Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 09 August 2021
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare
Volume 8 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.683858

Neuroendocrine and Cardiovascular Activation During Aggressive Reactivity in Dogs

  • Department of Animal Science, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, Domžale, Slovenia

Our aim was to investigate cardiovascular activation by measuring changes in facial and body surface temperature using infrared thermography, and neuroendocrine activation using salivary cortisol (CORT) and serotonin concentration (SER) in dogs exhibiting aggressive reactivity in real time. Based on two factors, owner-reported past aggressive behaviors, and detailed behavioral observations collected during a Socially Acceptable Behavior test consisting of 16 subtests and, each individual was categorized as aggressive or non-aggressive. CORT and SER showed no difference in neuroendocrine activity between dogs, but aggressive dogs with higher levels of aggression were found to have lower SER. Aggressive dogs also had an increase in facial temperature from pre-test values. The discovery of a correlation between tail wagging and left tail wagging with aggression level and aggression-related behaviors in aggressive dogs is further evidence of the right hemisphere specialization for aggression previously reported in the literature. This study provides the first evidence that both cardiovascular and neuroendocrine systems are activated during an active act of aggression in dogs.

Introduction

The response of animals to environmental stimuli, often referred to as reactivity (1), varies from individual to individual. When exposed to challenges, animals adopt different individual behavioral strategies or coping styles that are stable over a longer time (2). An individual coping style is an adaptive strategy characterized by a set of behaviors and physiological responses to reduce the impact of a stressor and is characteristic of a particular group of individuals (2). Animals, including dogs, can be described as proactive or reactive copers (3) and exhibit behavioral patterns that can distinguish them as aggressive or non-aggressive individuals, respectively (4). Aggressive reactivity in dogs, especially when directed toward humans, is a widely recognized problem that poses a public health and animal welfare concern (5). The behavior can be classified by its' motivation (territorial-, fear-related etc.) or target (human-, dog-directed etc.), but its' cause cannot always be determined. To date, various physiological factors underlying aggressive behavior have been studied to identify potential biomarkers of aggression, but certain gaps remain. Research (69) has primarily focused on comparing groups of dogs with or without a history of owner reported aggressive behavior, and has not aimed to examine physiological activation during an aggressive act. Evidence that considers physiological activation during an actual aggressive reactivity is therefore lacking. For the study of real-time behavior, it is recommended to measure multiple physiological parameters simultaneously (10), non-invasively, so that measurement devices and procedures do not interfere with behavioral responses (11).

Behavioral reactivity to external stimuli has been reported to be associated with cardiovascular parameters such as heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), and (body and facial) surface temperature. For example, recent evidence suggests that dogs with a history of biting incidents have poorer autonomic regulation, resulting in lower resting HRV (7), while dogs exhibiting aggressive reactivity to threatening stimuli have decreased HRV and increased HR (12). This latter result was based on measurements collected after the stimulus was applied, when the dog was standing still to avoid motion artifacts. In addition to motion artifacts, many researchers agree that the measurement of HR and HRV in moving dogs has other limitations. Sudden bursts of muscle activity during movement can lead to poor electrode conduction (13) and loss of contact or displacement of the electrodes can cause false signals (14). Another limitation is that the monitor strapped around the dog's chest can be intrusive, especially for dogs that are not used to wearing it, so habituation by wearing a dummy monitor may be required (13). An alternative measure that avoids any direct interaction during exercise and potentially alters behavioral responses is infrared thermography, which has been recognized as a useful tool for assessing cardiovascular reactivity in animals, including dogs (15). Findings in animals (16) suggest that it can be used to measure temperature changes associated with positive and negative affective states, as affective states can cause vascular activity that produces changes in heat production and release that lead to changes in surface temperature.

In terms of reactivity during negative affective states, dogs have been shown to have lower nasal temperature while alert when kenneled compared to a home environment (17). Other animal studies showed a decrease in nasal surface temperature in response to threatening stimuli [monkeys: (11, 18)] and a decrease in ocular bulb and periocular area temperature exposed to various stressors [rabbits: (19)]. The only two studies that observed cardiovascular activity during an aggressive act in animals were by Boileau et al. (20) and Rigternik et al. (21) and they reported inconsistent results. Boileau et al. (20) reported a decrease in dorsal surface temperature in pigs during a fight, while Rigternik et al. (21) found no differences between the control group and aggressive dogs that showed human-directed aggression.

The above cardiovascular parameters are closely related to the autonomic stress response “fight or flight”, which prepares an animal to react in a stressful situation (22). Simultaneously, cortisol, the primary stress hormone, is released (23) in conjunction with the production of the inhibitory neurotransmitter serotonin (24). Such neuroendocrine activation modulates cognitive and behavioral functions and determines coping behavior in humans (25) and in non-human animals (26). According to Bari and Robbins (27), serotonin helps both humans and animals to inhibit inappropriate learned behavior and choose adapted behavior. In humans, Montoya et al. (28) found that low serotonin concentration (SER) combined with high testosterone to cortisol concentration ratio (CORT) modulates impulsive aggression. Following owner-reported past aggressive behavior, researchers found that aggressive dogs had significantly lower serum SER levels (6, 8) and higher plasma CORT levels than non-aggressive dogs (9). These studies used an invasive approach when examining cardiovascular activity, which caused unnecessary stress to the animals (29). To avoid this, CORT and SER can be assessed by highly comparable saliva samples (30) and tested during short-term physiological reactivity (31).

Similar physiological reactivity is often reported in the expression of various behaviors. For example, fear and aggression in dogs have different behavioral expression but share similar neurochemistry, resulting in similar physiological reactivity (32). To observe physiological and behavioral parameters in a controlled environment, behavioral tests are the most objective research method. In our study, the Socially Acceptable Behavior (SAB) test, which is known to elicit aggression in aggressively-inclined dogs (33), was used to assess a dog's behavioral phenotype. This test is also known to have a very high predictability of dogs' future biting behavior and a very high correlation between dogs' biting behavior during the test and their biting behavior in the past (33). In our study, we focused on the expression of the behavior and not on motivation for such behavior or target. Police working dogs were selected for the aggressive group because, according to Haverbeke et al. (34), the vast majority of military working dogs behave aggressively during the SAB test. For the non-aggressive group, highly trained dogs (e.g., show, rescue, therapy dogs) known to behave calmly in a new and noisy environment and in the presence of unfamiliar people (35, 36), of the same sex and age were selected. According to the breed nomenclature of the Fédération Cynologique Internationale, the dogs studied were all from the same classified breed group - sheepdogs. A final chosen criterion for inclusion in the dog groups was the behavior shown during the test. We decided to categorize dogs as aggressive if they attacked at least once during the test (37). The dogs primarily placed in the aggressive dogs that failed to exhibit biting behavior and dogs in the non-aggressive group exhibiting biting behavior were excluded from the study.

To provide a comprehensive physiological profile of an aggressive dog, neuroendocrine and cardiovascular parameters were measured simultaneously and non-invasively during the behavioral test of aggressive reactivity. Our main predictions were that aggressive dogs would show neuroendocrine activation measured by increased salivary CORT, but decreased SER, with concomitant cardiovascular activation measured as decreased facial and body surface temperature.

Materials and Methods

The study was carried out between July and October of 2020 in Ljubljana, Slovenia and was approved by the Administration of the Republic of Slovenia for Food Safety, Veterinary Sector and Plant Protection (U34401-17/2020/10). The dog owners and handlers signed an informed consent form and were given the right to withdraw from the study at any time if the dog showed signs of stress or without giving a reason.

Animals

Two groups of dogs (aged between 12 and 36 months) with different behavioral backgrounds participated in the study. The aggressive group consisted of 16 male German and Belgian Shepherd police dogs that were reported to have been aggressive during training in the past. The non-aggressive group consisted of 15 male herding dogs of different breeds, trained to behave calmly in new situations and with no known history of aggression by humans. All dogs were without cardiovascular or sensory problems and all, except two in the non-aggressive group, were neutered. The police dogs were recruited through the Slovenian Ministry of Interior, and the privately owned dogs were recruited through Slovenian dog clubs and social media. To reach the test site, 19 dogs that were used to traveling longer distances traveled by car, while others were housed in kennels at the site.

Behavioral Recordings

Aggressive reactivity was assessed using the Socially Acceptable Behavior (SAB) test (33). The SAB test consisted of 16 subtests (Supplementary Table 1) and was administered outdoors in a specially set up test area (Figure 1) adapted from Planta and De Meester (33). Each subtest lasted 20 s, with the time in between kept as short as possible. The test was performed by the 3 experimenters. The lead experimenter instructed the owner/handler and guided him through the test, while the other two performed the tasks (e.g., pulling up the blanket). The total duration of the test was approximately 10 min per dog, mainly depending on the dog's cooperation in taking the thermographic images after each subtest. For safety reasons, the dogs were equipped with a harness, a leash, and an additional fixed leash [in subtests (1, 616)]. The owner/handler was present during subtests 1 through 7 and 16 and either held the dog on a short leash or the dog was tethered with a double 1.5 m fixed leash.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic plan of the test area.

Behavior was videotaped and coded using the Solomon coder (© 2019 by András Péter). Using the scoring method introduced by van der Borg et al. (37), an aggression and anxiety score was assigned to each dog. During each subtest, aggression was scored on a 3-point scale, with 0 points assigned if there were no signs of aggression, 1 point if the dog showed threat (e.g., growling, baring teeth, staring), and 2 points if the dog attacked (e.g., snapping, biting, lunging). Anxiousness was scored on a 5-point scale indicating whether the behavior was safe (0 points), unsafe (1 point), fearful (2 points), extremely fearful (3 points), and panicky (4 points). Scores were cumulative, with a maximum of 32 points for the aggression score and 64 points for the anxiety score. These scores represented the highest aggression and anxiety scores. More detailed behavioral reactivity during the SAB test was analyzed either as duration or frequency of occurrence using a predefined ethogram (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive ethogram of the observed behaviors during SAB test.

Reliability coding was performed for 20% of the videos. The consistency between two coders for frequencies using Cohen's Kappa (κ) was 0.87 and for continuous variables using an intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) was 0.96.

Sampling and Data Collection of the Salivary CORT and SER

Saliva samples for the assessment of salivary CORT and SER were collected on three occasions. Samples in the home environment (home samples) were collected when the dog was relaxed and resting. Pre-test samples were collected immediately before the start of the SAB test (approximately 5 minutes after arrival at the test area), while post-test samples for SER were collected immediately after the behavioral test and for CORT were collected 20 minutes later, as the dog's CORT peaks approximately 20 minutes after contact with a stressor (40). Samples were collected using commercially available cotton swabs in plastic tubes (Salivette®, Sarstedt, Germany), following the procedure described by Glenk et al. (41). For safety reasons, saliva samples were collected by the owner/handler. To avoid contamination of the samples, the dogs were not allowed to eat or drink for 30 min before sampling and the person collecting the sample wore latex gloves. Cotton swabs were rotated in both sides of the dog's cheek pouch until saturated with saliva, for at least 30 s. The cotton swabs were used to collect samples. Swabs were examined for visible contamination before being placed in plastic tubes and temporarily stored in a freezer at −20 °C before final storage. The samples were stored for 2–3 weeks. To obtain clear saliva, swabs were thawed and centrifuged at 1,500 g for 15 min at room temperature.

Commercial enzyme immunoassay kits (Cortisol free in Saliva ELISA DES6611; Demeditec Diagnostic Gmbh Germany and Serotonin Research ELISA DEE5900; Demeditec Diagnostic Gmbh Germany) were used for the determination of CORT and SER. ELISA kits have previously been used for CORT (42) and SER (43) assessment in dogs. Samples were tested in duplicate (1:10 dilution for SER samples). The sensitivity of the assay was 0.02 ng/mL for CORT and 0.005 ng/mL for SER. Although owners/handlers were familiar with the procedure, they were not always successful in collecting samples and from the total of 144 samples, saliva could not be extracted from 32 samples (22.2, 8.3% in the non-aggressive group and 13.9% in the aggressive group) due to limited sample volume. One outlier (home CORT in the aggressive group) exceeded the mean by more than 13 standard deviations and was removed from the statistical analysis.

Thermal Imaging Procedure

Surface temperature was measured three times using infrared thermography. Thermographic infrared images were taken with a portable thermographic camera (Optris PI 640). Images of the dog's facial area and body side were taken immediately before entering the test area (pre-test images) and immediately after the test was completed (post-test images). Body image were taken laterally, from a distance of approximately 2 m (from 1.8–2.5 m, depending on the dog's cooperation). The owner/handler stood sideways to the dog (out of the image) and held the dog by the leash. The side of the body from which the picture was taken was balanced between the dogs. Facial images were taken frontally, from a distance of 30–50 cm. Thermal images of the facial area (during the test images) were also taken during SAB the test, after completion of each subtest. As it is known that images taken in the field can be disturbed by the dog's coat characteristics, the distance between the subject and the camera, and environmental factors such as wind and humidity (4446). Air temperature (°C), humidity (%) and wind (km/h) were measured before taking pre-test images of an individual dog. Dog characteristics (body weight, coat length, and coat color) were also recorded. Thermal images were analyzed using Optris PI Connect software (Rel. 2.15.2219.0). The facial temperature (before, after and during the test) was calculated from the mean values of the warmest points in the image, while the mean value of the observed body side represented the body temperature (before and after the test).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SAS/STAT software, version 9.4 of the SAS System for Windows (© 2002-2012 SAS Institute Inc). After participating in the SAB test, 2 dogs from the non-aggressive group were excluded from the analysis because they showed biting behavior, and there were 5 dogs from the aggressive group that did not show even a single attack. The final non-aggressive group included 13 dogs, while the aggressive group included 11 dogs. Normal distribution for the quantitative traits was determined using the Shapiro-Wilk test. All reported P-values that were <0.05 were considered statistically significant or tended to be significant if P-values were <0.10. Means with standard deviations (SD) or percentages were calculated for descriptive statistics.

For behavioral analysis, due to low occurrence, the variables teeth baring (n = 0 occurrences) and yawning (n = 3 occurrences) were excluded, and the variables snapping and attacking, fleeing and retreating, growling, and snarling were combined into snapping/attacking, fleeing/retreating, and barking/ growling. For each variable, the sum of the occurrences collected during the subtests was used. The difference between groups in demographic variables, behavioral variables, and aggression and anxiety scores was examined. Independent samples t-test with the instruction PROC TTEST was used to compare normally distributed variables. Mann-Whitney U-test with the statement PROC NPAR1WAY was applied for variables that were not normally distributed.

Temperature change relative to baseline values was used for data analyzes. There were 18 (4.7%) missing values from images during the test due to technical problems. Physiological data were analyzed using the general linear model (GLM) procedure. For the three cardiovascular models (facial temperature, body temperature, and facial temperature during the test), the fixed effect of group (n = 2, aggressive and non-aggressive groups), coat color (n = 4; brown, black, tricolor, black-brown), side of measurement (n = 2; left and right; for the body temperature model only), and subtest (n = 16; for the facial temperature during the test model only) on surface temperature was tested. Models also included age, body weight, coat length, humidity, wind, and aggression and anxiety scores as covariates and dog as a repeated measure (for the facial temperature during test model only). For the neuroendocrine models, the fixed effect of group, time of sampling (n = 3; home, pre-test, post-test), and age, body weight, aggression score, and anxiety score as covariates were tested at CORT and SER. The dog was included as a repeated measure. When a significant effect was found, the LSMEANS and ESTIMATE statements were used to compare means and estimate contrast between factor levels. To find significant differences when more than two means were compared, a multiple post-hoc test Tukey-Kramer was used. Non-significant variables were eliminated and the final model consisted of significant effects only. The final models achieved R-squared values ranging from 0.23 to 0.27.

To test the association between behavioral and physiological variables within dog groups, Spearman's rank correlation was applied using the PROC CORR statement. For this analysis, in addition to behavioral and surface temperature variables, home values (home CORT and SER) and changes between pre- and post-test CORT and SER were used (CORT and SER change). Only strong correlations (r ≥ 0.7) are presented in this manuscript.

Results

The selected dogs were of similar age (aggressive dogs: n = 11 dogs; mean age: 20 ± 4.9 months; non-aggressive dogs: n = 13; mean age: 24 ± 7 months, t = 1.59, p = 0.13) and coat length (4.3 cm ± 0.9 vs. 6.4 cm ± 4; t = 1.70, p = 0.1), but aggressive dogs were heavier than non-aggressive dogs (33.6 kg ± 3.1 vs. 25.3 ± 9.3 kg; t = −2.84, p = 0.01). After data inspection, the two neutered dogs within the non-aggressive group did not stand out in their values for all physiological and behavioral parameters compared to the rest.

Behavioral Testing

Aggressive dogs had a higher aggression score (10.18 ± 6.31) than non-aggressive dogs (0.46 ± 0.66, Z = −4.20, p < 0.0001), but an indifferent anxiety score (aggressive dogs = 6.18 ± 2.71; non-aggressive dogs = 8.15 ± 4.81, Z = −0.84, p = 0.40). Individual aggressive dogs that snapped or attacked 3 to 26 times per test (mean: 14 ± 6.8) were more likely to show lower body posture, more movement, staring, snapping/attacking, rapid barking, tail wagging, and left tail wagging than non-aggressive dogs (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Behavioral differences in the SAB test between dog groups.

Measurement of the Salivary CORT and SER

Dog groups did not differ in CORT or SER, but timing of sampling influenced CORT (Table 3), with CORT tending to be lower at home than before the test (p = 0.06). As shown in Table 3, the covariate anxiety score was significant for CORT and SER, while the aggression score was significant for SER. Dogs with higher anxiety levels had higher CORT and SER, but those with higher aggression levels had lower SER.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effects of variables tested on CORT and SER.

Surface Temperature Measurements

Aggressive dogs (Δ = 1.81 °C, LSMEANS = 4.19) had a significantly greater change in facial temperature during the test than non-aggressive dogs (Δ = 0.98 °C, LSMEANS = −0.03, F = 57.75, p < 0.0001), but similar facial changes (non-aggressive dogs: LSMEANS = 1.52, aggressive dogs: LSMEANS = 0.03, F = 0.30, p = 0.59) and body surface temperature (non-aggressive dogs: LSMEANS = 0.71, aggressive dogs: LSMEANS = 1.19, F = 0.01, p = 0.94). The change in facial surface temperature during the test was influenced by three variables (Table 4). Longer coat, lower humidity, and stronger wind increased or tended to increase temperature. Although one effect of the subtest showed a trend, there were no significant changes between subtests (Supplementary Table 2).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of continuous variables tested on changes (Δ) in surface temperature.

Relationship Between Behavioral and Physiological Measures

Several highly significant correlations were found within each dog group (Figure 2). In the aggressive group, aggression score correlated positively with moving, staring, snapping/attacking, rapid barking, and left tail wagging. Rapid barking correlated positively with moving, staring, and high posture. Staring correlated positively with rapid barking and negatively with snout licking and cover seeking. Tail wagging was positively correlated with low body posture. Left tail wag (side wag bias) was positively related to aggression score, moving, and high posture and negatively related to neutral posture. High posture was also negatively correlated with neutral posture and cover seeking.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significant correlations between aggression and anxiety score, behavioral and physiological variables (when r ≥ 0.7) within (A) the aggressive group and (B) the aggressive group of dogs.

In the non-aggressive group, aggression score was positively correlated with low posture, growling/barking, and higher home CORT. Home CORT also correlated positively with growling/barking. Anxiety score correlated positively with stretching the leash. Low posture correlated positively with growling/barking, fleeing/retreating, and leash stretching. Neutral posture correlated negatively with snout licking and left tail wagging. SER change correlated positively with tail wagging.

Combining behavioral, neuroendocrine, and cardiovascular data, the aggressive dogs in our study were characterized by a lower posture with ears held back. They stared, snapped, or attacked, barked rapidly, and wagged their tails frequently, especially on the left side. The physiological profile included increased facial surface temperature and lower SER.

Discussion

In this study, simultaneous and non-invasive observation of behavioral, cardiovascular, and neuroendocrine changes during aggressive reactivity provided a profile of an aggressive dog that has never been presented before. Although we cannot fully support our hypothesis of neuroendocrine activation, as we found no difference in the serotonergic system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in aggressive dogs compared to non-aggressive dogs, our results suggest that higher levels of aggression are associated with lower levels of salivary SER. We confirmed cardiovascular activation, but the finding of increased facial temperature in aggressive dogs is contrary to our expectations. Our results also confirmed the importance of measuring tail wagging and side wagging in dogs when faced with emotional challenges.

Based on owner-reported history of aggression and display of biting behavior in a standardized dog behavior test, our dogs were successfully divided into the aggressive and non-aggressive groups. The phenotypic description of aggressive dogs with lower posture, ears held back, increased staring, snapping, attacking, and rapid barking was consistent with previous observations (34, 47). In addition to these known behaviors, we also observed and described for the first time an increased frequency of tail wagging and more frequent wagging to the left during aggression. Tail wagging is mainly reported in association with positive affective states in dogs (48, 49). However, Quaranta et al. (50) argued that dogs show asymmetric tail wagging in response to stimuli with different emotional valence. This asymmetry is due to differential activation of left and right brain structures (51). A higher amplitude of tail wagging on the right side has been found for stimuli that dogs perceive as positive, while the left side is perceived as negative (50). Measuring the frequency of tail wagging on each side and finding a high correlation of the amplitude of tail wagging to the left side with aggression level and aggression-related behaviors may indicate that left tail wagging is associated with aggressive behaviors. Because left tail wagging results from right hemisphere activation (51), our findings are consistent with other canine studies (52) and several other animal studies (53) that indicate right hemisphere specialization for the expression of intense emotions, including hostility and aggression. Further behavioral observations show that dogs had similarly low levels of anxiety and indifferent anxiety-related behavior whether or not they were characterized as aggressive. This, coupled with the fact that anxiety and aggression share similar physiological reactivity (32), leads us to believe that the physiological changes observed during the test are related only to aggression-related behaviors. We found that some of these behaviors, particularly aggressive threatening behaviors (e.g., growling, barking), were associated with CORT collected in the home neutral environment, but surprisingly only in dogs that do not normally respond aggressively (i.e., non-aggressive dogs).

To find an increase in pre-test CORT values compared to baseline values prior to test participation, albeit with a weak trend, could indicate emotional arousal rather than emotional valence according to Lewandowski et al. (54). Based on this and the evidence that physiology can be altered simply due to arriving in a new situation and meeting new people (55) or anticipation of an activity (56), we consider it less likely that the pretest release of CORT was triggered by transport-induced stress. Assuming that SER responds rapidly to environmental stimuli (24) and that no changes were found between home and pre-test SER, this further suggests that the factors that altered pre-test CORT did not represent a stressful experience for the dogs.

During testing, our results found similar activation of the stress and serotonergic systems in the two groups of dogs, which is not what would be expected based on the nature of the stimuli presented in the behavioral test (57) and based on previous research. We expected aggressive dogs to show higher HPA axis activity based on documentation in humans (58) or in dogs with a history of aggression (9) and during displays of aggression between dogs (59). Next, we expected these dogs to also have a lower SER because a reduced SER produces a generalized state of hyperirritability and lowers the threshold at which humans and animals respond to provocative stimuli (60). Our results were distinctive due to methodological differences and difficult to compare with other studies. Our dogs were tested during real-time aggression, whereas previous studies compared SER and CORT in dogs with or without owner-reported aggression history. Due to the fact that aggressive dogs are under the influence of an emotional attachment to their owner/handler (61), the owner/handler could represent a stress buffer for our dogs, influencing the dog's behavior and physiology, as previously observed for stimuli with a threatening approach (12), potentially masking the physiological changes that resulted from the aggression.

Because only a single bite attempt during the test was sufficient to classify the dog as aggressive, some dogs exhibited biting behavior on infrequent occasions, while some others attempted to bite up to 26 times during the test. Thus, the variability of aggression within the aggressive group was high. Highly aggressive dogs were found to have a lower SER, which is consistent with studies on dogs with a history of aggression (6, 8, 9). We find this result valid since it is known that SER plays a role in the neural control of aggression as an inhibitory regulator of aggressive reactivity (24) and dogs with a low SER have been associated with impaired impulse control (62). This phenomenon has been described as the serotonin deficiency hypothesis of aggression, demonstrating the inverse relationship between SER and aggression in humans (63) and non-human animals (64). Based on our results, it is reasonable to assume that neuromodulation, expressed as a lower SER, is evident only in dogs that exhibit high levels of aggressive behavior.

In addition to neuroendocrine activation, activation of the sympathetic nervous system leading to lower surface temperature has been documented in several animal studies when animals were presented with various aversive situations [monkeys: (18); dogs: (15, 17); rabbits: (19); pigs: (20)]. To our knowledge, only two studies examined surface temperature in an aggressive context [pigs: (20); dogs: (21)]. When comparing temperature change relative to baseline, Rigterink et al. (21) found an increase in eye temperature in both aggressive and non-aggressive dogs, whereas we found no such changes in facial or body surface temperature, regardless of aggression group. However, we believe that the discrepancy between the results is due to the fact that their aggressive group consisted of only 27% of dogs that showed aggressive reactivity during interaction with an unfamiliar person, whereas in our study all such dogs were included and their temperature changes were observed on a smaller area that is assumed to be highly reactive.

However, we observed an increase in facial surface temperature during an actual act of aggression in aggressive dogs, suggesting that aggression activates cardiovascular activation in real time, but not the stress axis, measured as increased salivary CORT. Assuming that eye temperature increases during both negative stressful experiences (15) and positive experiences in dogs of both sexes (49), the change in surface temperature could reflect emotional arousal but not necessarily emotional valence. This has also been suggested in pigs, where Boileu et al. (20) reported a decrease in dorsal surface temperature in pigs during social aggression in both winning and losing individuals. Ward et al. (65) reported an increase in aggressive behavior in males exposed to exercise-induced arousal, similar to what we found.

Our results further suggest that thermal images taken during the test may be considered a better indicator of cardiovascular activation after an aggressive response than the change in temperature before and after the test. This assumption should be taken with some caution, as an increase in surface temperature could be influenced by physical activity. Our aggressive dogs moved significantly more than non-aggressive dogs and exercise resulting in heat being dissipated through skeletal muscle, could lead to an increase in surface temperature (66). In addition, our study is the first field study of its kind to examine aggressive dogs outdoors, but this can be problematic for optimal thermal imaging data collection. These data are typically conducted indoors or in a controlled environment with constant temperature and humidity (11, 15, 19, 21). Our observation revealed that not only humidity and wind act as potential confounders on surface temperature, but also coat length.

In light of our findings, we believe that future studies of aggressive behavior in dogs should address certain methodological improvements. First, rather than looking for a specific phenotype, 'it would be preferable touse a dog as its own control (67), which could overcome the problems of inter-subject variability. Second, participants in our study found the method of saliva collection challenging, so we believe that an alternative method for easier and safer saliva collection, such as a collection tube or cotton head on a plastic handle (13), should be used, especially if a collecting individual is inexperienced and if the dogs involved are aggressive.

Conclusions

Although our study faces numerous methodological challenges, it represents an important step in simultaneously investigating animal behavioral and physiological responses in the field and in real time. Our work provides the first evidence that aggressive dogs can be characterized by serotonergic, measured as salivary SER, and cardiovascular features, measured as increased facial temperature, during an actual aggressive act. The discovery of novel aggression-related behaviors such as tail wagging and left tail wagging opens a new avenue for the study of lateralization in the context of aggression.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Administration of the Republic of Slovenia for Food Safety, Veterinary Sector and Plant Protection. Written informed consent was obtained from the owners for the participation of their animals in this study.

Author Contributions

EG and MZ: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, and writing—review and editing. EG: data collection, data curation, and writing—original draft preparation. MZ: supervision. Both authors approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

The neuroendocrine analysis was funded by the Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the dog owners and handlers who participated in this study. Special thanks go to the Service Dogs Training Section of the Slovenian police and Prison Administration of Republic of Slovenia. We are very grateful to Živa Hernaus and Špela Zarnik for helping with the testing or coding videos and Alja Willenpart for providing the testing area, and Nataša Debeljak for creating the main graphic.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fvets.2021.683858/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Sforzini E, Michelazzi M, Spada E, Ricci C, Carenzi C, Milani S, et al. Evaluation of young and adult dogs' reactivity. J Vet Behav. (2009) 4:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.09.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1999) 23:925–35. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Horváth Z, Igyártó BZ, Magyar A, Miklósi A. Three different coping styles in police dogs exposed to a short-term challenge. Horm Behav. (2007) 52:621–30. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Schjolden J, Backström T, Pulman KGT, Pottinger TG, Winberg S. Divergence in behavioural responses to stress in two strains of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchuss mykiss) with contrasting stress responsiveness. Horm Behav. (2005) 48:537–44. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Oxley JA, Christley R, Westgarth C. Contexts and consequences of dog bite incidents. J Vet Behav. (2018) 23:33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2017.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Çakiroglu D, Meral Y, Sancak AA, Çifti G. Relationship between the serum concentrations of serotonin and lipids and aggression in dogs. Vet Rec. (2007) 161:59–61. doi: 10.1136/vr.161.2.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Craig L, Meyers-Manor JE, Anders K, Sütterlin S, Miller H. The relationship between heart rate variability and dog aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2017) 188:59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.12.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. León M, Rosado B, García-Belenguer S, Chacón G, Villegas A, Palacio J. Assessment of serotonin in serum, plasma, and platelets of aggressive dogs. J Vet Behav. (2012) 7:348–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2012.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rosado B, Garcia-Belenguer S, León M, Chacon G, Villegas A, Palacio J. Blood concentrations of serotonin, cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone in aggressive dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2010) 123:124–30. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Reefmann N, Wechsler B, Gygax L. Behavioural and physiological assessment of positive and negative emotion in sheep. Anim Behav. (2009) 78:651–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ermatinger FA, Brügger RK, Burkart JM. The use of infrared thermography to investigate emotions in common marmosets. Physiol Behav. (2019) 211:112672. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gácsi M, Maros K, Sernkvist S, Faragó T, Miklósi A. Human analogue safe haven effect of the owner: behavioural and heart rate response to stressful social stimuli in dogs. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e58475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lensen RCMM, Betremieux C, Bavegems V, Sys SU, Moons CPH, Diederich C. Validity and reliability of cardiac measures during behavioural tests in pet dogs at home. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2017) 186:56–63. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Essner A, Sjöström R, Ahlgren E, Gustås P, Edge-Hughes L, Zetterberg L, et al. Comparison of polar® RS800CX heart rate monitor and electrocardiogram for measuring inter-beat intervals in healthy dogs. Physiol Behav. (2015) 138:247–53. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Travain T, Colombo ES, Heinzl E, Belluci D, Previde EP, Valsecci P. Hot dogs: thermography in the assessment of stress in dogs (Canis familiaris) – a pilot study. J Vet Behav. (2015) 10:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bouwknecht JA, Olivier B, Paylor RE. The stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm as a physiological animal model for anxiety: a review of pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2007) 31:41–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Part CE, Kiddie JL, Hayes WA, Mills DS, Neville RF, Morton DB, et al. (2014). Physiological, physical and behavioural changes in dogs (Canis familiaris) when kennelled: testing the validity of stress parameters. Physiol Behav. (2014) 133:260–71. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kuraoka K, Nakamura K. The use of nasal skin temperature measurements in studying emotion in macaque monkeys. Physiol Behav. (2011) 102:347–55. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ludwig N, Gargano M, Luzi F, Carenzi C, Verga M. Technical note: applicability of infrared thermography as a non invasive measurement of stress in rabbit. World Rabbit Sci. (2007) 15:199–206. doi: 10.4995/wrs.2007.588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Boileau A, Farish M, Turner SP, Camerlink I. Infrared thermography of agonistic behaviour in pigs. Physiol Behav. (2019) 210:112637. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Rigterink A, Moore GE, Ogata N. Pilot study evaluating surface temperature in dogs with or without fear-based aggression. J Vet Behav. (2018) 28:11–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Cannon WB. Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear, and Rage: an Account of Recent Researchers Into the Function of Emotional Excitement. New York, NY: D Appleton & Company. (1915). doi: 10.1037/10013-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Veissier I, Boissy A. Stress and welfare: two complementary concepts that are intrinsically related to the animal's point of view. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:429–33. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Summers CH, Winberg S. Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression. J Exp Biol. (2006) 209:4581–9. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Puglisi-Allegra S, Andolina D. Serotonin and stress coping. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 277:58–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.07.052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Bacqué-Cazenave J, Bharatiya R, Barrière G, Delbecque J-P, Bouguiyoud N. Di Giovanni, et al. Serotonin in animal cognition and behavior. Int J Mol Sci. (2020) 21:1649. doi: 10.3390/ijms21051649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. (2013) 108:44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Montoya ER, Terburg D, Bos PA, van Honk J. Testosterone, cortisol, and serotonin as key regulators of social aggression: A review and theoretical perspective. Motiv Emot. (2012) 36:65–73. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9264-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Cook NJ. Review: Minimally invasive sampling media and the measurement of corticosteroids as biomarkers of stress in animals. Can J Anim Sci. (2012) 92:227–59. doi: 10.4141/cjas2012-045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Vincent IC, Michell AR. Comparison of cortisol concentrations in saliva and plasma of dogs. Res Vet Sci. (1992) 53:342–5. doi: 10.1016/0034-5288(92)90137-Q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lensen CMM, Moons CPH, Diederich C. Saliva sampling in dogs: how to select the most appropriate procedure for your study. J Vet Behav. (2015) 10:504–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hydbring-Sandberg E, von Walter LW, Höglund K, Svartberg K, Swenson L, Forkman B. Physiological reactions to fear provocation in dogs. J Endocrinol. (2004) 180:439–48. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1800439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Planta JUD, De Meester RHWM. Validity of the Socially Acceptable Behavior (SAB) test as a measure of aggression in dogs towards non-familiar humans. Vlaams Diergeneeskd Tijdschr. (2007) 76:359–68.

Google Scholar

34. Haverbeke A, De Smet A, Depiereux E, Giffoy JM, Diedrich C. Assessing undesired aggression in military working dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2009) 117:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Beebe SC, Howell TJ, Bennett PC. Using scent detection dogs in conservation settings: a review of scientific literature regarding their selection. Front Vet Sci. (2016) 3:96. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2016.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Mongillo P, Pitteri E, Adamelli S, Bonichini S, Farina L, Marinelli L. Validation of a selection protocol of dogs involved in animal-assisted intervention. J Vet Behav. (2015) 10:103–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. van der Borg JAM, Beerda B, Ooms M, Silveira, de Souza A, van Hagen M, Kemp B. Evaluation of behaviour testing for human directed aggression in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2010) 128:78–90. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gähwiler S, Bremhorst A, Tóth K, Riemer S. Fear expressions of dogs during New Year fireworks: a video analysis. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:16035. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72841-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Foyer P, Svedberg AM, Nilsson E, Wilsson E, Faresjö Å, Jensen P. Behavior and cortisol responses of dogs evaluated in a standardized temperament test for military working dogs. J Vet Behav. (2016) 11:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hennessy MB, Williams MT, Miller DD, Douglas CW, Voith VL. Influence of male and female petters on plasma cortisol and behaviour: Can human interaction reduce the stress of dogs in a public animal shelter? Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1998) 61:63–77. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00179-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Glenk LM, Kothgassner OD, Stetina BU, Palme R, Kepplinger B, Baran H. Salivary cortisol and behavior in therapy dogs during animal-assisted interventions: a pilot study. J Vet Behav. (2014) 9:98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Dreschel NA, Granger DA. Methods of collection for salivary cortisol measurement in dogs. Horm Behav. (2009) 55:163–8. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Scarsella E, Cintio M, Iacumin L, Ginaldi F, Stefanon B. Interplay between neuroendocrine biomarkers and gut microbiota in dogs supplemented with grape proanthocyanidins: results of dietary intervention study. Animals. (2020) 10:531. doi: 10.3390/ani10030531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Church JS, Hegadoren PR, Paetkau MJ, Miller CC, Regev-Shoshani G, Schaefer AL, et al. Influence of environmental factors on infrared eye temperature measurements in cattle. Res Vet Sci. (2014) 96:220–6. doi: 10.1016/j.rvsc.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Jansson A, Lindgren G, Velie BD, Solé M. An investigation into factors influencing basal eye temperature in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) when measured using infrared thermography in field conditions. Physiol Behav. (2020) 228:113218. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. McCafferty DJ. The value of infrared thermography for research on mammals: previous applications and future directions. Mammal Rev. (2007) 37:207–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2907.2007.00111.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Netto WJ, Planta JUD. Behavioural testing for aggression in the domestic dog. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1997) 52:243–63. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01126-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. McGowan RT, Rehn T, Norling Y, Keeling LJ. Positive affect and learning: exploring the “Eureka Effect” in dogs. Anim Cogn. (2014) 17:577–87. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0688-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Travain T, Colombo ES, Grandi LC, Heinzl EUL, Pelosi A, Prato-Previde E, et al. How good is this food? A study on dogs' emotional responses to a potentially pleasant event using infrared thermography. Physiol Behav. (2016) 159:80–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Quaranta A, Siniscalchi M, Vallortigara G. Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr Biol. (2007) 17:R199–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Buxton DF, Goodman DC. Motor function and the corticospinal tracts in the dog and raccoon. J Comp Neurol. (1967) 129:341–60. doi: 10.1002/cne.901290405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Siniscalchi M, Bertino D, d'Ingeo S, Quaranta A. Relationship between motor laterality and aggressive behavior in sheepdogs. Symmetry. (2019) 11:233. doi: 10.3390/sym11020233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Rogers LJ. Relevance of brain and behavioural lateralization to animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2010) 127:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lewandowski GW Jr, Mattingly BA, Pedreiro A. Under pressure: the effects of stress on positive and negative relationship behaviors. J Soc Psychol. (2014) 154:463–73. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2014.933162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ng ZY, Pierce BJ, Otto CM, Buechner-Maxwell VA, Siracusa C, Werre SR. The effect of dog–human interaction on cortisol and behavior in registered animal-assisted activity dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2014) 159:69–81. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Horváth Z, Dóka A, Miklósi Á. Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directions. Horm Behav. (2008) 54:107–14. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. King T, Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ. Fear of novel and startling stimuli in domestic dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2003) 82:45–64. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00040-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Haller J, Mikics E, Halász J, Tóth M. Mechanisms differentiating normal from abnormal aggression: glucocorticoids and serotonin. Eur J Pharmacol. (2005) 526:89–100. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wormald D, Lawrence AJ, Carter G, Fisher AD. Physiological stress coping and anxiety in greyhounds displaying inter-dog aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2016) 180:93–9. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Berman ME, Tracy JI, Coccaro EF. The serotonin hypothesis of aggression revisited. Clin Psychol Rev. (1997) 17:651–65. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00039-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Gobbo E, Zupan M. Dogs' sociability, owners' neuroticism and attachment style to pets as predictors of dog aggression. Animals. (2020) 10:315. doi: 10.3390/ani10020315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Reisner IR, Mann JJ, Stanley M, Huang YY, Houpt KA. Comparison of cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolite levels in dominant-aggressive and non-aggressive dogs. Brain Res. (1996) 714:57–64. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01464-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Duke AA, Bègue L, Bell R, Eisenlohr-Moul T. Revisiting the serotonin-aggression relation in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2013) 139:1148–72. doi: 10.1037/a0031544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Ferrari PF, Palanza P, Parmigiani S, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA. Serotonin and aggressive behavior in rodents and nonhuman primates: predispositions and plasticity. Eur J Pharmacol. (2005) 526:259–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ward A, Mann T, Westling EH. Creswell, DJ, Ebert JP, Wallaert M. Stepping up the pressure: arousal can be associated with a reduction in male aggression. Aggress Behav. (2008) 34:584–92. doi: 10.1002/ab.20270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Cilulko J, Janiszewski P, Bogdaszewski M, Szczygielska E. Infrared thermal imaging in studies of wild animals. Eur J Wildl Res. (2013) 59:17–23. doi: 10.1007/s10344-012-0688-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Zupan M, Buskas J, Altimiras J, Keeling LJ. Assessing positive emotional states in dogs using heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiol Behav. (2015) 155:102–11. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dog aggression, physiology, cortisol, serotonin, surface temperature, tail wagging

Citation: Gobbo E and Zupan Šemrov M (2021) Neuroendocrine and Cardiovascular Activation During Aggressive Reactivity in Dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 8:683858. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.683858

Received: 22 March 2021; Accepted: 14 July 2021;
Published: 09 August 2021.

Edited by:

Jeremy N. Marchant, Livestock Behavior Research Unit (USDA-ARS), United States

Reviewed by:

Andrew David Fisher, The University of Melbourne, Australia
Gonzalo Chávez, Santo Tomás University, Chile

Copyright © 2021 Gobbo and Zupan Šemrov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Gobbo, gobbo.elena@gmail.com

Download