiate abstainers and non-abstainers in a cocaine clinical trial. J Addict Dis. (2009) 28:250–7. doi: 10.1080/10550880903028502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Nejtek VA, Kaiser KA, Zhang B, Djokovic M. Iowa Gambling Task scores predict future drug use in bipolar disorder outpatients with stimulant dependence. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 210:871–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Ghavidast M, Hassanvandi S. Examination of the relationship between severity of addiction and personality: the mediating effect of social alienation and emotional, cognitive regulation. Iranian J Psychiatry Behav Sci. (2023) 17(1):e123720. doi: 10.5812/ijpbs

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Kohno M, Loftis JM, Huckans M, Dennis LE, McCready H, Hoffman WF. The relationship between interleukin-6 and functional connectivity in methamphetamine users. Neurosci Lett. (2018) 677:49–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.04.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Stipacek A, Grabner RH, Neuper C, Fink A, Neubauer AC. Sensitivity of human EEG alpha band desynchronization to different working memory components and increasing levels of memory load. Neurosci Lett. (2003) 353:193–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.09.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. West R, Brown J. Theory of addiction. New Jersey, USA: Wiley-Blackwell (2013). doi: 10.1002/9781118484890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adverse childhood experiences, methamphetamine use disorder, decision-making, inhibitory control, perseveration

Citation: Ko C-H, Lu Y-C, Lee C-H and Liao Y-C (2024) The influence of adverse childhood experiences and depression on addiction severity among methamphetamine users: exploring the role of perseveration. Front. Psychiatry 15:1382646. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1382646

Received: 06 February 2024; Accepted: 25 April 2024;
Published: 14 May 2024.

Edited by:

Scott E. Hemby, High Point University, United States

Reviewed by:

Mo Daming, Hefei Fourth People’s Hospital, China
Bruno Kluwe-Schiavon, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States

Copyright © 2024 Ko, Lu, Lee and Liao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chun-Hung Lee, yuhsinliu87@gmail.com; Yu-Chi Liao, ycliao@asia.edu.tw

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.