Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 March 2024
Sec. Personality Disorders

Measurement invariance of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 across sex

Genevive Rivard,Geneviève Rivard1,2Yann Le Corff,,*Yann Le Corff1,2,3*Mlanie Lapalme,Mélanie Lapalme1,4Karine Forget,Karine Forget1,3
  • 1The Group for Research and Intervention on Children’s Social Adjustment (GRISE), Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
  • 2Département d’orientation Professionnelle, Faculté d’éducation, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
  • 3Département de Psychiatrie, Faculté de Médecine, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
  • 4Département de Psychoéducation, Faculté d’éducation, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

Introduction: There has been an international movement towards dimensional models of personality disorders (PDs) in the last decades, which culminated in the publication of the Alternative Model of Personality Disorders (AMPD) in the Emerging Measures and Models section of the DSM-5. This model was accompanied by a APA-sanctioned Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5) for the assessment of the AMPD pathological personality traits. One major issue with the assessment of personality disorders pertains to sex differences, and measurement invariance across sex in assessment instruments for PDs is necessary in order to ensure non-biased evaluations and to make valid comparisons between men and women. This study aimed to provide more information on measurement invariance across sex for the PID-5, using both the original scoring approach provided by the authors of the instrument and the scoring approach suggested by the APA in the published version of the PID-5.

Methods: This study was conducted with a sample of 2273 participants from the general Québec (Canada) adult population aged 18 to 90 years (M = 46.59; SD = 16.32; 51.8% women).

Results: The original scoring approach model showed good fit to data after freeing paths between certain traits and reached strict invariance. The APA scoring approach also showed good fit to data and reached strict invariance, but needed an adjustment (path freed between Emotional lability and Impulsivity in men) to reach scalar invariance.

Discussion: In line with previous research, the PID-5 is invariant across sex and the five-factor structure adjusts well to data. The APA scoring approach appears to attenuate the cross-loading problem observed with the original scoring approach. In light of these results, we recommend using the APA scoring approach to derive domain scores.

Introduction

Models of personality disorders (PDs) presented in international classifications, such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5; 1) and the International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition (ICD-10; 2), faced numerous criticisms regarding their validity and their clinical utility over the years (3, 4). In response to these criticisms, there has been an international movement towards dimensional models of personality disorders (PDs). This movement led to the introduction of the Alternative Model of Personality Disorders (AMPD) in the Emerging Measures and Models section of the DSM-5 (1), and five years later a similar model was also published in the ICD-11 (5) that became the official PDs model for all WHO member-states, who were expected to migrate to ICD-11 by January 2022 (6). These new models mark a significant shift in the way PDs are conceptualized (7), that is, from a categorical to a dimensional perspective. In the AMPD, personality disorders (PDs) are defined by two main Criteria: 1) the level of disturbance in self and interpersonal functioning (Criterion A); and 2) a set of 25 maladaptive personality traits (Criterion B) that can be grouped into five domains (8) closely resembling the dimensions of the Big Five model of personality (9). These two Criteria are both necessary to PD diagnosis, but the actual incremental value of one over the other is still subject to debate (10, 11).

With the publication of the DSM-5, the APA AMPD Workgroup developed an official measure to directly index Criterion B which was released with the DSM, the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5; 8). The PID-5 is a self-report questionnaire assessing the 25 maladaptive traits as well as the 5 higher order domains, using 220 self-descriptive items (an informant-report version was also developed; 12). These five domains are Antagonism (exaggerated sense of self-importance, antipathy towards others as well as unawareness of others’ needs and feelings), Disinhibition (impulsive behavior motivated by the need of an immediate gratification), Psychoticism (culturally eccentric or unusual behaviors or cognitions), Negative Affectivity (frequent and intense experiences of negative emotions), and Detachment (withdrawal from interpersonal interactions and a limited affective experience and expression). The 25 traits and their associations with the five domains are presented in Supplementary Table 1 of Supplementary Materials.

Since its publication a decade ago, the PID-5 has been translated in many languages, including French (13), and several studies provided strong support for its psychometric properties (14, 15). Most notably, systematic reviews have shown that the PID-5 has good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients being greater than.80 in most studies for domain scales and greater than .70 for trait scales (14, 15). Studies on its test-retest reliability have shown that domain and trait scores are stable across various time intervals (1622). The PID-5 was also shown to have good predictive validity, mostly regarding its capacity to predict categorical PDs (15), but also to predict psychosocial functioning (23) and other mental disorders (24, 25). The PID-5 also showed good convergent validity with other traits models at the domain-level, and with other relevant constructs at the trait-level (14, 15).

Several studies investigated the factor structure of the PID-5 (see 26, for a review) using either of the two possible structures/scoring approaches. In the first scoring approach, henceforth labelled the original scoring approach, all 25 trait scales are used as indicators of the five dimensions, while in the scoring approach adopted by the APA (2013; henceforth referred to as the APA scoring approach), only the three most relevant trait scales per domain were retained as indicators (i.e. those with the highest loadings). A meta-analysis on the structural validity of the PID-5 conducted by Watters and Bagby (27) has shown that substantial cross-loadings of traits on two or even more domains and inconsistencies across studies are often observed, especially when using the original scoring approach. Meanwhile, the APA scoring approach appeared to reduce cross-loadings of traits. Despite concerns regarding the two different scoring approaches leading to diverging results, results have shown that they both lead to similar domain scores (28). The APA scoring approach would even be preferable since interstitial traits are removed without losing information in the domains, and fewer items are thus required to access fairly similar information (28).

Despite the numerous validity studies on the PID-5, only five studies (2933) have investigated its measurement invariance across men and women, even though sex differences are a notable concern in the assessment of PDs (34, 35). Thus, to be able to ensure non-biased evaluations and to make valid comparisons between men and women, we must first ensure that the assessment instruments used are invariant across sex, that is, measurement invariance must be demonstrated (36). In the case of personality traits, measurement invariance assesses the equivalence in terms of structure and of meaning of PD domains across groups (in this study, between men and women) by progressively constraining the factor structure to equality across said groups. Thus, measurement invariance informs whether the same factors (domains) are observed across sex, if these factors are defined by the same indicators (trait scales), if an indicator has the same weight in factor definition and if measurement errors are similar between men and women (37).

Four of the five studies investigating measurement invariance across sex for the PID-5 used shorter versions of the instrument. Two of them (29, 31) used brief versions of the instrument; South et al. (29) used a 36-item version of the PID-5 that assesses the five traditional domains, and a sixth domain of compulsivity, while Gomez et al. (31) used the 25-item version developed by Krueger et al. (38). The structure of the 36-item version was invariant across sex, although strict invariance was not examined (29). The structure of the 25-item version was invariant across sex at the configural, metric and strict levels, but only reached partial scalar invariance. The third study (32), used the PID-5 Faceted Brief Form (PID-5-FBF; 39), a 100-item version, in three distinct samples: populational (n = 526; 49.8% women), private psychotherapy practice clients (n = 544; 64.0% women) and outpatients in a PD treatment clinic (n = 288, 61.5% women). In these three samples, strict invariance (using the original scoring approach) was supported, providing evidence for the absence of sex-related bias in the measure. The last study (33) tested measurement invariance of the PID-5-FBF (39) traits across sex and age in a Belgian community sample (n = 1930; 66.1% women). Of the 25 traits, 10 reached scalar invariance, 13 reached partial scalar invariance, while two reached metric invariance only (Emotional Lability and Separation Insecurity).

In the only study investigating measurement invariance across sex for the original 220-item PID-5 version, Suzuki et al. (30) examined whether its five-factor structure was invariant across men and women in a sample of 6,376 undergraduate students (4195 women, 2181 men) with a mean age of 19.48 years. The authors first conducted exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (EFA and CFA) to test the proposed structure of the PID-5, using the 25 traits as indicators. The CFA model did not adjust well to the data; therefore an EFA model was tested, leading to a 5-factor model that was similar to the one obtained by Krueger et al. (8) in the development of the PID-5. Then, the authors examined the measurement invariance of this model using exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM). Results supported the measurement invariance of the exploratory five-factor structure across sex at the configural (invariance of factors), metric (invariance of loadings) and scalar (invariance of intercepts) levels, which indicates that latent means can be compared across men and women (40). Thus, the PID-5 can be considered invariant across sex when using the original scoring approach (30), although no information was provided regarding the APA scoring approach that is sanctioned by the APA and used in research and clinical settings (28).

The present study

Given that measurement invariance across sex is crucial to make valid comparison between men and women (35), this study aimed to further examine measurement invariance across sex of the PID-5. Since only three studies (30, 32, 33) examined its measurement invariance across sex and used the original scoring approach, the present study aimed to replicate and extend these results by examining measurement invariance for both the original scoring approach and the APA scoring approach. Testing measurement invariance for the APA scoring approach seems particularly relevant, since many clinical decisions rely on it. Furthermore, although Suzuki et al. (30) concluded to scalar invariance, they did not test for strict invariance (invariance of residuals), while Leclerc et al. (32) did test and reach strict invariance but used the 100-item version of the PID-5. Thus, this study aimed to examine invariance of residuals across sex for the 220-item version of the PID-5, since it would inform whether the constructs have the same explicative value across sex (37). Finally, this study used a populational sample of adults that may be more representative of the general population than Suzuki et al. (30) student sample and Leclerc et al. (32) convenience community sample.

Method

Participants

The initial sample consisted of 2,505 participants. Since participation was voluntary, took place online without human proctoring, and came with incentives, it is possible that some participants responded carelessly (41). To detect, at least in part, invalid response profiles, long strings analyses were conducted. Participants with more than 32 consecutive identical responses to the PID-5 were removed from the sample. The cut-off for the PID-5 was set based on the fact that, considering the presence of reversed items, the longest possible sequence of coherent identical responses is 32. Therefore, 118 participants (4.7% of the sample) were removed from the sample due to probable careless responding. Additionally, a response inconsistency scale (42) was used to detect inconsistent responding. This scale includes 20 pairs of similar items. The total score represents the sum of the discrepancy between responses to these 20 pairs of items and a score of 17 or more indicates random responding (42). Thus, 117 participants (4.7% of the sample) reached the cutoff value. Only three participants reached the cutoffs for both long strings and inconsistent responding, leading to a final sample of 2273 participants.

Participants were aged between 18 and 90 years (M = 46.59; SD = 16.32; Mmen = 48.35; SDmen = 15.82; Mwomen = 44.94; SDwomen = 16.62). The sample included 1177 (51.8%) participants whose assigned sex at birth was female, and 1096 (48.2%) whose assigned sex at birth was male. Regarding education, 39.6% (41.8% of men, 37.4% of women) of the sample had a university degree, while 33.7% (29.7% of men, 37.6% of women) had a technical of pre-university college (CEGEP) diploma, 25.6% (27.5% of men, 23.8% of women) had a high school diploma or a specialized trade diploma, and 0.8% (for both men and women) did not complete high school. Regarding marital status, 56.7% (48.8% of men, 51.2% of women) of the sample was married or in a romantic relationship, 32.6% (34.3% of men, 31.1% of women) were single, and 10.4% (8.2% men, 12.5% women) were either widowed, divorced, or separated. Median household annual income was in the 60,000$ to 79,999$ CAN bracket for both men and women.

Measures

Personality Inventory for DSM-5

The French version (13) of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5; 8) was used in the present study. It includes 220 self-reported items rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 to 3 (0 = very false or often false, 1 = sometimes or somewhat false, 2 = sometimes or somewhat true, 3 = very true or often true). Since the French translation was conducted in Europe and the present study took place in Canada, slight vocabulary adjustments were made to five items to optimize their understandability. Cronbach alphas for the 25 trait scales varied from.72 to.96 for the original version (8), and from.68 to.95 for the French translation in a sample of Belgian, French, and Swiss university students (43). In the present study, they ranged from.75 to.95.

Procedure

Participants were recruited as part of a larger study on the assessment of the AMPD. The questionnaires were administered via Léger 360, which is the largest Survey firm in Canada. An email invitation was sent to a randomized sample of Léger 360’s panel of over 200,000 residents from the Province of Québec, Canada. Participants under 18 years of age or who reported a “very poor” understanding of the French language were excluded from the study (age and knowledge of the French language were filter questions). The questionnaires were administered online on Léger 360’s secure servers. Participants had the opportunity to save their responses and return to complete the questionnaire at another time. An email reminder was sent to the participants who had not completed the questionnaires one week after the initial invitation. The questionnaire was closed when the target sample size of approximately 2500 completed questionnaires was reached. The data collection began on June 18 and ended on July 15, 2019. Participants who completed the questionnaires received an incentive in the form of points added to their Léger 360 account and that can be exchanged for cash, gifts, or participation in prize draws. There were no missing data in the PID-5 items since the online questionnaire did not allow for missing responses (though missing responses were allowed for sociodemographic questions). The study received approval from the ethics board from the authors’ research institution, and all participants signed an online informed consent form.

Data analyses

All analyses were conducted using MPlus 8.8 (44). The MLR estimator was used in all factor analyses since it is relatively robust to non-normality, and PID-5 trait scores were not normally distributed in the present sample. To examine the PID-5 factor structure, an exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) with target rotation was used. An ESEM was preferred to a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) because it is less restrictive for a complex model where cross-loadings are expected and theoretically meaningful (45, 46). Target rotation in ESEM allows to target the cross-loadings on specific factors to be as close as possible to zero without applying a constraint; the a priori structure can thus be examined without imposing theoretically-unsound constraints on cross-loadings. To assess model fit, values for the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and Standardized Root Mean-square Residual (SRMR) were examined. For complex multidimensional models such as the PID-5, Hopwood and Donnellan (47) suggest cutoffs close to.10 for RMSEA, and close to.90 for CFI and TLI to assess model fit. Regarding the SRMR, a value below.08 can be considered acceptable (48). Both the original scoring approach (using the 25 traits as indicators) and the APA scoring approach (using 15 traits as indicators) were tested. Using the two structures obtained with target ESEM, four levels of measurement invariance were then tested: configural, metric, scalar, and strict.

First, the baseline model was tested to examine overall model fit for the whole sample. Then, this baseline model was progressively constrained according to levels of invariance that were reached. At each level, new parameters were constrained to equality to examine if the chosen model is still acceptable in both groups. Configural invariance was tested by fitting an unconstrained multigroup model using the same specifications across sex, thus testing if the model has the same number of factors across men and women. Metric invariance was tested by constraining factor loadings to equality across groups to determine whether each trait contributes to the latent variables (the five domains) similarly in men and women. Scalar invariance was tested by constraining intercepts to equality across groups to determine whether mean differences in the latent constructs capture all mean differences in the shared variance of the traits. Finally, strict invariance was tested by constraining residual variance to equality across groups to determine whether the sum of specific variance and error variance is similar across groups. To determine whether there is measurement invariance, ΔRMSEA and ΔCFI were used, since the Chi-Square test does not perform as well in ESEM as it does in CFA (49). The invariance hypothesis should only be rejected if the decrease in CFI is greater than .01 and the increase in RMSEA is greater than .015 (49). That is, if the supplementary constraints do not significantly decrease the fit of the model, measurement invariance is supported. Moreover, we used the w-coefficient to assess the effect size of each additional constraint on the Chi-Square statistic, as it was used in Suzuki et al. (30). The w-coefficient can be interpreted according to Cohen’s (50) standards, w = .1 representing a small effect size, therefore a small change between compared models (51).

Results

Baseline models

Before conducting invariance analyses, baseline models were tested using ESEM with target rotation. The first model to be tested was the five-factor structure with the 25 traits as indicators proposed by Krueger et al. (8). Fit indices showed mixed support for model fit, since the RMSEA, CFI and SRMR suggested an acceptable fit (according to 47 cutoffs) while the TLI indicated inadequate model fit. Modification indices (MI) for all pairs of indicators were examined to identify high measurement error covariances that penalized model fit. Measurement error covariance can indicate the presence of systematic (rather than random) errors and may derive from indicators characteristics such as overlap in content or the presence of a small subfactor within the factor (52). Respecified models were successively computed by freeing the path between Anhedonia and Depressivity (both traits of Detachment; MI = 308.60), Manipulativeness and Deceitfulness (both traits of Antagonism; MI = 327.23), and Rigid Perfectionism (Disinhibition) and Perseveration (Negative Affectivity; MI = 205.91). Following these specifications, the model reached a good fit to the data. Factor loadings for the modified baseline model are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Factor loadings – Original scoring approach modified baseline model.

Model fit for the APA scoring approach baseline model had an excellent fit to the data, which allowed to proceed to the invariance analyses. Factor loadings for this baseline model are presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Factor loadings – APA’s scoring approach baseline model.

Measurement invariance across sex

For the original scoring approach model (with the 25 traits as indicators), configural, metric, scalar, and strict measurement invariance were supported. As shown in Table 3, changes in RMSEA and CFI were within the recommended thresholds of 0.015 and 0.010, respectively (49). When constraining the number of factors across sex (configural invariance), fit indices were similar to those of the baseline model. Constraining factor loadings across groups (metric invariance) did not significantly reduce model fit. When constraining intercepts across groups (scalar invariance), model fit did not significantly worsen. Finally, when constraining residual variance across sex (strict invariance), fit indices did not significantly change. All w-coefficients were lower than 0.1, which indicates that effect sizes for changes in the chi-square statistic were very small when additional constraints were applied.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Fit indices and invariance test for PID-5 domains.

Results for the APA scoring approach (with 15 traits as indicators) are presented in Table 3. The configural invariance model had a good fit to the data which was similar to those of the baseline model. Metric invariance was also supported. However, when constraining the intercepts to equality across groups (scalar invariance), the change in CFI was greater than the 0.01 cutoff value suggested by (49). Examination of MI indicated that freeing the path between Emotional lability and Impulsivity in the men’s group could improve the model (MI = 71.92). Following this modification, model fit changes improved, thus allowing to reach (partial) scalar invariance. Finally, strict invariance was supported. Therefore, the PID-5 model based on the APA scoring approach can be considered invariant across sex. All w-coefficients were lower than 0.1, which indicates that effect sizes for changes in the chi-square statistic were very small when additional constraints were applied.

Since models are invariant across sex, mean and latent score comparisons between men and women have been calculated for each domain with both the original and the APA scoring approaches. Results are presented in Tables 47. For both scoring approaches, women obtained higher scores on the Negative Affectivity domain and men obtained higher scores for Detachment, Antagonism, and Psychoticism. All effect sizes are small, except for Antagonism for which the difference between men and women is moderate with both scoring approaches. Mean difference in Disinhibition is only significant and of small size with the original scoring approach (men scored higher than women). The same pattern of differences is observed with latent scores.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Sex comparisons for domain scores – original scoring approach.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Sex comparisons for domain latent scores – original scoring approach.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6 Sex comparisons for domain scores – APA scoring approach.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7 Sex comparisons for domain latent scores – APA scoring approach.

Discussion

This study examined the measurement invariance of the PID-5 across men and women, using both the original scoring approach, in which the 25 trait scales are used as indicators for the five domains, and the APA scoring approach, in which a reduced sample of the 15 most relevant trait scales are used as indicators. As prerequisite analyses for invariance testing, baseline models were computed and, in line with previous studies, results showed that the five-factor structure of the PID-5 had a good fit to the data with both scoring approaches. In the present study, most of the traits retained for the APA scoring approach had the strongest loadings on their respective domain when using the original scoring approach, which provides support for the APA model. The Antagonism domain appears to be less clearly defined in the original scoring approach, with several traits cross-loading on this dimension, that is, Hostility (λ = .43), Rigid perfectionism (λ = -.37) and Risk taking (λ = .37). This cross-loading of Hostility is consistent with other studies that showed that this trait is theoretically and statistically more associated to Antagonism than it is to Negative Affectivity (53).

Regarding baseline models, while the APA model reached a good fit to the data, adjustments were made for the original scoring approach model to reach an acceptable fit. Paths were successively freed, first between Depressivity and Anhedonia, second between Manipulativeness and Deceitfulness, and lastly between Rigid Perfectionism and Perseveration. In all three cases, these modifications make theoretical sense and may indicate overlap in content between the pairs of traits that is not explained by the factor. Indeed, Depressivity and Anhedonia (Detachment domain) are known to be closely related since the latter is an important aspect of depression (5456). Similarly, Manipulativeness and Deceitfulness (Antagonism domain) both imply dishonesty and the use of others to one’s benefit. Albeit Manipulativeness is mainly associated with manipulation of others while Deceitfulness is more associated with lying and using others, some items, for example “Sweet talking others helps me get what I want” (item 125, Manipulativeness) and “People don’t realize that I’m flattering them to get something” (item 56, Deceitfulness) are closely related may be responsible for the high MI. Regarding Rigid perfectionism and Perseveration, although they are not part of the same domain, they also are conceptually closely related, since they both imply rigidity and persistence (57). Past studies have shown that Rigid Perfectionism tends to load equally, if not more, onto the Negative Affectivity domain rather than onto the Disinhibition (21, 27, 58).

In accordance with our first hypothesis, strict invariance was supported for the original scoring approach model. This indicates that not only the domain constructs have the same meaning across sex (configural invariance), that traits have an equivalent strength of association to their latent domain construct (factor loadings) across sex (metric invariance), that a same observed score on trait scales indicates a same level on the latent domain variable (scalar invariance), but also that the level of unexplained variations in domain scores are the same across men and women, thus informing that the constructs have the same value and meaning across sex (59). However, the model had to be slightly modified by allowing correlations between pairs of traits, as discussed above.

The second hypothesis was that the five-factor structure of the PID-5 based on the APA scoring approach would be invariant across sex. To our knowledge, invariance for this model had never been tested. While configural and metric invariances were supported, to reach scalar invariance, the path between Emotional Lability (a trait in the Negative Affectivity domain) and Impulsivity (a trait in the Disinhibition domain) were freed, but only in the men’s group. These two traits are both core components of Borderline PD (60), and impulsivity is particularly observed in men (61, 62). Following this respecification of the model, strict invariance was also reached, meaning that constructs have the same explicative value across sex.

Overall, even though both models needed respecifications, the original scoring approach for the PID-5 including 25 traits and the APA scoring approach including 15 traits reached full (strict) invariance across sex. These results are in accordance with other evidence supporting the invariance of personality traits across sex (e.g. 63), and of the level of personality functioning (Criterion A; 34). The APA scoring approach model needed fewer modifications to reach full invariance. This better fit can probably be explained by the removal of interstitial traits, since they seem to be problematic in many structure analyses involving the PID-5 (26). Because the two approaches lead to similar scores, we recommend the use the APA scoring approach, which is the approach used by clinicians, over the original scoring approach when one is interested in using the five domain scores, since it appears to significantly attenuate the cross-loading of traits on domains.

For both scoring approaches, men obtained higher scores on Detachment, Antagonism, and Psychoticism domains, albeit differences were of small effect size, except for the Antagonism domain which had a moderate effect size for both mean and latent score differences. Women obtained higher scores on the Negative Affectivity domain with a small effect size for both mean and latent scores. These findings are consistent with the differences across sex observed by Suzuki et al. (30) for the PID-5, and more broadly with results regarding the Five Factor Model of personality (64). Men obtained significantly higher scores with small effect size on Disinhibition domain, but only with the original scoring approach, for both mean and latent scores. This could be explained by the fact that the Risk Taking trait is not included in the Disinhibition domain score in the APA scoring approach while it is in the original scoring approach; men scored significantly higher than women on Risk Taking with a moderate effect size.

Limitation and future directions

Although this sample is quite large and is representative of general population, which can provide normative data useful to clinicians and researchers [see (34), for French-Canadian normative data for the PID-5], the proportion of people suffering from severe personality pathology is probably low (as suggested by PID-5 scale scores). This study should thus be replicated in clinical samples. This study also only covers measurement invariance for the self-report version of PID-5. Future studies could assess measurement invariance for the informant version (12), in which both sex of the informant and the assessed could impact on results. Furthermore, this sample is representative of the French-speaking population of the Province of Québec (Canada) only and should be replicated in other cultures, especially non-Western and from developing countries (65). Also, the French version of the PID-5 was used in the present study and results may not generalize to other linguistic versions.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comité d’éthique de la recherche - Éducation et sciences sociales - Université de Sherbrooke. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

GR: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing. YL: Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. ML: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing. KF: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study has been funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Grant Number 419691).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1328937/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases – 10th edition. Gen ve, Switzerland: World Health Organization (1993).

Google Scholar

3. Widiger TA, Mullins-Sweatt SN. Five-factor model of personality disorder: A proposal for DSM-V. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2009) 5:197–220. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Waugh MH, Hopwood CJ, Krueger RF, Morey LC, Pincus AL, Wright AGC. Psychological assessment with the DSM-5 Alternative Model for Personality Disorders: Tradition and innovation. Prof Psychol: Res Pract. (2017) 48:79–89. doi: 10.1037/pro0000071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. World Health Organization. International classification of diseases – 11th revision(2019). Available at: https://icd.who.int/en.

Google Scholar

6. Bach B, Simonsen S. How does level of personality functioning inform clinical management and treatment? Implications for ICD-11 classification of personality disorder severity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2021) 34:54–63. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hopwood CJ, Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Widiger TA, Althoff RR, et al. The time has come for dimensional personality disorder diagnosis. Pers Ment Health. (2018) 12:82–6. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Krueger RF, Derringer J, Markon KE, Watson D, Skodol AE. Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. psychol Med. (2012) 42:1879–90. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Widiger TA, McCabe GA. The alternative model of personality disorders (AMPD) from the perspective of the five-factor model. Psychopathology. (2020) 53:149–56. doi: 10.1159/000507378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Morey LC, McCredie MN, Bender DS, Skodol AE. Criterion A: Level of personality functioning in the alternative DSM–5 model for personality disorders. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2022) 13:305–15. doi: 10.1037/per0000551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Zimmermann J, Kerber A, Rek K, Hopwood CJ, Krueger RF. A brief but comprehensive review of research on the alternative DSM-5 model for personality disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2019) 21:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-1079-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Markon KE, Quilty LC, Bagby RM, Krueger RF. The development and psychometric properties of an informant-report form of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5). Assessment. (2013) 20:370–83. doi: 10.1177/1073191113486513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Rossi G, Rolland JP, De Page L, Hennequin E, Pirrera S, Guelfi JD, et al. Questionnaire de personnalit pour le DSM-5 [Personality Inventory for DSM-5]: Official French trans-lation of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5) (reproduced and translated for research use with permission from the American Psychiatric Association) [Unpublished manuscript]. Personality and Psychopathology Research Group, Vrije Universiteit Brussel. (2012).

Google Scholar

14. Al-Dajani N, Gralnick TM, Bagby RM. A psychometric review of the personality inventory for DSM–5 (PID–5): current status and future directions. J Pers Assess. (2016) 98:62–81. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1107572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Barchi-Ferreira AMB, Osorio FL. The personality inventory for DSM-5: psychometric evidence of validity and reliability–updates. Harvard Rev Psychiatry (2020) 28(4):225–37. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Chmielewski M, Ruggero CJ, Kotov R, Liu K, Krueger RF. Comparing the dependability and associations with functioning of the DSM–5 Section III trait model of personality pathology and the DSM–5 Section II personality disorder model. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2017) 8:228. doi: 10.1037/per0000213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Adhiatma W, Halim MS. Structural validity and reliability of the Indonesian version of PID-5: Study on community and student sample. psychol Test Adaptat Dev. (2021) 2:62–71. doi: 10.1027/2698-1866/a000010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Coelho O, Pires R, Ferreira AS, Gonçalves B, AlJassmi M, Stocker J. Arabic version of the personality inventory for the DSM-5 (PID-5) in a community sample of united arab emirates nationals. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health: CP EMH. (2020) 16:180–8.

Google Scholar

19. Nagy ZS, Salgó E, Bajzát B, Hajduska-Dér B, Unoka ZS. Reliability and validity of the Hungarian version of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5). PloS One. (2022) 17:e0266201. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pires R, Sousa Ferreira A, Guedes D. The psychometric properties of the portuguese version of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Scandinavian J Psychol (2017) 58(5):468–75. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wright AG, Simms LJ. On the structure of personality disorder traits: conjoint analyses of the CAT-PD, PID-5, and NEO-PI-3 trait models. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2014) 5:43–54. doi: 10.1037/per0000037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Suzuki T, Griffin SA, Samuel DB. Capturing the DSM-5 alternative personality disorder model traits in the five-factor model’s nomological net. J Pers. (2017) 85:220–31. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wright AGC, Calabrese WR, Rudick MM, Yam WH, Zelazny K, Williams TF, et al. Stability of the DSM-5 section III pathological personality traits and their longitudinal associations with psychosocial functioning in personality disordered individuals. J Abnormal Psychol (2015) 124(1):199–207. doi: 10.1037/abn0000018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Few LR, Miller JD, Rothbaum AO, Meller S, Maples J, Terry DP, et al. Examination of the Section III DSM-5 diagnostic system for personality disorders in an outpatient clinical sample. J Abnormal Psychol. (2013) 122:1057–69. doi: 10.1037/a0034878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Zimmermann J, Altenstein D, Krieger T, Holtforth MG, Pretsch J, Alexopoulos J, et al. The structure and correlates of self-reported DSM-5 maladaptive personality traits: Findings from two German-speaking samples. J Pers Disord. (2014) 28:518–40. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2014_28_130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Clark LA, Watson D. The trait model of the DSM–5 alternative model of personality disorder (AMPD): A structural review. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2022) 13:328–36. doi: 10.1037/per0000568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Watters CA, Bagby RM. A meta-analysis of the five-factor internal structure of the Personality Inventory for DSM–5. psychol Assess. (2018) 30:1255–60. doi: 10.1037/pas0000605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Watters CA, Sellbom M, Bagby RM. Comparing two domain scoring methods for the Personality Inventory for DSM–5. psychol Assess. (2019) 31:1125–34. doi: 10.1037/pas0000739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. South SC, Krueger RF, Knudsen GP, Ystrom E, Czajkowski N, Aggen SH, et al. A population based twin study of DSM–5 maladaptive personality domains. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2017) 8:366–75. doi: 10.1037/per0000220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Suzuki T, South SC, Samuel DB, Wright AG, Yalch MM, Hopwood CJ, et al. Measurement invariance of the DSM–5 Section III pathological personality trait model across sex. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2019) 10:114–22. doi: 10.1037/per0000291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gomez R, Watson S, Brown T, Stavropoulos V. Personality inventory for DSM 5-Brief Form (PID-5-BF): Measurement invariance across men and women. Pers Dis: Theory Res Treat. (2023) 14(3):334–338. doi: 10.1037/per0000569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Leclerc P, Savard C, Sellbom M, Côté A, Nolin MC, Payant M, et al. Investigating the validity and measurement invariance of the personality inventory for DSM-5 faceted brief form among French-speaking clinical and nonclinical samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2023) 45:519–36. doi: 10.1007/s10862-022-10000-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Vergauwe J, Rouco V, Franssens R, Claes L, Bastiaens T, De Clercq B. Age and sex differences of the PID-5-100 maladaptive personality traits throughout adulthood. Pers Dis Theory Res Treat. (2023) 14(5):512–26. doi: 10.1037/per0000622

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Le Corff Y, Aluja A, Rossi G, Lapalme M, Forget K, García LF, et al. Construct validity of the dutch, english, french, and spanish LPFS-BF 2.0: Measurement invariance across language and gender and criterion validity. J Pers Dis (2022) 36(6):662–79. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2022.36.6.662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Paggeot A, Huprich S. Measurement invariance between genders on two measures of borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. (2018) 32:31–43. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2017_31_284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chen FF. What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2008) 95:1005–18. doi: 10.1037/a0013193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev Rev. (2016) 41:71–90. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Krueger RF, Derringer J, Markon KE, Watson D, Skodol AV. The Personality Inventory for DSM–5 (PID-5)-Adult. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). Available at: http://www.psychiatry.org/practice/dsm/dsm5/onlineassessment-measures.

Google Scholar

39. Maples JL, Carter NT, Few LR, Crego C, Gore WL, Samuel DB, et al. Testing whether the DSM-5 personality disorder trait model can be measured with a reduced set of items: An item response theory investigation of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5. psychol Assess. (2015) 27:1195–210. doi: 10.1037/pas0000120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Vandenberg RJ, Lance CE. A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Res Methods. (2000) 3:4–70. doi: 10.1177/109442810031002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Burchett D, Sellbom M, Bagby RM. Assessment of response bias in personality disorder research. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2023) 14:93–104. doi: 10.1037/per0000608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Keeley JW, Webb C, Peterson D, Roussin L, Flanagan EH. Development of a response inconsistency scale for the personality inventory for DSM–5. J Pers Assess. (2016) 98:351–9. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2016.1158719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Roskam I, Galdiolo S, Hansenne M, Massoudi K, Rossier J, Gicquel L, et al. The psychometric properties of the French version of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5. PloS One. (2015) 10:e0133413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Muthén and Muthén. Mplus. (2022). Available at: http://www.statmodel.com/.

Google Scholar

45. Marsh HW, Muthén B, Asparouhov T, Lüdtke O, Robitzsch A, Morin AY, et al. Exploratory structural equation modeling, integrating CFA and EFA: Application to students’ evaluations of University Teaching. Struct Equation Model. (2009) 16:439–76. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Marsh HW, Nagengast B, Morin AJ. Measurement invariance of big-five factors over the life span: ESEM tests of gender, age, plasticity, maturity, and la dolce vita effects. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:1194. doi: 10.1037/a0026913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hopwood CJ, Donnellan MB. How should the internal structure of personality inventories be evaluated? Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2010) 14:332–46. doi: 10.1177/1088868310361240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis (8th ed.). Andover, UK: Cengage (2018).

Google Scholar

49. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equation Model: A Multidiscip J. (2007) 14:464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York, NY: Routledge Academic (1988).

Google Scholar

51. Newsom JT. Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Routledge (2015).

Google Scholar

52. Byrne BM, Van de Vijver FJ. Testing for measurement and structural equivalence in large-scale cross-cultural studies: Addressing the issue of nonequivalence. Int J Test. (2010) 10:107–32. doi: 10.1080/15305051003637306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Watters CA, Sellbom M, Uliaszek AA, Bagby RM. Clarifying the interstitial nature of facets from the Personality Inventory for DSM–5 using the five factor model of personality. Pers Disorders: Theory Res Treat. (2019) 10:330–9. doi: 10.1037/per0000327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Treadway MT, Zald DH. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:537–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Pizzagalli DA. Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2014) 10:393–423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Rizvi SJ, Pizzagalli DA, Sproule BA, Kennedy SH. Assessing anhedonia in depression: Potentials and pitfalls. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 65:21–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 – Text Revision. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2022). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Crego C, Oltmanns JR, Widiger TA. FFMPD scales: Comparisons with the FFM, PID-5, and CAT-PD-SF. psychol Assessment. (2018) 30:62–73. doi: 10.1037/pas0000495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Byrne BM. Multigroup Comparisons. Testing for measurement, structural, and latent mean equivalence. In: Leong FTL, Bartram D, Cheung FM, Geisinger KF, Iliescu D, editors. The ITC international handbook of testing and assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2016). p. 377–94.

Google Scholar

60. Bach B, Sellbom M. Continuity between DSM-5 categorical criteria and traits criteria for borderline personality disorder. Can J Psychiatry. (2016) 61:489–94. doi: 10.1177/0706743716640756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Soloff PH, Kelly TM, Strotmeyer SJ, Malone KM, Mann JJ. Impulsivity, gender, and response to fenfluramine challenge in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2003) 119:11–24. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00100-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Sher L, Rutter SB, New AS, Siever LJ, Hazlett EA. Gender differences and similarities in aggression, suicidal behaviour, and psychiatric comorbidity in borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2019) 139:145–53. doi: 10.1111/acps.12981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Samuel DB, South SC, Griffin SA. Factorial invariance of the five-factor model rating form across gender. Assessment. (2015) 22:65–75. doi: 10.1177/1073191114536772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kajonius PJ, Johnson J. Sex differences in 30 facets of the five factor model of personality in the large public (N= 320,128). Pers Individ Dif. (2018) 129:126–30. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Schmitt DP, Realo A, Voracek M, Allik J. Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2008) 94:168–82. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Personality Inventory for DSM-5, maladaptive traits, personality disorders, Alternative Model of Personality Disorders, measurement invariance, scoring approaches

Citation: Rivard G, Le Corff Y, Lapalme M and Forget K (2024) Measurement invariance of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 across sex. Front. Psychiatry 15:1328937. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1328937

Received: 27 October 2023; Accepted: 21 February 2024;
Published: 08 March 2024.

Edited by:

Lionel Cailhol, University Institute in Mental Health of Montreal, Canada

Reviewed by:

Conal Monaghan, Australian National University, Australia
Serge Combaluzier, Université de Rouen, France
Colin Freilich, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States

Copyright © 2024 Rivard, Le Corff, Lapalme and Forget. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yann Le Corff, yann.le.corff@usherbrooke.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.