Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 August 2024
Sec. Autism
This article is part of the Research Topic Women in Psychiatry 2023: Autism View all 11 articles

Head circumference growth in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: trend and clinical correlates in the first five years of life

Lara Cirnigliaro&#x;Lara Cirnigliaro1†Luisa ClericLuisa Clericò1Lorenza Chiara RussoLorenza Chiara Russo1Adriana PratoAdriana Prato1Manuela CarusoManuela Caruso2Renata RizzoRenata Rizzo1Rita Barone,*Rita Barone1,3*
  • 1Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 2Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetology Center, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 3Research Unit of Rare Diseases and Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Oasi Research Institute-IRCCS, Troina, Italy

Background: Macrocephaly is described in almost 15% of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Relationships between head growth trajectories and clinical findings in ASD children show a high degree of variability, highlighting the complex heterogeneity of the disorder.

Objectives: The aim of this study was to measure differences of the early growth trajectory of head circumference (HC) in children with ASD and macrocephaly compared to ASD normocephalic children, examining clinical correlates in the two groups of patients.

Methods: HC data were collected from birth to 5 years of age in a sample of children with a confirmed diagnosis of ASD. Participants were classified into two groups: ASD macrocephaly (ASD-M, Z-scores ≥1.88 in at least two consecutive HC measurements), and ASD non-macrocephaly (ASD-N). Based on the distribution of HC measurements (Z-scores), five age groups were identified for the longitudinal study. Developmental and behavioral characteristics of the ASD-M children compared to the ASD-N group were compared by using standardized scores.

Results: 20,8% of the children sample met criteria for macrocephaly. HC values became indicative of macrocephaly in the ASD-M group at the age range from 1 to 6 months, and persisted thereafter throughout the first five years of age. ASD-M children showed significantly higher developmental quotients of Griffiths III B and D subscales compared to ASD-N group. No significant differences in the severity of ASD symptoms assessed by ADOS-2 were observed between ASD-M and ASD-N groups.

Conclusion: In this study HC size from birth to 5 years links to accelerated HC growth rate as early as the first 6 months of age in children with ASD and macrocephaly, preceding the onset and diagnosis of ASD. We found that in early childhood, children with ASD-M may exhibit some advantages in language and social communication and emotional skills without differences in autism severity, when compared with age-matched normocephalic ASD children. Longitudinal analyses are required to catch-up prospectively possible relationships between head size as proxy measure of brain development and neuro-developmental and behavioral features in children with ASD.

1 Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social communication impairment and restricted, repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behaviors, interests or activities (1). The rising global prevalence rate of ASD (2) and the complexity of the ASD etiology, clinical features and developmental trajectories, have prompted intensive research to identify specific biological markers and endophenotypes for earlier diagnosis and treatment (35). Accelerated head growth associated with brain enlargement is a commonly reported biological feature of ASD, affecting 14%–34% of patients (4, 610). Head circumference (HC) is an accurate, rapid, and inexpensive tool used by research as a proxy measure of brain growth in the assessment of children with ASD (11).

Macrocephaly (or macrocrania) is clinically defined as an abnormally large head with an occipitofrontal circumference (OFC) greater than the 97th percentile.

A higher frequency of head overgrowth has been reported in children with ASD compared to typically developing children (TD) at varying age ranges, emphasizing a certain variability HC with respect to gender and age in ASD subjects (5). The abnormal head growth trajectory starts early in postnatal life and continues until at least 5 years of age (12). Early cerebral overgrowth in children with ASD may be followed by volumetric regression throughout the childhood (13). However, a recent longitudinal study showed the persistence of brain enlargement from early to late childhood in a subset of patients with ASD compared to age and sex-matched control subjects. This finding was correlated with a greater increase in white matter volume and a slower decrease in grey matter volume over time in ASD patients (14). Neuroimaging studies described a generalized enlargement of frontal, temporal and parietal lobes, involving both gray and white matter (15) or mainly limited to the frontal lobe gray matter (13, 16).

Altogether, previous findings have generally been interpreted as reflecting excessive neurogenesis/neuronal proliferation and inappropriate synaptic pruning, which may underlie the increased brain size in patient subsets with ASD (17, 18). These neuropathological abnormalities would also result in cortical surface area overgrowth (19), especially in cortical areas related to sensory information processing (middle occipital cortex) in children at high risk for ASD than in those at low or no risk for ASD. Thus over-expansion of cortical surface area and related head size increase may represent an early event in a cascade leading to brain overgrowth and emerging ASD symptoms (20), supporting identification of children at risk of ASD with or without a history of regression (8, 10, 21).

The timing of HC increase in children with ASD and its relationship to the appearance of behavioral symptoms is unclear so far. Overall, the results of studies are inconsistent regarding age, gender and intelligence quotient (IQ) effects on the HC growth rate and the relationship of macrocephaly to the clinical features of ASD and their severity (8, 2224).

Clinical variables have been explored in order to identify meaningful subgroups that may share common genetic underpinning (8). Macrocephaly has been described as a clinical indicator of genetic subtypes of ASD. Historically, mutations in the gene phosphatase and tensin homolog (PTEN) were detected in a subset of individuals with large head and ASD (25).

Dysregulation of brain developmental processes due to multiple genomic variations in genes involved in cell proliferation (e.g., PTEN, mTOR pathway), chromatin remodeling (e.g., chromodomain helicase DNA binding protein 8, CHD8), protein transcription and translation and biological adhesion (WNT pathway) has been associated with ASD and macrocephaly (2630), in order to provide elucidation of genotype–phenotype correlations and new insights into different subtypes of ASD. Approximately 17–20% of children with ASD and macrocephaly have pathogenic PTEN mutations (27) showing a distinct neurobehavioral phenotype with cognitive impairment extended to adaptive behavior, sensory deficits, repetitive behavior and decreased memory function (3133).

In the effort to correlate different patterns of brain growth during development with the heterogeneous neurodevelopment trajectories of ASD in different subsets of affected individuals, the need for longitudinal analyses has been highlighted to carry out meaningful phenotyping (12, 22).

The present study was undertaken to assess longitudinal changes in head circumference by cross-sectional analyses in a sample of ASD children, with the aim of detecting significant differences in the growth trajectory of head circumference in children with macrocephaly compared to normocephalic children, in the first five years of life. We foresee that this study may identify a possible endophenotype of ASD associated with macrocephaly examining clinical correlates in the two groups of patients.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design

This cross-sectional study was performed in two phases. In the first one, HC measurements were collected from birth to 5 years of age in a sample of children with ASD consecutively observed during the study period. In the second phase, we compared ASD children with macrocephaly (ASD-M) and normocephalic children (ASD-N) using neurodevelopmental and neurobehavioral assessment tools.

Data used for this study were collected from the clinical files and obtained as part of clinical protocols for patients with ASD. The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee of Catania University Hospital. All procedures performed in the present study were in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments (2013). Written informed consent was obtained from both parents of each participant.

2.1.1 Participants and data collection procedure

At study entry, 79 subjects were recruited, between October 2022 and July 2023, from a clinical population with ASD, diagnosed at the Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Catania. Longitudinal auxological data (HC, height and weight) were collected from birth to 5 years of age using medical records from the Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit. Data on gestational age at birth were also collected.

The Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) a clinical diagnosis of ASD according to DSM-5 criteria and all testing measures, including the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2); 2) age at study time ≤ 5 years; 3) at least 3 HC measurements in the time period between 0 and 5 years of age.

Exclusion criteria included: 1) general overgrowth (head circumference, height and weight >97th percentile/>2 standard deviations, SD); 2) presence of microcephaly (head circumference <3rd percentile/<-2 SD); 3) lack of repeated measures of HC in the first five years of age.

All HC data refer to measurements obtained manually, using a non-stretchable tape measure placed over the maximum fronto-occipital head circumference.

HC, body length and weight measures at birth were converted into percentiles using the Italian Neonatal Study (INeS) charts, promoted by the Italian Society of Neonatology.

The Growth4 Software was used to calculate percentiles and SD values for each available measurement from the postnatal period to 5 years of age, according to the World Health Organization (WHO) growth charts.

HC measurements were normalized for sex and age by conversion to Z-scores based on the WHO mean values for healthy infants (World Health Organization 2006, Child Growth Standards).

Macrocephaly was defined as a HC greater than the 97th percentile, that is more than 1.88 SD above the normative mean (z score > 1.88) (8, 24, 34).

Participants were classified into two groups: ASD macrocephaly (ASD-M, Z-scores ≥1.88 in at least two consecutive HC measurements), and ASD non-macrocephaly (ASD-N).

Based on the distribution of HC measurements (Z-scores) in the study sample, five age ranges were identified: birth, 1–6 months, 8–18 months, 20–32 months, 33–60 months. In each age range, ASD-M and ASD-N patients were matched for age and sex.

All participants underwent neuropsychiatric assessment and clinical data were compared between the two study groups ASD-M/ASD-N. The clinical diagnosis of ASD was confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, second edition (ADOS-2). The Griffiths Scales of Childhood Development - 3rd edition (Griffiths III) was administered in order to assess the psychomotor development.

2.2 Standardized measures

Symptoms of ASD were established using the gold-standard tools for ASD diagnosis: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (35) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) (36). The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured, standardized assessment of core deficits in ASD. It contains five modules that are differentiated by children’s developmental and language levels. In the present study participants completed the Toddler Module (designed specifically for children 12–30 months old with limited language), the Module 1 (used for children aged from 31 months who do not consistently use phrase speech) or the Module 2 in a minority of children using phrase speech, but who were not verbally fluent. To allow comparisons among different modules, ADOS-2 scores (total score, Social Affect, SA, and Restricted and Repetitive Behaviour, RRB, scores) were converted to respective calibrated severity scores (CSS 1–10 indicating absence to severe autism) (3739).

An overall measure of children’s psychomotor development was provided by the Griffiths III assessment across five subscales (40). Subscale A (Foundations of Learning) assesses the ability of learning; subscale B (Language and Communication) evaluates the development of both receptive and expressive language and social communication abilities; subscale C (Eye and Hand Coordination) assesses visual perception and fine motor skills; subscale D (Personal-Social-Emotional) evaluates child’s ability to adapt, personal autonomy and early social and emotional development through items measuring imitation, joint attention, emotional recognition and empathy; subscale E (Gross Motor domains) refers to the child’s early development of postural control, gross body coordination, balance and visual-spatial coordination. Subscale raw scores and general development raw scores are calculated to determine the Developmental Age, Scaled Score and Development Quotient, according to the norm tables.

2.3 Statistical analysis

In the first phase of the study, the one-way ANOVA statistical test was initially applied to compare ages (months) as means (M) and standard deviations (SD) in the five subgroups (age ranges). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was then performed to find out possible significant differences on HC measurements (z-scores) between the two groups (ASD-M/ASD-N) in each age range.

In the second phase of the study, we used the independent samples t-test to compare the mean scores on each clinical assessment measure between the two groups (ASD-M/ASD-N).

The statistical significance level α was established at 0.05. All statistical tests were performed by using SPSS version 27 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA, IBM, Somers, NY, USA).

3 Results

3.1 Comparison of HC growth in children with ASD in the period from 0 to 5 years of age

Out of an initial sample of 79 subjects with ASD, thirty-four patients were excluded due to the unavailability of repeated HC measurement during the age 0–5 years. Forty-five children with a confirmed diagnosis of ASD (male to female ratio = 5:1; mean age: 4.4 ± 1.1) were included in two groups (ASD-M/ASD-N) based on HC measurements (Z-score). Ten children (20,8%) met criteria for macrocephaly (Z-scores ≥1.88). Routine laboratory analyses, extended metabolic screening and array-CGH analyses yielded normal results. Two patients were diagnosed with germline PTEN mutations (c.697C>T/p.Arg233 and c.62T>G/pPhe21Cys, respectively).

Demographic characteristics in ASD-M/ASD-N groups are reported in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic characteristics in ASD-M/ASD-N groups and in the total sample.

At each study interval, the mean age was not significantly different between the two groups ASD-M/ASD-N (birth: F(37)=2.28, p=0.14; 1–6 months: F(17)=2.74, p=0.12; 8–18 months: F(18)=0.92, p =0.35; 20–32 months: F(23)=2.82, p =0.11; 33–60 months: F(28)=0.16, p=0.69).

HC Z-score data in the ASD-M group compared to the ASD-N group at each age range are reported in Table 2. At birth HC measurements were in the normal range in both groups. However, mean Z-scores were significantly higher in the ASD-M group than in the ASD-N group (F=10.2, p=0.004).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Head circumference z-scores data in ASD-M group compared to ASD-N group at each age interval.

We found a significant increase of HC in the ASD-M group compared to the ASD-N group at each age interval (Table 2) suggesting that ASD-M patients showed an excessive HC growth in the study period than ASD-N subjects (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Differences in HC growth between ASD-M and ASD-N children from birth to 60 months. The box plot shows that children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and macrocephaly (ASD-M, red) had a more rapid HC growth than ASD normocephalic subjects (ASD-N, blue). The two patients harboring PTEN mutations (outliers) were not included in the HC growth analysis.

3.2 Neurobehavioral phenotype comparison between ASD-M and ASD-N children

In the second phase of the study, we sought to determine whether and to what extent the developmental and behavioral characteristics of the ASD-M group differed from the ASD-N group, by comparing the mean scores obtained on each clinical assessment tool (Table 3). At the time of assessment, chronological age (CA) (ASD-M mean age: 29.7 ± 9.4 months; ASD-N mean age: 34.6 ± 9.3 months) was not significantly different between the two groups (t=1.4, p=0.08), ruling out that possible clinical differences might be related to different CA in the group comparison.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Comparison of the Griffiths III and ADOS-2 scores between ASD-M and ASD-N study groups.

We first investigated about differences in the severity of autism symptoms by using the ADOS-2 total CSS. No significant differences were found comparing ASD-M children with ASD-N children (SA CSS: t=0.3, p=0.76; RRB CSS: t=0.8, p=0.81; CSS TOT: t=0.2, p=0.85).

As to the developmental profile on the Griffith III scales, the General Development Quotient (GDQ) was below the normal range in both groups, but was significantly higher in ASD-M children (mean GDQ = 68.2± 18.11) than in the ASD-N group (mean GDQ = 57.03 ± 12.77) (t= 2.16, p=0.02).

Then, development quotients (DQ) on each subscale of the Griffiths III were compared between the ASD-M and ASD-N groups. No significant differences were found for the subscale A (t=0.9, p=0.38), subscale C (t=1.1, p=0.27) and subscale E DQ (t=1.1, p=0.28). ASD-M children scored significantly higher on the subscale B (t=3.4, p=0.0016) and subscale D (t=2.2, p=0.032) than ASD-N children (Table 3).

4 Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the growth trajectory of HC in a sample of ASD children in the first five years of life. We sought to determine whether macrocephaly was correlated with other clinical characteristics in ASD children profiling a possible endophenotype associated with macrocephaly.

Macrocephaly was consistently reported in children with ASD than in neurotypical peers (5, 8, 10, 41), with an overall prevalence rate of 15.7% vs 3%, respectively (4).

In this study 20.8% of the total sample of ASD children developed macrocephaly during the first 5 years of life, a finding consistent with rates reported in the ASD literature (11, 34, 42, 43).

We found that ASD patients with macrocephaly (ASD-M group) presented a significantly larger HC size at birth compared to normocephalic ASD children (ASD-N group), although birth Z-scores were in the normal range in both groups. This result might indicate a possible prenatal brain overgrowth, consistent with observed late-gestational fetal HC overgrowth among children with ASD (44, 45). Most studies reported normal HC at birth in ASD infants (6, 10, 12, 4648), while a few studies described smaller HC at birth with an overt increase in later months in ASD infants compared to neurotypical children (11, 23).

In the current ASD-M sample, head circumference was found to be consistent with the definition of macrocephaly from the first six months of life (F=7.7, p=0.014) and persisted thereafter throughout the first five years of age. In line with previous studies, we found that an accelerated HC growth rate is present in the first year of life and precedes the onset and diagnosis in children with autism spectrum disorder (8, 10, 20, 21, 23). Increased rates of head growth in early childhood (13) were mantained until the age of 5 years in children with ASD (12, 46, this study) and were not followed by volumetric regression until at least late childhood (age 11) (14).

Brain size is positively correlated with cognitive function in typically developing individuals, however previous data are inconsistent regarding neurocognitive development in ASD children with macrocephaly (22). Moreover, brain and behavior relationships may develop at different times during development, illustrating the need of longitudinal analyses to achieve meaningful phenotyping. Notably, we found significant differences of ASD-M children compared to the ASD-N group, assessed by the Griffiths III developmental scales.

Increasing evidence suggests the validity of Griffiths III in describing specific developmental profiles in children with ASD. Recently different developmental profiles on the Griffiths III have been detected in children with ASD with respect to children with developmental delay (DD) (49). Griffiths III B and D-subscales, which probe language, social and emotional skills, have been shown to be the most impaired in ASD and the most predictive for ASD risk (4953). In this regard, we recently developed a novel level-2 ASD screener, the Developmental Autism Early Screening (DAES), by selecting the most predictive Griffiths III B- and D-subscales items for ASD risk in the first three years of age, which may differentiate children with ASD-risk from their peers with DD or with typical development (TD) (53).

In the present study we found that the Griffiths III DQ on the B and D subscale were significantly higher in ASD-M than in ASD-N group. This indicates that at the study time, children with ASD and macrocephaly showed less impairment in language, communication, social and emotional skills compared with age-matched normocephalic ASD children.

An advantage in language development and general IQ measures was reported in ASD patients with macrocephaly (9). Abnormal acceleration of HC in early life was associated with better adaptive functioning and less impairment in social and behavioral domains in macrocephalic children compared to normocephalic children with ASD. This observation led to the hypothesis that the accelerated head growth in early childhood may be a protective reaction in response to pathognomonic neurodevelopmental processes that contribute to ASD (8).

On the other hand, additional studies yielded conflicting results or failed to detect significant differences in the DQ and IQ scores between macrocephalic and normocephalic ASD patients (12, 34, 43). Lower IQ scores might be associated with a history of language and social skills regression in ASD children with macrocephaly (10, 22, 54). In some instances, better non-verbal than verbal performances were correlated with increased head size in ASD patients (41).

These discrepancies in the clinical correlates of HC in ASD may reflect abnormalities in neurodevelopmental trajectories of ASD children whereby different skill domains become increasingly uneven over time (41).

The association between the peculiar neurodevelopmental profile of ASD-M children and the underlying neuroanatomical abnormalities and pathophysiological mechanisms needs to be further investigated. The degree, rate and/or duration of the brain overgrowth may be related to neuroanatomical and clinical outcome of ASD neurophenotypes. An inappropriate synaptic pruning or arborization with increased axon and dendrite number and size produces too many connections in various brain areas (18). The better performance of ASD-M children in language, social and emotional skills might suggest that the brain areas involved in these domains are not directly affected by abnormal growth processes or, alternatively, are more active due to compensatory mechanisms at certain times of neurodevelopment.

Otherwise, we found no significant differences between ASD-M and ASD-N groups in the severity of ASD symptoms assessed by ADOS-2. This result is consistent with other studies that did not identify differences on ADOS (10, 12, 22), ADI-R (24) and CARS (55), used to measure ASD severity in the studied groups of ASD patients with macrocephaly.

There are certain limitations in this study. The sample is relatively small and available measurements of head circumference are not uniform in all considered age ranges. Moreover, we focused on the first 5 years of life. Further investigations are needed to compare the two groups (ASD-M/ASD-N) at older ages, in order to detect differences in head growth and developmental trajectories. A few studies have suggested that the differences between ASD children with macrocephaly and ASD normocephalic children may persist at older ages (7, 56). It is not clear whether a brain size normalization occurs in adolescence: discrepancies between studies might be explained by the age heterogeneity of participants and a selection bias due to the inclusion of more compliant patients with higher IQs in neuroimaging studies (14). Imaging and EEG studies may be informative to further investigating additional features of the ASD-M endophenotype related to the neuro-behavioral profile. The co-occurrence of temporal EEG abnormalities, regression and macrocephaly has been described in a previous study, in order to define anatomic or pathophysiologic subtypes of ASD, highlighting the crucial role of the temporal region in processing language and social stimuli (57).

5 Conclusions

Alterations in brain organization and developmental trajectory might be strong biological indicators of ASD subtypes that would be associated with different patterns of behavioral symptoms or co-occurring conditions (22). In this study HC measurements from birth to 5 years links to early accelerated HC growth rate as early as the first 6 months of age in children with ASD and macrocephaly, preceding the onset and diagnosis of ASD. This observation concurs with recent findings illustrating that sub-regional brain fetal measurements at 20 weeks and fetal HC at 28 weeks were positively associated with Q-CHAT scores at 18–20 months of age (45). In addition, we demonstrate that in early childhood, children with ASD-M may exhibit some advantages in language and social communication and emotional skills without differences in autism severity, when compared with age-matched normocephalic ASD children. In this regard, we emphasize the need of longitudinal analyses to catch-up prospectively possible relationships between head size as proxy measure of brain development and neuro-developmental and behavioral features. Prospective investigations in larger samples may well consider including ad-hoc genetic and technical investigations to understand the precise nature of the association between accelerated head growth in ASD and the clinical phenotype. This future perspective may increase knowledge about clinical outcome and guide the therapeutic choices.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comitato Etico Locale Catania 1. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the minor(s)’ legal guardian/next of kin for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

LaC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LuC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. LR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. AP: Writing – review & editing. MC: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. RR: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. RB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all participants and their families for their collaboration.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text Revision. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2022). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Maenner MJ, Warren Z, Williams AR, Amoakohene E, Bakian AV, Bilder DA, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2020. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2023) 72:1–14. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss7202a1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sacco R, Curatolo P, Manzi B, Militerni R, Bravaccio C, Frolli A, et al. Principal pathogenetic components and biological endophenotypes in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. (2010) 3:237–52. doi: 10.1002/aur.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sacco R, Gabriele S, Persico AM. Head circumference and brain size in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 234:239–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Crucitti J, Hyde C, Enticott PG, Stokes MA. Head circumference trends in autism between 0 and 100 months. Autism. (2020) 24:1726–39. doi: 10.1177/1362361320921037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology. (2001) 57:245–54. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.2.245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, Sparks BF, Singh N. Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology. (2002) . 59:175–83. doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.2.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Dementieva YA, Vance DD, Donnelly SL, Elston LA, Wolpert CM, Ravan SA, et al. Accelerated head growth in early development of individuals with autism. Pediatr Neurol. (2005) 32:102–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2004.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Sacco R, Militerni R, Frolli A, Bravaccio C, Gritti A, Elia M, et al. Clinical, morphological, and biochemical correlates of head circumference in autism. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 62:1038–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Nordahl CW, Lange N, Li DD, Barnett LA, Lee A, Buonocore MH, et al. Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschool-age boys with autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2011) 108:20195–200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107560108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mraz KD, Green J, Dumont-Mathieu T, Makin S, Fein D. Correlates of head circumference growth in infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Neurol. (2007) 22:700–13. doi: 10.1177/0883073807304005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Libero LE, Nordahl CW, Li DD, Ferrer E, Rogers SJ, Amaral DG. Persistence of megalencephaly in a subgroup of young boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2016) 9:1169–82. doi: 10.1002/aur.1643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Redcay E, Courchesne E. When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 58:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Lee JK, Andrews DS, Ozonoff S, Solomon M, Rogers S, Amaral DG, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of cerebral growth across childhood in boys and girls with autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 90:286–94. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sparks BF, Friedman SD, Shaw DW, Aylward EH, Echelard D, Artru AA, et al. Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology. (2002) 59:184–92. doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.2.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Carper RA, Courchesne E. Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:126–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gilbert J, Man HY. Fundamental elements in autism: from neurogenesis and neurite growth to synaptic plasticity. Front Cell Neurosci. (2017) 11:359. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Accogli A, Geraldo AF, Piccolo G, Riva A, Scala M, Balagura G, et al. Diagnostic approach to macrocephaly in children. Front Pediatr. (2022) 9:794069. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.794069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, Styner M, Chappell C, Smith RG, et al. Early brain overgrowth in autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2011) 68:467–76. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hazlett HC, Gu H, Munsell BC, Kim SH, Styner M, Wolff JJ, et al. Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder. Nature. (2017) 542:348–51. doi: 10.1038/nature21369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Webb JS, Nalty T, Munson J, Brock C, Abbott R, Dawson G. Rate of head circumference growth as a function of autism diagnosis and history of autistic regression. J Child Neurol. (2007) 22:1182–90. doi: 10.1177/0883073807306263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Amaral DG, Li D, Libero L, Solomon M, Van de Water J, Mastergeorge A, et al. In pursuit of neurophenotypes: The consequences of having autism and a big brain. Autism Res. (2017) 10:711–22. doi: 10.1002/aur.1755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Courchesne E, Carper R, Akshoomoff N. Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. JAMA. (2003) 290:337–44. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.3.337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, et al. Head circumference and height in autism: a study by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism. Am J Med Genet A. (2006) 140:2257–74. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Klein S, Sharifi-Hannauer P, Martinez-Agosto JA. Macrocephaly as a clinical indicator of genetic subtypes in autism. Autism Res. (2013) 6:51–6. doi: 10.1002/aur.1266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Reijnders MRF, Kousi M, van Woerden GM, Klein M, Bralten J, Mancini GMS, et al. Variation in a range of mTOR-related genes associates with intracranial volume and intellectual disability. Nat Commun. (2017) 8:1052. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00933-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sánchez-Puelles C, Calleja-Felipe M, Ouro A, Bougamra G, Arroyo A, Diez I, et al. PTEN activity defines an axis for plasticity at cortico-amygdala synapses and influences social behavior. Cereb Cortex. (2020) 30:505–24. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wu H, Li H, Bai T, Han L, Ou J, Xun G, et al. Phenotype-to-genotype approach reveals head-circumference-associated genes in an autism spectrum disorder cohort. Clin Genet. (2020) 97:338–46. doi: 10.1111/cge.13665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dingemans AJM, Truijen KMG, van de Ven S, Bernier R, Bongers EMHF, Bouman A, et al. The phenotypic spectrum and genotype-phenotype correlations in 106 patients with variants in major autism gene CHD8. Transl Psychiatry. (2022) 12:421. doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-02189-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Fu C, Ngo J, Zhang S, Lu L, Miron A, Schafer S, et al. Novel correlative analysis identifies multiple genomic variations impacting ASD with macrocephaly. Hum Mol Genet. (2023) 32:1589–606. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddac300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Busch RM, Srivastava S, Hogue O, Frazier TW, Klaas P, Hardan A, et al. Neurobehavioral phenotype of autism spectrum disorder associated with germline heterozygous mutations in PTEN. Transl Psychiatry. (2019) 9:253. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0588-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Uljarević M, Frazier TW, Rached G, Busch RM, Klaas P, Srivastava S, et al. Toward better characterization of restricted and repetitive behaviors in individuals with germline heterozygous PTEN mutations. Am J Med Genet A. (2021) 185:3401–10. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.62458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sharma A, Mehan S. Targeting PI3K-AKT/mTOR signaling in the prevention of autism. Neurochem Int. (2021) 147:105067. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2021.105067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. O’Reilly H, Thiébaut FI, White SJ. Is macrocephaly a neural marker of a local bias in autism? Dev Cognit Neurosci. (2013) 6:149–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (1994) 24:659–85. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lord C, Rutter M, Di Lavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition. Torrance: Western Psychological Services (2012).

Google Scholar

37. Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2009) 39:693–705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:2400–12. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1719-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Esler AN, Bal VH, Guthrie W, Wetherby A, Ellis Weismer S, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule, toddler module: standardized severity scores. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:2704–20. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2432-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Green E, Stroud L, O’Connell R, Bloomfield S, Cronje J, Foxcroft C, et al. Griffiths Scales of Child Development 3rd Ed. Part II: Administration and scoring. Oxford, UK: Hogrefe (2016).

Google Scholar

41. Deutsch CK, Joseph RM. Brief report: cognitive correlates of enlarged head circumference in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2003) 33:209–15. doi: 10.1023/A:1022903913547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Davidovitch M, Patterson B, Gartside P. Head circumference measurements in children with autism. J Child Neurol. (1996) 11:389–93. doi: 10.1177/088307389601100509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Miles JH, Hadden LL, Takahashi TN, Hillman RE. Head circumference is an independent clinical finding associated with autism. Am J Med Genet. (2000) 95:339–50. doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1096-8628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bonnet-Brilhault F, Rajerison TA, Paillet C, Guimard-Brunault M, Saby A, Ponson L, et al. Autism is a prenatal disorder: Evidence from late gestation brain overgrowth. Autism Res. (2018) 11:1635–42. doi: 10.1002/aur.2036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Aydin E, Tsompanidis A, Chaplin D, Hawkes R, Allison C, Hackett G, et al. Fetal brain growth and infant autistic traits. Mol Autism. (2024) 15:11. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lainhart JE, Piven J, Wzorek M, Landa R, Santangelo SL, Coon H, et al. Macrocephaly in children and adults with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1997) 36:282–90. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Torrey EF, Dhavale D, Lawlor JP, Yolken RH. Autism and head circumference in the first year of life. Biol Psychiatry. (2004) 56:892–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, Smith RG, Provenzale J, Ross A, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism: birth through age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2005) 62:1366–76. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Taddei M, Bulgheroni S, Toffalini E, Pantaleoni C, Lanfranchi S. Developmental profiles of young children with autism spectrum disorder and global developmental delay: A study with the Griffiths III scales. Autism Res. (2023) 16:1344–59. doi: 10.1002/aur.2953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Jansen JM, Green E, Stroud LA, Watson MB. Using the Griffiths III and Quartile Charts in assessing Autism Spectrum Disorder: a case study. J Educ Learn. (2020) . 9:30–40. doi: 10.5539/jel.v9n1p30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Li HH, Wang CX, Feng JY, Wang B, Li CL, Jia FY. A developmental profile of children with autism spectrum disorder in China using the griffiths mental development scales. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:570923. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Wang H, Du Y, Mao Z, Che Y, Li H, Ding L, et al. Use of the Griffiths mental development scale-Chinese in the assessment of children with autism spectrum disorder and global developmental delay/intellectual disability. Med (Baltimore). (2021) 100:e25407. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000025407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cirnigliaro L, Valle MS, Casabona A, Randazzo M, La Bruna F, Pettinato F, et al. The developmental autism early screening (DAES): A novel test for screening autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2024) 54:2418. doi: 10.1007/s10803-024-06274-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chaste P, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Murtha MT, Hus V, Lowe JK, et al. Adjusting head circumference for covariates in autism: clinical correlates of a highly heritable continuous trait. Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 74:576–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Fombonne E, Rogé B, Claverie J, Courty S, Frémolle J. Microcephaly and macrocephaly in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (1999) 29:113–9. doi: 10.1023/A:1023036509476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Denier N, Steinberg G, van Elst LT, Bracht T. The role of head circumference and cerebral volumes to phenotype male adults with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Behav. (2022) 12:e2460. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Valvo G, Baldini S, Retico A, Rossi G, Tancredi R, Ferrari AR, et al. Temporal lobe connects regression and macrocephaly to autism spectrum disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 25:421–9. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0746-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder, macrocephaly, head circumference, head growth trajectory, neurodevelopment, endophenotype

Citation: Cirnigliaro L, Clericò L, Russo LC, Prato A, Caruso M, Rizzo R and Barone R (2024) Head circumference growth in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: trend and clinical correlates in the first five years of life. Front. Psychiatry 15:1431693. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1431693

Received: 12 May 2024; Accepted: 23 July 2024;
Published: 06 August 2024.

Edited by:

Antonio Narzisi, Stella Maris Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Michele Roccella, University of Palermo, Italy
Marco Carotenuto, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Cirnigliaro, Clericò, Russo, Prato, Caruso, Rizzo and Barone. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rita Barone, cmJhcm9uZUB1bmljdC5pdA==

ORCID: Denis Gilbert, orcid.org/0000-0001-6561-2807

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.