Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 21 December 2020
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Perinatal Mental Health: Expanding the Focus to the Family Context View all 30 articles

Parental Cognitions About Sleep Problems in Infants: A Systematic Review

\nSusanne Knappe
Susanne Knappe1*Anna-Lisa PfarrAnna-Lisa Pfarr1Johanna PetzoldtJohanna Petzoldt1Samia Hrtling,Samia Härtling1,2Julia Martini,Julia Martini1,3
  • 1Institute of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
  • 2University of Meißen (FH) and Centre of Further Education, Meißen, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, Carl Gustav Carus University Hospital, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Introduction: Parental cognitions may directly and indirectly contribute to infant sleep outcomes. This review provides a systematic up-to-date overview of the associations between parental cognitions and infant sleep problems with special emphasis on temporal relationships and the content of parental cognitions.

Methods: A systematic literature research in PubMed and Web of Science Core Collection sensu Liberati and PRISMA guidelines was carried out in March 2020 using the search terms (parent* AND infant* AND sleep* problem*), including studies with correlational or control group designs investigating associations between parental cognitions and sleep problems in children aged 1–6 years.

Results: Twenty-three studies (published from 1985 to 2016) met inclusion criteria, of which 14 reported group differences or associations between parental sleep-related cognitions and child sleep outcomes. Nine papers additionally reported on the role of general parental child-related cognitions not directly pertaining to sleep. Findings from longitudinal studies suggest that parental cognitions often preceded child sleep problems. Cognitions pertaining to difficulties with limit-setting were especially prevalent in parents of poor sleepers and were positively associated with both subjective and objective measures of child sleep outcomes.

Conclusions: Parental cognitions appear to play a pivotal role for the development and maintenance of sleep problems in young children, arguing that parents' attitudes and beliefs regarding child sleep inadvertently prompts parental behavior toward adverse sleep in offspring. Associations are however based on maternal reports and small to moderate effect sizes. Thus, additional parental factors such as mental health or self-efficacy, as well as additional offspring factors including temperamental dispositions and regulatory abilities, require consideration in further studies.

Introduction

Infant sleep patterns develop rapidly over the 1st year of life. During the 1st months of development, infant sleep is fractionated into numerous sessions that last for about 4 h throughout the day. After 6 months, sleep becomes more stable, with night sleep duration increasing and daytime sleep decreasing simultaneously (1, 2). By the age of 12 months, most infants no longer need to be fed at night and sleep patterns further consolidate, with longer periods of uninterrupted sleep and a smaller number and duration of nighttime awakenings (1, 36). Albeit the majority of infants develop stable sleeping behaviors and self-soothing abilities at 12 months of age (7), sleep continues to be fragmented and disrupted in some infants after this period (8). Hence, sleep issues in toddlerhood and childhood are a major parental concern in childrearing, a primary reason for the interruption of parental sleep, and a common subject of referrals to professional care (911). According to parents and child-care professionals, bedtime, or night waking problems are the most common infant sleep problems (8, 10, 1214). Prevalence rates of infant sleep problems consistently range between 20 and 30 percent across studies in newborns, infants, and toddlers (12, 15, 16). When left untreated, they pose a substantial risk to persist (8) until preschool age (17), leading to a number of adverse outcomes in later child development such as behavioral problems or lower cognitive performance [(1820)]. In addition, sleep problems interfere with parental mental and physical health (21, 22) and are associated with increased levels of parenting stress (23, 24).

Risk factors for sleep problems include child variables such as temperament (25) and genetic factors (26), parent-centered variables such as parental mental health (22, 27) and psychological functioning (28), as well as marital stability (29, 30), and parenting behaviors promoting the implementation/consolidation of stable bedtime routines (3133) or parental involvement at nighttime (34) [see (35) for a recent overview]. Sadeh et al. (36) reviewed that parental behaviors such as interactions at bedtime, soothing strategies and limit-setting strategies were linked directly to infant sleep variables. In fact, active parental involvement at nighttime and excessive comforting have consistently been linked to sleep onset difficulties and night awakening problems in infants and young children (7, 34, 3742). Soothing, holding or feeding until the child falls asleep, as well as parental presence at bedtime in general, may interfere with the child's development of self-soothing abilities and falling asleep independently. Following Sadeh et al., excessive parental concerns regarding the limitation of personal involvement at nighttime were associated with disrupted sleep in toddlers and infants. In contrast, minimal parental assistance and early encouragement of infants' autonomy were associated with more consolidated sleep (36). As parental cognitions drive parents' behaviors around infant sleep, a better understanding of the temporal relationships and contents of parental expectations, attitudes, concerns, and beliefs is warranted to inform targeted behavioral interventions for improving infants' sleep.

In sum, this review contributes to the field by an actualized literature overview, considering specific parental cognitions, and extending the infant age range up to preschoolers. The term cognition is understood as perceptions, attitudes and beliefs regarding their child's behavior (here: child's sleep) including concerns, worries, and fears about the child's sleep. In this sense, Sadeh et al. included four studies directly examining the link between cognitions (defined as perceptions, attitudes, expectations, and interpretations about infant sleep) and sleep (4346), and recapitulate that parental cognitions that expressed difficulties with limit-setting were most strongly associated with problematic infant sleep behaviors, suggesting that together with parental health and feeding concerns, parental cognitions regarding limit-setting might be particularly important when dealing with sleep onset or night waking issues in young children. Since completion of their review 10 years ago (in 2010), additional studies have added to this empirical base. Furthermore, specific parental cognitions about infant sleep or bedtime situations [cf. (36)] should be broadened to more general cognitions on parental feeding and safety concerns as well as parental self-efficacy during bedtime procedure. These cognitions may remain relatively stable across a variety of parent-child interactions and thus affect child sleep quality even when not specifically related to sleep situations. As sleep problems have been shown to persist in older children such as preschoolers (8), we will also review research evidence pertaining to parental cognitions for sleep onset difficulties and night waking problems beyond infancy and toddlerhood, up to preschool age.

We systematically review associations of both specific and general parental child-related cognitions (expectations, attitudes, concerns, and beliefs) with sleep problems in toddlers and young children up to 6 years of age. Second, we expect parental cognitions regarding limit-setting and concerns about their child's well-being to be more strongly associated with sleep problems, as compared to other cognitions such as doubt about parenting competence. Based on previous findings on the direction of associations, we argue that parental cognitions in favor of limit-setting are associated with less sleep problems, whereas parental concerns are expected to be positively associated with sleep problems in children.

Materials and Methods

A systematic literature review sensu Liberati et al. (47) and the PRISMA guidelines of the electronic databases PubMed and Web of Science Core Collection was undertaken. The same search algorithm was also applied for random search in Ebscohost databases (including APA PsycArticles, APA PsycInfo, PSYNDEX Literature with PSYNDEX tests). Publications were considered eligible if original articles were published in English or German before March 2020. Given that stable sleep patterns in infants typically emerge at the end of the 1st year of life (48), and to account for school entrance as a developmental milestone (49), only studies that assessed outcome variables in toddlers and preschool children between the age of 12 months and 6 years were included. For studies examining children of mixed ages, a sample mean between 8 months and 6 years, as well as statistically controls for age in all relevant data analyses was required. Papers were included when the association between parental cognitions and child sleep variables was examined using either a correlational or control group design.

Variables Assessed

Parental cognitions were defined as any cognition pertaining to the (own) child, child behavior, interactions with the child and the relationship with the child, as well as cognitions regarding parenting and the own role as a parent. General cognitions refer to stable parental feeding and safety concerns as well as parental self-efficacy during, but not limited to, bedtime procedure. In contrast, specific cognitions pertain to specific infant sleep or bedtime situations. Studies assessing more general concepts not directly related to parenting and/or the child, for example overall self-efficacy in major depression, were excluded, as well as studies examining parental knowledge (rather than cognitions) on child sleep and healthy sleep practices. Studies mixing up cognitions and behaviors that were not measured independently, and therefore did not allow for separate interpretation of results, were also excluded [e.g., in case of the overall stress score on the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form [PSI-SF; (50)]].

Child sleep problems were defined by sleep onset difficulties including sleep onset latency, bedtime resistance, nocturnal awakening that require parental intervention [e.g., soothing strategies, reunions with the parent; [(48, 5153)]. Across classificatory systems and guidelines for clinically diagnosing sleep disorders in toddlers and young children, there seems to be common ground across parents and child-care professionals regarding the adversity of infant difficulties to initiate or maintain sleep during the night, which are referred to as bedtime or night waking problems. We thus only included studies that assessed sleep onset latency or the presence, number and duration of nocturnal awakenings in children from 1 to 6 years. Sleep efficiency describes the amount of time a child is asleep at night in proportion to the actual time spent in bed, including nocturnal awakenings and sleep onset latency. It was therefore considered as reliable proxy to the aforementioned variables. Likewise, in clinical samples, primary diagnostic criteria were required to comprise sleep onset latency and nocturnal awakenings, in separation from other sleep problems such as nightmares, somnambulism or sleep disordered breathing.

Search Strategy and Data Extraction

Identification of relevant papers was based on a systematic literature research in PubMed and Web of Science Core Collection in March 2020 (Figure 1). The same search algorithm was also applied for random search in Ebscohost databases (including APA PsycArticles, APA PsycInfo, PSYNDEX Literature with PSYNDEX tests) without identifying additional publications for the review. Searches were conducted in accordance with the PICO approach (47) using the search terms: (parent* AND infant* AND sleep* problem*), with key words referring to the targeted study population (infants), intervention or exposure (parenting), and control group and outcome (sleep problems). Screening of selected records and their reference lists, as well as Google Scholar and ResearchGate were used to identify publications not otherwise listed in data bases. Ninety-nine full-texts were assessed for eligibility, of which 77 were excluded. Hereupon, a full-text analysis was carried out on N = 23 original study papers. Data extraction using a fully standardized protocol (see Supplementary Table 1), as well as screenings and full-text analyses were performed by ALP, SK, and JM. Duplicates were removed manually. Studies were excluded when they did not meet inclusion criteria. Reasons for exclusion were lack of correlational analysis or control group design (N = 16 studies), lack of assessment on sleep problems (N = 13 studies), or parental cognitions (N = 27), study population older than 6 years (N = 10 studies), other than original study (N = 4 studies: book chapters, reviews, meta-analyses), language other than English or German (N = 3 studies), and full text unavailable (N = 3 studies).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of study selection.

Extracted data included general study information such as name of authors, year of publication, sample size, child age at sleep assessment, sample type (community/clinical) and study design, as well as study specific information on sleep variables and parental cognitions, correlation coefficients, p-values, effect sizes and directions of group effects and group comparisons. In acknowledgment of the cultural diversity in parental sleep practices and child sleep outcomes (54, 55), information about the cultural and ethnical background and socioeconomic status (SES) of study samples was also collected.

Results

Study Characteristics

The majority of original studies (15/23) were published in 2006 or later (range 1985–2016). All included papers were written in English. Sample sizes varied across studies, from four papers (studies no. 9, 11, 15, 17) reporting on N < 50 participants, to three national surveys (studies no. 16, 19, 22) examining up to N = 2,000 subjects. All studies were observational studies of which n = 17/23 included cross-sectional control-group and correlational designs, and n = 5 included a longitudinal design. One study [Morrel and Steele (43), study no. 3] had both a cross-sectional correlational design and a longitudinal approach. The majority of studies was based on community samples, while five studies (studies no. 4, 12, 14a, 14b, 17) reported on a clinical sample compared to a community sample. One study (no. 11) examined children clinically diagnosed with Williams Syndrome, while another study (no. 7) examined children of parents raised under communal sleeping arrangements, who were especially prone to problematic sleep-related cognitions. Most studies were based on Caucasian samples from Australia, Canada, the UK and the USA, with only one study from the USA reporting diversity in their sample (no. 13). Notably, a total of ten studies (studies no. 4, 6, 7, 10, 12, 14a, 14b, 18, 21, 23) were conducted in Israel, making this cultural group particularly well-represented in the body of evidence assembled here. Most families were indexed as having a middle-high SES, with only four studies assessing families of more diverse SES.

Most studies included infants up to 3 years, while three studies (studies no. 5, 8, 11) included children up to 5 years (referred to as “preschoolers”). One study (no. 10) examined children between 4 and 6 years of age.

Of the 23 included studies, n = 14 reported on sleep-related cognitions. Four of these studies were also reviewed by [Sadeh et al. (36)] (Supplementary Table 2). Meanwhile, nine studies assessed child-related cognitions not specifically referring to sleep (Supplementary Table 3). One study (no. 14a, 14b) reported on both sleep-related and child-related cognitions.

Question 1: Associations of Specific and General Parental Child-Related Cognitions With Sleep Problems

Results for Specific Sleep-Related Parental Cognitions

Fourteen studies reported on the association of sleep-related cognitions in parents with offspring sleep outcomes (Table 1). For child sleep outcomes, the number of nocturnal awakenings (Seven studies: 4, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11), average duration of nocturnal awakenings (Two studies: studies no. 4, 8), sleep onset latency (Two studies: studies no. 4, 7), and sleep efficiency (study no. 10) were considered (Table 1). Four studies (studies no. 1, 5, 9, 13) used composite scores of a combination of sleep variables. The other studies (studies no. 1-4, 12, 14a) reported sum scores for diverse outcome measures on infant sleep.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies on sleep related cognitions in parents and offspring sleep.

Results for General Child-Related Parental Cognitions

Significant associations between parental cognitions not specifically pertaining to child sleep and child sleep variables were expected. These findings are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Studies on child-related cognitions not referring to sleep.

As shown in Table 2, seven studies (studies no. 14b, 15-17, 19, 21, 24) reported findings based on cross-sectional case control or control group designs, while three studies (studies no. 18, 20, 22) reported findings based on longitudinal (i.e., prospective cohort-designs). With regard to offspring sleep variables, most studies compared sleep problems with a control group, or used a control-group design. Others created categorical variables, however, categorization criteria for the sleep problem group and control group varied across studies. One paper (no. 14b) compared a group of infants receiving professional help for their sleep problems to two control groups, another two papers (studies no. 17, 21) identified sleep problem groups according to Richman's criteria (1981). Three studies (studies no. 15, 18, 22) classified infants with regard to the presence/absence of night waking problems or bedtime resistance as well as the number of nocturnal awakenings per week or sleep duration of <6 consecutive hours per night (no. 19). Sleep variables further comprised the number and duration of nocturnal awakenings and sleep onset latency in minutes for the purpose of group categorization (studies no. 18, 20, 22).

Overall, 10 studies (studies no. 14b, 15-22, 24) reported on eight distinct cognitions (indicated in italics) and their associations with child sleep problems. One study (no. 19) however, did not find an association between maternal self-efficacy and overprotectiveness with nocturnal awakenings. Three studies (studies no. 15, 16, 17) found higher levels of maternal acceptance and more positive feelings and perceptions of the child in controls than in sleep problem groups. Of note, maternal disengagement and distortion were unrelated to sleep problems in offspring (17). One study (24) also reported significant correlations between infant nocturnal awakenings and impaired bonding, parental anger and rejection toward the infant, as well as accepting parental attitudes toward the child. Two studies (16, 21) observed that parents of infants with sleep problems felt more restricted by their parenting duties and perceived their child to dominate their lives more strongly than parents of controls. Evidence pertaining to parental self-efficacy and sense of parental impact was based on two longitudinal studies (20, 22), and was not observed in cross-sectional studies (19, 24). Simard et al. [(25); study no. 20] found that parents of children with a sleep onset latency longer than 15 min at the ages of 50 months, 5 and 6 years felt less self-efficient as a parent when the child was at the ages of 5, 17, and 29 months. These parents also had a lower sense of parental impact when the child was aged 5 and 17 months. However, these associations attenuated to non-significance when analyses were adjusted for maternal depression. Similarly, self-efficacy and sense of parental impact measured at ages 5, 17, and 29 months were significantly lower in parents of infants with nocturnal awakenings longer than 20 min at the age of 29 months (22), when compared to nocturnal awakenings of <20 min or control groups. Higher levels of maternal overprotection at the ages of 5, 17, and 29 months predicted child sleep problem group-membership at 29 months, but only in a prospective study (22; but not in 19).

Scher and Blumberg [no. 18 (68)] examined whether maternal separation anxiety at 6 months was linked to the infant being a “waker” at the age of 12 months, and whether the number of nocturnal awakenings and child sleep onset latency at 12 months differed according to maternal regulation beliefs. Their results showed that higher levels of separation-related concerns were reported in mothers of infants who woke up at least once per night. Moreover, mothers classified as “Bipolars” more frequently had infants with longer sleep onset latency at 12 months, compared to other groups. Also, nocturnal awakening was reported most often in infants of mothers with dysfunctional regulation beliefs (pertaining to co-regulation and facilitation) as compared to mothers with more functional beliefs (i.e., Regulators and Facilitators). Finally, Sadeh et al. [(63), study no. 14b] found that parents of infants with sleep problems attributed more distress to infant crying sounds compared to parents of infants without sleep problems, but not compared to married couples without children.

Question 2: Parental Cognitions Pertaining to Limit-Setting and Parental Concerns

Evidence on the particular role of limit-setting was found in nine studies (Table 1; 17 correlations; studies no. 1, 3, 4, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 12). As expected, cognitions on the importance of limit-setting were negatively correlated with problematic sleep behaviors, while difficulties with limit-setting were positively associated with sleep problems. Three studies compared a group of participants with sleep problems to a control group (studies no. 1, 3, 4). One of these studies (no. 12) did not reveal any differences in limit-setting between the groups (albeit differences were observed for doubts about managing infant sleep, and anger at infants' demands around). The remaining two studies (studies no. 1, 4) found that higher levels of unfavorable cognitions indicated difficulties with limit-setting in parents of poor sleepers. However, couples without children (i.e., hypothetical parents of poor sleepers) actually emphasized the importance of limit-setting more than parents of control infants (studies no. 4, 14a). Cognitions regarding limit-setting were more often reported in the sleep problem group (no. 3). Cognitions on limit-setting at 12 months also predicted the number of nocturnal awakenings at age 4 (no. 19). For one study (no. 12), no group differences emerged, and correlations were also not significant in a subsample of children clinically diagnosed for Williams Syndrome compared to significant findings in the comparison group (no. 11).

For parental concerns, as measured by the Maternal Cognitions about Infant Sleep Questionnaire (MCISQ) feeding and safety scales (studies no. 1, 4, 7, 12), only one out of four studies reported small effects, thereby linking feeding concerns to more nocturnal awakenings in community children, as well as to longer sleep onset latency (no. 7). Similarly, (61) (no. 12) found a greater level of parental feeding concerns in a selected sample of infants with sleep problems, as compared to control parents. Concerns about the infant being scared or distressed at night, combined with the urge to actively sooth and comfort the infant were examined in seven studies (studies no. 4, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 14a) of which five (studies no. 6, 7, 8, 11, 14a) reported positive associations with subjective measures of nocturnal awakenings. For three correlations, parental distress had been assessed up to 4 years before the report on child sleep outcome at 12 months (no. 6). Three other studies did not find associations with subjective or objective measures of child sleep (studies no. 4, 10, 11), however, in the Williams Syndrome subsample [(60); study no. 11] parental distress was associated with child sleep in the comparison group.

For cognitions expressing anger toward the infant and doubt about individual parenting competence (studies no. 1, 3, 4, 7, 9, 12), four studies (studies no. 1, 2, 3, 5) reported significant findings for composite scores on problematic sleep behaviors, as well as for the subjective number and duration of nocturnal awakenings and objective measures for sleep onset latency. Likewise, parents of infants with sleep problems scored higher on anger scales than parents of unaffected controls (studies no. 1, 3, 12). Parental doubts about their competencies were associated with objective (but not with subjective) sleep measures in one study (no. 4). Parental competency doubts were also positively associated with sleep onset latency and sleep problem composite scores, but negatively associated with the number and duration of nocturnal awakenings. These associations were weak to moderate. With regard to between-group differences, two studies reported more doubts in parents of infants with sleep problems (studies no. 1, 12). In contrast, one study found anger and doubt unrelated to nocturnal awakenings in infants (no. 9).

Apart from evidence for specific cognition scales, few studies reported on the common impact of parental cognitions on child sleep outcomes. For example, Morrell and Steele [(43); study no. 3] found that the combination of anger and cognitions on problems with limit-setting significantly predicted continuous sleep problem group- membership in toddlers. Similarly, a composite score of limit-setting, anger, and doubt was found to be significantly higher among parents of toddlers with sleep problems (no. 2), as well as being moderately associated with sleep problems in preschool children (no. 5). In a study by Teti and Crosby [(58); study no. 9], the combination of parental limit-setting and feeding concerns (but not helplessness and loss of control) was moderately associated with infant night waking. Finally, MCISQ total scores comprising all subscales of limit setting, anger, doubt, feeding and safety were significantly elevated in parents of children with sleep problems, compared to parents of controls (studies no. 1, 12). These scores were also positively correlated with child sleep dysregulation at 12 months (no. 13).

Discussion

This systematic review aims to provide a comprehensive and up-to-date overview on the role of parental cognitions for sleep onset difficulties and night waking problems in toddlers and young children up to the age of six. A total of 23 studies were reviewed and findings are in line with a previous meta-analysis (36). Our review revealed associations for both general and sleep-related cognitions in parents, indicating that parental cognitions do not need to specifically focus on the child's sleep in order to be associated with child sleep problems. Moreover, parental cognitions expressing difficulties with limit-setting and concerns about the child's well-being were more strongly associated with child sleep problems than other parental cognitions such as doubt about parenting competence.

The majority of studies reported correlations between sleep onset difficulties and night waking problems. Three studies reported higher levels of problematic cognitions in parents of infants with sleep problems, compared to parents of infants without sleep problems. Effect sizes of correlations and between-group analyses were mostly in the weak to moderate range, indicating a moderate but stable effect of parental cognitions in child sleep. This is in line with studies of parental cognitions in other domains of child development such as feeding problems (74) and externalizing behavior (75). Only a few studies reported strong effects (studies no. 1, 3, 11). Associations were also observed in three prospective studies (studies no. 3, 6, 10), indicating that problematic parental cognitions may not only support the persistence of sleep problems, but also promote the development of fragmented sleep patterns in infants. In particular, limit-setting emerged as a substantial correlate or even predictor for sleep problems in infants. Results from longitudinal studies (no. 9) showed that toddlers' nocturnal awakenings at 12 months were associated with problematic parental cognitions dating back as far as pregnancy. Thus, problematic parental cognitions may arise in response to child behavior, as well as shaping child development from the very beginning of infancy, implying the existence of a mutual relationship. This is in line with longitudinal studies on parental cognitions promoting child behavior [study no. 21; (7, 36)], though not necessarily vice versa [i.e., (75, 76)].

There was similar evidence for the role of more general child-related cognitions for child sleep problems. Indeed, most of the eight general cognitions we reviewed were similar to the specific sleep-related cognitions in parents, with comparable effect sizes, suggesting that adverse parental cognitions at nighttime may actually reflect more general problematic parenting beliefs or attitudes. For example, maternal acceptance, which was higher in parents of good sleepers, might reflect the same or a similar construct such as anger, albeit with inverse scale orientation. In line with these considerations, one study (no. 24) reported significant associations between anger and rejection toward the child with the number and duration of child nocturnal awakening. Another study (no.m 14b), assessing parental distress attribution to infant crying sounds found positive associations with infant night waking, suggesting that parents of children with sleep problems interpret their infants' crying at nighttime as a sign of distress, leading to more anguish to their child's crying at any given time or situation. Longitudinal studies further argued for the predictive role of parental concerns, attitudes and beliefs to affect (promote) sleep onset and night waking problems in toddlers and young children. In turn, positive cognitions such as parental self-efficacy or accepting attitudes toward the infant may serve as protective factors for infants at risk for problematic sleep behaviors. Further, parental attitudes and beliefs that are supportive of limit-setting might serve as protective factors for infants at risk of developing sleep problems.

The review also points to some differences between cognitions, as the majority of studies reported on cognitions pertaining to limit-setting (as compared to other cognitions), with cross-sectional as well as longitudinal evidence of associations with all sleep variables in both toddlers and preschoolers. Cognitions pertaining to limit-setting may affect a variety of different sleep outcomes in infants, and have been linked to child sleep more often than other cognitions. This finding is in line with studies highlighting the importance of limit-setting practices and behaviors when trying to overcome sleep problems in children (7, 34, 36, 37, 41, 42), suggesting that limit-setting cognitions and behaviors are closely interrelated in parents. This result further stresses the importance of altering limit-setting cognitions when dealing with child sleep problems. On the other hand, there were no studies examining more general parental difficulties with limit-setting in contexts other than at nighttime, a subject that may be of interest for future research.

It also remains to be discussed to which degree general cognitions related to sleep problems shape parental cognitions and behaviors in other contexts. Intuitively, problematic parental cognitions regarding to some child behaviors may also extend and generalize to child behaviors in other contexts (cf. study no. 12). However, parental feeding and safety concerns were only weakly associated with child sleep problems and reported on by a smaller number of studies than other cognitions such as doubt about the own competence as a parent.

Limitations

Findings of this review suggest that parental cognitions pertaining to limit-setting are more frequently involved in child sleep problems and prove more problematic for sleep outcomes than other cognitions. However, this systematic review does not qualify to determine effect sizes. Effect sizes of correlations and between-group analyses were mostly in the weak to moderate range. We did not perform a meta-analysis due to the considerable variability in sampling strategies, study designs and measurements of the majority of included studies. In particular, the reviewed studies included assessment instruments that do not clearly disentangle cognitive, emotional and behavioral outcomes (e.g., differentiation between parental cognitions, emotional responses, and actual parenting behaviors such as limit-setting). Also, the search terms sleep* problem* were included in this review, since this is an frequently used umbrella term in this multifaceted field of research. However, searching for the term “sleep” only would likely lead to more broader outcomes related to infant sleep. Adding the term “problem” denotes to a negative (deficit-focused) perspective on infant sleep; a maybe weaker (also less clinical-oriented) term could be “difficulties;” an even neutral term could be “behavior.” Since we defined sleep problems in line with current classification systems (see Methods), we aimed to capture clinically relevant (i.e., persisting and impairing) sleep behaviors in offspring, anticipating not to neglect significant contributions to this field of research. In addition, we also reviewed research evidence pertaining to parental cognitions for sleep onset difficulties and night waking problems beyond infancy and toddlerhood, up to preschool age.

Moreover, as there was no objective assessment of the quality of studies, more frequent and stronger effects for some cognitions might be due to publication bias or methodological differences across studies. Thus, we did not introduce an objective assessment of the quality of included studies such as systematically comparing information like number of participants, validity and reliability of measures or adjustment for confounding variables such as age of parents, infant sex, or time and place of sleep assessment. In particular, inter-parental differences in cognitions were not reported, nor was maternal depression, which is likely to be linked to parental cognitions (58, 61, 72) as well as child sleep problems [e.g., (77)]. Although many studies claim to also examine the mediating effects of parental cognitions on effective parental behavior toward child sleep variables (39, 5759, 78), detailed inspection of these analyses was beyond the scope of this paper. However, as disentangling these associations is of great interest for clinical practice and intervention programs, subsequent reviews on parental cognitions should focus on mediation effects. Conclusions are also limited by the sample size (i.e., 23 studies included) and to predominantly white and Caucasian families of middle-upper SES, limiting generalizability to other cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds. Few studies included preschoolers, albeit the link between parental cognitions and child sleep still exists at school age and during adolescence (7981). Similar to other reviews, this review is vulnerable to publication bias as only published studies and those to which we had access, were included. The majority of papers reported on maternal cognitions. Only four papers also included paternal cognitions, albeit differences between mothers and fathers were negligible.

Conclusion

Problematic parental cognitions pertaining to child sleep or other realms of child development may contribute to the development of bedtime problems and fragmented sleep patterns in young children. Moreover, negative parental cognitions toward parenting or child behavior may extend to a variety of different interaction contexts and parenting situations, thus affecting parental behavior and child development in more than one context. Finally, results imply that a large part of problematic cognitions in child sleep pertains to parental difficulties with limiting their involvement at nighttime. Problematic cognitions included troubles setting limits as well as concerns that infants might experience distress upon awakening at night, inferring that parents should directly help and sooth their infant. Though findings are based on small associations und may be considered preliminary in particular for maternal cognitions, they however suggest including parental cognitions for interventions on offspring sleep, for example targeting limit setting in CBT-based interventions including psychoeducation and cognitive techniques. Since child sleep is predictive for later (cognitive) development, early interventions targeting specific sleep-related as well as general parental cognitions are likely to modify adverse parent-child-interactions and developmental trajectories.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author Contributions

SK prepared the manuscript. ALP, SK, and JM carried out the systematic literature research. Analyses and interpretation of the data by SK, ALP, JM, JP, and SH. ALP, JP, and JM helped to draft the manuscript. JP, JM, and SH critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

OpenAccess of this article was supported by the Saechsische Landesbibliothek—Staats- und Universitaetsbibliothek Dresden.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.554221/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Table 1. Checklist of items to include when reporting a systematic review or meta-analysis.

Supplementary Table 2. Sleep-related cognitions—study sample and design characteristics.

Supplementary Table 3. Child-related cognitions (not referring to sleep)—study sample and design characteristics.

Footnotes

* ^References marked with an asterix (*) indicate studies included for review.

References

1. Sadeh A, Mindell JA, Luedtke K, Wiegand B. Sleep and sleep ecology in the first 3 years: a web-based study. J Sleep Res. (2009) 18:60–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00699.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Mindell JA, Leichman ES, Composto J, Lee C, Bhullar B, Walters RM. Development of infant and toddler sleep patterns: real-world data from a mobile application. J Sleep Res. (2016) 25:508–16. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Gaylor EE, Burnham MM, Goodlin-Jones BL, Anders TF. A longitudinal follow-up study of young children's sleep patterns using a developmental classification system. Behav Sleep Med. (2005) 3:44–61. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0301_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Galland BC, Taylor BJ, Elder DE, Herbison P. Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: a systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Med Rev. (2012) 16:213–22. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hysing M, Harvey AG, Torgersen L, Ystrom E, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Sivertsen B. Trajectories and predictors of nocturnal awakenings and sleep duration in infants. J Dev Behav Pediatrics. (2014) 35:309–16. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Weinraub M, Bender RH, Friedman SL, Susman EJ, Knoke B, Bradley R, et al. Patterns of developmental change in infants' nighttime sleep awakenings from 6 through 36 months of age. Dev Psychol. (2012) 48:1511–28. doi: 10.1037/a0027680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Burnham MM, Goodlin-Jones BL, Gaylor EE, Anders TF. Nighttime sleepwake patterns and self-soothing from birth to 1 year of age: a longitudinal intervention study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. (2002) 43:713–25. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer LJ, Sadeh A, American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings in infants and young children. Sleep. (2006) 29:1263–76.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Algarvio S, Isabel L, Maroco J. Parental concerns' prevalence and sociodemographic variables in general parenting. J Child Health Care. (2013) 17:204–14. doi: 10.1177/1367493512456107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Porter N, Ispa JM. Mothers' online message board questions about parenting infants and toddlers. J Adv Nursing. (2013) 69:559–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2012.06030.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mindell JA, Leichman ES, Puzino K, Walters R, Bhullar B. Parental concerns about infant and toddler sleep assessed by a mobile app. Behav Sleep Med. (2015) 13:359–74. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2014.905475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bruni O, Baumgartner E, Sette S, Ancona M, Caso G, Di Cosimo ME, et al. Longitudinal study of sleep behavior in normal infants during the 1st year of life. J Clin Sleep Med. (2014) 10:1119–27. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.4114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Byars KC, Yolton K, Rausch J, Lanphear B, Beebe DW. Prevalence, patterns, and persistence of sleep problems in the first 3 years of life. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:276–84. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hiscock H, Canterford L, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M. Adverse associations of sleep problems in Australian preschoolers: national population study. Pediatrics. (2007) 119:86–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. *Johnson N, McMahon C. Preschoolers' sleep behaviour: associations with parental hardiness, sleep-related cognitions and bedtime interactions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. (2008) 49:765–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01871.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Mindell JA, Meltzer LJ, Carskadon MA, Chervin RD. Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Med. (2009) 10:771–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Honaker SM, Meltzer LJ. Bedtime problems and night wakings in young children: an update of the evidence. Paediatric Respiratory Rev. (2014) 15:333–9. doi: 10.1016/j.prrv.2014.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Touchette E, Petit D, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir JY. Risk factors and consequences of early childhood dyssomnias: new perspectives. Sleep Med Rev. (2009) 13:355–61. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hemmi MH, Wolke D, Schneider S. Associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood: a meta-analysis. Archiv Disord Child. (2011) 96:622–9. doi: 10.1136/adc.2010.191312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Winsper C, Wolke D. Infant and toddler crying, sleeping and feeding problems and trajectories of dysregulated behavior across childhood. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2014) 42:831–43. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9813-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Hampton A, Wake M. Sleep problems in young infants and maternal mental and physical health. J Paediatrics Child Health. (2007) 43:66–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2007.01005.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hall WA, Moynihan M, Bhagat R, Wooldridge J. Relationships between parental sleep quality, fatigue, cognitions about infant sleep, and parental depression pre and post-intervention for infant behavioral sleep problems. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. (2017) 17:104. doi: 10.1186/s12884-017-1386-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Byars KC, Yeomans-Maldonado G, Noll JG. Parental functioning and pediatric sleep disturbance: an examination of factors associated with parenting stress in children clinically referred for evaluation of insomnia. Sleep Med. (2011) 12:898–905. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mindell JA, Goh D, Collins M, Bartle A, Kohyama J, Sekartini R, et al. Parent-perceived sleep problems and sleep goals in infants and toddlers: a global sample. Sleep. (2020) 43:A360. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa056.943

CrossRef Full Text

25. *Simard V, Nielsen TA, Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Montplaisir JY. Longitudinal study of preschool sleep disturbance: the predictive role of maladaptive parental behaviors, early sleep problems, and child/mother psychological factors. Archiv Pediatr Adoles Med. (2008) 162:360–7. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.4.360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Fisher A, van Jaarsveld CHM, Llewellyn CH, Wardle J. Genetic and environmental influences on infant sleep. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:1091–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cook F, Conway L, Gartland D, Giallo R, Keys E, Brown S. Profiles and predictors of infant sleep problems across the 1st year. J Dev Behav Pediatrics. (2020) 41:104–16. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. O'Connor TG, Caprariello P, Blackmore ER, Gregory AM, Glover V, Fleming P, et al. Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early Human Dev. (2007) 83:451–8. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kelly RJ, El-Sheikh M. Longitudinal relations between marital aggression and children's sleep: the role of emotional insecurity. J Family Psychol. (2013) 27:282–92. doi: 10.1037/a0031896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mannering AM, Harold GT, Leve LD, Shelton KH, Shaw DS, Conger RD, et al. Longitudinal associations between marital instability and child sleep problems across infancy and toddlerhood in adoptive families. Child Dev. (2011) 82:1252–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01594.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Mindell JA, Telofski S, Wiegenand B, Kurzt E. A nightly bedtime routine: impact on sleep in young children and maternal mood. Sleep. (2009) 32:599–606. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.5.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Staples AD, Bates JE, Petersen IT. Bedtime routines in early childhood: prevalence, consistency, and associations with nighttime sleep. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (2015) 80:141–59. doi: 10.1111/mono.12149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Mindell JA, Li AM, Sadeh A, Kwon R, Goh DYT. Bedtime routines for young children: a dose-dependent association with sleep outcomes. Sleep. (2015) 38:717–22. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cowie J, Palmer CA, Hussain H, Alfano CA. Parental involvement in infant sleep routines predicts differential sleep patterns in children with and without anxiety disorders. Child Psychiatry Human Dev. (2015) 47:636–46. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0597-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Belmon LS, Busch V, van Stralen MM, Stujnman DPM, Hidding L, Harmsen IA, et al. Child and parent perceived determinants of children's inadequate sleep health. A concept mapping study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1583. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051583

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sadeh A, Tikotzky L, Scher A. Parenting and infant sleep. Sleep Med Rev. (2010) 14:89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. DeLeon CW, Karraker KH. Intrinsic and extrinsic factors associated with night waking in 9-months-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. (2007) 30:596–605. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. *Morrell J, Cortina-Borja M. The developmental change in strategies parents employ to settle young children to sleep, and their relationship to infant sleeping problems, as assessed by a new questionnaire: the Parental Interactive Bedtime Behaviour Scale. Infant Child Dev. (2002) 11:17–41. doi: 10.1002/icd.251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. *Tikotzky L, Sadeh A. Maternal sleep-related cognitions and infant sleep: a longitudinal study from pregnancy through the 1st year. Child Dev. (2009) 80:860–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01302.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Mohsenian E, Tabibi Z, Mashhadi A. Settling to sleep strategies and sleeping problems among 1-2 year old infants. J Iranian Psychologists. (2014) 10:237–48.

Google Scholar

41. Sheridan A, Murray L, Cooper PJ, Evangeli M, Byram V, Halligan SL. A longitudinal study of child sleep in high and low risk families: relationship to early maternal settling strategies and child psychological functioning. Sleep Med. (2013) 14:266–73. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. St James-Roberts I, Roberts M, Hovish K, Owen C. Video evidence that parenting methods predict which infants develop long night-time sleep periods by 3 months of age. Primary Health Care Res Dev. (2017) 18:212–26. doi: 10.1017/S1463423616000451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. *Morrell J, Steele H. The role of attachment security, temperament, maternal perception, and care-giving behavior in persistent infant sleeping problems. Infant Mental Health J. (2003) 24:447–68. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. *Sadeh A, Flint-Ofir E, Tirosh T, Tikotzky L. Infant sleep and parental sleep related cognitions. J Family Psychol. (2007) 21:74–87. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. *Tikotzky L, Sharabany R, Hirsch I, Sadeh A. “Ghosts in the Nursery:” infant sleep and sleep-related cognitions of parents raised under communal sleeping arrangements. Infant Mental Health J. (2010) 31:312–34. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. *Morrell J. The role of maternal cognitions in infant sleep problems as assessed by a new instrument, the maternal cognitions about infant sleep questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. (1999) 40:247–58. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. Ann Internal Med. (2009) 151:65–94. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

48. ZERO TO THREE. Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood: Revised edition (DC: 0-3R). Washington, DC: ZERO TO THREE. (2005).

49. Grotz T. Die Bewältigung des Übergangs vom Kindergarten zur Grundschule in Abhängigkeit von der vom Kind wahrgenommenen Unterstützung. In: A Panagiotopoulou, H Brügelmann, editors. Grundschulpädagogik meets Kindheitsforschung. VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. (2003). p. 111–4. Available online at: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-322-80948-3_15 (accessed March 4, 2020).

Google Scholar

50. Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index: Manual, 3rd Edn. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (1995).

Google Scholar

51. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 3rd Edn. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine. (2014).

Google Scholar

52. Anders T, Goodlin-Jones B, Sadeh A. Sleep disorders. In: CHJ Zeanah, editor. Handbook of infant mental health, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press. (2000). p. 326–38.

Google Scholar

53. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. (DSM-5) (Vol. 5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Davis NS. Cosleeping in urban families with young children in the United States. Pediatrics. (1984) 74:171–82.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

55. Mindell JA, Sadeh A, Kohyama J, How TH. Parental behaviors and sleep outcomes in infants and toddlers: a cross-cultural comparison. Sleep Med. (2010) 11:393–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Richman N. (1981). A Community survey of characteristics of one- to two-year-olds with sleep disruptions. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 20, 281–291. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60989-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. *Coulombe JA, Reid GJ. Agreement with night-waking strategies among community mothers of preschool-aged children. J Pediatric Psychol. (2012) 37:319–28. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. *Teti DM, Crosby B. Maternal depressive symptoms, dysfunctional cognitions, and infant night waking: the role of maternal nighttime behavior. Child Dev. (2012) 83:939–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01760.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. *Tikotzky L, Shaashua L. Infant sleep and early parental sleep-related cognitions predict sleep in pre-school children. Sleep Med. (2012) 13:185–92. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. *Axelsson EL, Hill CM, Sadeh A, Dimitriou D. Sleep problems and language development in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. (2013) 34:3988–96. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. *Golik T, Avni H, Nehama H, Greenfeld M, Sivan Y, Tauman R. Maternal cognitions and depression in childhood behavioral insomnia and feeding disturbances. Sleep Med. (2013) 14:261–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. *Lemery-Chalfant K, Clifford S, McDonald K, O'Brien TC, Valiente C. Arizona twin project: a focus on early resilience. Twin Res Human Genetics. (2013) 16:404–11. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. *Sadeh A, Juda-Hanael M, Livne-Karp E, Kahn M, Tikotzky L, Anders TF, et al. Low parental tolerance for infant crying: an underlying factor in infant sleep problems? J Sleep Res. (2016) 25:501–7. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Littell R. C., Milliken G. A., Stroup W. W., Wolfinger R. D., Schabenberger O. (2006). SAS for Mixed Models, Second Edition. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.

Google Scholar

65. *Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Davis N S. Sleep problems seen in pediatric practice. Pediatrics. (1985) 75:477–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

66. *Scott G, Richards MP. Night waking in 1-year-old children in England. Child. (1990) 16:283−302. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.1990.tb00662.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. *Benoit D, Zeanah CH, Parker KCH, Nicholson E, Coolbear J. “Working model of the child interview:” infant clinical status related to maternal perceptions. Infant Mental Health J. (1997) 18:107–21. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199721)18:1<107::AID-IMHJ8>3.0.CO;2-N

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. *Scher A, Blumberg O. Night waking among 1-year olds: a study of maternal separation anxiety. Child. (1999) 25:323–34. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.1999.00099.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. *Touchette E, Petit D, Paquet J, Boivin M, Japel C, Tremblay RE, et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across early childhood. Archiv Pediatr Adoles Med. (2005) 159:242–9. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.3.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. *Millikovsky-Ayalon M, Atzaba-Poria N, Meiri G. The role of the father in child sleep disturbance: child, parent, and parent-child relationship. Infant Mental Health J. (2015) 36:114–27. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. *Zaidman-Zait A, Hall WA. Children's night waking among toddlers: relationships with mothers' and fathers' parenting approaches and children's behavioural difficulties. J Adv Nurs. (2015) 71:1639–49. doi: 10.1111/jan.12636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. *Tikotzky L. Postpartum maternal sleep, maternal depressive symptoms and self-perceived mother–infant emotional relationship. Behav Sleep Med. (2016) 14:5–22. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2014.940111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Zeifman D. M. (2003). Predicting adult responses to infant distress: adult characteristics associated with perceptions, emotional reactions, and timing of intervention. Infant Ment. Health J. 24, 597–612. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Blissett J, Meyer C, Farrow C, Bryant-Waugh R, Nicholls D. Maternal core beliefs and children's feeding problems. Int J Eating Disord. (2005) 37:127–34. doi: 10.1002/eat.20070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Barnett MA, Shanahan L, Deng M, Haskett ME, Cox MJ. Independent and interactive contributions of parenting behaviors and beliefs in the prediction of early childhood behavior problems. Parenting Sci Practice. (2010) 10:43–59. doi: 10.1080/15295190903014604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Farrow C, Blissett J. Maternal cognitions, psychopathologic symptoms, and infant temperament as predictors of early infant feeding problems: a longitudinal study. Int J Eating Disord. (2006) 39:128–34. doi: 10.1002/eat.20220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Petzoldt J, Wittchen H-U, Einsle F, Martini J. Maternal anxiety versus depressive disorders: specific relations to infants' crying, feeding and sleeping problems. Child Care Health Dev. (2016) 42:231–45. doi: 10.1111/cch.12292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Reader JM, Teti DM, Cleveland MJ. Cognitions about infant sleep: interparental differences, trajectories across the 1st year, and coparenting quality. J Family Psychol. (2017) 31:453–63. doi: 10.1037/fam00002831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Meltzer LJ, Mindell JA. Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: a pilot study. J Family Psychol. (2007) 21:67–73. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Montgomery P, Wiggs L. Definitions of sleeplessness in children with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): implications for mothers' mental state, daytime sleepiness and sleep-related cognitions. Child Care Health Dev. (2015) 41:139–46. doi: 10.1111/cch.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Ng AS, Dodd HF, Gamble AL, Hudson JL. The relationship between parent and child dysfunctional beliefs about sleep and child sleep. J Child Family Stud. (2013) 22:827–35. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9637-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infant, child, sleep problem, parental, maternal, paternal cognition

Citation: Knappe S, Pfarr A-L, Petzoldt J, Härtling S and Martini J (2020) Parental Cognitions About Sleep Problems in Infants: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 11:554221. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.554221

Received: 22 April 2020; Accepted: 20 November 2020;
Published: 21 December 2020.

Edited by:

Antje Horsch, University of Lausanne, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Katie Moraes de Almondes, Federal University of Rio Grande Do Norte, Brazil
Caroline L. Horton, Bishop Grosseteste University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Knappe, Pfarr, Petzoldt, Härtling and Martini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Susanne Knappe, susanne.knappe@tu-dresden.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.