Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 17 April 2024
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare

Factors associated with behavioral euthanasia in pet dogs

  • 1School of Animal Sciences, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University (Virginia Tech), Blacksburg, VA, United States
  • 2Department of Animal Behavior, Ecology, & Conservation, Canisius University, Buffalo, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Statistics, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University (Virginia Tech), Blacksburg, VA, United States
  • 4Virginia-Maryland College of Veterinary Medicine, Department of Population Health Sciences, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University (Virginia Tech), Blacksburg, VA, United States

When pet dogs demonstrate certain serious problem behaviors, this may lead owners to choose behavioral euthanasia. However, research on behavioral euthanasia of pet dogs is sparse and previously published papers have not specifically sampled owners who made the decision to euthanize for behavior. The Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs Questionnaire was created to collect a wide range of information from dog owners who made behavioral euthanasia decisions. Using this survey, we explored the types of behaviors associated with behavioral euthanasia. Human-directed aggression, especially toward adults living in the household, was the most frequently reported reason for choosing behavioral euthanasia; followed by aggression toward other animals, especially other dogs living in the same household. The majority of dogs displaying human-directed or other animal-directed aggression were reported to have bitten and broken skin, and many of these had bitten in multiple or severe incidents. Most dogs had lived in their homes and displayed problem behaviors for over a year prior to behavioral euthanasia, and the euthanasia occurred at a variety of ages, from less than 1 year old to 18 years old. Additional research is required to understand environmental or nonbehavioral factors contributing to the behavioral euthanasia of companion animals, as well as the psychosocial and emotional impact of behavioral euthanasia on the human experience. Understanding the behavioral factors associated with behavioral euthanasia can direct resources toward problem behavior interventions, improve public education about animal behavior, and strengthen the human-animal bond.

1 Introduction

According to the American Pet Products Association, 66% of U.S. households include companion animals (1). Owners are estimated to spend over $143 billion dollars caring for their companion animals in 2023 (1). A majority of these households (75%) have dogs, and those dogs are considered to be “family” by 86% percent of owners (25).

There are a number of potential challenges of living with and caring for dogs (6, 7). Dealing with problem behaviors can be one of those challenges, as demonstrated by studies reporting that 40–92% of dogs present with behavior issues (816). Understanding the occurrence and prevalence of problem behaviors in pet dogs can be challenging due to the diverse range of descriptions, terminology, and categorization used in research (1720). For example, the broad single category of “aggression” has included barking, non-injurious bites during play, and even injurious or fatal bites across studies (16, 21, 22). Within the category of aggression, researchers have classified aggressive behaviors by victim, circumstance or trigger, and motivation (17, 2326).

When pet dogs display problem behaviors, this can result in negative consequences for the community, the human-animal bond, the owner, and the dogs themselves (16, 21, 2735). Poor human-animal relationships and euthanasia decisions can also negatively impact professionals such as dog behavior consultants and veterinarians (36).

If behavior problems are seen as severe by the dog’s owners, such as aggression, owners might seek assistance from veterinary and animal behavior professionals or choose to remove the dog from their home by rehoming them to another household or relinquishing them to an animal shelter or rescue (17, 19, 26, 3739). When considering these negative consequences (such as having to seek professional help or rehoming the dog), aggressive behavior was identified as the most commonly reported problem behavior by multiple studies (17, 19, 23, 26, 37, 40).

Problem behaviors that increase the risk of harm or decrease welfare for the dog or others around them may result in euthanasia of the dog, despite the dog being physically healthy (41, 42). This is commonly known as behavioral euthanasia (BE). Few studies have attempted to identify the prevalence or risk factors for behavioral euthanasia in owned dogs (28, 4355). Several of these studies have focused on prevalence at a population level and have not delved into the specifics of the behaviors exhibited by those dogs, while others looked at risk factors within a smaller subset of dogs.

Despite the limited research on this topic, dog behavior practitioners are drawing attention to this area through conferences, online courses, articles, and blogs (5665).

Previous research suggests that the primary behavioral cause for euthanasia is aggression. In one study that investigated why dogs were surrendered for behavioral reasons to an animal shelter, (47) found that the most common reasons were human-directed aggression, bites to people or animals, or animal-directed aggression. This is in agreement with other studies that indicate aggression, particularly aggression toward people, is a major factor in behavioral euthanasia decisions (28, 4346, 4855).

However, the existing studies do not investigate these behaviors in-depth to evaluate commonalities in severity levels or details of aggression. Currently, practitioners must rely largely on anecdotal evidence when it comes to the behaviors most commonly associated with euthanasia in pet dogs. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to identify which specific behavioral factors in dogs were associated with behavioral euthanasia decisions by owners using the Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs Survey.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Subjects

Subjects were primarily recruited online from social media groups focusing on behavioral euthanasia, dog behavior, or dog ownership. Additional recruitment strategies included email list-servs and emails with veterinary behaviorists and trainers. The resulting sample is one of convenience rather than a statistically derived sampling method. Participation in the study was limited to adults in the United States who had euthanized a pet dog due to behavior problems any time on or after January 1st, 2017. The dog must have been owned by the respondent, not a dog in a shelter or foster environment.

In order to minimize potential recall bias, the sample was limited to those who had euthanized a dog for behavior concerns within approximately the 5 years immediately prior to survey completion (66).

2.2 Questionnaire

We designed a questionnaire to collect a broad range of information about dogs that were euthanized for behavior-related issues. Due to the exploratory nature of this research, only a portion of the questions used in this questionnaire could be obtained from validated, previously published questionnaires. For example, several questions about the euthanized dog and its living situation were adapted from questionnaires used in the Dog Aging Project (67). Human demographic information questions were retrieved from the 2020 U.S. Census (68). The remaining questions were written based on the consensus opinion of more than 20 experts in dog behavior. These experts included board certified veterinary behaviorists, animal behavior researchers, certified dog behavior consultants, and dog trainers who specialize in complex cases.

The resulting Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs questionnaire was designed to focus on simple descriptions and lay terminology wherever possible. This limits potential differences in responses based on level of behavioral education. Questions used situational descriptions such as whether the dog showed aggression when “approached while eating,” vs. potential labels or causes for that behavior (e.g., dominance or resource guarding).

The questionnaire consisted of 118 questions designed to cover a wide range of information about dogs that were euthanized for behavioral reasons. Respondents were asked for basic signalment and information about the dog, their living situation, and their acquisition. They then ranked up to three categories of behavior as being the primary reasons for the euthanasia (aggression toward people; aggression toward dogs or other animals; fear, anxiety, or stress; compulsive behaviors; separation anxiety; and other behaviors). These ranked categories determined which survey sections respondents would see later in the survey, with branching logic showing the respondent detailed behavior questions related to the behaviors they had listed. The questionnaire also included questions about treatments or interventions respondents had attempted prior to euthanasia as well as non-behavioral factors that contributed to the euthanasia decision.

Behavior questions were written based on results and methods from previous research as well as anecdotal experience from behavior experts (28, 4355). These questions include information about types of behaviors, specific targets or circumstances for those behaviors, the severity involved, and any precursors to those behaviors.

In order to understand comorbid behaviors and patterns, most questions allowed respondents to select as many responses as applied. Thus response count (total number of responses submitted by all respondents) and respondent count (total number of respondents who answered that question) are listed throughout.

This research project was approved by the Virginia Tech Institutional Review Board, with exemption under 45 CFR 46.104(d) category (ies) 2(i) with IRB number 22–311. The first page of the survey included a consent form.

Summary descriptive statistics were calculated using JMP Pro Version 16.1.0 (69) (RRID: SCR_022199). All figures were created using R (70) [RRID: SCR_001905; (71), RRID: SCR_014601; (72), RRID: SCR_024824; (73), RRID: SCR_024825; (74), RRID: SCR_024826].

3 Results

We recruited a total of 800 respondents who met the selection criteria. Of these, 729 completed the questions about dog signalment and sourcing, and 690 completed the ranking of primary behaviors that led to the decision to euthanize. Finally, 575 respondents completed the entire survey through the demographic questions at the end. Data analysis of each question included all responses to that question; therefore, the response count varies by question (see Supplementary Figure for details).

Of the 575 respondents who provided demographic information, 94.5% identified as women and 91.3% identified as White, Non-Hispanic. Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 65+ years old, but over one third of all respondents reported being ages 25–34 at the time of the dog’s euthanasia. Almost two thirds of respondents reported having a bachelor’s degree or higher level of education or training. At the time of euthanasia, 34.1% of respondents had a household annual income of $50,000–$99,000. The next most common income range was $100,000–$199,000 (29.1%), followed by under $50,000 per year (21.2%).

The respondents reported on 729 dogs, which included 69.4% males and 30.6% females, with 89.2% of all dogs being spayed or neutered. These dogs ranged from three kilograms to 102 kilograms in weight, with a median of 27 kilograms and a mean of 28 kilograms. Over half of the dogs were acquired prior to 1 year of age.

3.1 Primary behavior problems that led to euthanasia

The “Primary Behaviors” question in the Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs questionnaire asked respondents to rank up to three behavior categories that contributed to their behavioral euthanasia decision. In this study, 690 respondents completed the survey section that provided information on the primary, secondary, and tertiary behavioral reason for euthanasia.

Aggression toward people was the most commonly reported behavior category at 33.8% of all responses (78.6% of respondents), followed by aggression toward dogs or other animals with 27.9% of responses (64.7% of respondents; Figure 1). Fear, anxiety, and stress accounted for 22.6% of responses (52.8% of respondents), with separation anxiety and compulsive behaviors represented in 6.0 and 5.7% of responses, respectively, (13.9 and 13.2% of respondents). Only 4.0% of responses were for the “Other” category of behavior (9.0% of respondents).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Primary behavior categories that led to owners’ behavioral euthanasia decision. Legend: average rank: aggression toward people 1.33, aggression toward animals 1.73, fear, anxiety, or stress 2.17, separation anxiety 2.35, compulsive behavior 2.31, other 2.53. Total number of responses (regardless of rank) by behavior type. Total response count = 1,604, total number of respondents n = 690. Respondents could rank up to three behaviors. Percentage represents portion of respondents (690), not portion of responses.

Of the 19.7% who only chose a single behavior, 63.2% of responses were aggression toward people and an additional 30.2% were aggression toward dogs or other animals. There were only six respondents who selected fear, anxiety, or stress as the only problem behavior leading to euthanasia. Two respondents chose “Other,” and one chose compulsive behavior.

In addition to calculating total number of responses per category, we also separated them by participants’ ranking (first, second, or third most problematic; Figure 2). Aggression toward people was the most highly ranked as a problematic behavior, with an overall average ranking of 1.33/3 ignoring non-rankings. It was listed as the primary behavior that led to a euthanasia decision by 57.4% of all respondents and was ranked in the top three factors by 78.6% of all respondents. Of the 542 respondents who included aggression toward people as one of their top three reasons for euthanasia, 73.1% marked it as the number one cause, with 20.8% marking it second and 6.1% marking it as the third most important factor.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Problem behaviors leading to behavioral euthanasia, ranked as primary, secondary, and tertiary reasons. AggP, aggression toward people. AggD, aggression toward dogs or other animals. FAS, fear, anxiety, or stress. SA, separation anxiety. Com, compulsive behavior. Legend: Rank: 1st (black), 2nd (medium gray), 3rd (lightest gray). Total response count = 1,609, total number of respondents n = 690.

While aggression toward people was commonly ranked as the most important reason for euthanasia, categories like fear, anxiety, and stress or compulsive behaviors were more likely to be ranked second or third. Of those who marked fear, anxiety, or stress as one of their ranked behaviors, only 20.0% ranked it first vs. 42.6% who ranked it second and 37.4% who ranked it third.

Comorbidities were common among the various categories of behavior, with 80.1% of respondents selecting more than one primary behavior. The most common combinations of categories were aggression toward people followed by aggression toward animals, with 26.8% of respondents choosing this option. The next most common combination was aggression toward people followed by fear, anxiety, or stress (12.9% of respondents).

3.2 Time in the home, age, and onset of behavior problems prior to euthanasia

The majority of dogs had been in the home for at least a year prior to euthanasia, and 42.3% had been in the home more than 3 years. Additionally, most dogs began displaying the problem behavior at least 1 year prior (60.4%, Figure 3). Twenty six percent of dogs had only shown the behavior for 3–12 months prior to euthanasia, and only 5.9% of dogs began the behavior less than 1 month before.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Time since onset of problem behavior (s) by length of time in home at time of behavioral euthanasia. Legend: time since onset (from lightest color to darkest): less than 1 week, 1–4 weeks, 1–3 months, 3–12 months, 1–3 years, More than 3 years. n = 639.

The dogs represented in this study were euthanized at ages ranging from under 1 year old to 18 years old, with a median of 3 years old and a mean of 4.31 years old (Figure 4). Only 3.6% of dogs were less than 1 year old at euthanasia, and 6.6% were 10 years old or older. Of those older dogs, 81.0% had been in the home for at least 3 years. However, the onset of problem behaviors within all of the older dogs was varied. Of these dogs that were euthanized at age 10 or older, 42.9% had demonstrated the problem behavior (s) for at least 3 years. However, only 21.4% had shown it for one to 3 years and another 23.8% demonstrated the problem behavior from three to 12 months. The remaining 11.9% of dogs 10 or older only began exhibiting the behavior issue less than 3 months prior to euthanasia.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Age of the dog at time of behavioral euthanasia. n = 634, mean age = 4.31 years, SD = 308.

In addition to questions about age and onset of behavior problems, respondents were asked to identify whether there was a single behavioral incident or final incident (compared to no specific incident) that contributed to the behavioral euthanasia decision. The majority of respondents indicated that behavioral euthanasia was based on a final incident (77.9%), vs. 5.8% indicating a single incident and 16.3% reporting that no specific incident led to the decision.

3.3 Aggression toward people

3.3.1 Targets

Of the 542 respondents who selected aggression toward people as a primary behavior leading to the decision to euthanize, 476 responded to questions about the types of targets of the dog’s aggression. These types included familiar adults or unfamiliar children, for example, and respondents were able to choose multiple targets of aggression (Figure 5). Most respondents (83.8%) selected multiple targets of aggression, with a mean of 3.2 categories of people the dog aggressed toward. The remaining 16.2% selected a sole target for the dog’s aggression toward people. Of these 16.2%, over half reported aggression solely toward adults in the home.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Total responses for potential targets of aggression toward people. Total response count 1,532. The 476 respondents who said that their dog’s aggression toward people was one of the primary reasons for euthanasia were able to choose as many categories as applied.

Aggression solely toward adults in the home was more prevalent than expected: 10.3% of all respondents who answered the section about aggression toward people (n = 476) indicated that the dog was only aggressive toward adults in the home. The next most common response pattern (7.4%) was aggression toward all adults, followed by 4.6% reporting all options (children and adults).

Aggression solely toward children was rare, reported by only 2.7% of respondents. Even more uncommon was aggression specifically to a single category of child, with 1.9% of respondents marking children in the home as the target of aggression and 0.4% noting familiar children not living in the home as the sole target.

3.3.2 Predictability and triggers

Respondents were asked about their ability to predict their dog’s aggression toward people as well as their ability to notice warning signals prior to aggression. Forty two percent of respondents indicated that they could predict aggressive reactions “most of the time” or “always,” while only 30.9% of respondents in this section said the dog showed clear warning signals “most of the time” or “always.” However, both categories demonstrated wide variation ranging from “always” to “never.”

Respondents also reported the scenarios or triggers that led to aggression toward people. On average, respondents chose 5.12 of the 15 listed circumstances, with only 12.6% of the 476 respondents selecting a single trigger. The most common circumstances included being approached by an unfamiliar person while on leash (9.3% of respondents) or when a person moved quickly or erratically (8.8% of respondents). Other common responses were when delivery workers approached the house or when being touched or handled (7.7% each).

3.3.3 Bites and severity

Of the respondents that indicated their dog showing aggression toward people, 77.5% reported that the dog had bitten and broken skin on a person at least once. The median number of bites was three, and outliers reported as many as 50 to 100 skin breaking bites (Figure 6). Of the 369 dogs who had broken skin, 22.8% had bitten only once. However, almost 36.9% of biting dogs had bitten people at least four times, and 12.7% were reported to have bitten 10 or more times.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Number of skin-breaking bites reported per dog with aggression toward people. n = 369 and the two highest outliers (50 bite and 100 bites) were broken from the main axis for better visual representation of the main data. The upper and lower limits of the box denote the 25 and 75th percentiles at two bites and five bites, respectively. The median was three bites. The whiskers show the lower and upper limits of the observed values (excluding outliers) at one bite and eight bites, respectively.

If respondents indicated their dog had bitten a person and broken skin, they were asked to describe the severity of the inflicted bite wounds (Table 1). Respondents were able to select multiple wound types based on the bites their dog had inflicted. The most common wound type, accounting for 70.5% of total responses, was “1–4 small punctures from a single bite.” While 37.4% of bites were the least severe type noted above, an additional 18.4% of responses were small tears or lacerations. However, 41.9% of reported bites were more severe and would likely involve multiple bites within the same incident and/or require stitches. Of the reported bites, 29.1% would be considered most severe (including nine or more punctures, multiple or large tears or lacerations, or crushing injuries and broken bones).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Types of wounds inflicted in skin-breaking bites to people by dogs with aggression toward people.

In addition to assessing the overall prevalence of different types of bites, we analyzed the data to look at the most severe bite per dog. If a respondent indicated several types of bites, the category of their worst bite was recorded. When only looking at the highest severity level per dog, 37.6% of dogs had a bite in the “most severe” category.

While wounds were reportedly treated at home without professional medical attention 35.9% of the time, 10.8% of all reported bites required more than 10 stitches, other surgical intervention, or hospital admission. When considering just the worst reported bite per dog, the percent of bites requiring the aforementioned types of extensive medical treatment rises to 13.8%.

3.4 Aggression toward dogs or other animals

3.4.1 Targets

Aggression toward dogs or other animals was the second most commonly reported problem behavior that led to behavioral euthanasia, selected by 447 participants; 394 of these respondents answered the detailed questions pertaining to that aggression. Aggression toward dogs made up the majority of animal-directed aggression, with 62.7% of those 394 respondents reporting that their dog was only aggressive to other dogs, not to other animals. Overall, aggression toward dogs was reported by 76.7% of respondents who said their dog showed aggression toward animals. Among dogs that showed aggression toward other animals, the most common target of that aggression was another dog (or dogs) in the home (31.2% of 394; Table 2). The next most common targets were unfamiliar dogs (27.9%) and familiar dogs not living in the home (17.6%). The most common responses/combinations for this section were either dogs in the home (18.1% of respondents), unfamiliar dogs (10.7% of respondents), or a combination of the two (11.0%).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Total responses of types of targets for dogs’ aggression toward other dogs or other animals.

Of the respondents who reported their dog showing aggression toward other animals (n = 394), 133 indicated a single type of target of aggression. Of those 133, 66.2% aggressed toward other dogs in the home and 27.1% aggressed toward unfamiliar dogs. Only five respondents listed aggression toward cats in the home as the sole aggression toward other animals. No other targets (such as wildlife, unfamiliar cats, or livestock) were listed as sole choices, instead only being reported in combination with aggression toward dogs.

3.4.2 Triggers

Respondents were asked to indicate what circumstances or triggers would cause aggression toward other animals (Table 3). While the responses varied widely, the most commonly reported triggers were: being approached while on leash (10.4%), an escalation in play/excitement (9.5%), being approached while engaging with a preferred object like a toy or a bone (9.4%), and predatory behavior (9.4%).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Environmental stimuli/circumstances for aggression toward dogs or other animals.

3.4.3 Bites and severity

Of the 394 dogs that were reported to aggress toward other animals, 69.5% had previously bitten an animal and broken skin. Of the dogs that had bitten, 16.4% had done so only once. Another 21.2% had broken skin twice, with 19.6% biting three times. Almost 44% of dogs that had previously bitten had broken skin four times or more, while 13.9% had done it at least 10 times.

Of the skin-breaking bites to other animals, 29.4% were a single bite with 1–4 small punctures (Table 4). When looking at the dogs that had bitten (n = 274), 10.9% of respondents in this category reported that the other animal had been killed from the bite incident, and 6.6% said the other animal was wounded badly enough to be euthanized. Upon closer examination of the instances where the dog either fatally attacked another animal or caused injuries severe enough to necessitate euthanasia, the predominant factor was again aggression toward dogs. The other animals in these cases included dogs (18), cats (4), and wildlife (3) based on the free text responses, while other participants did not note the type of animal. According to notes provided by respondents, one dog was responsible for the deaths of two cats and another dog in the household. Additionally, another dog was reported to have caused the deaths of two dogs within the household.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Types of wounds inflicted in skin-breaking bites to dogs or other animals.

4 Discussion

The Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs Questionnaire was used to investigate factors associated with behavioral euthanasia. Aggression was the primary problem behavior reported by respondents that led to euthanasia. This is consistent with previous studies that identified aggression as the primary behavior concern that led to owners visiting a veterinary behaviorist or choosing to euthanize (17, 19, 26, 28, 46, 47).

Within the larger category of aggression, aggression toward people was both the most reported problem behavior overall and most likely to be selected as the primary reason for euthanasia. This was consistent with the findings of Anderson et al. (17), which showed the majority of aggression cases in a particular behavior clinic were human-directed aggression. This may be due to the safety risks involved with aggression to humans, or might indicate a prioritization of our own species’ safety (75, 76). When only one category of human-directed aggression was reported, aggression toward adults living in the home was the most frequently selected option. At behavior specialty clinics, owner-directed aggression is a commonly-identified type of human-directed aggression exhibited by dogs (23, 26, 53). This might suggest increased difficulty managing aggression toward household members vs. a visitor who is present less often. Aggressing toward household members might also contribute to breaking the human-animal bond resulting in a behavioral euthanasia decision (30, 41, 52, 77).

Aggression toward children was reported less often than aggression toward adults and was rarely selected as the sole target of the dog’s aggression. This is contrary to national dog bite data reporting that children are more frequently taken to the emergency department for dog bites than adults (33). However, it is also possible that post-bite treatment is more regularly sought for children due to the prevalence of bites to the face or higher bite severity to children (33, 78). Children familiar with and living with the dog were selected more frequently than unfamiliar children. This aligns with previous research which indicates that most pediatric bites were caused by a dog familiar to the child (79). While bites or aggression toward children occur frequently according to dog bite statistics, they do not appear to be a common reason for behavioral euthanasia unless combined with other behavior problems. This area warrants future research due to the serious physical and mental health ramifications of bites to children.

When considering the frequency and severity of dog bites, most of the dogs reported to have shown aggression toward people had bitten someone and broken skin. About two-thirds of those dogs had bitten up to three times, but the remaining dogs (36%) had bitten four or more times. While the number of bites alone is a strong indicator for overall behavioral concerns, the severity of the bites might also greatly influence potential euthanasia decisions. While many participants indicated that their dog had inflicted at least one minor bite of one to four small punctures, a substantial portion of responses indicated more severe bites (including those involving many punctures or large tears or lacerations, and those requiring extensive medical treatment). These suggest a heightened public safety risk and impacts on individuals and communities by way of injuries and medical expenses (28, 80). These findings are also consistent with previous research indicating that a history of bites, particularly severe bites, was a significant risk factor for behavioral euthanasia (52, 53).

Aside from human-directed aggression, aggression toward other animals was selected second overall to aggression toward people. Aggression toward other dogs was selected significantly more often than aggression toward non-dog animals. Aggression toward other dogs living in the same household was slightly more frequently reported than toward unfamiliar dogs (31% vs. 28%). However, when participants indicated that their dog only aggressed toward one type of target, other dogs in the home were the sole target more than twice as often as unfamiliar dogs (66% vs. 27%). This might be because dogs living in multi-dog households have more opportunity for intraspecific conflict than those that only encounter other dogs outside the home. This highlights the crucial need for a dog to safely coexist with humans and other dogs in their living environment, as there are serious consequences when they do not.

Aggression toward other dogs was reported more than three times as often as aggression toward other species. Unfamiliar cats, wildlife, and livestock were never the sole target of dog’s animal-directed aggression in this survey, and familiar cats living in the home were rarely marked as the sole target of the dog’s aggression. This might suggest that aggression toward non-dog animals is considered a less serious problem behavior and less likely to result in behavioral euthanasia. Aggression toward non-dog animals is even less likely to result in behavioral euthanasia if the other animals do not share the same household.

For dogs whose owners reported that they displayed aggression toward other animals, a majority of them had bitten another animal and broken skin at least once. Of the dogs who had caused injury to another animal, a significant percentage had done so multiple times (80.9%) and 10.9% had killed or fatally wounded the other animal in one or more incidents. This suggests that owners typically managed and/or sought to resolve this problematic behavior over extended periods of time and after damaging incidents, instead of hastily considering behavioral euthanasia decisions.

Predictability and the presence of warning signals are both considered prognostic factors for aggression risk, and may be useful in evaluating risk of behavioral euthanasia, but have not been objectively assessed (81, 82). Respondents indicated more confidence in predicting the situation or event that could trigger their dog’s aggression than in identifying behaviors that serve as precursors to aggression (e.g., warning signals) in the moment, although participants ranged from “always” to “never” in both predicting aggressive incidents and identifying warning signals. This supports recent work showing that misinterpreting dog behavior can contribute to increased incidents of aggression toward people (83). However, over one-third of respondents indicated being able to predict aggressive incidents most or all of the time, indicating that even with long-term, known aggression issues, perceived predictability of aggression is not enough to prevent behavioral euthanasia for aggression.

Experiencing more than one problem behavior was commonly reported by owners that euthanized a dog for behavior. A large percentage of respondents (80.3%) selected and ranked up to three separate problem behaviors as contributing to their behavioral euthanasia decision for their dog. The data indicate that owners commonly face more than one problem behavior. The most frequently reported comorbidities were aggression toward people and aggression toward other animals. Other studies have also found that many owners reported comorbid behavior problems (23, 24, 48, 84).

Non-aggressive problem behavior (s) were also selected as comorbid factors influencing a behavioral euthanasia decision. Non-aggressive behaviors were most likely to be in combination with aggressive behavior toward humans or other animals, and listed as the second or third behavioral factor contributing to a euthanasia decision. The prominence of aggression over non-aggressive behaviors emphasizes that problem behaviors that increase risk to others, especially risk to humans, are overwhelmingly more influential in behavioral euthanasia decisions than problem behaviors that only pose welfare concerns for the individual dog. This conflicts with recommendations from professionals to consider poor behavioral quality of life as a reason for euthanasia (58, 85).

The results from the Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs survey indicate that behavioral euthanasia can occur at any age during a dog’s life. Previous studies of behavioral euthanasia have focused on dogs up to 3 years of age (43, 49, 55). However, results from our survey indicated that behavioral euthanasia is not limited to young dogs and can occur in dogs of all ages, from puppies to senior dogs. Additional research is required to understand how factors such as age-related issues or physical or cognitive decline may complicate the ability to manage problem behavior. In the future, researchers exploring behavioral euthanasia should expand their samples to include dogs of all ages.

Finally, most dogs in the sample population lived in their homes for at least 1 year or more, displayed problem behavior (s) for over 1 year, and were involved in multiple aggressive incidents resulting in injury prior to owners electing behavioral euthanasia, indicating that owners made these decisions over time and with extensive consideration for safety.

4.1 Limitations and future directions

The Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs questionnaire was designed to collect data from owners of dogs that were euthanized due to behavior concerns. While the sample used a limited time frame for the euthanasia, recall bias still impacts the retrospective data. Future research should be conducted to assess the reliability and validity of the survey instrument. Additional research could address the recall bias limitation by working with veterinary clinics to provide the survey to owners nearer to the time of behavioral euthanasia rather than retrospectively.

Additionally, the mode of recruitment might have produced a biased sample. The online survey distribution restricted potential respondents to those who had internet access via computer or mobile phone. Researchers chose to recruit a purposive, self-selecting sample of respondents to begin exploring this topic, which may have resulted in a highly motivated sample that was primarily white, female, and highly educated. This bias could be mitigated with future research, for example in-depth interviews of behavior professionals and quantitative study of the prevalence of behavioral euthanasia in veterinary clinics.

However, dog ownership, human-animal relationships, and perceptions of dogs varies greatly by geography, culture, religion, and other societal factors (8690). Cross-cultural research would uncover similarities and differences in the non-behavioral factors influencing behavioral euthanasia decisions across cultures. Factors like housing, source of the dog, access to animal behavior professionals, implementation of treatment interventions, and non-behavioral factors may also contribute to a behavioral euthanasia decision and should be considered for analysis in future studies.

This paper only addresses behavioral euthanasia in pet dogs; future research should also focus on other species, allowing for comparisons of behavioral euthanasia decisions across those species. Additionally, evaluating the impact of environments (e.g., animals in shelters, foster homes, or even in laboratories), and other life course factors (e.g., owner parental status or life changes such as deaths in the household or moving to a new home) on behavioral euthanasia decisions would help investigate the multifactorial nature of this issue. Future research using in-depth interviews of owners who made the decision to euthanize will help provide clarity and detail about the influence of living situation and non-behavioral factors on behavioral euthanasia decisions.

Research into behavioral euthanasia is understandably challenging. Assessing the incidence, prevalence, change over time, geography, or other factors of behavioral euthanasia in animals is difficult to explore given the lack of centralized reporting of causes of death for the veterinary field similar to reporting in the sheltering industry (e.g., Shelter Animals Count) or vital records (e.g., death certificates for humans) that provides a means to identifying when and why animals were euthanized for behavior (39, 91, 92). With some previous studies estimating that behavior could be the reason for up to one third of all euthanasia in dogs ages three and under, this topic should be further studied in detail (43, 55).

The current study focused on problem behaviors in pet dogs that contribute to the behavioral euthanasia decision by pet dog owners. Research focused on the human experience of the behavioral euthanasia decision process and the impact on the lives and mental health of the remaining family and owner, and the social stigma experienced by humans who made behavioral euthanasia decisions could result in de-stigmatizing this topic and increase the availability of psychosocial support for those grieving after experiencing a behavioral euthanasia (93). Both quantitative surveys and in-depth interviews can help better understand the emotional impacts of behavioral euthanasia on owners, caregivers, and professionals (such as shelter workers, trainers, and veterinarians).

This study is not generalizable to all behavioral euthanasia decisions for pet dogs. Results indicate that multiple factors might contribute to a behavioral euthanasia decision; therefore, individual criteria should not be interpreted to predict behavioral euthanasia decisions for individual pet dogs. Rather, as an exploratory survey, it was intended to uncover insights about a little-researched topic to drive future research design. Future research could look for matched cohorts of dogs with the same problem behavior or combinations thereof that were not euthanized to understand non-behavioral factors that influence behavioral euthanasia decisions.

Additional data from the Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs Questionnaire could be analyzed to provide insights into the interventions or treatment options pursued by owners prior to behavioral euthanasia, as well as commonalities in factors like housing, source of the dog, and non-behavioral factors that led to the euthanasia decision.

5 Conclusion

The Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs survey is the first exploratory project seeking to understand how problem behaviors in dogs are associated with an owner’s decision to euthanize their dog for behavior concerns. Aggression toward people, followed by aggression toward other animals are the main drivers of behavioral euthanasia in this sample. Safety risks to our own species and to those of any species living in the same household as the dogs who were euthanized were paramount in the decision process. Understanding the behavioral factors that contribute to behavioral euthanasia decisions by owners can help direct additional resources toward successful problem behavior interventions. Improving public education on dog behavior has the potential to strengthen the bond between humans and animals, enhance the quality of life for both, and reduce safety risks in households and communities with multiple species. This, in turn, could lead to a decrease in behavioral euthanasia for pet dogs.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: Virginia Tech Data Repository at https://doi.org/10.7294/25222304.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Virginia Tech Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MW: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AG: Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. AR: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. EF: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author (s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the following individuals (in no particular order) for their contributions and expertise in designing the Behavioral Euthanasia in Pet Dogs Questionnaire and editing this manuscript: Trish McMillan, Sue Alexander, James Serpell, Lauren Powell, Carlo Siracusa, Liz Hare, Jessica Hekman, Michael Shikashio, James Crosby, Malena DeMartini, Moira Hechenleitner, Amy Pike, Chris Pachel, Lore Haug, Krista Sirois, Sarah Bond, Juliana DeWillems, Kristen-Lee Thanawala, Sawyer Butterfield, Tegan MacDonald, Tiffany Score, Jessica Harder-Leeson, Ana Sofia Goncalves Costa, Eduardo Fernandez, Emily Mason, Marshall Whitmire, Eileen Anderson, Bob Cohen, and Lauren Tsao.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fvets.2024.1387076/full#supplementary-material

References

1. APPA. (2023). Pet industry market size, trends & ownership statistics. Available at: https://www.americanpetproducts.org/press_industrytrends.asp

Google Scholar

2. American Veterinary Medical Association. (2022). 2022 AVMA pet ownership and demographics sourcebook. Available at: https://ebusiness.avma.org/files/ProductDownloads/eco-pet-demographic-report-22-low-res.pdf

Google Scholar

3. American Veterinary Medical Association. AVMA pet ownership and demographics sourcebook: 2017–2018 edition. (2019).

Google Scholar

4. Laurent-Simpson, A . Just like family: How companion animals joined the household. New York, US: NYU Press (2021).

Google Scholar

5. Megna, M. (2023). Pet ownership statistics 2023. Forbes Advisor. Available at: https://www.forbes.com/advisor/pet-insurance/pet-ownership-statistics/#sources_section

Google Scholar

6. Applebaum, JW, Tomlinson, CA, Matijczak, A, McDonald, SE, and Zsembik, BA. The concerns, difficulties, and stressors of caring for pets during COVID-19: results from a large survey of US pet owners. Animals. (2020) 10:1882. doi: 10.3390/ani10101882

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. McConnell, AR, Brown, CM, Shoda, TM, Stayton, LE, and Martin, CE. Friends with benefits: on the positive consequences of pet ownership. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2011) 101:1239–52. doi: 10.1037/a0024506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Adams, GJ, and Clark, WT. The prevalence of behavioural problems in domestic dogs; a survey of 105 dog owners. Aust Vet Pract. (1989) 19:135–137.

Google Scholar

9. Campbell, WE . The prevalence of behavioral problems in American dogs. Mod Vet Pract. (1986) 67:28–31.

Google Scholar

10. Chung, TH, Park, C, Kwon, YM, and Yeon, SC. Prevalence of canine behavior problems related to dog-human relationship in South Korea-a pilot study. J Vet Behav: Clinical Applications and Res. (2016) 11:26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Didehban, N, Pourmahdi Borujeni, M, Avizeh, R, and Mosallanejad, B. Problematic behaviors in companion dogs: a survey of their prevalence and associated factors. J Vet Behav. (2020) 39:6–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2020.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Martínez, ÁG, Santamarina Pernas, G, Diéguez Casalta, FJ, Suárez Rey, ML, and De la Cruz Palomino, LF. Risk factors associated with behavioral problems in dogs. J Vet Behav: Clinical Applications and Res. (2011) 6:225–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2011.01.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Salonen, M, Sulkama, S, Mikkola, S, Puurunen, J, Hakanen, E, Tiira, K, et al. Prevalence, comorbidity, and breed differences in canine anxiety in 13,700 Finnish pet dogs. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:2962. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59837-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Voith, VL . Attachment of people to companion animals. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. (1985) 15:289–95. doi: 10.1016/S0195-5616(85)50301-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wells, DL, and Hepper, PG. Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2000) 69:55–65. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00118-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Yang, J, Langford, F, and Kiddie, J. Risk factors for aggressive behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris), as reported by owners in mainland China. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2021) 234:105211. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105211

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Anderson, KH, Yao, Y, Perry, PJ, Albright, JD, and Houpt, KA. Case distribution, sources, and breeds of dogs presenting to a veterinary behavior Clinic in the United States from 1997 to 2017. Animals. (2022) 12:576. doi: 10.3390/ani12050576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mills, DS . Medical paradigms for the study of problem behaviour: a critical review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2003) 81:265–77. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00286-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ramos, D, Reche-Junior, A, Henzel, M, and Mills, DS. Canine behaviour problems in Brazil: a review of 180 referral cases. Vet Rec. (2020) 186:e22. doi: 10.1136/vr.105539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Stelow, E . Diagnosing behavior problems: a guide for practitioners. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. (2018) 48:3. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2017.12.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Guy, NC, Luescher, UA, Dohoo, SE, Spangler, E, Miller, JB, Dohoo, IR, et al. A case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour, and their victims. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2001) 74:43–57. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00155-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Guy, NC, Luescher, UA, Dohoo, SE, Spangler, E, Miller, JB, Dohoo, IR, et al. Demographic and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2001) 74:15–28. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00153-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bamberger, M, and Houpt, KA. Signalment factors, comorbidity, and trends in behavior diagnoses in dogs: 1, 644 cases (1991-2001). J Am Vet Med Assoc. (2006) 229:1591–601. doi: 10.2460/javma.229.10.1591

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Borchelt, PL . Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Appl Anim Ethol. (1983) 10:45–61. doi: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90111-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Luescher, AU, and Reisner, IR. Canine aggression toward familiar people: a New look at an old problem. Vet clinics of North America-Small Animal Prac. (2008) 38:008. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.04.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Takeuchi, Y, Ogata, N, Houpt, KA, and Scarlett, JM. Differences in background and outcome of three behavior problems of dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2001) 70:297–308. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00156-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bennett, PC, and Rohlf, VI. Owner-companion dog interactions: relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2007) 102:65–84. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.03.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Blackshaw, JK . An overview of types of aggressive behaviour in dogs and methods of treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1991) 30:351–61. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(91)90140-S

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Brown, CR, Edwards, S, Kenney, E, Pailler, S, Albino, M, Carabello, M, et al. Family quality of life: pet owners and veterinarians working together to reach the best outcomes. J Am Vet Med Assoc. (2023) 261:1–6. doi: 10.2460/javma.23.01.0016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Buller, K, and Ballantyne, KC. Living with and loving a pet with behavioral problems: pet owners’ experiences. J Vet Behav. (2020) 37:41–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2020.04.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Houpt, KA, Honig, SU, and Reisner, IR. Breaking the human-companion animal bond. J Am Vet Med Assoc. (1996) 208:1653–9. doi: 10.2460/javma.1996.208.10.1653

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kuntz, K, Ballantyne, KC, Cousins, E, and Spitznagel, MB. Assessment of caregiver burden in owners of dogs with behavioral problems and factors related to its presence. J Vet Behav. (2023) 64-65:41–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2023.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Loder, RT . The demographics of dog bites in the United States. Heliyon. (2019) 5:e01360. doi: 10.1016/J.HELIYON.2019.E01360

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Shaw, JK, and Lahrman, S. The human–animal bond–a brief look at its richness and complexities In: Canine and feline behavior for veterinary technicians and nurses. Eds. JK Shaw and D Martin. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2023) 88–105.

Google Scholar

35. Weiss, E, Slater, M, Garrison, L, Drain, N, Dolan, E, Scarlett, JM, et al. Large dog relinquishment to two municipal facilities in New York city and Washington, D.C.: identifying targets for intervention. Animals. (2014) 4:409–33. doi: 10.3390/ani4030409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Matte, A, Khosa, D, Coe, J, and Meehan, M. Impacts of the process and decision-making around companion animal euthanasia on veterinary wellbeing. Vet Rec. (2019) 185:480. doi: 10.1136/vr.105540

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Coe, JB, Young, I, Lambert, K, Dysart, L, Nogueira Borden, L, and Rajić, A. A scoping review of published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2014) 17:253–73. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2014.899910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Landsberg, GM . The distribution of canine behaviour cases at three behaviour referral practices. Vet Med. (1991) 86:1011–1018.

Google Scholar

40. Hawes, SM, Kerrigan, JM, Hupe, T, and Morris, KN. Factors informing the return of adopted dogs and cats to an animal shelter. Animals. (2020) 10:1573. doi: 10.3390/ani10091573

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Grigg, EK, and Donaldson, TMC. Helping clients cope with grief associated with euthanasia for behavior problems In: Pet loss, grief, and therapeutic interventions: Practitioners navigating the human-animal bond. Eds. L Kogan and P Erdman. New York, US: Routledge (2019).

Google Scholar

42. Pugliese, M, Falcone, A, Alibrandi, A, Zirilli, A, and Passantino, A. Risk factors regarding dog euthanasia and causes of death at a veterinary teaching hospital in Italy: preliminary results. Vet Sci. (2022) 9:554. doi: 10.3390/vetsci9100554

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Boyd, C, Jarvis, S, McGreevy, PD, Heath, S, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC, et al. Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged under three years attending primary-care veterinary practices in England. Anim Welf. (2018) 27:251–62. doi: 10.7120/09627286.27.3.251

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Col, R, Day, C, and Phillips, CJC. An epidemiological analysis of dog behavior problems presented to an Australian behavior clinic, with associated risk factors. J Vet Behav: Clinical Applications and Res. (2016) 15:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Edney, ATB . Reasons for the euthanasia of dogs and cats. Vet Rec. (1998) 143:114. doi: 10.1136/vr.143.4.114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gorodetsky, E . Epidemiology of dog and cat euthanasia across Canadian prairie provinces. Canadian Vet J La Revue Vétérinaire Canadienne. (1997) 38:649–52.

Google Scholar

47. Kass, PH, New, JC, Scarlett, JM, and Salman, MD. Understanding animal companion surplus in the United States: relinquishment of nonadoptables to animal shelters. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2001) 4:237–48. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0404_01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lund, JD, Agger, JF, and Vestergaard, KS. Reported behaviour problems in pet dogs in Denmark: age distribution and influence of breed and gender. Prev Vet Med. (1996) 28:33–48. doi: 10.1016/0167-5877(96)01015-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. O’Neill, DG, Church, DB, McGreevy, PD, Thomson, PC, and Brodbelt, DC. Longevity and mortality of owned dogs in England. Vet J. (2013) 198:638–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.09.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Patronek, GJ, and Dodman, NH. Attitudes, procedures, and delivery of behavior services by veterinarians in small animal practice. J Am Vet Med Assoc. (1999) 215:1606–11. doi: 10.2460/javma.1999.215.11.1606

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Pegram, C, Gray, C, Packer, RMA, Richards, Y, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC, et al. Proportion and risk factors for death by euthanasia in dogs in the UK. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:9145. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-88342-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Reisner, IR, Erb, HN, and Houpt, KA. Risk factors for behavior-related euthanasia among dominant-aggressive dogs: 110 cases (1989-1992). J Am Vet Med Assoc. (1994) 205:855–63. doi: 10.2460/javma.1994.205.06.855

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Siracusa, C, Provoost, L, and Reisner, IR. Dog- and owner-related risk factors for consideration of euthanasia or rehoming before a referral behavioral consultation and for euthanizing or rehoming the dog after the consultation. J Vet Behav: Clinical Applications and Res. (2017) 22:46–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2017.09.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Stead, AC . Euthanasia in the dog and cat. J Small Anim Pract. (1982) 23:37–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-5827.1982.tb01633.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Yu, Y, Wilson, B, Masters, S, van Rooy, D, and McGreevy, PD. Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged three years under attending primary-care veterinary practices in Australia. Animals. (2021) 11:493. doi: 10.3390/ani11020493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. De Marinis, A . (2022). What are the options for dogs with aggression? Available at: https://aggressivedog.com/2022/07/12/what-are-the-options-for-dogs-with-aggression/

Google Scholar

57. DeGioia, P. (2013). Euthanizing aggressive dogs: Sometimes It’s the best choice. VetzInsight. Available at: https://www.vin.com/vetzinsight/default.aspx?pid=756&id=5912453

Google Scholar

58. Gibeault, S . When to consider behavioral euthanasia American Kennel Club (2021).

Google Scholar

59. Lilly, M. L. (2020). CEU event: behavioral euthanasia: considerations for when and how (#209). Ohio veterinary medical association. Available at: https://www.ccpdt.org/certification/continuing-education/events/16088

Google Scholar

60. Marrs, M. (2021). Euthanizing an aggressive dog and dealing with the guilt: How to move forward. Available at: https://www.k9ofmine.com/euthanizing-aggressive-dog-guilt/

Google Scholar

61. Rayment, D . (2020). Making the impossible choice …. Available at: https://siscomiyagi.com/2020/10/02/making-the-impossible-choice/

Google Scholar

62. Stremming, S. (2017). We need to talk about behavioral euthanasia. Available at: https://thecognitivecanine.com/blog/we-need-to-talk-about-behavioral-euthanasia/

Google Scholar

63. Summerfield, J. (2017). Harsh truths and difficult choices: The reality of behavioral euthanasia. Available at: http://www.drjensdogblog.com/harsh-truths-and-difficult-choices-the-reality-of-behavioral-euthanasia/

Google Scholar

64. Thomas, K . (2020). Euthanasia for behavior, part two: How do you decide? Available at: https://k9turbotraining.com/euthanasia-for-behavior-part-two-how-do-you-decide/

Google Scholar

65. Walker, M. (2020). Two words: Behavioral euthanasia. Medium. Available at: https://medium.com/@doggydivas71/two-words-behavioral-euthanasia-37da8f7cbbc0

Google Scholar

66. Jacob, ME, and Ganguli, M. Epidemiology for the clinical neurologist. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 138:3–16. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-802973-2.00001-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Creevy, KE, Akey, JM, Kaeberlein, M, Promislow, DEL, Barnett, BG, Benton, B, et al. An open science study of ageing in companion dogs. Nature. (2022) 602:51–7. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04282-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. SAS Institute Inc. JMP® pro software SAS Institute Inc. (2021).

Google Scholar

70. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2023).

Google Scholar

71. Wickham, H . ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag (2016).

Google Scholar

72. Xu, S, Chen, M, Feng, T, Zhan, L, Zhou, L, and Yu, G. Use ggbreak to effectively utilize plotting space to deal with large datasets and outliers. Front Genet. (2021) 12. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2021.774846

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Petukhov, V., van den Brand, T., and Biederstedt, E. (2023). ggrastr: Rasterize Layers for 'ggplot2'. R package V 1.0.2. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggrastr

Google Scholar

74. Pedersen, T. (2023). Patchwork: The composer of plots. R package V 1.1.3. Available at: https://rdrr.io/cran/patchwork/.

Google Scholar

75. Lee, TS, Creech, TG, Martinson, A, Nielsen, SE, Jakes, AF, Jones, PF, et al. Prioritizing human safety and multispecies connectivity across a regional road network. Conservation Sci Prac. (2021) 3. doi: 10.1111/csp2.327

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Petrinovich, L, O’Neill, P, and Jorgensen, M. An empirical study of moral intuitions: toward an evolutionary ethics. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1993) 64:467–78. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.467

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Grigg, EK . Helping clients facing behavior problems in their companion animals In: Clinician’s guide to treating companion animal issues. Eds. L Kogan and C Blazina. London, UK: Academic Press. (2019).

Google Scholar

78. Fein, J, Bogumil, D, Upperman, JS, and Burke, RV. Pediatric dog bites: a population-based profile. Inj Prev. (2019) 25:290–4. doi: 10.1136/injuryprev-2017-042621

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Jakeman, M, Oxley, JA, Owczarczak-Garstecka, SC, and Westgarth, C. Pet dog bites in children: management and prevention. BMJ Paediatrics open. (2020) 4:726. doi: 10.1136/bmjpo-2020-000726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Weiss, HB, Friedman, DI, and Coben, JH. Incidence of dog bite injuries treated in emergency departments. JAMA J Am Med Assoc. (1998) 279:51–3. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.1.51

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Haug, L. (2011). Treat or euthanize? Putting it all together: prognostic indicators. DVM 360. Available at: https://www.dvm 360.com/view/treat-or-euthanize-putting-it-all-together-prognostic-indicators

Google Scholar

82. Haug, L. (2011). Treat or euthanize? Helping owners make critical decisions regarding pets with behavior problems. DVM 360. Available at: https://www.dvm 360.com/view/treat-or-euthanize-helping-owners-make-critical-decisions-regarding-pets-with-behavior-problems

Google Scholar

83. Parkinson, C, Herring, L, and Gould, D. Public perceptions of dangerous dogs and dog risk Edge Hill University (2023).

Google Scholar

84. Wright, JC, and Nesselrote, MS. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1987) 19:169–78. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(87)90213-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Euthanasia for behavioral issues: A complicated and difficult decision. The Ohio State University Veterinary Medical Center. (2014). Retrieved December 16, 2022, from https://www.vet.osu.edu/vmc/sites/default/files/files/companion/HTB/behavioral_euthanasia_printable_2014.pdf

Google Scholar

86. Herzog, H, and Rowan, A. Geography, demography, and patterns of pet-keeping: the case of dogs In: International Society for Anthrozoology (2019)

Google Scholar

87. Volsche, S, Frailey, F, Ihara, N, and Nittono, H. Sex and parental status impacts human-to-pet attachment and caregiving attitudes and behaviors in a Japanese sample. Anthrozoös. (2022) 36:1–13. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2022.2149926

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Volsche, S, Mukherjee, R, and Rangaswamy, M. The difference is in the details: attachment and cross-species parenting in the United States and India. Anthrozoös. (2022) 35:393–408. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2021.1996026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Volsche, S, Schultz, S, Alsaifi, S, Melamies, M, and Pulkkinen, J. Parental status influences human-to-pet caregiving behaviors, attachment, and attitudes in a Finnish sample. Human-animal. Dermatol Int. (2023) 11. doi: 10.1079/hai.2023.0019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Wan, M, Kubinyi, E, Miklósi, Á, and Champagne, F. A cross-cultural comparison of reports by German shepherd owners in Hungary and the United States of America. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2009) 121:206–13. doi: 10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2009.09.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Lundquist, JH, Anderton, DL, and Yaukey, D. Demography: The study of human population. Long Grove, Illinois: Waveland Press (2018).

Google Scholar

92. Vital Records. (2023). National Archives. Retrieved July 4, 2023, from https://www.archives.gov/research/vital-records

Google Scholar

93. Doka, KJ . Disenfranchised grief: an exposition and update In: Readings in thanatology. Ed. JD Morgan . New York, US: Routledge. (2020). 275–83.

Google Scholar

Keywords: euthanasia, behavior problems, behavioral euthanasia, canine behavior, aggression, dogs, behavior, behavioral problems

Citation: Hitchcock M, Workman MK, Guthrie AP, Ruple A and Feuerbacher EN (2024) Factors associated with behavioral euthanasia in pet dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1387076. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1387076

Received: 16 February 2024; Accepted: 02 April 2024;
Published: 17 April 2024.

Edited by:

Flaviana Gottardo, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marta Brscic, Department of Animal Medicine Production and Health, Italy
Phyllis Erdman, Washington State University, United States

Copyright © 2024 Hitchcock, Workman, Guthrie, Ruple and Feuerbacher. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Miranda Hitchcock, mirandah@vt.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.