Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 July 2025

Sec. Autism

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1444675

Acoustic features of emotional expression in 5-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder

Daichi Okuizumi,Daichi Okuizumi1,2Kazunori TeradaKazunori Terada3Azusa IshiiAzusa Ishii1Yoshimasa OhmotoYoshimasa Ohmoto4Hitomi ShimizuHitomi Shimizu1Akira ImamuraAkira Imamura5Ryoichiro IwanagaRyoichiro Iwanaga5Hirokazu Kumazaki,*Hirokazu Kumazaki1,2*
  • 1Department of Neuropsychiatry, Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Nagasaki University, Nagasaki, Japan
  • 2Hokusuikai Kinen Hospital, Ibaraki, Japan
  • 3Faculty of Engineering, Department of Electrical, Electronic, and Computer Engineering, Gifu University, Gifu, Japan
  • 4Faculty of Informatics, Department of Behavior Informatics, Shizuoka University, Shizuoka, Japan
  • 5Unit of Medical Science, Nagasaki University Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Nagasaki, Japan

Background: Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) exhibit poor prosodic performance, which is associated with their poor language and social skills. Prosody serves important communicative functions not only at grammatical and pragmatic levels but also at the emotional level. This study investigates the acoustic features of emotional expression in children with ASD compared to typically developing (TD) children, within a narrowly defined age cohort restricted to 5-year-old participants.

Methods: Nineteen children with ASD and 19 TD children, aged 5 years, participated in this study. We investigated the differences in the fundamental frequency (f0) ranges in three emotional expression settings (i.e., neutral, liking, and disliking).

Results: The f0 range in the neutral setting was greater in children with ASD than in TD children (p = 0.04). There were no significant differences in the f0 range between the three settings in the ASD group (p = 0.61). There were significant differences between the neutral and liking settings (p < 0.01) and the liking and disliking settings (p < 0.01) in the TD group. In the ASD group, a negative correlation was observed between the f0 range in the liking setting and the Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition T-score (p < 0.01).

Discussion: By focusing on the relationship between acoustic features and emotional expression setting and by restricting the age of participants, our results demonstrate the trend of acoustic features in children with ASD. To deepen the understanding of the relationship between f0 and emotion, future studies investigating prosody in a range of emotional expression settings are needed.

1 Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that causes significant social, communication, and behavioral challenges. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the US estimates that one in 36 children has ASD (1). Children with ASD may have difficulty developing language skills and understanding what others say (2), which can limit their opportunities in higher education and employment, resulting in an overall negative impact on their quality of life (3). Additionally, they often struggle to communicate nonverbally through hand gestures, eye contact, facial expressions, and prosody (4).

Prosody is concerned with the suprasegmental features of speech and refers to speech rhythm as well as affective, pragmatic, and syntactic communicative functions (5, 6). Prosody operates at various levels, enabling speakers to construct their speech using expressive language. Children with ASD have some prosodic differences such as atypical intonation (a monotone intonation and robot-like voice), incorrect word stress, speech rhythm differences (too slow or too alert), difficulty using a high or low pitch and controlling intensity, poor resonance (nasalization and pharyngeal resonance) and voice quality (7, 8). Poor prosodic performance may lead to poor language skills in children with ASD (9). Prosodic deficits represent some of the most significant barriers to social integration and acceptance (8), leading to impaired social functioning.

Prosody serves important communicative functions not only at grammatical and pragmatic levels but also at the emotional level (10, 11). The fundamental frequency (f0) is defined as the lowest frequency of the periodic waveform. F0 measures have often been used to identify specific acoustic markers of prosody that differentiate basic emotions (1215). ASD is associated with impairments in processing one’s own and others’ emotions (16). Grossman et al. (17) reported that the f0 range of individuals with ASD was wider than that of typically developing (TD) individuals when expressing emotions related to gladness, fear, anger, and surprise, in a sample of participants aged 8 to 19 years (17). Hubbard and Trauner (18) reported that the f0 range of individuals with ASD was not significantly different from that of TD individuals in the context of happiness, sadness, and anger, in participants aged 6 to 18 years (18). Hubbard et al. (19) found that the f0 range of individuals with ASD was wider than that of TD individuals in emotional expression contexts involving happiness, sadness, and anger, in participants aged 18 to 50 years (19). However, they reported that the f0 range of individuals with ASD was not significantly different from that of TD individuals for neutral topics. Therefore, the results of previous studies investigating the difference in the f0 range between individuals with ASD and their TD peers according to their emotion are inconsistent. One plausible source of these inconsistencies is the ambiguity of emotion-category labels. The emotion concepts in preschool children are still coarse: between 2 to 5 years of age they tend to group facial expressions mainly by valence and only gradually learn to single out specific categories such as sadness or fear (20). Moreover, constructionist accounts propose that these categories are dynamically assembled from context rather than fixed universals (21, 22). Mapping affect onto the continuous core-affect (valence–arousal) axes may therefore offer a more stable basis for comparing prosody across studies.

The correlation between prosodic features and age is complex, and interactions between them should be considered. It is well known that prosodic features are significantly correlated with speaker age (23). The prosodic features of school-aged children change with age due to factors such as acquiring accents (8). To deepen understanding of the prosodic features, research targeting children before entering elementary school is needed. In our preliminary study (unpublished), which involved children under 4 years of age, understanding the experiment’s explanation proved too difficult, and many participants dropped out. This confounding factor should be minimized by using participants within a narrow age range, restricted to 5 years.

In our preliminary study, we confirmed that 5-year-old children could express emotions of liking and disliking. However, role-playing tasks (i.e., story replay and demonstration tasks) seemed to be difficult for them to complete. By conducting a task that involves showing pictures that provoke the emotions of liking and disliking, it is possible to explore whether emotions can be conveyed through prosody.

McAlpine et al. (24) reported no significant differences in the production of rate, loudness, or pitch between children with ASD and those with TD aged between 24 and 68 months. However, the ASD group exhibited atypical stress patterns significantly more often, such as misplaced stress in multisyllabic words and reduced stress. Yoshimatsu and Umino (25) found that children with ASD scored lower on both prosody comprehension and prosody expression tests compared to typically developing 5-year-old controls. These findings suggest that, among children with ASD around the age of five, prosodic variability and atypicalities are frequently observed.

In this study, we investigated the f0 of children with ASD compared to their TD peers, restricting the sample to 5-year-olds, across different emotional expression settings (i.e., neutral, liking, and disliking). We predicted that our results would reflect a primary difference in the acoustic features of emotional expression in children with ASD.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

This study was approved by Hokusuikai Kinen Hospital Institutional Review Board (No. 081). The legal guardians of the participants provided written informed consent, and the participants provided assent to participate in this study. All procedures involving human participants were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. After receiving a complete explanation of the study, all participants and their guardians agreed to participate. All participants and their guardians provided written informed consent. The inclusion criteria for the ASD group were as follows: they had a diagnosis of ASD based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) by a supervising study psychiatrist (26); they were 5 years of age; and their IQ scores were 70 or higher. During enrollment, the diagnoses of all participants were confirmed by a psychiatrist with more than 15 years of experience in ASD using standardized criteria derived from the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO), which has demonstrated good psychometric properties (27, 28).

Children with TD were recruited from a public offering. The inclusion criteria for the TD group were: children had to be 5 years of age, have a Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2) T-score of 59 or lower, and attend a mainstream preschool with no evidence of intellectual impairment. A total of 19 children with ASD and 19 TD participants were included in this study.

Parents of children in both groups completed the SRS-2 (1) to screen for clinically significant autistic symptoms. Higher scores on the SRS-2 indicate a higher degree of autistic traits. Raw SRS-2 scores were converted to T-scores (with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10) for each sex. We classified the data as TD based on a cutoff value “59” according to the previous study (29).

The participants also completed the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) (30). The SCQ is frequently used as a screening tool in ASD research. It was designed as a questionnaire version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; (31)), the gold-standard developmental history measure widely used in research and clinical practice. In this study, we did not set a cutoff based on the SCQ score and only used the SRS-2 to gather children with TD.

Full-scale IQ scores were obtained using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III).

There were significant differences in the SCQ total scores (p < 0.01) and SRS-2 T-scores (p < 0.01) between children with ASD and their TD peers. Details of the demographic data are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of participants.

2.2 Materials

In this study, we categorized the participants’ speech into three emotional expression settings: neutral, liking, and disliking. These settings were selected because they occupied distinct regions along the valence-arousal and valence dimensions (32). Utterance in the liking setting evoked high arousal and positive valence, whereas utterance in the disliking setting evoked low arousal and negative valence.

Teaching materials were created using vocabulary acquired before 3 years of age, based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MB-CDIs) (33), which evaluate language development based on parent reports. The teaching materials included actions and pictures of the categories. The pictures were displayed on a tablet. We used 10 action-picture cards (throwing a ball, eating rice, swimming in the pool, walking on the road, waking up in the morning, cutting a tree, sitting on a chair, kicking the ball, drinking water, and putting on clothes). Examples of action pictures are illustrated in Figure 1. We also used 14 categories of picture cards (animals, sea creatures, 4-wheeled vehicles, vehicles, indoor toys, outdoor toys, food, vegetables, fruits, teenagers, colors, outings, occupations, and characters). In each picture card category, six nouns corresponded to one category. An example category image is depicted in Figure 2.

Figure 1
An illustration of a person in a green outfit enthusiastically playing dodgeball, with one leg slightly lifted and arms in motion, as a ball approaches from the side.

Figure 1. Example of action pictures.

Figure 2
Illustration featuring six cartoon animals in a grid: a giraffe, a gorilla, and a dog on the top row; a rabbit, a cow, and a horse on the bottom row. Each animal is shown with a playful expression.

Figure 2. Example of category pictures.

2.3 Procedure

Speech recordings in the three emotional expression settings were conducted in the following order: neutral, liking, and then disliking. Examiners of emotional expressions were blinded to the diagnosis group.

In the neutral setting, 10 action-picture cards were displayed one after the other, and participants were instructed to verbally name each action (e.g., “throw a ball”). After displaying all 10 action-picture cards, participants took a 1-min break before repeating the exercise. Each picture card was displayed, and the examiners asked the participants to name the object shown. Since the examiner did not utter the name of the object, the participants were unable to imitate their utterances directly. If the participants gave an incorrect response or did not answer, the data were removed from the analysis, and the examiners displayed the next action-picture card.

In the liking and disliking settings, 14 category picture cards were displayed individually. Participants were encouraged to select a card and express their liking as “I like giraffes.” Subsequently, they were encouraged to select another card and express their dislike as “I don’t like gorillas.” If the participants provided an incorrect response or did not answer, the examiners displayed the next picture card. The participants checked their expressed emotions using a 5-point Likert scale after completing all the sentence tests. The recording equipment consisted of a microphone (the Shure MV7) and an audio capture system (the TASCAM PortaCapture X8). The microphone was maintained at a constant distance from the participants during recording. The recording parameters included a 48-kHz sampling frequency and 24-bit quantization.

2.4 Criteria for utterance selection

To obtain relatively comparable speech samples from participants’ utterances, only complete utterances containing both a noun and a verb were included in the analysis, provided they did not meet any of the exclusion criteria. Utterances were excluded if they were questions, fillers, repetitive speech, consisted only of a noun or a verb, were interrupted by the examiner, were unintelligible, were directed toward someone else in the room and unrelated to the task, or were abandoned. Only complete utterances containing more than two words were considered. In the neutral setting, action utterances (e.g., “throw a ball”) were selected, whereas in the liking and disliking settings, preference utterances (e.g., “I like giraffes”) were chosen. These exclusion criteria were applied to ensure consistency in utterance length and type across participants.

2.5 Measurement

The audio data for analysis consisted of utterances in three settings. Using sound editing software (free software, Praat version 6.2.14), the sampling frequency of the recorded data was converted to 24 kHz, and the audio data were isolated for each sentence. We checked all audio data for artifacts that might interfere with accurate pitch analysis (e.g., background noise, coughs, or artifacts from glottal stops) and removed them. For each sentence, we extracted f0 (in Hz) with a timestep of 0.01 seconds. The f0 range, which measures the extent to which an individual’s pitch varies during speech, was calculated by subtracting the minimum f0 value from the maximum f0 value obtained from each sentence. First, the f0 range of each sentence was calculated for each emotional expression setting. The f0 range in each setting was subsequently averaged across all sentences.

2.6 Data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 24.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Differences in the number of speeches between children with ASD and TD were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U tests. Kruskal–Wallis tests were used to determine if differences in the f0 range existed in each emotional expression setting (i.e., neutral, liking, disliking) between children with ASD and TD. The Friedman test was used to determine the f0 range in each setting in children with ASD and TD. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were used to explore the relationships between the f0 range and the SRS-2 T-score and between the f0 range and their IQ in each setting in children with ASD.

3 Results

No significant difference was observed between the number of sentences in children with ASD and TD (neutral setting, U = 121.50, p = 0.09; liking setting, U = 131.00, p = 0.15; disliking setting, U = 142.00, p = 0.27). The details are presented in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The number of sentences in children with ASD and TD children in Neutral, Liking, and Disliking settings.

The f0 range in the neutral setting was greater in children with ASD than TD children (H = 4.24, df = 1, p = 0.04). There were no significant differences in the f0 range across groups for the liking setting (H = 0.79, df = 1, p = 0.37) or disliking setting (H = 1.26, df = 1, p = 0.26) between children with ASD and TD children.

There were no significant differences in the f0 range between the three settings in the ASD group (χ² (2) = 1.00, p = 0.61). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests revealed no significant differences between the neutral and liking settings (p = 0.31), the neutral and disliking settings (p = 0.62), or the liking and disliking settings (p = 0.62) in the ASD group. There were significant differences in the f0 range between the three settings in the TD group (χ² (2) = 13.37, p < 0.01). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests revealed that the liking setting had a greater f0 range than the neutral context (p < 0.01) and the disliking setting (p = 0.01) in the TD group. There were no significant differences between the neutral and disliking settings in the TD group (p = 0.87). The details are illustrated in Figure 3.

Figure 3
Box plot comparing fundamental frequency (f0) range in Hertz for Neutral, Liking, and Disliking conditions between ASD and TD groups. ASD (blue) generally shows a higher f0 range than TD (orange) across conditions. Notable differences with statistical significance are indicated by asterisks: * for p<0.05, ** for p<0.01.

Figure 3. The f0 range in neutral, liking, and disliking settings in children with ASD and TD. The f0 range in neutral, liking, and disliking settings in children with ASD and TD is shown. Error bars indicate ±1 standard error of the mean. In the neutral setting, children with ASD exhibited a significantly wider f0 range than those with TD (p = 0.04). Among children with TD, significant differences in the f0 range were observed between the liking and neutral settings (p < 0.01), and between the liking and disliking settings (p < 0.01).

In children with ASD, a negative correlation was observed between the f0 range in the liking setting and the SRS-2 T-score (r = -0.60, p < 0.01). There were no significant differences in the correlations between the SRS-2 T-score and f0 range in the neutral (r = -0.17, p = 0.49) and disliking settings (r = -0.38, p = 0.12). The details are presented in Table 3. There were no significant differences between IQ and f0 in the neutral (r = 0.11, p = 0.67), liking (r = 0.18, p = 0.46), or disliking (r = 0.40, p = 0.09) settings. The details are presented in Table 3.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The correlations between SRS-2 T-scores and IQ, and f0 range of neutral, liking, and disliking settings.

4 Discussion

This study investigated the f0 of children with ASD compared to their TD peers, restricting the age range of participants to 5 years, across different emotional expression settings (i.e., neutral, liking, and disliking) within a narrow age range restricted to 5 years. The results revealed that the f0 range in the neutral setting was greater in children with ASD than in TD children. This suggests that in emotionally neutral situations, children with ASD may show greater pitch variation compared to TD children. There were no significant differences in the f0 range between the three settings (i.e., neutral, liking, disliking) in the ASD group, whereas significant differences in the f0 range were observed between the three settings in the TD group. These findings suggest that children with ASD have difficulty varying pitch according to their emotions, whereas children with TD emphasize positive emotions in their speech. In children with ASD, a negative correlation was observed between the f0 range in the liking setting and the SRS-2 T-score, suggesting that increased severity of autistic traits is associated with reduced pitch variation. Our results indicate a trend in the acoustic features of children with ASD.

Previous studies (3437) have reported that the f0 range in general conversations in individuals with ASD was higher than that in TD individuals, which is consistent with the results of this study, that the f0 range in the neutral setting in children with ASD was greater than that in TD children. No significant differences were observed in the f0 range between the ASD and TD groups in the liking and disliking settings. This finding is consistent with those of Hubbard and Trauner (18), who reported no significant differences in the f0 range between ASD and TD groups aged 6 to 21 years during emotional expression. Conversely, Hubbard et al. (19) reported significant differences in the f0 range between ASD and TD groups aged 21 to 41 years during emotional expression. According to a previous study by Lee et al. (38), f0 variability usually decreases with age, beginning around 10 years of age. A previous meta-analysis (7) suggested that pitch difference between individuals with ASD and those with TD were significant during adulthood compared to other age groups, which may explain the discrepancies between the findings of Hubbard and Trauner (18) and those reported by Hubbard et al. (19). Given these factors, the finding of the present study–that there were no significant differences in the F0 range between the ASD and TD groups in both the liking and disliking settings–is understandable.

Contrary to their TD peers, children with ASD are unable to change pitch in areas where it is usually emphasized and are unable to adjust pitch depending on the communication situation (39). These results may explain why children with TD emphasize positive emotions in their speech, whereas children with ASD have difficulty varying pitch according to their emotions.

Nakai et al. (40) reported that pitch variation in word utterances was negatively correlated with the severity of autistic traits (40). A previous study also found that pitch measures extracted from the ADOS-2 conversational task were significantly negatively correlated with the SRS-2 T-score (41).

By focusing on the relationship between acoustic features and emotional expression settings, and by restricting the age range of participants, our study found a negative correlation between the f0 range in the liking condition and autistic traits, consistent with previous findings (8, 40). Given that the f0 range is associated with emotional expression (1215) and that emotional expression in favorable settings is linked to social functioning (42), the f0 range in emotional contexts may reflect social dysfunction in children with ASD.

When considered in the context of previous studies (8, 40), our findings support the notion that greater severity of autistic traits is associated with reduced f0 range, even in positive emotional contexts. These results underscore the potential future applications of (i) developing voice-based biomarkers utilizing the f0 range, and (ii) implementing interventions targeting emotional prosody.

Our findings—that children with ASD have difficulty modulating pitch according to emotional context, particularly in the lower f0 range of the liking condition, which is associated with greater autistic traits—can be interpreted within the framework of neurodevelopmental theories of emotional processing and prosody.

Emotional prosody processing is typically associated with right-hemisphere brain regions, including the right superior temporal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus (43, 44). Atypical development or reduced activation in these regions has been reported in individuals with ASD (45), which may underlie difficulties in modulating vocal pitch to match emotional valence.

Furthermore, theory of mind (ToM)—the ability to infer others’ mental and emotional states—is also implicated in prosodic expression (46). If children with ASD have reduced ToM abilities, they may not only struggle to interpret others’ emotional prosody but may also have difficulty expressing their own emotions vocally in socially appropriate ways.

These findings suggest that a reduced f0 range in children with ASD, even in positive emotional contexts, reflects underlying neurodevelopmental mechanisms affecting both emotional processing and social communication. This highlights the importance of incorporating such theoretical frameworks when developing voice-based biomarkers and interventions targeting emotional prosody.

4.1 Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, the sample size was relatively small, and most participants were male. Wehrle et al. (47) described the prosody of men with ASD as more exaggerated than that of women. Furthermore, parents have reported that boys with ASD are more likely to speak with an “unusual tone of voice” than girls (48). Therefore, gender differences in prosody may exist among children with ASD. Second, we did not conduct formal IQ testing in children with TD and instead relied only on the records of a normal preschool performance for those enrolled in this study. However, all the children attended mainstream preschools with no evidence of intellectual impairment. We confirmed that 5-year-old children with average intellectual and verbal competencies could complete the experimental process in our preliminary experiments. These results also demonstrated that IQ was not correlated with the f0 range in children with ASD. Previous longitudinal research (49) suggests that age-appropriate performance in preschool reliably predicts later cognitive functioning, including IQ. Therefore, assuming average IQ in TD children based on typical preschool performance may be justified, although we acknowledge the limitations of inferring cognitive level without formal testing. This limitation should be considered when interpreting between-group comparisons, as individual differences in cognitive ability within the TD group may not be fully accounted for in the present study. Third, since the participants were 5 years old, it was difficult to perform an experiment involving complex procedures. This study focused on the neutral, liking, and disliking settings. It is generally accepted that there are various emotions such as happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, fear, and surprise.

4.2 Future research directions

To address these limitations and promote advanced understanding, we highlight the following: First, studies with larger sample sizes, including numerous female participants are required to validate the results. Second, further research assessing IQ in TD children is needed. Third, further studies are needed to investigate f0 in a variety of emotional expression settings in order to advance our understanding of the relationship between f0 and emotion.

5 Conclusion

This study identified the acoustic features according to each emotional expression setting in children with ASD compared to TD children. Given the relationship between prosody, language skills, and social dysfunction, focusing on prosodic features screening children whose social function is poor, the significance of focusing on prosody is great. Further studies are needed to investigate f0 in a variety of emotional expression settings, in order to deepen our understanding of the relationship between f0 and emotion.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Hokusuikai Kinen Hospital Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

DO: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KT: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AIs: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YO: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HS: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AIm: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RI: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HK: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported in part by JST, CREST (grant number JPMJCR21D4), Japan.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the children and their families who participated in this study. Moreover, we thank all the team members of Nagasaki University for their incredible work on this project.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted without any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Cakir J, Frye RE, and Walker SJ. The lifetime social cost of autism: 1990–2029. Res Autism Spec Disord. (2020) 72:101502. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101502

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Vogindroukas I, Stankova M, Chelas EN, and Proedrou A. Language and speech characteristics in autism. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2022) 18:2367–77. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S331987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Al Jaffal M. Video-based interventions for adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Int J Ment Health Promot. (2023) 25:881–90. doi: 10.32604/ijmhp.2023.028982

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Strickland DC, Coles CD, and Southern LB. A transition to employment program for individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:2472–83. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1800-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Belyk M and Brown S. Perception of affective and linguistic prosody: An ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci. (2014) 9:1395–403. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. McCann J, Peppé S, Gibbon FE, O’Hare A, and Rutherford M. Prosody and its relationship to language in school-aged children with high-functioning autism. Int J Lang Commun Disord. (2007) 42:682–702. doi: 10.1080/13682820601170102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Asghari SZ, Farashi S, Bashirian S, and Jenabi E. Distinctive prosodic features of people with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis study. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:23093. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-02487-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Eigsti IM, de Marchena AB, Schuh JM, and Kelley E. Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: A developmental review. Res Autism Spec Disord. (2011) 5:681–91. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Peppé S, McCann J, Gibbon F, O’Hare A, and Rutherford M. Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2007) 50:1015–28. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ma W and Thompson WF. Human emotions track changes in the acoustic environment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2015) 112:14563–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515087112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Paul R, Augustyn A, Klin A, and Volkmar FR. Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:205–20. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1999-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Banse R and Scherer KR. Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1996) 70:614–36. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.3.614

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Juslin PN and Laukka P. Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychol Bull. (2003) 129:770–814. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pell MD, Paulmann S, Dara C, Alasseri A, and Kotz SA. Factors in the recognition of vocally expressed emotions: A comparison of four languages. J Phon. (2009) 37:417–35. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2009.07.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Thompson WF and Balkwill LL. Decoding speech prosody in five languages. Semiotica. (2006) 2006:407–24. doi: 10.1515/SEM.2006.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child. (1943) 2:217–50.

Google Scholar

17. Grossman RB, Edelson LR, and Tager-Flusberg H. Emotional facial and vocal expressions during story retelling by children and adolescents with high-functioning autism. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2013) 56:1035–44. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0067)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hubbard K and Trauner DA. Intonation and emotion in autistic spectrum disorders. J Psycholinguist Res. (2007) 36:159–73. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9037-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hubbard DJ, Faso DJ, Assmann PF, and Sasson NJ. Production and perception of emotional prosody by adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2017) 10:1991–2001. doi: 10.1002/aur.1847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Widen SC and Russell JA. Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cognit Dev. (2008) 23:291–312. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Russell JA and Barrett LF. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1999) 76:805–19. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.805

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Barrett LF. Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2006) 10:20–46. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Müller C. Speaker Classification I: Fundamentals, Features, and Methods. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer (2007). doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-74200-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. McAlpine A, Plexico LW, Plumb AM, and Cleary J. Prosody in young verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord. (2014) 41:120–32. doi: 10.1044/cicsd_41_S_120

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Yoshimatsu Y and Umino A. Characteristics of the understanding and expression of emotional prosody among children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Open Access. (2016) 6. doi: 10.4172/2165-7890.1000185

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. United States: American Psychiatric Association Publishing (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J, and Taylor C. The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: Algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:327–42. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, and Larcombe M. The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: Background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:307–25. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Constantino JN and Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale. 2nd ed. Kamio Y, editor. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusya (Original Work Published 2012), Trans (2017).

Google Scholar

30. Rutter M, Bailey A, and Lord C. The social communication questionnaire. Western psychol Serv. (2003).

Google Scholar

31. Lord C, Rutter M, and Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord Rev version. (1994) 24:659–85. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev. (2003) 110:145–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fernald A and Morikawa H. Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American Mothers Speech to infants. Child Dev. (1993) 64:637–56. doi: 10.2307/1131208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Eni M, Dinstein I, Ilan M, Menashe I, Meiri G, and Zigel Y. Estimating autism severity in young children from speech signals using a deep neural network. IEEE Access. (2020) 8:139489–500. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2020.3012532

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Godel M, Robain F, Journal F, Kojovic N, Latrèche K, Dehaene-Lambertz G, et al. Prosodic signatures of ASD severity and developmental delay in preschoolers. NPJ Digit Med. (2023) 6:99. doi: 10.1038/s41746-023-00845-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Nadig A and Shaw H. Acoustic and perceptual measurement of expressive prosody in high-functioning autism: Increased pitch range and what it means to listeners. J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 42:499–511. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1264-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Sharda M, Subhadra TP, Sahay S, Nagaraja C, Singh L, Mishra R, et al. Sounds of melody-Pitch patterns of speech in autism. Neurosci Lett. (2010) 478:42–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lee S, Potamianos A, and Narayanan S. Acoustics of children’s speech: Developmental changes of temporal and spectral parameters. J Acoust Soc Am. (1999) 105:1455–68. doi: 10.1121/1.426686

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. DePape A-MR, Chen A, Hall GBC, and Trainor LJ. Use of prosody and information structure in high functioning adults with autism in relation to language ability. Front Psychol. (2012) 3:72. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Nakai Y, Takashima R, Takiguchi T, and Takada S. Speech intonation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Dev. (2014) 36:516–22. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2013.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Asgari M, Chen L, and Fombonne E. Quantifying voice characteristics for detecting autism. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:665096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.665096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Van Kleef GA, Homan AC, Beersma B, Van Knippenberg D, Van Knippenberg B, and Damen F. Searing sentiment or cold calculation? the effects of leader emotional displays on team performance depend on follower epistemic motivation. Acad Manag J. (2009) 52:562–80. doi: 10.5465/AMJ.2009.41331253

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ross ED. The Aprosodias. Functional-anatomic organization of the affective components of language in the right hemisphere. Arch Neurol. (1981) 38:561–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1981.00510090055006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Kirk E and Sharma S. Mind-mindedness in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spec Disord. (2017) 43–44:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, and Dapretto M. Reading affect in the face and voice: Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:698–708. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S, Hill JJ, and Rutherford MD. The ‘reading the mind in the voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. (2007) 37:1096–106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Wehrle S, Cangemi F, Hanekamp H, Vogeley K, and Grice M. Assessing the intonation style of speakers with autism spectrum disorder. Speech Prosody. (2020) 2020:809–13. doi: 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. de Giambattista C, Ventura P, Trerotoli P, Margari F, and Margari L. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: Focus on high functioning children and adolescents. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:539835. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.539835

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Józsa K, Amukune S, Zentai G, and Barrett KC. School readiness test and intelligence in preschool as predictors of middle school success: Result of an eight-year longitudinal study. J Intell. (2022) 10:66. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence10030066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, prosody, acoustic feature, F0, emotion

Citation: Okuizumi D, Terada K, Ishii A, Ohmoto Y, Shimizu H, Imamura A, Iwanaga R and Kumazaki H (2025) Acoustic features of emotional expression in 5-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychiatry 16:1444675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1444675

Received: 06 June 2024; Accepted: 15 July 2025;
Published: 28 July 2025.

Edited by:

Cristina Costescu, Babeş-Bolyai University, Romania

Reviewed by:

Mohammed Al Jaffal, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia
Mrinmoy Chakrabarty, Indraprastha Institute of Information Technology Delhi, India

Copyright © 2025 Okuizumi, Terada, Ishii, Ohmoto, Shimizu, Imamura, Iwanaga and Kumazaki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hirokazu Kumazaki, a3VtYXpha2lAdGlhcmEub2NuLm5lLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.