Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychiatry, 16 May 2024
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic International Day of Happiness: Happiness and Wellbeing in the Age of Climate Catastrophe View all 7 articles

On proposing relational environmental metaphors to stimulate engagement and foster well-being in the midst of climate change

  • School of Counselling, Psychotherapy and Spirituality, Faculty of Human Sciences, Saint Paul University, Ottawa, ON, Canada

Messages regarding climate change that are intended to stimulate responsible engagement can impact our mental health in both positive and negative ways, which in turn can increase or limit the potential engagement being sought through those very messages. Increasingly alarmist environmental metaphors are being brought into question due to their possibly detrimental impact on mental health and well-being, and in their place, relational environmental metaphors are proffered to instill hopeful and constructive individual and collective engagement for responsible climate action. This article discusses how both alarmist and relational environmental metaphors interact with eco-emotions. It proposes, in light of concepts arising from Porges’ Polyvagal Theory − on the psychophysiology of autonomic states created in contexts of threatening cues and feelings of safety and connection −, that relational environmental metaphors are preferable for stimulating responsible collective engagement and fostering global well-being in the midst of climate change.

Introduction

The psychological and physiological benefits of regular contact with nature for mental health and well-being, whether through green spaces (i.e. parks, forests, gardens, fields) or blue spaces (i.e. lakes, rivers, oceans) are well documented (13). However, climate change is disrupting that relationship (47). Furthermore, children and adolescents are playing less outdoors than previous generations (8, 9). They are also increasingly eco-anxious (10), in the context of media reports covering extreme events linked to climate change such as record droughts, floods, or hurricanes (11). This ecological crisis in turn leads to the use of ever more alarming metaphors (e.g. the house is burning) by politicians (e.g. 12), activists (e.g. 13) and journalists in the hopes of encouraging more eco-responsible individual and collective behaviours.

Any message regarding climate change can negatively impact the mental health of the target audience, short of including actions to support their psychological and social well-being (14). Negative psychological health is linked to a lack of positive climate engagement, or what Gifford (15) calls “dragons of inaction” (16, 17). We thus argue for the importance of constructive and hopeful, yet accurate, environmental metaphors to stimulate individual and collective engagement and foster global well-being in the midst of climate change (18: 19). Namely, we focus on the long-term merits of using relational (or ‘co-benefit’) metaphors rather than alarmist (or ‘risk reduction’) metaphors.

The purpose of this article is to provide some conceptual support as well as empirical evidence taken from previous research in order to show the benefits of using environmental and, specifically, climate-related messages drawn from metaphors that evoke positive feelings of safety, (inter)relationship and hope, as opposed to metaphors based on threat and fear (“risk reduction”) metaphors.

Method, main sources and overview of this study

Methodologically, this study borrows an interdisciplinary perspective, drawing from various fields of research such as environmental (and climate change) communications (i.e. metaphors, 20, 21), the science of eco-emotions (22), and elements from neuroscience (via Polyvagal Theory, 23, 24). This article uses a combination of a semi-structural review (25) and a variation of a form of narrative review (22, 26). In order to do so, some guiding principles for the selection and prioritization of sources were used, such as peer-reviewed research in academic journals. There are five main parts to this article.

The first part is based on literature review, and presents some relevant research on climate metaphors. The provided selection of climate metaphors took into account space limitations. Since it would be a huge task to present all of them, and somehow impossible to make strong claims of their universality, considering the wealth of cultural diversity within multiple countries and languages, it was decided not to try to discuss all the possibilities. This article was thus partly based on a semi-structural review method recommended for such topics where many disciplines are involved and the sources differ from each other (25). In short, this article focuses on commonly encountered climate metaphors. In terms of procedural approach, database searches were made using relevant keywords such as climate + environmental + metaphors, and various related words. While not exhaustive, the main sources come from empirical studies and/or recent research published within the past ten years (19, 2729).

The second part provides a brief overview of eco-emotions, focusing mainly on the breakthrough work of Pihkala (30). The third part presents the theoretical framework, calling upon the Polyvagal theory in particular. Taking into consideration space limitations, the fourth part presents some results from the application of the Polyvagal theory to two contrasting types of environmental metaphors (“The earth is our home” versus “Our house is burning”). Finally, in the fifth part, strengths and limitations of the study are discussed, along with some possible implications for future research.

Let us first discuss how metaphors facilitate climate change communications and campaigns before presenting recent findings on the science of eco-emotions as important drivers of climate action. Then, we will delve into Polyvagal Theory to deepen our understanding of the psychophysiology of autonomic states created in contexts of threatening cues and feelings of safety and connection (23, 24). We will discuss how feelings of safety can lead to enhanced collaborative behaviours necessary for collective engagement (23, 24), particularly in the face of climate change.

Climate metaphors

Metaphors have been used by scientists, political leaders, environmental activists, and journalists (20, 21, 31) to communicate complex issues linked to climate change – and to trigger a response (32). Metaphors offer a way of setting the unfamiliar in familiar terms by carrying over knowledge from past experiences (20, 33). They are relevant to how we make sense of complex problems (27). Metaphors can influence people because they are more emotionally evocative than comparable literal communications (19, 34).

While a metaphorical representation facilitates understanding of certain characteristics of something, metaphors can be misleading if relied upon too heavily, in that by their very nature they create only partial insights (35). Because metaphors are not neutral ways of representing and perceiving reality, one needs to acknowledge the profound implications of commonly using a particular metaphor over another for it promotes particular readings of an event or issue (20, 36, 37). Thibodeau et al. (19) present three main ways metaphors can influence people and thus serve as effective tools of climate change communications: (1) by evoking emotions; (2) by providing a mental model for thinking (i.e. framing) the domain they describe (i.e. referring to the natural world as a complex system – say like globalisation); (3) by being accurate.

Commonly encountered environmental metaphors include ‘the house is burning’, ‘war on warming’, ‘tipping point’, and ‘greenhouse gases’, (see Table 1 for more examples). While these metaphors convey the intended meaning and enhance comprehension of climate change (33), their anxiety-inducing nature may have unintended consequences as they impact our ability to respond. For ‘alarmist’ metaphors may reduce our ability to engage collaboratively on sustainable solutions due to neurophysiological processes involved in associative learning linking thoughts with autonomic states (24). Metaphors that rely on an alarmist or ‘risk reduction’ framework (19) may be useful in the short term to capture people’s attention, but can become detrimental by maintaining people in a state of vigilance from which they will grow fatigued (38), as they did with prolonged covid-19 precautionary measures.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Commonly encountered climate metaphors and possible associated eco-emotions.

Researchers (18, 27) have observed a tendency to overuse risk reduction metaphors, instead of more relational (hopeful/optimistic or ‘co-benefit’) ones, providing a reductionist representation of climate change and/or a separation between humans and nature – as if unrelated. The overuse of such risk reduction metaphors feeds narratives which tend to reduce the complexity of climate change and the richness of the human-nature relationship to a single dichotomous impacted/not-impacted scenario, reinforcing the economic principles of cost–benefit analysis. “This representation undermines public understanding of and engagement with climate change by marginalising subordinate policy framings which do not align with the prevailing dichotomous framing” (27, p. 34).

Although fear-based messaging is used to promote pro-ecological behaviour from the general public (39), one may wonder about its medium and long term effectiveness – especially among young people who feel more directly impacted, leading to feelings of eco-anxiety, paralysis or depression (10). How can anyone feel calm or relaxed in his/her own home while constantly being reminded that ‘the house is burning’ outside (e.g. while listening to the news)?

According to Rucińska & Fondelli (40), humans can be influenced by metaphors not only due to their intellectual ability to understand the conceptual implications (20), but also because metaphors can be defined and operationalized in more dynamic (i.e. embodied, enactive, and ecological) ways (41). For metaphors can convey many subtle and complex emotions. Table 1 summarizes the commonly encountered climate metaphors and possible related eco-emotions in scientific literature. While not exhaustive, this list provides a brief overview which shows that “negative” climate metaphors and eco-motions are far more prominent than the “positive” ones.

The role of eco-emotions

Interesting new advancements have recently been made in the fields of emotions studies, environmental psychology, climate emotions and eco-emotions. Works by Albrecht (42) and Pihkala (22) Pihkala (30) both acknowledge the wide scope, complexity, depth, and diversity of climate emotions, as well as the need to embrace the coexistence of their darker side (undesirable, negative, painful) and brighter side (pleasant, positive, desirable) with regards to our current natural environment. Based on their proposed typologies, both researchers propose a higher ratio for unpleasant (“negative”) versus pleasant (“positive”) eco-emotions (about 3:2 for 22; and 2:1 for 42). This seems to make sense: when things are difficult, stressful or problematic, the human mind may search for answers to explain such lack of coherence or clarity, in order to calm the state of activation (triggered by such problem) and find peace of mind (43, 44). In sum, people dealing with climate anxiety (or eco-anxiety) can experience a wide range of affects and respond differently, from high distress and even clinical symptoms (45) to finding meaning in times of adversity (46), the latter observed in terms of “practical eco-anxiety” (i.e. engaging in adapted and healthy ways despite feeling anxious about climate change, 47).

Furthermore, the study of eco-emotions is relevant to experiences of psychological resistances in the context of climate change (15). By responding to the messages conveyed by emotions − which, etymologically speaking, refer to the adaptive function of “setting in motion” (42)−, humans can better guide and direct behavior to better meet their needs in order to move forward. This requires a deep understanding of eco-emotions enabling us to transcend the psychological barriers of inaction (48).

Comparing various approaches for understanding social behavior and adaptive responses to stress, we can also draw parallels with climate emotions. Alarmist (risk reduction) climate metaphors (e.g. ‘our house is burning’) correspond to the classic ‘fight or flight’ response to threats (49), while “co-benefit” metaphors (e.g. ‘our common home’) resonate with the Tend and Befriend theory (50). Since the quality of attachment informs the level of safety and satisfaction within human relationships, Hlay et al. (51)’s findings suggest that secure attachments are linked to prosocial reactions to stress, namely tending to and befriending others (50).

Amidst an ongoing debate regarding the relevance of using fear-based messages versus hope-based messages in order to motivate people to adopt a more sustainable lifestyle (39), some research points in favor of using more hope-based (and/or less fear-based) messages (5256). In short, “constructive hope” can promote pro-environmental behavior (57), policy support and political engagement (58), especially when it is understood in terms of trusting that climate change can be mitigated by solution-oriented individual and collective action (59). After all, climate messages without negative emotional content (i.e. fear-based) tend to be better received by people in general (60).

Theoretical framework: Polyvagal Theory

Socio-cultural contexts can influence people’s ability to use as well as respond to metaphors, which both in turn impact their effectiveness (35). Furthermore, given that polyvagal theory can help shed light on various autonomic states within threatening versus safe relational contexts (23, 24, 61), we thus argue the following: this conceptual framework − which provides a neurological and physiological grounding − can support the use of positive metaphors in climate communications, particularly via climate metaphors.

In the case of relational (or “co-benefit”) environmental metaphors which may be seemingly less dire from an intellectual point of view, from the perspective of Polyvagal Theory (23, 24), such environmental metaphors may lead to greater collective involvement. Especially, if these metaphors are delivered within trusted communities and in ways that support co-regulation so as to engender feelings of safety that are maintained within individuals’ ventral vagal systems (24). Indeed, “Polyvagal Theory provides an innovative scientific perspective … of safety that incorporates an understanding of neuroanatomy and neurophysiology” (24, p. 1). Autonomic states can be regulated to promote a feeling of safety from within, which in turn leads to collaborative behaviour and social engagement (23, 24). Polyvagal Theory also validates collective involvement in promoting opportunities to experience co-regulation that contributes to feelings of safety as a reciprocal process (23, 24).

Polyvagal Theory stipulates that bidirectional pathways connect the brain’s motor cortex to the autonomic nervous system through the ventral vagal system, a “family of neural pathways” that wander (‘vagus’ is latin for ‘wanderer’) throughout the body (23). Whereas the dorsal vagal system regulates organs below the diaphragm, including the digestive system, and the sympathetic nervous system regulates blood circulation, heart rhythms and body temperature, providing energy to the system, the ventral vagal system oversees the entire autonomic nervous system, “holding the sympathetic and dorsal vagal systems in a warm embrace” (62, p.11). When the ventral vagal system is engaged as a person’s autonomic state, a feeling of safety is created.

From the perspective of Polyvagal Theory, “life is experienced from the inside out”, and these “experiences are carried in autonomic pathways” (62, p. 33). Perceptions of emotional and/or physical threats are experienced as sympathetic and dorsal vagal dysregulation, whereas feelings of safety and connection, of collaboration, creativity, and openness to possibilities are experienced in the ventral vagal autonomic state. It is important to note that in response to the same environmental context, individuals may experience differing autonomic states (24).

As Dana (62) points out, one can learn to regulate autonomic threat responses by bringing explicit awareness to previously implicit experiences. In doing so, one can interrupt the usual adaptive survival responses that typically prevent the autonomic nervous system from finding safety in connection, and thus effectively engage the “ventral vagal safety circuit” (62, p.36). Social connection is perceived as a result of ventral vagal involvement through co-regulation, but also as a “neuromodulator” that reciprocally supports the individual’s ability to maintain ventral vagal involvement, with its resulting positive effects of connection, mutual help, and cooperation (24). “Polyvagal Theory emphasizes that resilience reflects a physiological state, which is sufficiently resilient to recover from disruptions, support feelings of safety, and connect with others via an active social engagement system” (24, p. 11).

Porges (24) entertains the hope that spontaneous social engagement can emerge from communities which foster co-regulated feelings of safety, and states that “introduction of cues of safety would be a functional antidote to threat reactions by reducing the associative links between feelings of threat and thoughts and actions” (24, p.4). Furthermore, Polyvagal Theory links self-regulation and co-regulation of autonomic responses to affective experience, emotional expression, facial gestures, vocal communication, and social behavior (23). Thus, cues of safety need to be understood from the perspective of pro-social cues and social cognition.

Applying Porges’s theory in the context of climate change: an illustration

Given the complexity and plurality of people’s emotional, behavioral and adaptive responses, according to Wright (61), Porges’ model seems useful since it provides a conceptual framework with promising heuristic potential for accounting for and/or categorizing diverse behavioral and emotional responses related to perceptions of climate change. Wright (61) suggests that in the context of climate change, people’s responses, thoughts, and feelings can be mapped following some nervous system responses. For example, social engagement responses may relate to any regulated state involving a sense of safety, curiosity, calmness, compassion and presence. However, under initial stress (or nervous activation), individuals enter into the fight or flight mode, where anger, rage or frustration are related to the fight mode, and the panic, fear, anxiety, worry, dread may echo the flight mode. Finally, feelings of hopelessness may relate to the freeze mode, and so forth (61).

Let us apply the Polyvagal Theory perspective to the two aforementioned types of opposing (i.e. alarmist/”risk reduction” versus relational/”co-benefit”) environmental metaphors. When a communicator uses a more relational metaphor such as “The earth is our home”, listeners may subjectively associate his message with the calm autonomic state which is regulated by the ventral vagal pathway, supporting homeostatic functions (i.e. health, growth, restoration) (24). This in turn activates the social engagement system which sends back signals of security (non verbal cues and facial expressions) among them and to the speaker that functionally regulate (via neuroception) autonomic states of calmness (24). Through this co-regulation, potential threat reactions can be neutralized in favor of establishing and nurturing trusting relationships (24). Furthermore, these feelings of safety not only allow sociality but can also foster problem solving and creativity, by enabling efficient access to the cortical regions (i.e. higher brain structures), optimizing health and performance (24), two important determinants of well-being (63, 64).

Another framework well-known in positive psychology may support this overall argument: the so-called “Broaden and Build theory” of positive emotions (65, 66). This theory, by adding an evolutionary dimension, may further support the psychophysiological grounding proposed by Porges (24). In brief, negative affect (emotions) narrow attentional scope, draw on existing skills and resources and arise in relation to threats which, from an evolutionary perspective, usually demand immediate attention and response (66). By contrast, positive affects (joy, pleasure, interest, etc.) broaden attentional scope, fostering the use of a wider repertoire of thoughts and actions (66). This allows for creative exploration and innovation leading to developing new skills and capabilities (such as self-regulation and emotional regulation). One may see a potential link with the ventral vagal system being the anatomical-functional instantiation of this evolved set of tendencies. Incidentally, this also echoes research on “play”, or how the creative and adaptive skills development that comes from the “playful approach” (not only for children) requires a secure, low threat “relaxed field” (67) and non-stressed frame of mind (68, p. 140).

By contrast to fostering such a secure (and thus engaging) social response via positive emotions, if someone decides to use a popular alarmist climate metaphor (i.e. “our house is burning”) with the same audience, people may feel somehow threatened, thus stressed and/or anxious, due to a subjective interpretation activating shared autonomous states of defense which disrupt homeostatic functions (24). In sum, from a psychophysiological point of view, aforementioned, we argue that the use of frequent relational (or inclusive “co-benefit”) environmental metaphors would lead to greater collective involvement if delivered within trusted communities and in ways that support co-regulation (i.e. fostering feelings of safety that are maintained within individuals’ ventral vagal systems, 24). Thus, not only the wording, but the context and manner of delivery will be important to incite greater collective engagement (24). Table 2 summarizes this by contrasting two different environmental (or climate) metaphors using similar semantic terms, house/home. Let us underline that for most people, the notions of ‘home’ or ‘house’ refer to that of a refuge (i.e. a nocturnal symbol, 70): a place where one feels safe, can rest and enjoy life in a peaceful way, as opposed to daytime struggles linked to work, performance, achievement and so on (70).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Contrasting two types of environmental (or climate) metaphors.

Implications and concluding remarks

In this article, we argue that, when using environmental metaphors, we need to shift from an alarmist (or ‘risk reduction’, based on threat and fear) framework to a more relational (or ‘co-benefit’) framework (19, 71, 72) using metaphors that evoke positive feelings of safety, (inter)relationship and hope, and so forth. Based on ground work from co-emotions studies, research on stress responses, partly on Positive Psychology (i.e. Fredrickson’s model on positive emotions), and using the Polyvagal Theory, we find that metaphors focusing on climate risks may trigger debilitating fear and anxiety, especially in more vulnerable people, potentially contributing to mental health issues or prompting people to disengage (15, 53, 73).

Thus instead, and based on a more relational perspective linked to eco-emotions (74), we may pair messages acknowledging potential risks from climate hazards with messages of hope, in order to help people feel more empowered and in control of their well-being (75). To this end, we argue that metaphors offering a relational (or ‘co-benefit’) perspective rather than a ‘risk reduction’ framework such as ‘the earth is our home’ (19), or ‘our common home’ (69), may prove more successful in fostering individual and collective engagement towards global well-being in the midst of climate change (21, 71, 72). Indeed, certain metaphors favouring a human-environment relationship can lead people to adopt a more nuanced and responsible understanding of their place within the natural world (19). Metaphors grounded in a relational perspective can be viewed as more constructive and hopeful, as they favour a more systemic framework adaptation of the human-environment relationship, echoing Gillespie’s recommendation that we reframe climate change as a systemic problem (76), while focusing less on individual responsibility.

This is drawn from understanding the widespread use and psychologically influential nature of metaphors in human language and communication (e.g., the seminal works of 20), and placing it within the context of work on ‘‘eco-emotions’’ (eco-anxiety in all its forms (including ‘practical eco-anxiety’, a relatively ‘desirable’ state), depression, despair, frustration, guilt, etc.) and the tabulation of use of metaphors in climate communications. Also, by underpinning the rationale for the use of more positive metaphors by using the ‘Polyvagal Theory’ − particularly through emphasising the role of the ‘ventral vagal system’ (as opposed to the autonomic and dorsal vagal systems) and its alignment with cooperative and trusted social connection (in a reciprocal process) −, such provides a neurological and physiological grounding that supports the use of positive metaphors in climate communications.

The main limitation of this perspective paper is that it is solely conceptual (for now) and borrows various notions from diverse fields. In order to empirically support this perspective, future research may use qualitative and innovative methods to examine the same topic, such as Online photovoice (OPV) to explore people’s experiences of climate change (7780) and Online Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (OIPA) to understand people’s use of environmental metaphors (81).

As Marks et al. (82) have indicated, when young people can share eco-emotions and collective engagement, this can decrease their climate distress and despair, while engendering hope (83, 84), such as (‘active’ or ‘constructive’ hope; 83) which requires holding two competing realisations in mind such as: (1) the crisis is real and (2) change for a sustainable future is possible, if we keep imagining the possible while acting together now. By contrast, overuse of alarmist metaphors may notably leave youth feeling so unsafe as to stunt their ability to discover, enjoy, protect and reflect on how to engage in sustainable activity. We thus need environmental metaphors that are grounded in empirical findings yet at the same time stimulate individual and collective engagement. Such environmental metaphors may be beneficial for the current social discourse (notably inspiring political leaders), ecological education (i.e. fostering hope for future generations), and ecotherapy (i.e. helping clients deal with negative climate emotions).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

CB: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Resources, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. L-MC: Writing – review & editing, Resources.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The authors gratefully acknowledged the financial assistance received from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada: Grand number: SSHRC IG-SS (435-2021-0418).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Zelenski JM, Nisbet E. Happiness and feeling connected: The distinct role of nature relatedness. Environ Behav. (2014) 46, 1:3–23. doi: 10.1177/0013916512451901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Van Gordon W, Shonin E, Richardson M. Mindfulness and nature. Mindfulness. (2018) 9:1655–58. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0883-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. White PM, Alcock I, Grellier J, Wheeler BW, Hartig T, Warber SL, et al. Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and well-being. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:7730. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Doherty TJ, Clayton S. The psychological impacts of global climate change. Am Psychol. (2011) 66:265–76. doi: 10.1037/a0023141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Clayton S, Manning CM, Krygsman K, Speiser M. Mental health and our changing climate: Impacts, implications, and guidance. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association and ecoAmerica (2017).

Google Scholar

6. Mullins JT, White C. Temperature and mental health: Evidence from the spectrum of mental health outcomes. J Health Economics. (2019) 68:102240. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2019.102240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Généreux M, Lansard A-L, Maltais D, Gachon P. (2020). Institut national de santé publique du Québec.

Google Scholar

8. Young L. Canadian kids aren’t playing outside enough, experts say, and it’s harming their health (2019). Available online at: https://globalnews.ca/news/5987487/outdoor-plAy-children-Canada/.

Google Scholar

9. Save the Children. Only one in four children play out regularly on their street compared to almost three-quarters of their grandparents generation (2023). Available online at: https://www.savethechildren.org.uk/news/media-centre/press-releases/children-today-62-percent-less-likely-to-play-outside-than-their#:~:text=Just%2027%25%20of%20children%20said,in%20just%20a%20few%20generations (Accessed March 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

10. Hickman C, Marks E, Pihkala P, Clayton S, Lewandowski RE, Mayall EE, et al. Climate anxiety in children and young people and their beliefs about government responses to climate change: a global survey. Lancet Planetary Health. (2021) 5:e863–73. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(21)00278-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. IPCC. Climate change 2022: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability. Contribution of working group II to the sixth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change. Pörtner H-O, Roberts DC, Tignor M, Poloczanska ES, Mintenbeck K, Alegría A, Craig M, Langsdorf S, Löschke S, Möller V, Okem A, Rama B, editors. Cambridge, UK and New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press (2022). 3056. doi: 10.1017/9781009325844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Chirac J. Déclaration de J. Jacques Chirac, Président de la République, sur la situation critique de l’environnement planétaire et les propositions de la France pour un développement durable, Johannesburg, le 2 Septembre 2002 (2002). Available online at: https://www.elysee.fr/jacques-chirac/2002/09/02/declaration-de-m-jacques-chirac-president-de-la-republique-sur-la-situation-critique-de-lenvironnement-planetaire-et-les-propositions-de-la-France-pour-un-developpement-durable-johannesburg-le-2-septembre-2002 (Accessed January 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

13. Thunberg G. Address at World Economic Forum: Our House Is On Fire. Davos, Switzerland: Iowa State University, Archives of Women’s Political Communication (2019). Available at: https://awpc.cattcenter.iastate.edu/2019/12/02/address-at-davos-our-house-is-on-fire-jan-25-2019/.

Google Scholar

14. Clayton S, Manning C. Psychology and climate change: Human perceptions, impacts, and responses. Cambridge, MA: Elsevier Academic Press. (2018).

Google Scholar

15. Gifford R. The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. Am Psychol. (2011) 66:290–302. doi: 10.1037/a0023566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gifford R, Nilsson A. Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: A review. Int J Psychol. (2014) 49:141–57. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gifford R, Lacroix K, Chen A. Understanding responses to climate change. In: Psychology and climate change. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (2018). p. 161–83. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-813130-5.00006-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Antal M, Drews S. Nature as relationship partner: an old frame revisited. Environ Educ Res. (2015) 21:7:1056–78. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2014.971715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Thibodeau PH, McPherson Frantz C, Berretta M. The earth is our home: systemic metaphors to redefine our relationship with nature. Climatic Change. (2017) 142:287–300. doi: 10.1007/s10584-017-1926-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lakoff G, Johnson M. Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (1980).

Google Scholar

21. Lakoff G. Why it matters how we frame the environment. Environ Communication. (2010) 4:70–81. doi: 10.1080/17524030903529749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Pihkala P. Toward a taxonomy of climate emotions. Front Climate. (2022) 3:738154. doi: 10.3389/fclim.2021.738154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, and self-regulation (Norton series on interpersonal neurobiology). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company (2011). Kindle Edition.

Google Scholar

24. Porges SW. Polyvagal theory: A science of safety. Front Integr Neurosci. (2022) 16:871227. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.871227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Snyder H. Literature review as a research methodology: an overview and guidelines. J Bus. Res. (2019) 104:333–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.07.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rozuel C, Bellehumeur CR. Contextualizing eco-anxiety and eco-anger: tentative responses to visceral and numinous emotions. J analytical Psychol. (2022) 67:1431–51. doi: 10.1111/1468-5922.12870

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Shaw C, Nerlich B. Metaphor as a mechanism of global climate change governance: a study of international policies 1992–2012. Ecol Economics. (2015) 109:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mangat R, Dalby S. Climate and wartalk: Metaphors, imagination, transformation. Elem Sci Anth. (2018) 6:58. doi: 10.1525/elementa.313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Van der Hel S, Hellsten I, Steen G. Tipping points and climate change: metaphor between science and the media. Environ Communication. (2018) 12:605–20. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2017.1410198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pihkala P. Anxiety and the ecological crisis: an analysis of eco-anxiety and climate anxiety. Sustainability. (2020) 12:7836. 1-20. doi: 10.3390/su12197836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Larson B. Metaphors for environmental sustainability: Redefining our relationship with nature. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press (2011). doi: 10.2307/j.ctt5vm557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. IPCC. Climate change 2021: the physical science basis. Contribution of working group I to the sixth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change. Masson-Delmotte V, Zhai P, Pirani A, Connors SL, Péan C, Berger S, Caud N, Chen Y, Goldfarb L, Gomis MI, Huang M, Leitzell K, Lonnoy E, Matthews JBR, Maycock TK, Waterfield T, Yelekçi O, Yu R, Zhou B, editors. Cambridge, United Kingdom and New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press (2021). 2391. doi: 10.1017/9781009157896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Armstrong AK, Krasny ME, Schuldt JP. Communicating climate change: A guide for educators. Ithaca: Cornell University Press (2018) p. 70–4.

Google Scholar

34. Gibbs R. The poetics of mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press (1994).

Google Scholar

35. Morgan G. Images of organization. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage (2006).

Google Scholar

36. Lule J. War and its metaphors: news language and the prelude to war in Iraq 2003. Journalism Studies. (2004) 5(2):179–90. doi: 10.1080/1461670042000211168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Musolff A. Metaphor scenarios in public discourse. Metaphor and Symbol (2006) 21(1):23–38. doi: 10.1207/s15327868ms2101_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gilford D, Moser S, DePodwin B, Moulton R, Watson S. The emotional toll of climate change on science professionals. Eos. (2019) 100. doi: 10.1029/2019EO137460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hine DW, Phillips WJ, Cooksey R, Reser JP, Nunn P, Marks ADG, et al. Preaching to different choirs: how to motivate dismissive, uncommitted, and alarmed audiences to adapt to climate change? Glob Environ Change. (2016) 36:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2015.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rucińska Z, Fondelli T. Enacting metaphors in systemic collaborative therapy. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:867235. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.867235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Anderson M. Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artif Intell. (2003) 149:91–130. doi: 10.1016/S0004-3702(03)00054-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Albrecht GA. Earth emotions: new words for a new world. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press (2019). doi: 10.7591/j.ctvfc5563

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ritchie TD, Sedikides C, Wildschut T, Arndt J, Gidron Y. Self-concept clarity mediates the relation between stress and subjective well-being. Self Identity. (2011) 10, 4:493–508. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2010.493066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Geurtzen N, Keijsers GPJ, Karremans JC, Tiemens BG, Hutschemaekers GJM. Patients' perceived lack of goal clarity in psychological treatments: Scale development and negative correlates. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2020) 27:915–24. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Clayton S. Climate anxiety: psychological responses to climate change. J Anxiety Disord. (2020) 74:102263. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Frankl VE. Man’s search for meaning. New York, NY: Washington Square Press (1985).

Google Scholar

47. Kurth C, Pihkala P. Eco-anxiety: What it is and why it matters. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:981814. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.981814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lacroix K, Gifford R. Psychological barriers to energy conservation behavior: the role of worldviews and climate change risk perception. Environ Behav. (2018) 50:749–80. doi: 10.1177/0013916517715296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Cannon WB. The wisdom of the body. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. (1932).

Google Scholar

50. Taylor SE. Tend and befriend theory. In: Van Lange PAM, Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET, editors. Handbook of theories of social psychology. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications Ltd (2012). p. 32–49. doi: 10.4135/9781446249215.n3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hlay JK, Johnson BN, Levy KN. Attachment security predicts tend-and-befriend behaviors: A replication. Evolutionary Behav Sci. (2022) 17(2):103–22. doi: 10.1037/ebs0000284. Advance online publication.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. O’Neill S, Nicholson-Cole S. Fear won’t do it" promoting positive engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. Sci Commun. (2009) 30:355–79. doi: 10.1177/1075547008329201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Stern PC. Fear and hope in climate messages. Nat Climate Change. (2012) 2:572–3. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1610

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chapman DA, Lickel B, Markowitz EM. Reassessing emotion in climate change communication. Nat Climate Change. (2017) 7:850–852. doi: 10.1038/s41558-017-0021-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. de Vries G. Public communication as a tool to implement environmental policies. Soc Issues Policy Rev. (2020) 14:244–72. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS. Understanding (and reducing) inaction on climate change. Soc Issues Policy Rev. (2020) 14:3–35. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12058.22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Ojala M. Hope in the face of climate change: Associations with environmental engagement and student perceptions of teachers’ emotion communication style and future orientation. J Environ Educ. (2015) 46(3):133–48. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1021662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Marlon JR, Bloodhart B, Ballew MT, Rolfe-Redding J, Roser-Renouf C, Leiserowitz A, et al. How hope and doubt affect climate change mobilization. Front Commun. (2019) 4:20. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Brosch T. Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: A review. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2021) 42:15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bloodhart B, Swim JK, Dicicco E. “Be worried, be VERY worried:“ preferences for and impacts of negative emotional climate change communication. Front Commun. (2019) 3:63. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Wright E. Climate stress lives in the body. Medium (2021). Available at: https://emily-wright.medium.com/climate-stress-lives-in-the-body-41df4af3e902.

Google Scholar

62. Dana D. Polyvagal exercises for safety and connection: 50 client-centered practices (Norton series on interpersonal neurobiology). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company (2020). Kindle Edition.

Google Scholar

63. Seligman ME. Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York, NY: Atria Paperback (2012).

Google Scholar

64. Kern M, Waters L, Alder A, White M. Assessing employee wellbeing in schools using a multifaceted approach: Associations with physical health, life satisfaction and professional thriving. Psychology. (2014) 5:500–13. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.56060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Fredrickson BL. What good are positive emotions? Rev Gen Psychol. (1998) 2:300–19. doi: 10.1037//1089-2680.2.3.300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Fredrickson BL. Positivity : Top-notch research reveals the 3 to 1 ratio that will change your life. Three Rivers Press: New York (2009).

Google Scholar

67. Burghardt GM. The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2005). doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3229.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Gray P. Free to learn: why unleashing the instinct to play will make our children happier, more self-reliant, and better students for life. New York: Basic Books (2013).

Google Scholar

69. Francis P. Laudato si. Encyclical Letter of the Holy Father (On care for our common home) (2015). Available online at: http://w2.vatican.va/content/Francesco/en/encyclicals/documents/papaFrancesco_20150524_enciclica-laudato-si.html (Accessed November 10, 2022).

Google Scholar

70. Durand G. Les structures anthropologiques de l’imaginaire. 12e éd. Paris, France: Dunod (2016). doi: 10.3917/dunod.duran.2016.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Bain PG, Hornsey MJ, Bongiorno R, Jeffries C. Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers. Nat Climate Change. (2012) 2:600–3. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Bain PG, Milfont TL, Kashima Y, Bilewicz M, Doron G, Garðarsdóttir RB, et al. Co-benefits of addressing climate change can motivate action around the world. Nat Climate Change. (2016) 6:154–7. doi: 10.1038/nclimate2814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Dodgen D, Donato D, Kelly N, La Greca A, Morganstein J, Reser J, et al. Mental health and well-being. The impacts of climate change on human health in the United States: A scientific assessment. Washington, DC: U.S. Global Change Research Program (2016) p. 217–46. doi: 10.7930/J0TX3C9H

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Kałwak W, Weihgold V. The relationality of ecological emotions: an interdisciplinary critique of individual resilience as psychology's response to the climate crisis. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:823620. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.823620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Maibach EW, Leiserowitz A, Roser-Renouf C, Mertz CK. Identifying like-minded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: an audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PloS One. (2011) 6:e17571. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Gillipsie S. Climate crisis and consciousness. United States: Routledge (2020).

Google Scholar

77. Tanhan A. Utilizing Online Photovoice (OPV) methodology to address biopsychosocial spiritual economic issues and wellbeing during COVID-19: Adapting OPV to Turkish. Turkish. Studies. (2020) 15:1029–86. doi: 10.7827/TurkishStudies.44451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Tanhan A, Strack RW. Online photovoice to explore and advocate for Muslim biopsychosocial spiritual wellbeing and issues: Ecological systems theory and ally development. Curr Psychol. (2020) 39:2010–25. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00692-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Doyumğaç İ, Tanhan A, Kıymaz MS. Understanding the most important facilitators and barriers for online education during COVID-19 through online photovoice methodology. Int J Higher Educ. (2021) 10:166–90. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v10n1p166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Volkodav TV, Volkodav EV. Harnessing the power of online photovoice in cross-cultural English language education: a study among students of psychology and pedagogy faculty. Koncept. (2023) 10:166–84. doi: 10.24412/2304-120X-2023-11102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Subasi Y. College belonging among university students during COVID-19: An Online Interpretative Phenomenological (OIPA) Perspective. J Happiness Health. (2023) 3:109–26. doi: 10.47602/johah.v3i2.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Marks E, Atkins E, Garrett JK, Abrams JF, Shackleton D, Hennessy L, et al. Stories of hope created together: A pilot, school-based workshop for sharing eco-emotions and creating an actively hopeful vision of the future. Front Psychol. (2023) 13:1076322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1076322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Ojala M. Hope and climate change: the importance of hope for environmental engagement among young people. Environ Educ Res. (2012) 18:625–42. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2011.637157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hickman C. We need to (find a way to) talk about … eco-anxiety. J Soc Work Pract. (2020) 34:411–24. doi: 10.1080/02650533.2020.1844166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: climate change, well-being, environmental metaphors, climate metaphors, human-environment relationship, climate engagement, Polyvagal Theory, eco-emotions

Citation: Bellehumeur CR and Carignan L-M (2024) On proposing relational environmental metaphors to stimulate engagement and foster well-being in the midst of climate change. Front. Psychiatry 15:1377205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1377205

Received: 26 January 2024; Accepted: 26 April 2024;
Published: 16 May 2024.

Edited by:

Andrew Kemp, Swansea University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Ahmet Tanhan, Economic and Social Research Center (ESAM), Türkiye
Kevin Moore, Lincoln University, New Zealand

Copyright © 2024 Bellehumeur and Carignan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian R. Bellehumeur, cbellehumeur@ustpaul.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.