EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 April 2025

Sec. Autism

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1604144

This article is part of the Research TopicVisuospatial and Visuoconstructional Abilities and Disorders Across the Life SpanView all 7 articles

Editorial: Visuospatial and visuoconstructional abilities and disorders across the life span

  • 1Developmental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Caserta, Italy
  • 2Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 3Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
  • 4Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Caserta, Italy

Visuospatial cognition covers a wide range of non-verbal cognitive abilities that are pivotal for enabling individuals to interact with the environment effectively, and are strongly linked to drawing and assembling (visuoconstructional) activities. This link is evident both in childhood, when the progressive maturation of visuospatial cognition parallels changes in performance on spatial construction tasks (1, 2), and in adulthood, when acquired visuospatial deficits and disorders of spatial construction are frequently observed together following brain damage (35). Visuospatial skills are affected by a large variety of factors, spanning from demographics (e.g., sex, age, and education) to individual (e.g., personality, autistic traits, and spatial self-evaluations) and cultural (e.g., gender stereotypes) differences (69).

The present Research Topic offers the latest evidence on visuospatial and visuoconstructional abilities and disorders across the lifespan, encompassing studies on typically and atypically developing children (Ebert et al.; Orefice et al.), healthy adults (Ricci et al.; Muffato et al.), and individuals with acquired brain lesions (Lederer et al.; Moretta et al.).

Ebert et al. investigated the link between implicit and explicit spatial gender stereotypes and mental rotation performance in preschool-aged typically developing children. Children displayed explicit stereotypes in favor of boys, stronger in boys than girls. Evidence of implicit stereotypes favoring boys was also found in the overall sample, but independently from sex. Although no clear relationships were found between stereotypes and mental rotation, a positive association emerged in girls between implicit stereotypes favoring their own sex and mental rotation. These findings support the existence of explicit spatial gender stereotypes as early as at pre-school age.

Orefice et al. investigated several aspects of visuospatial cognition in children with Developmental Visuospatial Disorder (DVSD) and Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Compared with typical controls, both groups showed significantly lower performance on a visuospatial perspective-taking task (VSPT), with the DVSD group scoring the lowest. Interestingly, group moderated the relationship between VSPT and fine-motor coordination, showing an opposite pattern between the clinical groups (a positive association for the DVSD group and the reverse pattern in the DCD group). These findings suggest that children with DVSD tend to use a strategy relying on fine-motor skills when performing VSPT task, whereas DCD children would not. A strict link between visuomotor coordination and other high-order visuospatial abilities has also been reported in typical development (10, 11).

Ricci et al. assessed the interaction between visuospatial attention and action control in healthy adult individuals. The authors tested the effect of the hand starting position (near or far from the body) on the subjective midpoint’s localization in a radial line bisection task. Results showed that participants bisected radial lines farther than the true center in both near and far conditions, although bisection errors were greater in the near condition. These findings provide new evidence for a classical model postulating close relationships between attention and action (12).

Moreover, Muffato et al. examined the ability to navigate combining scales of sense of direction and spatial representation, spatial anxiety and attitude in exploring across the adult lifespan. A distinct age-related pattern in the different self-evaluations emerged, with a linear positive trend for a sense of direction, and an age breakpoint (66 and 71 years) for the other two tasks, with an increase in spatial anxiety and a decrease in attitude towards exploring. Moreover, men produced higher self-ratings than women for sense of direction and attitude towards exploring (and a lower spatial anxiety); higher education was associated with lower spatial anxiety and higher sense of direction. These results contribute to shed light on individual differences in navigational skills across the adult lifespan, thus further enlarging the range of issues relevant to visuospatial cognition.

Finally, two studies assessed visuospatial cognition in adult patients with brain lesions. Lederer et al. investigated the kinematics of grasping toward common objects in an adult with acquired visual agnosia who showed considerable residual everyday skills. The patient was able to recognize most colored objects with a variable delay, but he was strongly impaired in recognizing objects in a color-masked condition, and his kinematic grasping performance was significantly slower than that of matched healthy controls, both when he was instructed to grasp-then-name and to name-then-grasp the target objects. These findings suggest that color information can be processed by the dorsal stream and contribute to visuomotor transformations (13).

In a systematic review, Moretta et al. examined a well-known disorder of spatial cognition, i.e., unilateral spatial neglect, but focusing on a peculiar manifestation implying a defective exploration of the vertical dimension of space. Analysis of the available literature revealed that vertical neglect is more common in lower than upper space, is often associated with attentional bias in the horizontal dimension, and can co-occur with radial space impairment, particularly in the near radial. Vertical neglect is most often assessed by paper-and-pencil tasks, such as line bisection, but computer-based tasks are increasingly employed. Furthermore, the authors also found that the most frequent etiology is vascular, especially ischaemic, involving diverse subcortical and cortical areas, although the temporal lobe is most often affected.

The heterogeneous range of abilities and disorders included within the broad domain of spatial cognition and the variety of methods featured in the present Research Topic highlighted, on the one hand, the complexity of this field of investigation and, on the other hand, delineated the potential for establishing a shared framework. This common ground could help integrate approaches that have largely evolved along separate lines of research.

Author contributions

IZ: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PJ: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. IM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LT: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Stiles J, Akshoomoff N, Haist F. The development of visuospatial processing. In: Rubenstein JLR, Rakic P, editors. Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: neural circuit development and function in the brain, vol. 3 . Elsevier, Amsterdam (2013). p. 271–96.

Google Scholar

2. Zappullo I, Conson M, Zoccolotti P, Trojano L, Senese VP. Building blocks and drawing figures is not the same”: neuropsychological bases of block design and Rey figure drawing in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychol. (2021) 27:371–89. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2020.1862075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Trojano L. Constructional apraxia from the roots up: Kleist, Strauss, and their contemporaries. Neurol Sci. (2020) 41:981–8. doi: 10.1007/s10072-019-04186-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Trojano L. Hemispheric asymmetries in the control of upper limb movements. Handb Clin Neurol. (2025) 208:393–405. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-443-15646-5.00024-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Trojano L, Conson M. Visuospatial and visuoconstructive deficits. Handb Clin Neurol. (2008) 88:373–91. doi: 10.1016/S0072-9752(07)88019-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Rosselli M, Ardila A. The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: a critical review. Brain Cogn. (2003) 52:326–33. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00170-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Conson M, Senese VP, Zappullo I, Baiano C, Warrier V, LabNPEE Group, et al. The effect of autistic traits on disembedding and mental rotation in neurotypical women and men. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:4639. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-08497-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Carbone E, Meneghetti C, Borella E. The influence of personality traits and facets on visuo-spatial task performance and self-assessed visuo-spatial inclinations in young and older adults. PloS One. (2019) 14:e0220525. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Moè A, Pazzaglia F. Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learn Individ Dif. (2006) 16:369–77. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Senese VP, Zappullo I, Baiano C, Zoccolotti P, Monaco M, Conson M. Identifying neuropsychological predictors of drawing skills in elementary school children. Child Neuropsychol. (2020) 26:345–61. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2019.1651834

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Zappullo I, Senese VP, Trojano L, Cecere R, Conson M. Specific and shared cognitive predictors of drawing and block building in typically developing children. Front Hum Neurosci. (2024) 18:1436362. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1436362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rizzolatti G, Riggio L, Sheliga BM. Space and selective attention. In: Umiltà C, Moscovitch M, editors. Attention and Performance XV. Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (1994). p. 231–65.

Google Scholar

13. Claeys KG, Dupont P, Cornette L, Sunaert S, Van Hecke P, De Schutter E, et al. Color discrimination involves ventral and dorsal stream visual areas. Cereb Cortex. (2004) 14:803–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visuospatial, visual agnosia, unilateral spatial neglect, mental rotation, perspective taking, navigation

Citation: Zappullo I, Conson M, Jansen P, Mammarella IC and Trojano L (2025) Editorial: Visuospatial and visuoconstructional abilities and disorders across the life span. Front. Psychiatry 16:1604144. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1604144

Received: 01 April 2025; Accepted: 10 April 2025;
Published: 28 April 2025.

Edited and Reviewed by:

Antonio M. Persico, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Zappullo, Conson, Jansen, Mammarella and Trojano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Isa Zappullo, aXNhLnphcHB1bGxvQHVuaWNhbXBhbmlhLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.