Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 04 July 2019
Sec. Social Neuroscience

Emotional Prosody Effects on Verbal Memory in Euthymic Patients With Bipolar Disorder

Mario Altamura*Mario Altamura1*Licia SantamariaLicia Santamaria1Antonella EliaAntonella Elia1Eleonora AngeliniEleonora Angelini1Flavia A. PadalinoFlavia A. Padalino1Claudia AltamuraClaudia Altamura1Caterina PaduloCaterina Padulo2Nicola MammarellaNicola Mammarella2Antonello BellomoAntonello Bellomo1Beth FairfieldBeth Fairfield2
  • 1Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Psychiatry Unit, University of Foggia, Foggia, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychological, Health and Territorial Sciences, University of Chieti, Chieti, Italy

A growing body of evidence suggests that emotional prosody influences the ability to remember verbal information. Although bipolar disorder (BD) has been shown to be associated with deficits in verbal memory and emotional processing, the relation between these processes in this population remains unclear. In the present study, we aimed to investigate the impact of emotional prosody on verbal memory in euthymic BD patients compared with controls. Participants were randomly divided into three subgroups according to different prosody listening conditions (a story read with a positive, negative, or neutral prosody) and effects on a yes–no recognition memory task were investigated. Results showed that euthymic bipolar patients remembered comparable numbers of words after listening to the story with a negative or neutral prosody but remembered fewer words after listening to the positive version compared with healthy controls. Results suggest that verbal memory is hindered in BD patients after listening to the story read with a positive prosody. This recognition bias for information with a positive prosody may lead to negative intrusive verbal memories and poor emotion regulation.

Introduction

The affective connotation attributed to a word or a sentence during speech includes a series of non-lexical cues, such as intonation, intensity, and timing that together or alone communicate the emotional reaction or state of the speaker (1, 2). This affective connotation, commonly called prosody, is crucial in communication since a word (or a sentence) can assume a positive or negative meaning independent of the valence of the words themselves. Indeed, prosody is a powerful contextual modulator of verbal content and emphasizes the importance of investigating how different populations of individuals process prosody.

In general, the ability to correctly identify, interpret, and use affective prosody emerges around the age of 9 years (3, 4) and then declines with age (5, 6). Older adults, in fact, generally have difficulty processing prosody [for a review, see Ref. (7)], although the reasons behind this difficulty remain controversial. Changes in bottom-up auditory processing may affect processing of prosody and, certainly, modifications in auditory perception often accompany the developmental trajectory (e.g., hearing loss, different voice pitch, pausing, and duration thresholds and discrimination). More interesting, differences in vocal emotional processes may also specifically affect the processing affective prosody beyond these auditory changes independent of age.

It is now widely recognized that affective information can have both enhancing and impairing effects on various cognitive processes (e.g., memory), depending on a range of factors. For instance, attentional and working memory capacity may play an important role in determining whether and how emotions influence declarative memory [see for reviews Refs. (8, 9)]. On the one hand, a large body of evidence suggests that affective information is generally processed and remembered better than neutral information (e.g., 10). For example, McGaugh and colleagues (11) demonstrated that stories with emotional content are remembered better than similar stories lacking emotional implications. On the other hand, affective content represents additional information that needs to be manipulated and may undermine rather than facilitate memory performance (1214). Indeed, studies have suggested that focusing on affective prosody does not always translate into enhanced memory formation (15, 16) since the affective information contained in prosody may also be considered as an additional piece of information that needs to be processed, creating additional requests for cognitive resources (17). For example, Fairfield et al. (16) found that listening to a story read with an affective prosody hindered performance on a subsequent recognition task in healthy older adults compared with younger adults.

Last, some studies have found gender effects on emotion recognition showing that females outperformed males (18) (19). Indeed, it may be that females are generally the primary caretakers and may have evolved greater facility in identifying emotion due to their importance in long-term parental bonding (20). Other studies, however, failed to reproduce gender differences in emotion recognition ability (21, 22). In particular, a study by Ross and Monnot (23) found no gender differences in affective prosody recognition.

Regarding patients with bipolar disorders (BDs), numerous studies have found general impairments in working memory, and verbal and episodic memory in euthymic patients compared to healthy controls [for reviews, see Refs. (2426)], whereas studies focusing on alterations in the perception of affective stimuli (27, 28) and the impact of affective information on memory in these individuals have produced divergent findings (29). For example, Kauer-Sant’Anna et al. (30) tested memory for affective and neutral story content in euthymic BD participants and healthy controls and found no memory enhancement for the affective content of the story compared with healthy controls. Differently, other studies found typical emotional enhancement effects in both remitted patients and healthy controls in facial recognition tasks and affective information processing (3134). Thus, the exact nature and the role of affective information on memory in euthymic BD participants remain somewhat unclear.

More importantly, growing evidence suggests that BD patients are impaired in their ability to identify the affective prosody of neutral content sentences (35, 36). Some studies have found general deficits across emotion categories in both male and female populations (37, 38) and in remitted patients with BD (37, 39). Other studies have found more gender-specific deficits. Bozikas et al. (40) found impaired perception of affective prosody specific to females and to certain emotions (i.e., fear and surprise), whereas Van Rheenen and Rossell (36) found impaired emotional prosody recognition specific to men and for the processing of positive intonation. These deficits may be linked to deficits in emotion processing brain regions and a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study suggested that patients with BD may lack the typical right lateralization temporal lobe response to emotional prosody compared to controls (41). Indeed, converging evidence indicates that the right hemisphere has a predominant role in processing emotions from the tone of voice (42, 43) and functional imaging studies have found the right prefrontal cortex plays a critical role in perceiving and recognizing emotional prosody (44, 45).

In sum, although some findings suggest that individuals with BD may have diminished perception of emotional prosody, no study to our knowledge has examined the effect of affective prosody on memory. In this study, we aimed to investigate emotional prosody effects on information processing and verbal memory in healthy adults and BD type I patients using a carefully controlled auditory procedure. In particular, since we were specifically interested in investigating how affective prosody is used during memory processing and remembering, we asked participants to listen to a brief story that contained neutral target words but that were read with a specific affective prosody (positive, negative, neutral) and then to recognize target words. In this manner, the task allowed us to investigate how listeners process affective prosody and to detect subsequent differences in the ability to discriminate words belonging to the story. We adopted a single story with a controlled neutral semantic content to avoid content-prosody interactions within the three versions. In this way, any differences could be attributed to the emotional connotation of the voice rather than the content of the story per se or, in other words, to the way the voice conveyed an emotional meaning to the text. This may help clarifying whether healthy adults and BD patients have specific preferences for affective information in auditory processing and whether this affects later processing. In addition, we used a between-subject design to avoid potential carryover effects of different vocal emotions on later processing. We predicted that although studies have shown that BD patients may have general working memory difficulties, in our study they may not show lower accuracy scores for general word memory since the recognition memory test was administered immediately after the study session. Instead, we expected difficulties with affective prosody in patients compared to healthy controls, but no group differences for word memory with neutral prosody in line with previous studies showing impaired memory retrieval for affective stimuli (30, 41, 46).

Understanding the effects of prosody on declarative memory in BD may provide critical insights into identifying potential factors that cause patients with BD to exhibit poor emotion regulation, to be vulnerable to relapse or to engage in risk taking behavior.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 48 individuals diagnosed with BD type I and a control group of 48 adults recruited from the local community, who did not meet current or past criteria for any Axis I disorder as defined by the Diagnostic Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). BD diagnoses were confirmed using the structured clinical interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5-CV) by licensed clinical psychologists (47). Exclusion criteria for the bipolar patients included other axis I diagnoses. Exclusion criteria for all participants included report of a history of severe head trauma, stroke, neurological disease, severe medical illness, or alcohol or substance abuse in the past 6 months. Current symptoms of mania were measured using the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (48). Current symptoms of depression were measured using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) (49). To ensure symptom recovery, BD patients were included only if they had a score of ≤ 8 on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) and the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (40). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (50) was used to measure positive and negative state affect (current mood). The PANAS is a self-report tool commonly used to assess positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). The PANAS has two 10-item scales: one for PA and one for NA. Responses are rated on a 5-point scale from very slightly (1) to very much (5) (50). All participants completed the forward and backward digit spans of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised measuring working memory capacity (WAIS-R; 51). Auditory acuity was evaluated using a screening procedure developed by Reilly et al. (52). To pass screening, participants had to successfully identify tones presented monaurally to either ear at 25 dB HL (1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz), followed by presentation of the same frequency tones to the contralateral ear. Controls and BD type 1 patients were randomly divided in three groups of 16 subjects each and assigned to one of the three prosody conditions (positive, negative and neutral). There were no differences between groups for general working memory abilities (controls: forward, p values > .12; backward digit, p values > .13; patients: forward, p values > .07; backward digit, p values > .09) nor for current mood (controls: positive PANAS, p values > .26; negative PANAS, p values > .55; patients: positive PANAS, p values > .36; negative PANAS, p values > .11). All participants took part in the study on a strictly volunteer basis and received no payment per participation. They received no other compensation. Healthy controls were recruited from the local Chieti area through a list of volunteers willing to participate in experiments in our lab. Patients were recruited from follow-up visits to a psychiatric unit at the University of Foggia.

Next, we conducted a post hoc power analysis with the program G*Power3 (53) to find out whether our experimental design had enough power to detect an effect of prosody on declarative memory in BD. The sample size of 96 was used for the statistical power analyses and alpha level of was p < .05. A post hoc power analysis revealed that on the basis of main effects, interaction and between-groups comparison the observed power was close to.86 with an observed effect size of f = .34 [medium-sized effect according to Cohen, (54)]. Approximately, 65 participants per group would be needed to obtain statistical power at the recommended .95 level and a large effect size [ f = .40; Cohen, (55)].

Participants gave written informed consent after the procedures were fully explained. Participants were not explicitly informed about the purpose of the study. The present study is in accordance with the Helsinki declaration and was approved by the local institutional review board.

Materials and Procedures

We used a single neutral content short story composed of 112 words, 31 of which were neutral target words as previously done by Fairfield et al. (16). Target words were selected from the Italian version of the Affective Norms for Emotional Words (ANEW) (56) and had the following characteristics: mean frequency of use, 127.4 (SD = 97.9), mean valence of 5.07 (SD = 0.28) and mean arousal 5.03 (SD = 0.45). The story was read and recorded by a 28-year-old European professional actor in three versions that differed for affective prosody. The actor read one version with a happy prosody, one with a sad prosody, and the last with a neutral prosody. The positive version of the story lasted 72 s, the negative version lasted 72 s, and the neutral version lasted 70 s. Participants listened to the audio traces through a pair of Bose headphones in a room with a noise level below 25 dB. The three traces were presented at an average of 70 dB, comparable to the level of conversational speech. Pitch levels were within a range of 75 to 385 Hz. To evaluate the prosody manipulation, we carried out a preliminary rating study in our laboratory. An independent group of 30 older adults (M age = 70.9 years; SD = 4.9 years; 15 males) rated the three versions according to valence on a 7-point scale (1, absolutely negative to 7, absolutely positive). We chose to control the prosody manipulation in a group of older adults since much research has shown that emotion and affective information processing impairments emerge with age (57). Our reasoning was that if healthy older adults could correctly detect the prosody of each story, then the versions could be considered positive, negative, or neutral.

Prosody was effective in changing the direction of rating as participants rated the story as more positive (M = 6.4, SD = .68) when the actor read the story with a positive intonation, more negative (M = 2.1, SD = .74) with a negative intonation, and neutral (M = 4.2, SD = .75) when the story was read with no particular intonation, F (2, 27) = 91,752, p < .001, η² = .87. Finally, to control for comprehension and intelligibility, we asked the independent group to answer five comprehension questions regarding the story. We did not find any difference in comprehension between prosody conditions (F (2, 27) = .34, p = .71, η² = .02).

Study Phase

We adopted the same procedure as that used in a previous study (16). Participants listened to a short story and were instructed to pay attention to what the story was about. We presented our study as a comprehension study so participants did not know that a recognition memory test would follow.

Test Phase

The recognition phase began after a 3-min interval during which participants completed the Pattern Comparison Test (58). Participants were presented with a surprise yes-no auditory recognition memory task. For the recognition memory test, the 31 studied words were mixed with 20 new words to create a list of 51 items. The new words were comparable to the studied words, mean frequency of use, 121.9 (SD = 102.9), mean valence of 5.18 (SD = 0.26), and mean arousal 5.03 (SD = 0.60). To make the recognition phase comparable across groups, participants listened to the list of words read by an experimenter blind to the experimental hypothesis and who was not involved in recording of words. Participants answered “yes” if they thought they had heard the word during the story or “no” if they felt it was new. Before leaving, participants also answered five comprehension questions as an index of their ability to pay attention to the story.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants are presented in Table 1. All patients were receiving appropriate medication, lithium, atypical antipsychotics, and antidepressants, as monotherapy or in various combinations: 18 were on an atypical antipsychotic, 3 on classical antipsychotics, 40 on a mood stabilizer, and 3 on an antidepressant. Daily dosage of each antipsychotic was converted into a daily equivalent dosage of chlorpromazine (59). If patients were treated with a combination of antipsychotics, all obtained equivalent dosages of chlorpromazine were summed. All participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal visual and auditory acuity. We carried out a preliminary analysis for gender differences and comprehension scores. We did not find any significant effects regarding gender (F (1,84) = .269, p = .61), so we excluded this variable from the analysis. We also collected accuracy to the five comprehension questions as an index of participants’ processing of the story. Accuracy in all participants was high > 60%.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic and clinical participant characteristics.

We carried out a 2 (group: controls vs BD type 1 patients) × 3 (prosody: positive, negative, neutral) mixed ANOVA on recognition scores, calculated as hits-false alarms (HITs-FAs), that is the difference between the proportion of items correctly remembered and the proportion of new words recognized as old in line with the Signal Detection Theory (60). We found no main effect of group F (1, 90) = 1,410, p = .24, η² = .02. Healthy controls and BD type 1 patients remembered a comparable number of words across all three conditions (positive, .33 vs. 26, p = .11; negative, .31 vs. 38, p = .20; neutral, .49 vs. 38, p = .09). There was a significant main effect of Prosody, F(2, 90) = 5,805, p < .01, η² = .10. Accuracy improved when the story was read with a neutral prosody compared to the emotional prosody (.44 vs. .32). Finally, the two-way interaction between group and affect neared significance, F (2, 90) = 3.063, p = .05; η² = .06. Planned comparisons revealed that both positive and negative prosody hindered recognition memory in healthy controls, whereas recognition memory was hindered in BD type 1 patients only after listening to the story read with a positive prosody (p < .001). Recognition accuracy is reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Recognition accuracy.

We used Pearson’s correlations to investigate the association between working memory capacity (as measured by forward and backward digit span) and performance on recognition task within the patient group only. There were no significant correlations between recognition accuracy and the two working memory tasks (p > 0.05 in all cases). Moreover, there were no significant correlations between recognition scores and chlorpromazine dosage equivalent (p > 0.05).

Discussion

In this study, we examined the effects of the affective cues contained in prosody on verbal information processing and memory in BD patients and healthy controls with an affective prosody manipulation. To our knowledge, this is the first study to present detailed data on affective prosody effects on verbal memory among individuals with BD. The main results can be summarized as follows. First, both healthy participants and patients with BD remembered target words better when they were not embedded in an emotional vocal context (i.e., neutral vocal prosody). Second, the valence of affective prosody affected the pattern of recognition memory in patients and controls differently. Recognition performance was specifically affected by positive prosody in the BD patients who remembered fewer words after listening to the story read with a positive prosody respect to the patients who listened to the story on a negative or neutral prosody. Instead, healthy controls remembered fewer target words after listening to the story with a negative or a positive prosody.

Results showed a clear memory advantage for neutral prosody compared to affective prosody and are inconsistent with previous literature that shows how affective information is generally processed and remembered better than neutral information (6164). Our divergent results might be due to differences in study stimuli and/or the nature of memory tested. Indeed, previous studies have generally focused on verbal memory impairments for words with positive or negative valence, whereas our study tested the effects of vocal prosody on neutral word recognition. Noteworthy, our findings are in line with previous prosody studies that revealed poorer memory for sentences spoken with a positive or negative intonation compared with a neutral one (65, 66). The deleterious effects of affective prosody on word recognition accuracy are consistent with the assumption that focusing on affective cues contained in prosody does not always guarantee better memory. In particular, it has been suggested that the vocal prosody might draw attention to non-verbal aspects of the stimulus (e.g., voice identity), thus leaving fewer resources for the encoding and consolidation of verbal aspects [e.g., Refs. (15, 16)]. Our findings suggest that these general effects of vocal prosody on word recognition may be relatively intact in both healthy adults and patients with BD.

The pattern of recognition as a function of the emotional prosody valence differed between patients and controls. Importantly, the differential responses between healthy subjects and euthymic bipolar patients cannot be explained by cognitive impairment in the patient group since we found no significant correlations between working memory performance and recognition scores. Furthermore, results cannot be explained by drug treatment since memory performance of patients was not significantly correlated. Recognition performance was specifically affected by positive prosody in the BD patients who remembered comparable numbers of words after listening to the story with a negative or neutral prosody but remembered fewer words after listening to the positive version. This diverges from previous emotional memory studies showing that participants with BD have impaired memory retrieval for negative emotional information (30, 46). However, it should be noted that those early studies examined the impact of affective stories on recall instead of affective cues contained in prosody. Our findings are in line with results obtained by Van Rheenen and Rossell (36) who demonstrated impaired ability to recognize positive intonations in patients with BD compared with healthy controls.

With regard to the nature of memory tested, results are in line with the transfer-appropriate processing theory that posits a general memory advantage when study and test processes match (6769). In fact, our findings suggest that when participants listened to the story presented with a neutral prosody and were later tested for a series of old and new neutral items read with a neutral intonation, memory performance benefited from the neutral match between prosody and the words to be remembered. Differently, when prosody and words to-be-remembered were unmatched (affective prosody and neutral words), subsequent recognition memory was affected by the study phase intonation mismatch.

Regarding brain lateralization, another possible interpretation of our findings is that, contrary to healthy individuals, euthymic bipolar patients are constrained in their ability to engage affective processing of positive prosody, which may be related to a dysfunctional prefrontal regulation of emotional memory processing (41).

Finally, our study has several limitations. Our study includes prosody effects on only one neutral story and in a small sample. In particular, the small sample size was due to a limited number of patients who met inclusion criteria from the Psychiatry Unit in Foggia. Foggia is a small city in the southern part of Italy and accordingly receives a smaller number of psychiatric patients, in general, and BD patients, in particular. In line with the small number of eligible BD patients, we recruited only 48 healthy controls. Consequently, caution is recommended before placing too great a significance on the results without also considering effect sizes. Future studies should increase the number of participants and adopt a variety of stories to better specify prosody effects. Results of our study may reflect the different emotional intensity attributed to the processed intonations. Unfortunately, we did not ask the participants to rate the intensity of the material. Future experiments might request participants to rate the stimuli for both valence as well as for arousal. Despite these caveats, the present study provides important insights into underlying cognitive mechanisms associated with emotional prosody in BD. Specifically, results indicated that verbal memory was hindered in BD patients after listening to the story read with a positive prosody. The effects of emotional prosody on verbal memory exhibited by euthymic patients with BD are likely to have important clinical implications since identifying potential factors that cause patients with BD to exhibit poor emotion regulation may clarify vulnerability to relapse or risk-taking behavior. Moreover, the existence in the BD euthymic phase of alterations in the memory processes for the positive emotional stimuli might make patients more vulnerable to developing negative intrusive memories and depressive symptoms.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Department of Psychological, Health and Territorial Sciences’ ethical committee.

Author Contributions

MA and BF conceived and designed the experiments. LS, AE, EA, and FP performed the experiments. CA, BF, and CP analyzed data. MA and BF wrote the article. AB and NM discussed the results and provided comments.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Van Rijn S, Aleman A, van Diessen E, Berckmoes C, Vingerhoets G, Kahn RS. What is said or how it is said makes a difference: role of the right fronto-parietal operculum in emotional prosody as reveled by repetitive TMS. Eur J Neurosci (2005) 21:3195–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04130.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE. Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev (2002) 22:789–832. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00130-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Friend M. Developmental changes in sensitivity to vocal paralanguage. Dev Sci (2000) 3:148–62. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Friend M, Bryant J. A developmental lexical bias in the interpretation of discrepant messages. Merrill Palmer Q (2000) 46:140–67.

Google Scholar

5. Allen R, Brosgole L. Facial and auditory affect recognition in senile geriatrics, the normal elderly and young adults. Int J Neurosci (1993) 68:33–42. doi: 10.3109/00207459308994257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kiss I, Tricia E. Age-related decline in perception of prosodic affect. Appl Neuropsychol (2001) 8:251–4. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0804_9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ruffman T, Henry JD, Livingstone V, Phillips LH. A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2008) 32:863–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat Rev Neurosci (2006) 7:54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Tyng CM, Amin HU, Saad MNM, Malik AS. The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Front Psychol (2017) 8:1454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Buchanan TW, Adolphs R. The role of the human amygdala in emotional modulation of long-term declarative memory. In: Moore S, Oaksford M, editors. Emotional cognition: from brain to behavior. Amsterdam: John Benjamins (2002). p. 9–34. doi: 10.1075/aicr.44.02buc

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. McGaugh JL, Cahill L, Roozendal B. Involvement of the amygdala in memory storage: interaction with other brain systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93:13508–14. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bergmann HC, Rijpkema M, Fernández G, Kessels RP. The effects of valence and arousal on associative working memory and long-term memory. PLoS One (2012) 7(12):e52616. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fairfield B, Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, Palumbo R. Running with emotion: when affective content hampers working memory performance. Int J Psychol (2015) 50:161–4. doi: 10.1002/ijop.2015.50.issue-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fairfield B, Altamura M, Padalino AF, Balzotti A, Di Domenico A, Mammarella N. False memories for affective information in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry (2016) 7:191. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pichora-Fuller MK. Effects of vocal emotion on memory in younger and older adults. Exp Aging Res (2016) 42:14–30. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2016.1108734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fairfield B, Di Domenico A, Serricchio S, Borella E, Mammarella N. Emotional prosody effects on verbal memory in older and younger adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn (2017) 24:408–17. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1219690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schirmer A. Mark my words: tone of voice changes affective word representations in memory. PLoS One (2010) 5:9080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mill A, Allik J, Realo A, Valk R. Age-related differences in emotion recognition ability: a cross-sectional study. Emotion (2009) 9(5):619. doi: 10.1037/a0016562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ruffman T, Henry JD, Livingstone V, Phillips LH. A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2008) 32:863–81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

20. Babchuk WA, Hames RB, Thompson RA. Sex differences in the recognition of infant facial expressions of emotion: the primary caretaker hypothesis. Ethol Sociobiol (1985) 6(2):89–101. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(85)90002-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Circelli KS, Clark US, Cronin-Golomb A. Visual scanning patterns and executive function in relation to facial emotion recognition in aging. Aging Neuropsychol C (2013) 20(2):148–73.

Google Scholar

22. Paulmann S, Schmidt P, Pell M, Kotz SA. Rapid processing of emotional and voice information as evidenced by ERPs. In: Barbosa PA, Madureira S, Reis C, editors. Proceedings of the Conference on Speech Prosody 2008 (2008) pp. 205–9.

Google Scholar

23. Ross ED, Monnot M. Affective prosody: what do comprehension errors tell us about hemispheric lateralization of emotions, sex and aging effects, and the role of cognitive appraisal. Neuropsychologia (2011) 49(5):866–77.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord (2006) 93:105–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Torres IJ, Boudreau VG, Yatham LN. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2007) Suppl. 434:17–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01055.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Dittmann S, Hennig-Fast K, Gerber S, Seemüller F, Riedel M, Emanuel Severus W, et al. Cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II patients. Bipolar Disord (2008) 10:877–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00640.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Vaskinn A, Sundet K, Friis S, Simonsen C, Birkenæs AB, Engh JA, et al. The effect of gender on emotion perception in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2007) 116:263–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00991.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Vederman AC, Weisenbach SL, Rapport LJ, Leon HM, Haase BD, Franti LM, et al. Modality-specific alterations in the perception of emotional stimuli in bipolar disorder compared to healthy controls and major depressive disorder. Cortex (2012) 48:1027–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mercer L, Becerra R. A unique emotional processing profile of euthymic bipolar disorder? A critical review. J Affect Disord (2013) 146:295–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Kauer-Sant’Anna M, Yatham LN, Tramontina J, Weyne F, Cereser KM, Gazalle FK, et al. Emotional memory in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry (2008) 192:458–63. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.040295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Altamura M, Padalino AF, Stella E, Balzotti A, Bellomo A, Palumbo R, et al. Facial emotion recognition in bipolar disorder and healthy aging. J Nerv Ment Dis (2016) 204:188–93. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lex C, Meyer TD, Marquart B, Thau K. No strong evidence for abnormal levels of dysfunctional attitudes, automatic thoughts, and emotional information-processing biases in remitted bipolar I affective disorder. Psychol Psychother (2008) 81:1–13. doi: 10.1348/147608307X252393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Derntl B, Seidel EM, Kryspin-Exner I, Hasmann A, Dobmeier M. Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Br J Clin Psychol (2009) 48:363–75. doi: 10.1348/014466509X404845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Van der Gucht E, Morriss R, Lancaster G, Kinderman P, Bentall RP. Psychological processes in bipolar affective disorder: negative cognitive style and reward processing. Br J Psychiatr (2009) 194:146–51. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.047894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Rossel SL, Van Rheenen TE, Groot C, Gogos A, O’Regan A, Joshua NR. Investigating affective prosody in psychosis: a study using the comprehensive affective testing system. Psychiatry Res (2013) 210:896–900. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Van Rheenen TE, Rossell SL. Auditory-prosodic processing in bipolar disorder: from sensory perception to emotion. J Affect Disord (2013) 151:1102–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hofer A, Baumgartner S, Benecke C, Biedermann F, Deisenhammer EA, Hausmann A, et al. Perception of affective prosody in remitted patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res (2010) 2(117):213–4. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Deveney CM, Brotman MA, Decker AM, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Affective prosody labeling in youths with bipolar disorder or severe mood dysregulation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2012) 53:262–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02482.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hoertnagl CM, Yalcin-Siedentopf N, Baumgartner S, Biedermann F, Deisenhammer EA, Hausmann A, et al. Affective prosody perception in symptomatically remitted patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res (2014) 158:100–4. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bozikas VP, Kosmidis MH, Tonia T, Andreou C, Focas K, Karavatos A. Impaired perception of affective prosody in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2007) 19:4. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19.4.436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mitchell RLC, Elliot R, Barry M, Cruttenden A, Woodruff PWR. Neural response to emotional prosody in schizophrenia and in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatr (2004) 184:223–30. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.3.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Tucker DM, Watson RT, Heilman KM. Discrimination and evocation of affectively intoned speech in patients with right parietal disease. Neurology (1977) 27:947–50. doi: 10.1212/WNL.27.10.947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Blonder L, Bowers D, Heilman K. The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain (1991) 114:1115–27. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.3.1115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Buchanan TW, Lutz K, Mirzazade S, Specht K, Shah NJ, Zilles K, et al. Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (2000) 9:227–38. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00060-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. George MS, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ketter TA, Kimbrell TA, Heilman KM, et al. Understanding emotional prosody activates right hemisphere regions. Arch Neurol (1996) 53:665670. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1996.00550070103017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Jacobs RH, Pavuluri MN, Schenkel LS, Palmer A, Khushbu S, Vemuri D, et al. Negative emotion impacts memory for verbal discourse in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord (2011) 13:287–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00922.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. First M, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatry Association (2015).

Google Scholar

48. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry (1978) 133(5):429–35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1960) 23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol (1988) 54:1063–70. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation (1981).

Google Scholar

52. Reilly J, Troiani V, Grossman M, Wingfield A. An introduction to hearing loss and screening procedures for behavioral research. Behav Res Methods (2007) 39:667–72. doi: 10.3758/BF03193038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods (2007) 39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Cohen LJ. The probable and the provable. (1977).

Google Scholar

55. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge (2013).

Google Scholar

56. Montefinese M, Ambrosini E, Fairfield B, Mammarella N. The adaption of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) for Italian. Behav Res Methods (2014) 46:887–903. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0405-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Demenescu LR, Kato Y, Mathiak K. Neural processing of emotional prosody across the adult lifespan. Biomed Res Int (2015) 2015(590216):9. doi: 10.1155/2015/590216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Salthouse TA. Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing. Psychol Sci (1991) 2(3):179–83.

Google Scholar

59. Gardner DM, Murphy AL, O’Donnell H, Centorrino F, Baldessarini RJ. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:686–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Tanner WP Jr., Swets JA. A decision-making theory of visual detection. Psychol Rev (1954) 61(6):11. doi: 10.1037/h0058700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ochsner KN. Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. J Exp Psychol Gen (2000) 129:242–61. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.2.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Windmann S, Kutas M. Electrophysiological correlates of emotion-induced recognition bias. J Cogn Neurosci (2001) 13:577–92. doi: 10.1162/089892901750363172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kensinger EA, Corkin S. Memory enhancement for emotional words: are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Mem Cognit (2003) 31:1169–80. doi: 10.3758/BF03195800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Whalley HC, McKirdy J, Romaniuk L, Sussman J, Johnstone EC, Wan HI, et al. Functional imaging of emotional memory in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord (2009) 11:840–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00768.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Kitayama S. Remembrance of emotional speech: improvement and impairment of incidental verbal memory by emotional voice. J Exp Soc Psychol (1996) 32: 289–308. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1996.0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Schirmer A, Chen CB, Ching A, Tan L, Hong RY. Vocal emotions influence verbal memory: neural correlates and interindividual differences. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2013) 13:80–93. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0132-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Morris CD, Bransford JD, Franks JJ. Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav (1977) 16:519–33. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Greenberg SN, Tokarev J, Estes Z. Affective orientation influences memory for emotional and neutral words. Am J Psychol (2012) 125:71–80. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.125.1.0071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, Palumbo R, Fairfield B. When green is positive and red is negative: aging and the influence of color on emotional memories. Psychol Aging (2016) 31:914–26. doi: 10.1037/pag0000122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bipolar disorder, vocal prosody, emotion, memory, recognition, comprehension

Citation: Altamura M, Santamaria L, Elia A, Angelini E, Padalino FA, Altamura C, Padulo C, Mammarella N, Bellomo A and Fairfield B (2019) Emotional Prosody Effects on Verbal Memory in Euthymic Patients With Bipolar Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 10:466. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00466

Received: 21 March 2019; Accepted: 12 June 2019;
Published: 04 July 2019.

Edited by:

Philipp Kanske, Dresden University of Technology, Germany

Reviewed by:

Gadi Gilam, Stanford University, United States
Sarah Kittel-Schneider, University Hospital Frankfurt, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Altamura, Santamaria, Elia, Angelini, Padalino, Altamura, Padulo, Mammarella, Bellomo and Fairfield. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mario Altamura, m_altamura@virgilio.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.