Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 30 May 2023
Sec. Anxiety and Stress Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards a Mechanistic Understanding of Depression, Anxiety, and Their Comorbidity: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience View all 9 articles

Blunted diurnal interleukin-6 rhythm is associated with amygdala emotional hyporeactivity and depression: a modulating role of gene-stressor interactions

Yuko Hakamata,,
Yuko Hakamata1,2,3*Hiroaki HoriHiroaki Hori3Shinya MizukamiShinya Mizukami4Shuhei IzawaShuhei Izawa5Fuyuko YoshidaFuyuko Yoshida3Yoshiya MoriguchiYoshiya Moriguchi3Takashi Hanakawa,Takashi Hanakawa6,7Yusuke InoueYusuke Inoue8Hirokuni TagayaHirokuni Tagaya2
  • 1Department of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience, Toyama University School of Medicine, Toyama, Japan
  • 2Department of Health Science, Kitasato University School of Allied Health Sciences, Sagamihara, Kanagawa, Japan
  • 3Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan
  • 4Department of Radiological Technology, Kitasato University School of Allied Health Sciences, Sagamihara, Kanagawa, Japan
  • 5Occupational Stress and Health Management Research Group, National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health, Kawasaki, Kanagawa, Japan
  • 6Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan
  • 7Department of Integrated Neuroanatomy and Neuroimaging, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
  • 8Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Kitasato University School of Medicine, Sagamihara, Kanagawa, Japan

Background: The immune system has major roles in the brain and related psychopathology. Disrupted interleukin-6 secretion and aberrant amygdala emotional reactivity are well-documented in stress-related mental disorders. The amygdala regulates psychosocial stress-related interleukin-6 affected by related genes. These led us to comprehensively examine the relationship between interleukin-6, amygdala activity, and stress-related mental symptoms under gene-stressor interactions.

Methods: One hundred eight nonclinical participants with various levels of anxiety/depression underwent magnetic resonance imaging scans during an emotional face task for amygdala activity and saliva collection (at 10-time points across 2 days) for the total output and diurnal patterns of interleukin-6. Gene-stressor interactions between rs1800796 (C/G) and rs2228145 (C/A) and stressful life events for the biobehavioral measures were explored.

Results: The blunting of interleukin-6 diurnal pattern was associated with hypoactivation of the basolateral amygdala in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces (t = 3.67, FWE-corrected p = 0.003), and was predominantly observed in individuals with rs1800796 C-allele homozygotes and negative life changes in the past year (F = 19.71, p < 0.001). When considered in a comprehensive model, the diminished diurnal pattern predicted greater depressive symptoms (β = −0.40), modulated by the amygdala hypoactivity (β = 0.36) and rs1800796-stressor interactions (β = −0.41; all p < 0.001).

Conclusion: Here we show that the blunted interleukin-6 diurnal rhythm predicts depressive symptoms, modulated by amygdala emotional hyporeactivity and gene-stressor interactions. These findings indicate a potential mechanism underlying vulnerability to depressive disorders, suggesting their early detection, prevention, and treatment through the understanding of immune system dysregulation.

1. Introduction

The immune system has major roles in the brain and related mental health conditions. Despite the mechanisms of their link remaining unknown, previous research has indicated that extreme stress can disrupt the immune system regulation (1). For example, excessive secretion of glucocorticoids causes dendritic shrinking and eventually neuronal death in stress-related brain regions such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (PFC) (2) via its inducing glutamate release (3) and thereby N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor overstimulation (4, 5), while it elicits dendritic lengthening in the amygdala (particularly its basolateral part) (6). In addition, the exaggerated glucocorticoid secretion produces oxidative stress in the long term via free radical formation through mitochondrial dysfunction (1). These detrimental effects on the organism can activate the immune system (7). Hence, the immune system can be disrupted when exposed to excessive stressors, possibly leading to mental disorders (810).

Although peripheral inflammatory cytokines do not readily pass through the blood–brain barrier (BBB), they can penetrate the central nervous system under specific conditions, such as BBB alteration (11, 12), causing neuroinflammation through glial cell activation (13). Glial cells can become activated by stress as well as infection (14), specifically in the amygdala when exposed to psychosocial stressors (15, 16). The amygdala modulates behavioral and physiological responses to stressors (1), determining their emotional significance (17). Furthermore, the amygdala regulates the expression of stress-related inflammatory cytokines (18, 19) as well as corticotropin-releasing factors (20), suggesting its critical role in the immune system and stress-related mental disorders.

Supporting this idea, immune system perturbation and aberrant amygdala emotional reactivity have been reported in stress-related mental disorders. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) (2123) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (2427) as compared to healthy controls, the amygdala primarily exhibits stronger responses to emotionally negative (vs. neutral) stimuli, whereas its dorsal and lateral parts exhibit significantly weaker responses simultaneously (23, 25). Dysfunctional activity during emotional processing has been also found in the hippocampus and PFC in PTSD (25, 26, 28) and MDD (2123) with some inconsistent findings (29), whose regions are implicated in cognitive deficits such as in learning, memory, and decision making (30, 31). Moreover, previous immunological studies have demonstrated increased levels of IL-6 and C-reactive protein (CRP) but not IL-1β or tumor necrosis factor (TNF)-α (32) in MDD, and increased levels of these markers in PTSD (33, 34). Above all, disrupted IL-6 secretion has been commonly observed and thus conceivably underlies the pathophysiology of stress-related mental disorders.

IL-6, which is primarily secreted by T cells, macrophages, and adipocytes, can function as either an anti-or pro-inflammatory cytokine (35), playing a primary role in inflammation (36). IL-6 expression is regulated by related genes and their interactions with stressors (37, 38). In particular, representative single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), including rs1800795/6 (IL6: C/G) and rs2228145 (IL-6 receptor, IL6R: C/A) (39, 40), impact circulating IL-6 levels (41, 42), such that the C substitution of IL6 is reported to be associated with lower IL-6 levels (40, 43) while that of IL6R is associated with higher IL-6 levels (42, 44). Moreover, IL-6 levels possess a clear diurnal rhythm characterized by two peaks and troughs during a 24-h period (45, 46). In particular, the pattern of an evening trough to a midnight peak is predominantly observed in both saliva and blood (4548). Plasma and saliva IL-6 levels are weakly-to-moderately correlated (4951), despite the detection rate in the saliva being reportedly higher and more stable than that in the blood (52). We previously reported that individuals with a history of childhood abuse, a significant risk factor for developing stress-related mental disorders (53), exhibit a diminished salivary IL-6 diurnal rhythm (54). Similarly, flattened plasma IL-6 diurnal patterns have also been reported in individuals with combat-zone experience, regardless of the presence/absence of PTSD (55), suggesting that exposure to intense stressors potentially leads to disruption of the IL-6 diurnal patterns through allostatic overload (1). Thus, alterations in the diurnal patterns and total output of IL-6 may be the key factors underlying one’s vulnerability to stress-related mental disorders, through gene-stressor interactions.

Despite the lack of comprehensive research on the relationship between IL-6 levels and amygdala emotional reactivity in the context of depression/anxiety, several seminal studies have investigated the relationship between IL-6 levels and amygdala emotional reactivity in general. No significant difference was found in amygdala activity in response to emotional stimuli between a control group and a group with increased IL-6 levels, induced by the administration of a low-dose bacteria/toxin (56, 57). Similarly, no significant differences were observed in the relationship between the circulating IL-6 levels and amygdala emotional reactivity in healthy participants (58) and breast cancer survivors who completed treatment (59). By contrast, other studies focusing on psychosocial stress in individuals without apparent psychopathology demonstrated significant correlations between increased IL-6 levels and amygdala activity, as well as its functional connectivity (FC) with the dorsomedial PFC while receiving negative feedback (60). A significant interaction was also reported between a poverty stressor and amygdala emotional reactivity on a composite of inflammatory markers, including IL-6 (61). Accordingly, psychosocial stressors are likely to connect IL-6 secretion, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depression/anxiety through interacting with genetic factors.

Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the relationship between IL-6 levels, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depression/anxiety, considering gene-psychosocial stressor interactions, in a sizeable community sample with various levels of depression/anxiety. Specifically, we hypothesize that: (1) disrupted IL-6 secretion (i.e., increased total output or diminished diurnal rhythm) is associated with perturbed (i.e., increased or decreased) amygdala activity in response to emotional stimuli; (2) disrupted IL-6 secretion is correlated with greater depressive/anxiety symptoms; and (3) disrupted IL-6 secretion can be predicted by the interactions between psychosocial stressors and IL6 or IL6R SNPs. Additionally, given the amygdala’s regulatory role in IL-6 secretion, we examine the pathways from the amygdala to IL-6 secretion and depression under gene-stressor interactions. Further, we explore correlations between IL-6 levels and FC throughout the brain.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

We recruited participants who did not receive a psychiatric diagnosis and psychotropic medication, since either considerably affects the levels of immune/inflammatory markers and amygdala emotional reactivity (62, 63). The individuals’ risk of developing stress-related mental disorders was assessed by measuring the current stress-related symptoms (i.e., depression/anxiety) to examine their relationships with the variables of interest based on the perspective of the dimensional model for mental disorders (64). This model postulates that the manifestation of mental disorders can be dimensional, but not categorical, including individuals with various levels of correspondent mental symptoms from none to moderate (i.e., risk individuals) and severe (i.e., patients) (64).

Participants were recruited via websites, magazine advertisements, and billboards at Kitasato University from people in the community who dwelled in Tokyo and surrounding cities such as Kanagawa, Chiba, Saitama, and Ibaraki prefectures. The eligibility criteria were as follows: (1) 18–59 years old; (2) no current Axis-I psychiatric disorders or substance-abuse history as determined by the 4th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders by an experienced clinician (YH); (3) no major medical illnesses; (4) no regular intake of psychotropics, steroids, or opioids; (5) no metal nor medical appliance on/inside the body; (6) no history of brain injury or trauma with loss of consciousness over 10 min; and (7) no habitual intake of medicine for pain or fever (e.g., loxoprofen or acetaminophen) or excessive caffeine (> 400 mg/day) that could affect the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal (65). Of the 134 recruited participants, 11 did not meet the inclusion criteria due to a history of brain trauma with loss of consciousness (> 10 min, n = 1), epilepsy (n = 1), orthodontic appliances (n = 2), immune-related diseases (n = 3, including 1 hyperlipidemia, 1 systemic lupus erythematosus, and 1 ulcerative colitis case), and regular intake of loxoprofen for migraines (n = 4). In total, 123 participants provided written informed consent.

Additionally, this study shared the sample of our previous studies as follows: 50.0% (66), 75.9% (67), 76.8% (68), and 91.7% (54), although there was no overlap with Izawa et al. (46) and Hakamata et al. (69). Studies by Hakamata et al. (6668) aimed to examine the relationship between resting-state FC and memory biases, and the study by Hori et al. (54) examined the relationship between childhood abuse and IL-6 levels. However, the current study focused on the relationship between IL-6 levels, depression, and amygdala activity during an emotional task in terms of gene-stressor interactions. Thus, the focus of the current study was entirely different from those of the previous studies.

This study was approved by the Kitasato University Medical Ethics Organization (C17-126 and G18-02 for genome) and the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry Ethics Committee (A2018-064) as part of a research project to examine cognitive bias and related neurobiological mechanisms; and was conducted in compliance with ethical guidelines issued by the National Ministry of Health, Labor, and Welfare and Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Study procedures

Participants underwent psychological assessment and MRI scans on the same day; saliva was collected five times daily on two consecutive weekdays within 2 weeks of the initial assessment day. Saliva was also collected from a subset of the participants (n = 73) for DNA extraction. The sample attrition was because the genetic part of this study (G18-02) was approved by the ethics committee in June 2018, which was about 8 months after the main part started (C17-126, approved in October 2017). Participants provided the following information on potential IL-6 confounders: age, sex, body mass index (BMI), daily caffeine intake, smoking habits, monthly alcohol consumption, and years of education. For women, menstrual status information was also obtained. Scoring details of BMI, daily caffeine intake, monthly alcohol consumption, and menstrual status are described in Supplementary material S1.1.

2.3. Psychological assessment

2.3.1. Depressive/anxious symptoms

Depressive symptoms over the past 2 weeks were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (70, 71), comprising 21 items rated on a 4-point scale (from 0: “None” to 3, depending on severity). Scores ≥14 indicate the presence of significant depressive symptoms (70, 71). Additionally, subscales from the 54-item Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL) (72, 73) were employed to assess anxiety (8 items) and depressive (13 items) symptoms in the past week; each item was rated on a 4-point scale (1: “None” to 4, depending on frequency).

2.3.2. Psychosocial stressors

Psychosocial stressors were identified by assessing stressful life events experienced in the past year (e.g., divorce or separation from a partner, death of a family member, and taking a new job), using the Life Experiences Survey (LES) that comprises 57 items rated on a 7-point scale (from−3: “Extremely negative” to +3: “Extremely positive”) (74). We used the Japanese version of LES that has been standardized (75).

Scores were summed for negatively-and positively-rated life events, respectively, as negative and positive impact scores. These scores were treated as absolute values (74, 75). To assess the overall impact of the negative and positive life events that occurred in each individual, we used the balanced impact score calculated by subtracting the positive impact score from the negative impact score (74, 75). Values more than zero on this index indicate the negative life changes (NLC) experienced on the whole, while zero indicates no such NLC experienced.

According to Iwamitsu et al. (75), the balanced impact score has higher test–retest reliability (r = 0.65) than the negative impact score (r = 0.50) and positive impact score (r = 0.46; p < 0.01 for all). This score also has higher criterion-related validity: r = 0.40 with state anxiety assessed by Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (76) and r = 0.41 with depression by Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS) (77) (p < 0.01 for both), as compared to negative impact score (r = 0.29 for STAI and r = 0.31 for SDS) and positive impact score (r = 0.10 for STAI and r = 0.08 for SDS) alone.

In the case that the balanced impact score did not follow a normal distribution, participants were classified into two groups for gene-stressor interaction analysis: those with NLC and without NLC. The classification was used for clarity, but results with this score as a continuous variable are also reported in Supplementary material S2.4 for reference.

2.4. Measurement of IL-6 levels

IL-6 levels were measured based on saliva collected five times daily over two consecutive typical weekdays: upon awakening (T1), 30 min after awakening (T2), midday (11:30–12:30) (T3), in the evening (17:30-18:30) (T4), and at bedtime (T5).

2.4.1. Saliva collection procedures

Saliva collection was conducted as per the guidelines for best practices (78). An experimenter carefully explained and demonstrated saliva collection procedures for each participant, and each received written instructions describing the saliva collection procedures. The participant was encouraged to practice taking a saliva sample with the experimenter present. Specifically, participants were instructed to collect their saliva in a microtube by passively drooling for approximately 3 min and to concurrently access a customized web-entry form via their smartphone so that the exact time was automatically recorded whenever they collected saliva.

In addition, on the customized web-entry form, participants recorded the exact time they went to bed and woke up (i.e., sleep duration). They also rated their sleep quality ranging from 1 (did not sleep well) to 3 (slept well) upon awakening, their perceived stress ranging from 1 (did not perceive stress) to 4 (high-stress levels), and physical health conditions of the day ranging from 1 (bad) to 3 (typical) at bedtime. Further, women answered an extra question on their menstrual status at the starting day of saliva collection to validate their menstrual cycle provided on the assessment day.

Saliva was collected on two consecutive, typical weekdays using personalized kits with 20 color-coded tubes for each sample time (e.g., blue for bedtime), which were labeled with the date and time of measurement. Participants chose a typical weekday within 2 weeks after the initial assessment to begin collecting saliva. Also, for naturalistic measurement (i.e., under participants’ usual routine), the collection time of T1 and T5 were not specifically designated as long as the participant did not engage in shift work. They were instructed never to collect saliva on days when they would be engaged in any special activity or relaxing at home. During the two consecutive days, as well as the night before, the participants were required not to consume alcohol. Similarly, taking any food or drink besides water, exercising, tooth-brushing, and showering or bathing was not allowed 1 h before saliva collection.

In the cases required to collect saliva away from home (e.g., office or school), two freezer packs and a cooler bag were provided to each participant designed to keep the saliva refrigerated below 3°C for at least 8 h at an outdoor temperature of 25°C. Participants were instructed to store collected samples in their home freezers below −18°C as soon as possible when they returned home. Within 3 days of completing saliva collection, the samples were transported to the National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health in a frozen state (to author SI).

2.4.2. IL-6 assay

For the salivary assay, slowly thawed samples were centrifuged (3,000 rpm, 1710-g force) for 10 min, and the concentration of IL-6 (pg/ml) was quantified by a Human IL-6 Quantikine HS ELISA (enzyme-linked immunoassay) Kit, 3rd Generation (R&D Systems, United States). Then, IL-6 levels at each time point were adjusted for total salivary protein levels at the corresponding time point (mg/ml) measured by a Bradford Protein Assay Kit (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc., United States) [i.e., IL-6 (pg/ml) divided by total protein (mg/ml)]. Intra-and inter-assay coefficients of variation for IL-6 were 4.4 and 9.9%, respectively. Detailed assay procedures are reported in our previous study (54).

2.4.3. Calculation of IL-6 indices

2.4.3.1. Total output

We calculated the area under the curve with respect to ground level (AUCg) based on the following standard formula (79): [(T1 + T2) × TimeT2-T1 (h)/2] + [(T2 + T3) × TimeT3-T2 (h)/2] + [(T3 + T4) × TimeT4-T3 (h)/2] + [(T4 + T5) × TimeT5-T4 (h)/2].

2.4.3.2. Diurnal patterns

Izawa et al. (46) measured IL-6 levels at seven-time points and revealed that salivary IL-6 has a biphasic diurnal pattern, as seen with plasma IL-6, despite slight differences in observed time of peaks and troughs. In plasma, a peak tended to appear between 16:00 and 18:00, with a trough around 21:00; a second larger peak emerged from midnight to early morning, once again reaching a trough around 8:00 (45, 47, 48, 80). In contrast, saliva had a small peak at 16:00, a trough around 18:00–19:00, and another far larger peak appeared at midnight (46). Unlike in plasma, the high IL-6 levels at midnight were sustained in saliva upon waking (7:00–8:00). This is potentially due to the accumulation of inflammatory proteins secreted during the night in saliva since salivary protein levels are reportedly high at awakening (81). Thus, the IL-6 diurnal pattern was indexed by subtracting T4 from T5 levels to assess the increase from an evening trough to a midnight peak. Due to the high salivary IL-6 levels in the morning and the insufficient number of time points, Cosine or Fourier analysis was not applied to the current data. However, the results for other potential indices (e.g., T1–T4 and T5–T3) are also reported in Supplementary Table S3. The index values were averaged across both collection days to capture an individual’s basal IL-6 secretion patterns. Positive values indicate the presence of a diurnal increase, while negative and zero values indicate its absence (i.e., a diminished/blunted or disrupted pattern).

2.5. Magnetic resonance imaging scans

Anatomical and functional MRIs were acquired using a 32-channel phased-array head coil, 3.0 T scanner (Discovery MR750; GE Healthcare, United States). Structural images were acquired using a 3D T1-weighted sequence [slice thickness without slice gap = 1.0 mm, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, matrix = 256 × 256, repetition time (TR) = 6.4 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.6 ms, inversion time (TI) = 400 ms, and flip angle (FA) = 14°]. For functional images, data were acquired using fast-gradient echo-planar T2*-weighted imaging with five dummy volumes at the beginning of the session. Each functional volume comprised 38 transverse slices (slice thickness = 3.0 mm, slice gap = 1.0 mm, FOV = 192 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, TR = 3,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, and FA = 90°).

For preprocessing and analyses of brain activity and FC, we used SPM121 and CONN Functional Connectivity Toolbox, version 18b,2 respectively; both were operated on the Matlab 2019b platform.3

2.6. Functional magnetic resonance imaging preprocessing

For brain activity analysis, the following preprocessing procedures were performed using SPM12: realignment, slice timing correction, co-registration of the structural image to the functional images, segmentation of the T1 structural volume image, spatial normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and Gaussian spatial smoothing [full width at half maximum (FWHM): 6 mm] (82). No excessive head motion (i.e., > 3 mm translation/rotation in any direction) was observed in the realignment.

For FC analysis, the aforementioned preprocessing procedures were similarly performed using a default pipeline with outlier detection (“scrubbing” to eliminate excessive head motion). In the outlier detection, three confounders were removed via principal-component-based noise-correction (“CompCor” method) (83): signal noise from the white matter and cerebrospinal fluid; within-subject covariates, including head-motion artifacts and scrubbing parameters; and the main condition effect convolved with a hemodynamic response function. The band-pass filter was set with a frequency window of 0.008–0.12 Hz. FWHM was 6 mm.

2.7. Functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment

We employed a modified version of the face-house matching task (84), which is widely used to capture the amygdala activity in response to emotional stimuli (85), as previously reported (69). As illustrated in Figure 1, four frames are displayed for 2,000 ms, with either the two horizontal or vertical frames being highlighted in yellow. Then, after a fixation at the center of the screen presented for 1,000 ms, they were replaced by two faces (either neutral or fearful ones of the same individuals) in the location of the non-highlighted frames and two houses in the location of highlighted frames for 250 ms. Participants were instructed to always attend to the two highlighted frames and judge whether the subsequent houses are identical while faces are simultaneously appearing.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The face-house matching task. The task was created by Vuilleumier et al. (84). A total of 8 different facial stimuli (4 women and 4 men) were selected from the ATR database (DB99; ATR-Promotions, Inc., Kyoto, Japan). An example of a fearful facial stimulus borrowed from the NimStim set of facial expressions (86) is shown here, as the ATR prohibits the publication of facial stimuli. The stimulus intervals varied randomly between 3.5 and 12.5 s for a total of 48 trials, with a mean of 7.5 s. The task duration was 8 min 51 s, including the five dummy scans. The positions and emotional valences of faces were randomly presented. Both faces in each pair had the same facial expression on a given trial.

For face stimuli, we used eight different individuals’ faces with fearful and neutral expressions (four women and four men), selected from an established standard facial database for the Japanese population (DB99; ATR-Promotions, Inc., Kyoto, Japan). We used eight houses, as used in the original study (84). All pictures were black and white photographs (visual angle 18° × 13°) presented on a black background and projected through a mirror mounted onto the head coil. The stimulus presentation schedule was controlled by the E-prime 2.0 Professional software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) in synchronization with MR signals. The stimulus intervals varied randomly between 3.5 and 12.5 s for a total of 48 trials (mean: 7.5 s). The task duration was 8 min 51 s, including the five dummy scans. The positions and emotional valences of faces were randomly presented. To assess amygdala emotional reactivity, we compared responses to fearful vs. neutral faces.

2.8. DNA extraction and SNP genotyping

For genotyping, saliva samples were collected from each participant with an Oragene DNA Kit (DNA Genotek Inc., Canada). We specifically studied the rs1800796 (IL6) and rs2228145 (IL6R) SNPs. The IL6 gene is located on chromosome 7p21, with the well-documented SNP, rs1800795 (−174G/C), located at its promoter region. However, the minor allele frequency of rs1800795 is known to be 0% in the Japanese population according to SNPedia (87). Therefore, we focused on rs1800796 (−572C/G) instead, which is also located in the promoter region of IL6 in strong linkage disequilibrium with rs1800795. Like rs1800795, rs1800796 is known to also affect circulating IL-6 levels (23). The IL6R gene is located on chromosome 1q21, and its missense variant, rs2228145 (Asp358Ala; A/C), located at exon 9, maps to the IL-6R cleavage site.

Genomic DNA was purified from centrifuged saliva using the ethanol precipitation protocol and the prepIT.L2P manual purification protocol (DNA Genotek Inc., Canada). IL6 rs1800796 (assay ID: C___11326893_10) and IL6R rs2228145 (assay ID: C___16170664_10) were genotyped using the TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assays. The polymerase chain reaction was carried out using GeneAce Probe qPCR Mixα (Nippon Gene, Japan) under the following conditions: 1 cycle at 95°C for 10 min followed by 45 cycles at 95°C for 15 s and 60°C for 1 min. The allele-specific fluorescence was measured with an ABI PRISM 7900 Sequence Detection System (Applied Biosystems, United States).

The numbers of participants with rs1800796 C/C, C/G, and G/G genotypes were 41 (56.2%), 24 (32.9%), and 8 (11.0%), respectively; and those with rs2228145 A/A, A/C, and C/C genotypes were 26 (35.6%), 37 (50.7%), and 10 (13.7%), respectively. The genotype frequency for these two SNPs did not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium: χ2(1) = 2.20, p = 0.14, and χ2(1) = 0.31, p = 0.58, respectively. Due to the low frequency of minor alleles, participants were assigned to the following groups: “C/C” or “C/G + G/G” for rs1800796 and “C/C” or “A/A + A/C” for rs2228145.

2.9. Data analyses

2.9.1. Relationships between IL-6 levels, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depressive/anxiety symptoms

After preprocessing fMRI data, we performed a 1st-level analysis using a general linear model to create a linear contrast map between the two conditions (i.e., fearful > neutral) for each participant. Then, using these contrast maps, we performed a 2nd-level analysis to explore regions exhibiting significant differences concerning measured IL-6 levels throughout the brain. For the amygdala, small volume correction (SVC) was applied to its main subnuclei, the basolateral nucleus (BLA) and centromedial nucleus (CEM), using the Jüelich histological atlas (Figure 2).4 The statistical threshold was set at p < 0.0125 (at peak level) for the amygdala (based on the number of bilateral subnuclei). For other brain regions, family-wise error (FWE)-corrected p < 0.05 (at peak level) was applied.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Anatomical definition of two major subnuclei of the amygdala complex: centro-median nucleus (CEM) and latero-basal nucleus (BLA). Dark blue: Right CEM. Red: Right BLA. Light Blue: Left CEM. Yellow: Left BLA. Center MNI coordinate and volume: (Right CEM) (23, −5, −14); 222 mm3; (Right BLA) (26, −1, −24), 1,699 mm3; (Left CEM) (−21, −6, −14), 345 mm3; (Left BLA) (−23, −3, −25), 1938 mm3. The definitions are based on Jüelich histological atlas (cyto-and myelo-architectonic segmentations: https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases/Juelich).

We further performed a correlation analysis to examine the relationship between amygdala activity, IL-6 indices, and depressive/anxiety symptoms, using the β values of the amygdala subnuclei that were extracted based on the anatomical mask of Jüelich atlas with MarsBaR version 0.43 (i.e., as the whole subnuclei values).5 Additionally, despite our primary interest in the amygdala, results of the hippocampus were also reported due to its adjacency to the amygdala. Statistical threshold was set at p < 0.0045 with the Bonferroni correction.

2.9.2. Effects of gene-stressor interactions on IL-6 secretion

Next, a three-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted for the observed IL-6 index with SNPs (rs1800796 and rs2228145) and the NLC grouping as between-subject factors to determine whether gene-stressor interactions predict a disrupted IL-6 secretion pattern. If significant interaction effects were found, post hoc analysis was performed for each level of a corresponding variable.

For the LES treated as continuous variable, analytic procedures are described in Supplementary material S2.4.

Additionally, this study was the first to examine the interaction between IL-6-related SNPs and psychosocial stressors on IL-6 diurnal index, and therefore a priori sample size estimation could not be performed. However, for a confirmatory purpose, we performed a post hoc power analysis using G*Power 3 (88), based on the effect that was observed to be significant in this study.

2.9.3. Path analysis on the relationship between IL-6 secretion, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depressive/anxiety symptoms under gene-stressor interactions

Focusing on the variables found to be significant in the above-mentioned analyses, we performed path analysis to comprehensively evaluate the pathways from amygdala emotional reactivity to IL-6 secretion and depressive/anxiety symptoms under gene-stressor interactions. A gene-stressor interaction affecting IL-6 levels was calculated by multiplying the NLC group (i.e., NLC+, 1; or NLC−, 0) and SNP type (e.g., for rs1800796, C/C type, 1; or C/G + G/G type, 0), and included in the model for DNA subsample. Standardized β values were calculated as each path coefficient. The results of path analysis in the whole sample (i.e., excluding the gene-stressor interaction) are also reported.

2.9.4. IL-6-related brain functional connectivity

To explore any FC associated with the IL-6 index throughout the brain, we performed a correlation analysis using the CONN default atlas (i.e., comprised of FSL Harvard-Oxford atlas6 and Automated Anatomical Labeling atlas7), covering the whole brain. For the amygdala only, we employed the Jüelich histological atlas, instead of the CONN atlas, to define its subnuclei (i.e., BLA and CEM on both sides). Thus, the total number of regions was 134, including the amygdala subnuclei, hippocampus, and PFC. The time series of BOLD signals were averaged across all voxels within each region, and FC values between the regions were calculated bivariately. False-discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied for multiple tests.

For all analyses, the effects of age, sex, BMI, and sleep duration were controlled since they were identified as IL-6 index confounders (see Supplementary Tables S1,S2; Supplementary material S2.1). Additionally, effects of the time interval between MRI scans and saliva collection and handedness, defined by the Edinburgh inventory (89), were controlled for in fMRI-related analysis. All analyses, except the whole-brain activity and FC analyses, were performed with SPSS version 27.0 J.8 The statistical threshold was set at p < 0.05 (2-sided) unless otherwise stated.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Of the 123 enrolled participants, two did not provide saliva samples and one had an MRI data writing error. For the IL-6 assay, the data whose absorbance values could not be measured with an absorptiometer were excluded from the analysis. Consequently, 1,119 (93.7%) of the total 1,200 IL-6 measurements (i.e., the remaining 120 individuals with 10 measurements each) were included. When IL-6 data was available for only one of the 2 days (i.e., data missing from any time point for either day), the two-day average data was substituted by this single-day data. Of the 120 participants, 12 were excluded from the entire analysis because their IL-6 data were not available for one or more corresponding time points for both days. Thus, we analyzed the data of 108 participants as a final data set (Table 1). Of these participants, two women regularly took oral contraceptives and none were engaged in shift work. Although no participant met the criteria for a current MDD episode, 12 of them had significant depression levels defined by the BDI-II cut-off score, indicating that they were at a high-risk state for MDD. These individuals had moderate levels of depression (BDI-II: Mean ± SD = 22.6 ± 8.5), and were not significantly different from non-depressive individuals in terms of age and sex (age: t = 0.34, p = 0.74, Mean ± SD = 26.8 ± 11.9 and 27.9 ± 11.4, respectively and sex: χ2 = 0.68, p = 0.54, 66.7% and 54.2%, respectively, for female ratios).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participants’ characteristics and psychological and biological data.

Saliva collection time at T1, T2, T3, T4, and T5 was as follows: 7:09 (± 1:16), 7:46 (± 1:17), 12:06 (± 0:20), 18:03 (± 0:19), and 0:20 (± 1:09), respectively. Average and standard errors of raw IL-6 values at each time point are shown in Table 1 and Figure 3. The observed IL-6 diurnal pattern was similar to that reported in our previous study (46), confirming that the salivary IL-6 levels were properly measured in this study. Since IL-6 diurnal index and total output did not follow a normal distribution, they were square-root transformed (W = 0.65, p < 0.001; W = 0.71, p < 0.001, respectively).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. IL-6 levels measured at each time point. Raw values (winsorized) are illustrated. Saliva collection time at T1, T2, T3, T4, and T5 was as follows: 7:09 (± 1:16), 7:46 (± 1:17), 12:06 (± 0:20), 18:03 (± 0:19), and 0:20 (± 1:09), respectively. For the numeric values, see Table 1. Bars indicate the standard error of the means. IL-6, interleukin-6.

Likewise, the balanced impact score of LES did not follow a normal distribution (W = 0.95, p < 0.001), and thus participants were classified into those who experienced NLC and those who did not.

For fMRI data, 11 participants were unable to respond by the time the next screen was presented in more than 30% of the total trials since the timing of stimulus presentation was synchronized with that of each MR signal. The overall hit ratio was 85.3 ± 11.4% in the 97 participants, indicating that they concentrated well on the task. In addition, the time of fMRI scans was 13:39 (± 1:22). The scanning time did not affect amygdala activity (see Supplementary material S2.3).

Furthermore, there was no significant difference between the whole sample (n = 108) and the DNA subsample (n = 73) in age, sex, IL-6 diurnal index, or total IL-6 output (t = 1.10, df = 179, p = 0.27; X2 (1, 181) = 4.2, p = 0.55; t = 0.58, df = 179, p = 0.57; t = −0.36, df = 179, p = 0.72; respectively).

3.2. Relationships between IL-6 levels, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depressive/anxiety symptoms

Whole-brain analysis revealed that IL-6 diurnal index had a significantly positive association with left BLA activity to fearful (vs. neutral) faces (MNI: −30 −7 −22, t = 3.67, FWE-corrected p = 0.003, 54 mm3; Figure 4), such that the more the IL-6 diurnal index diminished, the more the BLA activity decreased. No other significant cluster was found in the right BLA, CEM, and other brain regions. For IL-6 total output, no significant correlation was found with activity in any brain region.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. BLA activity in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces is significantly correlated with the IL-6 diurnal index. Effects of age, sex, BMI, sleep duration, handedness, and time interval from MRI scans to saliva collection were controlled for. (A) Significant peak cluster correlated with the IL-6 diurnal index. (B) Scatter plot on the relationship between the IL-6 diurnal index and left BLA activity. BLA, basolateral amygdala; IL-6, interleukin-6; BMI, body mass index.

Additional correlation analysis showed that the IL-6 diurnal index was associated with BDI-II depressive symptoms only, with increased blunting of the IL-6 diurnal pattern corresponding with greater depressive symptoms, although this correlation failed to reach the Bonferroni-corrected statistical threshold (r = −0.24, p = 0.018). The IL-6 diurnal index did not correlate with HSCL anxiety and depressive symptoms (Table 2). Despite the distinct correlation with the IL-6 diurnal index (r = 0.30, p = 0.002) as observed in the whole-brain analysis, the left BLA activity was uncorrelated with BDI-II depressive symptoms (r = 0.04, p = 0.67), although it had a relative correlation with HSCL anxiety symptoms (r = 0.20, p = 0.04).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between IL-6 indices, amygdala subnuclei activity to fearful (vs. neutral) faces, and stress-related mental symptoms.

Although the linear relationship between BDI-II depression and IL-6 diurnal index slightly failed to survive the Bonferroni correction, we also compared depressive individuals defined by the BDI-II cut-off score (n = 12) and non-depressive ones (n = 96) with ANCOVA controlling for age, sex, sleep duration, and BMI, in consideration of a possible non-linearity of biomarkers as suggested in the allostatic load model (1). As a result, we found a significant difference between the two groups (F[1,102] = 14.8, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.13). The depressive group had a significantly blunted IL-6 diurnal pattern (mean ± SD = −0.08 ± 2.98) as compared to the non-depressive group (mean ± SD = 2.13 ± 2.00; Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Significant group difference between depressive and non-depressive individuals in IL-6 diurnal secretion. Effects of age, sex, BMI, and sleep duration were controlled for. The depressive group had a significantly blunted IL-6 diurnal pattern (mean ± SD = −0.08 ± 2.98) as compared to the non-depressive group (mean ± SD = 2.13 ± 2.00) [F (1,102) = 14.8, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.13]. IL-6, interleukin-6; BMI, body mass index.

3.3. Effects of gene-stressor interactions on IL-6 secretion

Three-way ANCOVA demonstrated a significant rs1800796 × NLC interaction effect [F(1,61) = 15.1, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.20], with a significant main effect detected for NLC [F(1,61) = 4.26, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.07]. No other significant effects were observed: F (1,61) = 0.68, p = 0.41 for IL6; F(1,61) = 0.44, p = 0.51 for IL6R; F(1,61) = 0.00, p = 0.99 for NLC × IL6R; F(1,61) = 0.05, p = 0.82 for IL6 × IL6R; F(1,61) = 0.69, p = 0.41 for NLC × IL6 × IL6R.

When the significant rs1800796 × NLC effect was parsed into a simple main effect, a significant difference was found in the C/C group [n = 41, F(1,35) = 19.71, p < 0.001], but not in the G/C + G/G group [n = 32, F(1,26) = 1.22, p = 0.28]. Specifically, in the C/C group, the IL-6 diurnal index was significantly diminished under the presence of NLC (n = 16, mean ± SD = −0.32 ± 3.22, −0.80 ± 1.11 in Z score) but not under its absence (n = 25, mean ± SD = 2.80 ± 1.56, 0.22 ± 0.81 in Z score; Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Significant gene-stressor interaction impacts IL-6 diurnal secretion. The dotted line indicates the mean values of the IL-6 diurnal index (square-rooted): 1.69 ± 2.42. Error bars indicate the standard error of the means. Effects of age, sex, BMI, and sleep duration were controlled for. Significant difference found in the C/C group [n = 41, F(1,35) = 19.71, p < 0.001], but not in the G/C + G/G group [n = 32, F(1,26) = 1.22, p = 0.28]. In the C/C group, the IL-6 diurnal index significantly diminished under the presence of NLC (n = 16, mean ± SD = −0.32 ± 3.22, -0.80 ± 1.11 in Z score) but not under its absence (n = 25, mean ± SD = 2.80 ± 1.56, 0.22 ± 0.81 in Z score). IL-6, interleukin-6; BMI, body mass index; NLC, negative life change.

Additionally, our sample size for DNA analysis was observed to have a power of 0.99 to detect the observed effect size for the IL6 × NLC interaction (i.e., partial η2 = 0.20) at an α = 0.05.

Further, a multiple regression analysis where the LES balanced impact score was included as a continuous variable similarly found the rs1800796 × LES interaction effect (β = 0.82, t = 2.66, p = 0.010; see Supplementary material S2.4), confirming the IL6 SNP-psychosocial stressor interaction effect on the IL-6 diurnal index.

3.4. Path analysis on the relationship between IL-6 diurnal index, BLA activity, and depressive symptoms under gene-stressor interaction

Since the correlation of IL-6 diurnal index with BDI-II depression was still relatively significant, we proceeded to an exploratory path analysis on possible pathways from BLA activity to the IL-6 diurnal index and BDI-II depression under the rs1800796 × NLC interaction.

Based on the significant rs1800796 × NLC effect on IL-6 diurnal index and the relative correlation, Path analysis demonstrated that the diminished IL-6 diurnal index specifically explained greater depressive symptoms (β = −0.40, p < 0.001; respectively), being affected by the left BLA activity and the rs1800796 × NLC interaction (β = 0.36, p < 0.001; β = −0.41, p < 0.001; respectively; Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Comprehensive relationship between IL-6 diurnal index, BLA activity, and depression under gene-stressor interaction. Effects of age, sex, BMI, sleep duration, handedness, and time interval from MRI scans to saliva collection were controlled for. The values of left BLA activity were extracted based on the anatomical definition of Jüelich histological atlas. Depressive symptoms were assessed with BDI-II. (A) Results of path analysis (n = 73). Standardized β values are shown as each path coefficient. (B) Scatter plot on the relationship between the IL-6 diurnal index and depressive symptoms. Dots shown in orange color indicate individuals with significant levels of depression (i.e., scores ≥14 on BDI-II; n = 11 in DNA sample). BLA, basolateral amygdala; IL-6, interleukin-6; BMI, body mass index; IL6, rs1800796 single-nucleotide polymorphism; NLC, negative life change; BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory-II.

Additionally, the path coefficients in the DNA subsample were comparable to those in the whole sample where the gene-stressor interaction was not included (BLA–IL-6: β = 0.29, p = 0.002; IL-6–depression: β = −0.28, p = 0.004; BLA–depression: β = 0.16, p = 0.10), supporting the observed relationships.

3.5. IL-6-related brain functional connectivity

Whole-brain FC analysis revealed that the IL-6 diurnal index was significantly associated with increased FC between the CEM and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) on the right side (t = 3.92, FDR-corrected p = 0.012), as well as other cortical regions (Figure 8). All the significant FC values are reported in Table 3.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Exploratory whole-brain ROI-based FC analysis: correlations with the IL-6 diurnal index. Controlled for age, sex, BMI, sleep duration, handedness, and the time interval between MRI scans and saliva collection. The color bar indicates t values. Red and blue lines indicate increased and decreased FC, respectively. The width of lines is proportionate to statistical significance values. ROI, region of interest; FC, functional connectivity; IL-6, interleukin-6; BMI, body mass index; postCG, postcentral gyrus; sLOC, superior lateral occipital cortex; aPaHC, anterior parahippocampal gyrus; CO, central operculum cortex; PO, parietal operculum cortex; PT, planum temporale; SCC, supracalcarine cortex; HC, hippocampus; CEM, centromedial amygdala; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; pSTG, posterior division of superior temporal gyrus; FP, frontal pole.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The results of whole-brain FC analysis: correlations with IL-6 diurnal index.

To summarize, the diminished IL-6 diurnal pattern was associated with the hypoactivation of the left BLA to fearful (vs. neutral) faces, and was predominantly observed in individuals with the C-allele homozygotes of IL6 and NLC in the past year. When considered comprehensively, the blunted diurnal pattern predicted greater depressive symptoms, modulated by the amygdala hypoactivity and rs1800796-NLC interactions.

4. Discussion

The current study investigated the relationship between IL-6 levels, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depression/anxiety, considering gene-psychosocial stressor interactions in nonclinical participants with various levels of depression/anxiety. We specifically hypothesized that disrupted IL-6 secretion would be associated with perturbed amygdala emotional reactivity and greater depressive/anxiety symptoms; and that it would be predicted by the interactions between psychosocial stressors and IL6/IL6R.

In support of our hypotheses, we demonstrated that a diminished IL-6 diurnal index was associated with decreased amygdala activity, especially in the BLA, in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces, as well as greater depressive symptoms, through a comprehensive examination. The interaction between NLC and the IL6 SNP explained the diminished IL-6 diurnal pattern. The current study provides evidence on the relationship between IL-6 secretion, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depression, indicating that disrupted IL-6 secretion could affect greater depressive symptoms, being modulated by amygdala hyporeactivity and gene-psychosocial stressor interactions.

Specifically, the IL-6 diurnal pattern, but not the total IL-6 output, was associated with BLA response to emotional stimuli. The lack of association between total IL-6 output and amygdala activity is partially consistent with several previous findings (5659). First, this could potentially be because the IL-6 diurnal index and amygdala emotional responses capture a dynamic facet of reactivity rather than a static component. Moreover, while the IL-6 diurnal index was associated with depressive symptoms, the total IL-6 output did not correlate with anxiety or depressive symptoms. This was possibly because the participants involved did not have psychiatric diagnoses, suggesting the diurnal pattern of IL-6, rather than the total output, to be a more sensitive measure to the link between the immune system and mental health problems in the general population.

Importantly, the blunting of the IL-6 diurnal pattern was associated with both decreased amygdala responses to fearful faces and greater depressive symptoms. These findings are partly consistent with previous meta-analyses on the weakened emotional responses of the dorsal and lateral amygdala in PTSD (25) and MDD (23), as well as with previous findings on a flattened IL-6 diurnal pattern in individuals who underwent childhood trauma or combat-zone deployment (54, 55), which confer a risk of developing stress-related mental disorders (90, 91). The hypoactivation or lack of activation of the amygdala toward emotional stimuli has been implicated in both emotional and autonomic blunting (25, 92). The blunting of IL-6 diurnal secretion might be associated with the declined regulatory function of stress-related cytokines’ expression (18, 19) and weakened emotional reactivity (25, 92) in the amygdala, which potentially increases vulnerability to depression characterized by blunted affect such as anhedonia and learned helplessness (93). These findings may lead to the development of biomarkers for the early detection of depressive disorders, although the causal relationships between the variables are subject to scrutiny.

The amygdala regulates stress-related inflammatory cytokine expression via the norepinephrine system (18, 94). The BLA rapidly processes a potential threat, while the CEM elicits emotional responses, including norepinephrine secretion. More specifically, the CEM, where alpha adrenoreceptors are richly expressed (95, 96), receives catecholaminergic innervation from the locus coeruleus (97). Despite tight connections between BLA, CEM, and adjacent hippocampus (98), the activity of the BLA survived the correction for multiple testing (Table 2). This might have been because the BLA is reported to be susceptible to the effects of stress, such as dendritic remodeling, as mentioned earlier (6). In addition, this is also potentially due to the amygdala response elicited by the current emotional task being more relevant to the left BLA (84). Indeed, our previous study observed distinct activity in the amygdala, including the BLA on the left side (69), supporting the notion that this task is particularly sensitive to the left BLA. Although a consensus has not been reached on functional lateralization of the amygdala, some evidence suggests that the left side might be more strongly involved in emotion than the right (99). Moreover, although the Jüelich histological atlas is a reliable source in amygdala dissection, given the task-dependent activity and limit of fMRI spatial resolution, the CEM and hippocampus may also impact the IL-6 diurnal pattern, warranting further investigation.

Indeed, FC analysis demonstrated a significant positive association between the IL-6 diurnal pattern and FC between CEM and MFG, largely corresponding to the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces. Specifically, the more the IL-6 diurnal pattern diminished, the more this FC weakened. For the DLPFC, a previous positron emission tomography study reported a positive correlation between immune marker levels, including natural killer and helper T cell levels, and regional cerebral blood flow during a mental arithmetic task with low controllability (100). Moreover, an fMRI study revealed that IL-6 levels were negatively correlated with MFG activity, which decreased depending on an increase in cognitive load during the N-back task (101). Therefore, CEM-DLPFC connectivity might affect the secretion of immune/inflammatory markers according to cognitive load and stress controllability. Given the fMRI temporal resolution, the causal relationship between BLA activity and CEM-related FC remains unclear. Nevertheless, CEM connectivity might impact IL-6 diurnal rhythm modulation, given norepinephrine’s immune regulatory role (19). However, this is only a speculation that requires longitudinal investigation.

Furthermore, we found that the IL6 SNP-psychosocial stressor interaction predicted a diminished IL-6 diurnal pattern. Notably, individuals with the C/C genotype exhibited distinctively weakened (approaching −1 SD) patterns when experiencing NLC in the past year, whereas those with C/G and G/G genotypes did not. These findings suggest that diurnal IL-6 secretion may be influenced by gene-stressor interactions, supporting previous research (37, 38). Although future research needs to elucidate the exact mechanism, the blunting of IL-6 diurnal secretion might be possibly due to excessive stress (54), as the blunting of glucocorticoid secretion has been similarly observed after extreme stress (102). Given that the C substitution of rs1800796 has been reported to be associated with lower IL-6 levels (40, 43), individuals with the C/C genotype might be vulnerable to the disrupted regulation of IL-6 when exposed to stressors as compared to G-allele carriers. The effect of IL6-stressor interactions on the amygdala was not shown in this study, but the expression of IL-6 might be dampened in the amygdala, which might partially contribute to the blunted affect of depression (94). The diathesis-stress model for the development of MDD (103) partly accords with this possibility, and the disruption of the IL-6 diurnal rhythm, influenced by the individual’s genetic predispositions and the intensity of environmental stress, might be a key predictor for depression. However, this speculation needs to be thoroughly investigated in future research.

4.1. Limitations and future directions

Several limitations should be considered when interpreting these results. First, our sample for the SNP-related analysis was not very large, although the sample size was sufficiently powered to detect the observed significant interaction effect according to post hoc power analysis. Still, future research with larger sample sizes is necessary. Second, information on oral hygiene of the participants was unavailable in this study. However, even if the IL-6 levels at each time point were affected by inflammation due to oral hygiene, it would not significantly undermine the quality of IL-6 diurnal index data as it was calculated based on differences between two-time points, conceivably eliminating the systematic error due to such inflammation. In future research, information on oral hygiene should be obtained. Third, this was a cross-sectional study and did not reveal potential causal relationships between IL-6 levels, amygdala emotional reactivity, and depressive symptoms. Future research should elucidate these causal relationships by simultaneously performing fMRI and other imaging techniques with a higher temporal resolution, such as electroencephalography. Furthermore, the development of indices to assess individuals’ regulatory functions of the immune system beyond acute inflammation (i.e., not only phasic but also tonic changes of the immune system) might enable the early detection of high-risk individuals whose psychopathology is yet to be manifested and the effective prevention for them by ameliorating their immune system dysregulation. To capture such baseline regulatory functions, consecutive measurements of unstimulated biological markers on a daily basis, such as IL-6 diurnal patterns measured in this study and spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations measured by resting-state fMRI, might bring a new insight to psychiatry research.

4.2. Conclusion

We showed that blunting of the IL-6 diurnal pattern was associated with both amygdala (especially BLA) hypoactivity in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces and greater depressive symptoms in a sizeable community sample with various levels of depression. Disrupted IL-6 diurnal rhythm was explained by the interaction between IL6 polymorphism and stressful life events. These findings represent the first evidence that diminished IL-6 diurnal rhythm could affect depression, modulated by amygdala emotional hyporeactivity and the interaction between the individual’s genetic predisposition and the intensity of environmental stress. Developing an understanding of this blunted diurnal IL-6 rhythm, a possible precipitant in stress-related mental disorders such as MDD, is expected to facilitate their early detection, prevention, and treatment such as pharmacotherapy to ameliorate the disrupted secretion of inflammatory markers.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Kitasato University Medical Ethics Organization and the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YH designed the study, wrote the protocol, conducted the psychological assessment and statistical analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. HH conducted DNA-related statistical analyses and revised the first draft of the manuscript. SM conducted the MRI scans. SI conducted the IL-6 assay. FY performed the IL-6 assay and SNP genotyping. YM and TH supervised the fMRI analyses. YI and HT provided support during the recruitment of participants and assessment. All the authors made critical revisions to the intellectual content of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The authors received a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) (Nos. 18H01094 and 22H01092 to YH) and a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) (No. 20 K07937 to HH) from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science, research grants from the Uehara Memorial Foundation (to YH), the Suzuken Memorial Foundation, and Terumo Life Science Foundation (to HH). Funding sources did not influence any aspect of the work, including design, data collection, analyses, interpretation, writing, or submission, detailed in the current manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all study participants; Noriko Fukuzato, Aki Shingu, and Namiko Ogawa for measuring IL-6 levels; and Yasuko Omura, Masato Suzuki, Takahiro Mizoguchi, Kanta Okuzumi, Ayaka Ichikawa, Miyu Onoue, Yuri Nohara, Momoka Haginoya, and Sakura Wada for data entry and research assistance.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1196235/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

1. McEwen, BS, Bowles, NP, Gray, JD, Hill, MN, Hunter, RG, Karatsoreos, IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:1353–63. doi: 10.1038/nn.4086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ankarcrona, M, Dypbukt, JM, Bonfoco, E, Zhivotovsky, B, Orrenius, S, Lipton, SA, et al. Glutamate-induced neuronal death: a succession of necrosis or apoptosis depending on mitochondrial function. Neuron. (1995) 15:961–73. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90186-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Treccani, G, Musazzi, L, Perego, C, Milanese, M, Nava, N, Bonifacino, T, et al. Stress and corticosterone increase the readily releasable pool of glutamate vesicles in synaptic terminals of prefrontal and frontal cortex. Mol Psychiatry. (2014) 19:433–43. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Cameron, HA, McEwen, BS, and Gould, E. Regulation of adult neurogenesis by excitatory input and NMDA receptor activation in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. (1995) 15:4687–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-06-04687.1995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Magariños, AM, Verdugo, JM, and McEwen, BS. Chronic stress alters synaptic terminal structure in hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1997) 94:14002–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.25.14002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Laine, MA, and Shansky, RM. Rodent models of stress and dendritic plasticity–implications for psychopathology. Neurobiol Stress. (2022) 17:100438. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2022.100438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Du, J, Wang, Y, Hunter, R, Wei, Y, Blumenthal, R, Falke, C, et al. Dynamic regulation of mitochondrial function by glucocorticoids. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2009) 106:3543–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812671106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Tanaka, M, Szabó, Á, Spekker, E, Polyák, H, Tóth, F, and Vécsei, L. Mitochondrial impairment: a common motif in neuropsychiatric presentation? The link to the tryptophan-Kynurenine metabolic system. Cells. (2022) 11:2607. doi: 10.3390/cells11162607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Battaglia, S, Di Fazio, C, Vicario, CM, and Avenanti, A. Neuropharmacological modulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate, noradrenaline and Endocannabinoid receptors in fear extinction learning: synaptic transmission and plasticity. Int J Mol Sci. (2023) 24:5926. doi: 10.3390/ijms24065926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. McEwen, BS. Glucocorticoids, depression, and mood disorders: structural remodeling in the brain. Metabolism. (2005) 54:20–3. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2005.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dantzer, R, O'Connor, JC, Freund, GG, Johnson, RW, and Kelley, KW. From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:46–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Huang, X, Hussain, B, and Chang, J. Peripheral inflammation and blood-brain barrier disruption: effects and mechanisms. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2021) 27:36–47. doi: 10.1111/cns.13569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hashioka, S, Wu, Z, and Klegeris, A. Glia-driven neuroinflammation and systemic inflammation in Alzheimer's disease. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2021) 19:908–24. doi: 10.2174/1570159X18666201111104509

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Saijo, K, and Glass, CK. Microglial cell origin and phenotypes in health and disease. Nat Rev Immunol. (2011) 11:775–87. doi: 10.1038/nri3086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Calcia, MA, Bonsall, DR, Bloomfield, PS, Selvaraj, S, Barichello, T, and Howes, OD. Stress and neuroinflammation: a systematic review of the effects of stress on microglia and the implications for mental illness. Psychopharmacology. (2016) 233:1637–50. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4218-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bender, CL, Calfa, GD, and Molina, VA. Astrocyte plasticity induced by emotional stress: a new partner in psychiatric physiopathology? Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 65:68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ledoux, J. Emotional networks in the brain In: M Lewis and J Haviland, editors. Handbook of emotions. New York: The Guilford Press (1993). 109–18.

Google Scholar

18. Johnson, JD, Campisi, J, Sharkey, CM, Kennedy, SL, Nickerson, M, Greenwood, BN, et al. Catecholamines mediate stress-induced increases in peripheral and central inflammatory cytokines. Neuroscience. (2005) 135:1295–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Finnell, JE, Moffitt, CM, Hesser, LA, Harrington, E, Melson, MN, Wood, CS, et al. The contribution of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in the emergence of defeat-induced inflammatory priming. Brain Behav Immun. (2019) 79:102–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2019.01.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gray, TS, and Bingaman, EW. The amygdala: corticotropin-releasing factor, steroids, and stress. Crit Rev Neurobiol. (1996) 10:155–68. doi: 10.1615/critrevneurobiol.v10.i2.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hamilton, JP, Etkin, A, Furman, DJ, Lemus, MG, Johnson, RF, and Gotlib, IH. Functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis and new integration of base line activation and neural response data. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:693–703. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11071105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Fitzgerald, PB, Laird, AR, Maller, J, and Daskalakis, ZJ. A meta-analytic study of changes in brain activation in depression. Hum Brain Mapp. (2008) 29:683–95. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Palmer, SM, Crewther, SG, Carey, LM, and Team, SP. A meta-analysis of changes in brain activity in clinical depression. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:1045. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lee, MS, Anumagalla, P, and Pavuluri, MN. Individuals with the post-traumatic stress disorder process emotions in subcortical regions irrespective of cognitive engagement: a meta-analysis of cognitive and emotional interface. Brain Imaging Behav. (2021) 15:941–57. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00303-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Etkin, A, and Wager, T. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:1476–88. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Patel, R, Spreng, RN, Shin, LM, and Girard, TA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and beyond: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:2130–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Suarez-Jimenez, B, Albajes-Eizagirre, A, Lazarov, A, Zhu, X, Harrison, BJ, Radua, J, et al. Neural signatures of conditioning, extinction learning, and extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:1442–51. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Thome, J, Terpou, BA, McKinnon, MC, and Lanius, RA. The neural correlates of trauma-related autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Depress Anxiety. (2020) 37:321–45. doi: 10.1002/da.22977

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Müller, VI, Cieslik, EC, Serbanescu, I, Laird, AR, Fox, PT, and Eickhoff, SB. Altered brain activity in unipolar depression revisited: Meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. JAMA Psychiat. (2017) 74:47–55. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Price, RB, and Duman, R. Neuroplasticity in cognitive and psychological mechanisms of depression: an integrative model. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:530–43. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0615-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chen, C, Takahashi, T, Nakagawa, S, Inoue, T, and Kusumi, I. Reinforcement learning in depression: a review of computational research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:247–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Haapakoski, R, Mathieu, J, Ebmeier, KP, Alenius, H, and Kivimäki, M. Cumulative meta-analysis of interleukins 6 and 1β, tumour necrosis factor α and C-reactive protein in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain Behav Immun. (2015) 49:206–15. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Passos, IC, Vasconcelos-Moreno, MP, Costa, LG, Kunz, M, Brietzke, E, Quevedo, J, et al. Inflammatory markers in post-traumatic stress disorder: a systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression. Lancet Psychiatry. (2015) 2:1002–12. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00309-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yang, JJ, and Jiang, W. Immune biomarkers alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2020) 268:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.02.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fisman, EZ, and Tenenbaum, A. The ubiquitous interleukin-6: a time for reappraisal. Cardiovasc Diabetol. (2010) 9:62. doi: 10.1186/1475-2840-9-62

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Tanaka, T, Narazaki, M, and Kishimoto, T. IL-6 in inflammation, immunity, and disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. (2014) 6:a016295. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a016295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cole, SW, Arevalo, JM, Takahashi, R, Sloan, EK, Lutgendorf, SK, Sood, AK, et al. Computational identification of gene-social environment interaction at the human IL6 locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2010) 107:5681–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911515107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tartter, M, Hammen, C, Bower, JE, Brennan, PA, and Cole, S. Effects of chronic interpersonal stress exposure on depressive symptoms are moderated by genetic variation at IL6 and IL1β in youth. Brain Behav Immun. (2015) 46:104–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Stephens, OW, Zhang, Q, Qu, P, Zhou, Y, Chavan, S, Tian, E, et al. An intermediate-risk multiple myeloma subgroup is defined by sIL-6r: levels synergistically increase with incidence of SNP rs 2228145 and 1q21 amplification. Blood. (2012) 119:503–12. doi: 10.1182/blood-2011-07-367052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Fernandes, MT, Fernandes, KB, Marquez, AS, Cólus, IM, Souza, MF, Santos, JP, et al. Association of interleukin-6 gene polymorphism (rs 1800796) with severity and functional status of osteoarthritis in elderly individuals. Cytokine. (2015) 75:316–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2015.07.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Terry, CF, Loukaci, V, and Green, FR. Cooperative influence of genetic polymorphisms on interleukin 6 transcriptional regulation. J Biol Chem. (2000) 275:18138–44. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M000379200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Rothaug, M, Becker-Pauly, C, and Rose-John, S. The role of interleukin-6 signaling in nervous tissue. Biochim Biophys Acta. (2016):1863. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamcr.2016.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wei, Q, Chen, X, and Chen, H. Association of Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms of the IL-6, IL-10, and TNF-α genes with susceptibility to gestational diabetes mellitus. Genet Test Mol Biomarkers. (2020) 24:390–8. doi: 10.1089/gtmb.2020.0069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Van Dongen, J, Jansen, R, Smit, D, Hottenga, JJ, Mbarek, H, Willemsen, G, et al. The contribution of the functional IL6R polymorphism rs 2228145, eQTLs and other genome-wide SNPs to the heritability of plasma sIL-6R levels. Behav Genet. (2014) 44:368–82. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9656-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Vgontzas, AN, Bixler, EO, Lin, HM, Prolo, P, Trakada, G, and Chrousos, GP. IL-6 and its circadian secretion in humans. Neuroimmunomodulation. (2005) 12:131–40. doi: 10.1159/000084844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Izawa, S, Miki, K, Liu, X, and Ogawa, N. The diurnal patterns of salivary interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein in healthy young adults. Brain Behav Immun. (2013) 27:38–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nilsonne, G, Lekander, M, Åkerstedt, T, Axelsson, J, and Ingre, M. Diurnal variation of circulating Interleukin-6 in humans: a Meta-analysis. PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0165799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Agorastos, A, Hauger, RL, Barkauskas, DA, Moeller-Bertram, T, Clopton, PL, Haji, U, et al. Circadian rhythmicity, variability and correlation of interleukin-6 levels in plasma and cerebrospinal fluid of healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 44:71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. La Fratta, I, Tatangelo, R, Campagna, G, Rizzuto, A, Franceschelli, S, Ferrone, A, et al. The plasmatic and salivary levels of IL-1β, IL-18 and IL-6 are associated to emotional difference during stress in young male. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:3031. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21474-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cullen, T, Thomas, AW, Webb, R, and Hughes, MG. The relationship between interleukin-6 in saliva, venous and capillary plasma, at rest and in response to exercise. Cytokine. (2015) 71:397–400. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2014.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Fernandez-Botran, R, Miller, JJ, Burns, VE, and Newton, TL. Correlations among inflammatory markers in plasma, saliva and oral mucosal transudate in post-menopausal women with past intimate partner violence. Brain Behav Immun. (2011) 25:314–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.09.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Byrne, ML, O'Brien-Simpson, NM, Reynolds, EC, Walsh, KA, Laughton, K, Waloszek, JM, et al. Acute phase protein and cytokine levels in serum and saliva: a comparison of detectable levels and correlations in a depressed and healthy adolescent sample. Brain Behav Immun. (2013) 34:164–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Scott, KM, Smith, DR, and Ellis, PM. Prospectively ascertained child maltreatment and its association with DSM-IV mental disorders in young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2010) 67:712–9. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hori, H, Izawa, S, Yoshida, F, Kunugi, H, Kim, Y, Mizukami, S, et al. Association of childhood maltreatment history with salivary interleukin-6 diurnal patterns and C-reactive protein in healthy adults. Brain Behav Immun. (2022) 101:377–82. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2022.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Agorastos, A, Hauger, RL, Barkauskas, DA, Lerman, IR, Moeller-Bertram, T, Snijders, C, et al. Relations of combat stress and posttraumatic stress disorder to 24-h plasma and cerebrospinal fluid interleukin-6 levels and circadian rhythmicity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2019) 100:237–45. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Harrison, NA, Brydon, L, Walker, C, Gray, MA, Steptoe, A, and Critchley, HD. Inflammation causes mood changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:407–14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Leschak, CJ, Hornstein, EA, Byrne Haltom, KE, Johnson, KL, Breen, EC, Irwin, MR, et al. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity differentiates sick from healthy faces: associations with inflammatory responses and disease avoidance motivation. Brain Behav Immun. (2022) 100:48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2021.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Alvarez, GM, Hackman, DA, Miller, AB, and Muscatell, KA. Systemic inflammation is associated with differential neural reactivity and connectivity to affective images. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2020) 15:1024–33. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Muscatell, KA, Eisenberger, NI, Dutcher, JM, Cole, SW, and Bower, JE. Links between inflammation, amygdala reactivity, and social support in breast cancer survivors. Brain Behav Immun. (2016) 53:34–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Muscatell, KA, Dedovic, K, Slavich, GM, Jarcho, MR, Breen, EC, Bower, JE, et al. Greater amygdala activity and dorsomedial prefrontal-amygdala coupling are associated with enhanced inflammatory responses to stress. Brain Behav Immun. (2015) 43:46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.06.201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Miller, GE, White, SF, Chen, E, and Nusslock, R. Association of Inflammatory Activity with Larger Neural Responses to threat and reward among children living in poverty. Am J Psychiatry. (2021) 178:313–20. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20050635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Wang, L, Wang, R, Liu, L, Qiao, D, Baldwin, DS, and Hou, R. Effects of SSRIs on peripheral inflammatory markers in patients with major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav Immun. (2019) 79:24–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2019.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Nord, CL, Barrett, LF, Lindquist, KA, Ma, Y, Marwood, L, Satpute, AB, et al. Neural effects of antidepressant medication and psychological treatments: a quantitative synthesis across three meta-analyses. Br J Psychiatry. (2021) 219:546–50. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2021.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Helzer, J, Kraemer, H, Krueger, R, Wittchen, H, Sirovatka, P, and Regier, D. Dimensional approaches in diagnostic classification: Refining the research agenda for DSM-V. New York: American Psychiatric Pub Inc. (2008).

Google Scholar

65. Temple, JL, Bernard, C, Lipshultz, SE, Czachor, JD, Westphal, JA, and Mestre, MA. The safety of ingested caffeine: a comprehensive review. Front Psych. (2017) 8:80. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Hakamata, Y, Mizukami, S, Izawa, S, Moriguchi, Y, Hori, H, Kim, Y, et al. Basolateral amygdala connectivity with Subgenual anterior cingulate cortex represents enhanced fear-related memory encoding in anxious humans. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 5:301–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.11.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Hakamata, Y, Mizukami, S, Izawa, S, Moriguchi, Y, Hori, H, Matsumoto, N, et al. Childhood trauma affects autobiographical memory deficits through basal cortisol and prefrontal-extrastriate functional connectivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2021) 127:105172. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Hakamata, Y, Mizukami, S, Izawa, S, Okamura, H, Mihara, K, Marusak, H, et al. Implicit and explicit emotional memory recall in anxiety and depression: role of basolateral amygdala and cortisol-norepinephrine interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2022) 136:105598. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105598

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hakamata, Y, Sato, E, Komi, S, Moriguchi, Y, Izawa, S, Murayama, N, et al. The functional activity and effective connectivity of pulvinar are modulated by individual differences in threat-related attentional bias. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:34777. doi: 10.1038/srep34777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Ball, R, and Ranieri, W. Comparison of Beck depression inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. (1996) 67:588–97. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kojima, M, Furukawa, TA, Takahashi, H, Kawai, M, Nagaya, T, and Tokudome, S. Cross-cultural validation of the Beck depression inventory-II in Japan. Psychiatry Res. (2002) 110:291–9. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00106-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Derogatis, LR, Lipman, RS, Rickels, K, Uhlenhuth, EH, and Covi, L. The Hopkins symptom checklist (HSCL): a self-report symptom inventory. Behav Sci. (1974) 19:1–15.

Google Scholar

73. Nakano, K. Stress management. 2nd ed. Tokyo, Japan: Kongo Press (2016).

Google Scholar

74. Sarason, IG, Johnson, JH, and Siegel, JM. Assessing the impact of life changes: development of the life experiences survey. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1978) 46:932–46. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.46.5.932

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Iwamitsu, Y, Yasuda, H, Kamiya, M, Wada, M, Nakajima, K, Ando, N, et al. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the life experiences survey. Jpn J Stress Sci. (2008) 23:239–49.

Google Scholar

76. Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, and Lushene, RE. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1970).

Google Scholar

77. Zung, WW. A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1965) 12:63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Padilla, G, Calvi, J, Taylor, M, and Granger, D. Saliva collection, handling, transport, and storage: special considerations and best practices for interdisciplinary salivary bioscience research In: D Granger and M Taylor, editors. Salivary bioscience: Foundations of interdisciplinary saliva research and applications. New York, USA: Springer (2020). 21–47.

Google Scholar

79. Pruessner, JC, Kirschbaum, C, Meinlschmid, G, and Hellhammer, DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2003) 28:916–31. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Straub, RH, and Cutolo, M. Circadian rhythms in rheumatoid arthritis: implications for pathophysiology and therapeutic management. Arthritis Rheum. (2007) 56:399–408. doi: 10.1002/art.22368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Shirakawa, T, Mitome, M, and Oguchi, H. Circadian rhythms of S-IgA and cortisol in whole saliva–compensatory mechanism of oral immune system for nocturnal fall of saliva secretion. Pediatr Dent J. (2004) 14:115–20. doi: 10.1016/S0917-2394(04)70017-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Friston, K, Ashburner, J, Frith, C, Poline, J, Heather, J, and Frackowiak, R. Spatial registration and normalization of images. Hum Brain Mapp. (1995) 2:165–89. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Murphy, K, Birn, RM, Handwerker, DA, Jones, TB, and Bandettini, PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage. (2009) 44:893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Vuilleumier, P, Armony, JL, Driver, J, and Dolan, RJ. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. (2001) 30:829–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00328-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Tamietto, M, and de Gelder, B. Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2010) 11:697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn2889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Tottenham, N, Tanaka, JW, Leon, AC, McCarry, T, Nurse, M, Hare, TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. (2009) 168:242–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Cariaso, M, and Lennon, G. SNPedia: a wiki supporting personal genome annotation, interpretation and analysis. Nucleic Acids Res. (2012) 40:D1308–12. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkr798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, AG, and Buchner, A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Oldfield, RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Coenen, P, and Van der Molen, HF. What work-related exposures are associated with post-traumatic stress disorder? A systematic review with meta-analysis. BMJ Open. (2021) 11:e049651. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-049651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Scott, JC, Matt, GE, Wrocklage, KM, Crnich, C, Jordan, J, Southwick, SM, et al. A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull. (2015) 141:105–40. doi: 10.1037/a0038039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Phan, KL, Britton, JC, Taylor, SF, Fig, LM, and Liberzon, I. Corticolimbic blood flow during nontraumatic emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2006) 63:184–92. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Wang, S, Leri, F, and Rizvi, SJ. Anhedonia as a central factor in depression: neural mechanisms revealed from preclinical to clinical evidence. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 110:110289. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Gyoneva, S, and Traynelis, SF. Norepinephrine modulates the motility of resting and activated microglia via different adrenergic receptors. J Biol Chem. (2013) 288:15291–302. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M113.458901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Pascual, J, del Arco, C, González, AM, and Pazos, A. Quantitative light microscopic autoradiographic localization of alpha 2-adrenoceptors in the human brain. Brain Res. (1992) 585:116–27. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91196-l

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Pieribone, VA, Nicholas, AP, Dagerlind, A, and Hökfelt, T. Distribution of alpha 1 adrenoceptors in rat brain revealed by in situ hybridization experiments utilizing subtype-specific probes. J Neurosci. (1994) 14:4252–68. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-07-04252.1994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Asan, E. The catecholaminergic innervation of the rat amygdala. Adv Anat Embryol Cell Biol. (1998) 142:1–118. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-72085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Felix-Ortiz, AC, Beyeler, A, Seo, C, Leppla, CA, Wildes, CP, and Tye, KM. BLA to vHPC inputs modulate anxiety-related behaviors. Neuron. (2013) 79:658–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Sergerie, K, Chochol, C, and Armony, JL. The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:811–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Ohira, H, Isowa, T, Nomura, M, Ichikawa, N, Kimura, K, Miyakoshi, M, et al. Imaging brain and immune association accompanying cognitive appraisal of an acute stressor. NeuroImage. (2008) 39:500–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Dev, SI, Moore, RC, Soontornniyomkij, B, Achim, CL, Jeste, DV, and Eyler, LT. Peripheral inflammation related to lower fMRI activation during a working memory task and resting functional connectivity among older adults: a preliminary study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2017) 32:341–9. doi: 10.1002/gps.4482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Hakamata, Y, Suzuki, Y, Kobashikawa, H, and Hori, H. Neurobiology of early life adversity: a systematic review of meta-analyses towards an integrative account of its neurobiological trajectories to mental disorders. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2022) 65:100994. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.100994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Colodro-Conde, L, Couvy-Duchesne, B, Zhu, G, Coventry, WL, Byrne, EM, Gordon, S, et al. A direct test of the diathesis-stress model for depression. Mol Psychiatry. (2018) 23:1590–6. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interleukin-6, depression, basolateral nuclear complex, magnetic resonance imaging, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, single nucleotide polymorphism, circadian rhythm, immune system

Citation: Hakamata Y, Hori H, Mizukami S, Izawa S, Yoshida F, Moriguchi Y, Hanakawa T, Inoue Y and Tagaya H (2023) Blunted diurnal interleukin-6 rhythm is associated with amygdala emotional hyporeactivity and depression: a modulating role of gene-stressor interactions. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1196235. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1196235

Received: 29 March 2023; Accepted: 28 April 2023;
Published: 30 May 2023.

Edited by:

Masaru Tanaka, University of Szeged (ELKH-SZTE), Hungary

Reviewed by:

Chong Chen, Yamaguchi University Graduate School of Medicine, Japan
Simone Battaglia, University of Bologna, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Hakamata, Hori, Mizukami, Izawa, Yoshida, Moriguchi, Hanakawa, Inoue and Tagaya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuko Hakamata, aGFrYW1hdGFAbWVkLnUtdG95YW1hLmFjLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.