Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 20 August 2025

Sec. Addictive Disorders

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1531443

This article is part of the Research TopicExecutive Functions, Reward Systems and Addiction in Adolescents and Young AdultsView all 8 articles

Integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control among substance abusers at the alternative rehabilitation communities in Eastern Thailand

Juthamas Haenjohn*Juthamas Haenjohn*Warakorn SupwirapakornWarakorn SupwirapakornJatuporn NamyenJatuporn Namyen
  • Department of Research and Applied Psychology, Faculty of Education, Burapha University, Chonburi, Thailand

Introduction: Inhibitory control (IC) deficit among substance abusers is a potential consequence of detrimental drug use and can also serve as a risk factor for drug-seeking behaviors, compromising substance abuse treatment and leading to drug relapse. This study examined the efficacy of an integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control (ICNIC intervention program) among substance abusers.

Methods: A total of 30 substance abusers were recruited and randomly assigned to either an ICNIC training group and a non-training control group. The ICNIC training group participated in a 12-session intervention program and a regular rehabilitative program at alternative treatment centers. The control group did not receive ICNIC training, but participated in only the regular rehabilitative program. IC was assessed using self-report measures and a cognitive performance task. Data were collected at three points: before ICNIC training, after ICNIC training, and at a 3-week follow-up ICNIC training. Statistical analyses were implemented.

Results: The primary findings showed that substance abusers exhibited greater self-reported self-efficacy of behavioral IC after the ICNIC training, as assessed by the self-report measure, Behavioral Inhibitory Control Inventory – Substance Use (BICI-SU). There were no improvements in the ‘Go’ or ‘Stop’ trial accuracy (neither within-subject improvement nor between-group difference at the post-training or follow-up periods), and SSRT reaction time (neither pre- to post-training within-subject improvement nor between-group difference at post-training or follow-up periods), as measured by Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST). Moreover, the lack of improved accuracy coincided with slower responses to ‘Go’ stimuli in the ICNIC training group as compared to the non-training control group. Thus, the ICNIC training improved self-reported self-efficacy in IC and, therefore, may be associated with an improved and cautious response strategies for IC paradigms, resulting in slower response times. However, these strategies did not translate to improved response accuracy during the SSRT.

Conclusion: The implementation of an ICNIC intervention program as a supplementary cognitive rehabilitation approach could potentially enhance self-efficacy of behavioral IC and improve response strategies among substance abusers. With further investigation, the program may be expected to contribute to an increase in cognitive control and promote behavioral changes that lead to positive therapeutic outcomes.

1 Introduction

1.1 Drug addiction and drug-induced cognitive changes

Drug addiction has been increasing worldwide, with an approximated 296 million drug users in 2021, and the number continues to grow. The World Drug Report (2023) indicated that an estimated 1 in 17 people had used drugs in the past year. Cannabis was the most commonly used drug worldwide with 219 million users, followed by opioids (60 million users), amphetamines (36 million), cocaine (22 million), and ecstasy-type substances (20 million) (1).

In Thailand, the rise of drug use is a major public health concern with the growing drug use and treatment admissions among illicit drug users, including crystal methamphetamine, methamphetamine tablets, ecstasy, ketamine, cannabis herb, heroin, kratom, and cocaine (2). Furthermore, the widespread use of drug cocktails, known as “happy water,” has been raising the alarm for nighttime partygoers. Happy water is a psychoactive beverage that can be produced by mixing synthetic drugs into a sweetened drink, typically including methamphetamine, ecstasy, ketamine, caffeine, diazepam, and tramadol all in one (3).

Evidence shows that amphetamine-type stimulants (ATS) are the most commonly used drugs in Thailand. ATS consist of synthetic drugs such as amphetamine, methamphetamine, ephedrine, pseudoephedrine, and ecstasy (MDMA) along with its derivatives (4). With its psychoactive properties, ATS exert potent neurotoxic and neurocognitive effects upon the brain (5). Both human and experimental animal studies have shown the neurotoxic and neuroinflammatory effects of amphetamine-related drugs that can lead to neuronal cell death, such as oxidative stress, excitotoxicity, reactive gliosis, cell apoptosis, and DNA damage (6, 7), subsequently resulting in cognitive deficits and neuropsychiatric symptoms among abusers. As a result, several previous findings have indicated that chronic methamphetamine use can result in cognitive impairment, including memory loss, attention deficit, impulsivity, executive dysfunction, learning impairment, and poor decision making (8). Additionally, methamphetamine users often show manifestations of psychiatric symptoms such as dependence, agitation, anxiety, hallucinations, paranoia, aggression, and psychosis (6). Previous study among Thai methamphetamine abusers has shown that these users with psychiatric symptoms exhibited even higher cognitive impairment than those without psychiatric symptoms, such as psychosis (9).

Chronic drug use induces significant changes in brain regions associated with cognitive functions, leading to cognitive deficits (10). Studies using functional and structural neuroimaging techniques in methamphetamine abusers have demonstrated that chronic drug use can cause damage to numerous brain regions in the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes. Some of these regions play a significant role in cognitive function, addiction, and emotional regulation. For example, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is an essential brain region for executive functions. The striatum functions in reward processes and addiction. The limbic system plays a role in emotional regulation, impulsivity, and aggressive behavior among abusers (11). Thus, brain damage associated with chronic psychoactive drug use results in cognitive deficits and neuropsychiatric symptoms, which subsequently has a detrimental impact on addiction treatment and drug use relapse.

As evaluated by the standardized cognitive screening tool, Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), approximately 70% of chronic methamphetamine abusers exhibit cognitive impairments (12). Previous meta-analysis has shown predominant deficits in impulsivity, reward processing, and social cognition and moderate deficits in executive functions, attention, working memory, visual memory, verbal learning and memory, and language/verbal fluency (13). Impairment of these higher brain cognitive processes is the adverse consequence of illicit drug usage on PFC, which is brain region responsible for executive functions (14).

Individuals with different types of substance use disorders (SUDs), including alcohol, cannabis, opioids, and stimulants, also exhibit cognitive impairments (15) with deficits in several domains, such as executive functions (e.g., shifting, inhibition, and working memory), attention (e.g., selective attention, attentional biases towards drug-related stimuli), memory (e.g., episodic memory), visuospatial abilities, and reward-based decision-making (15, 16).

The most common cause of cognitive deficits in substance abusers, however, is linked to prolonged drug use. For example, acute methamphetamine users exhibit an enhanced cognitive performance in terms of visuospatial perception, attention, and inhibition while, on the contrary, long-term use of methamphetamines is often linked to cognitive impairments (17).

Inhibitory control means the ability to self-regulate, suppress distractions to maintain focus, and appropriately manage thoughts. Inhibitory control signifies the human capability to think before acting, resist temptations, and overcome unexpected obstacles or problems (18, 19). Inhibitory control has been conceptualized as one of core components of executive functions (20). Diamond’s conceptual framework of executive functions is comprised of three core components: inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. Diamond has conceptualized inhibitory control as the capacity to control one’s behavior to overcome an instinctive reaction, such as the drive to use drugs, and instead carry on with tasks to achieve higher order goals (18).

Individuals with SUDs exhibit particular cognitive deficits in executive functions. The association between poor inhibitory control and an increased risk of substance abuse in adolescents has also been emphasized (21). Indeed, adolescence is the period of an on-going development of inhibitory control (22). Neuroimaging studies have implicated the brain neural networks and the contribution of impaired inhibitory control as a risk factor on developing substance abuse (2325). In parallel, prolonged drug use can lead to deficits in inhibitory control among many drug users, subsequently leading to cravings and relapse following periods of abstinence (26, 27). Previous studies demonstrated that dysfunction of frontal brain regions involved with inhibitory control may lead to the loss of control of the limbic system. Thus, when experiencing drug cravings or withdrawal symptoms, the brain’s higher cognitive processes are unable to effectively inhibit the drive of drug seeking behaviors (28, 29).

1.2 Cognitive rehabilitation in drug addiction context

Drug-induced cognitive deficits in substance abusers can be restored using two approaches: computerized cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation (16). Cognitive training contributes to an improvement in cognitive function in individuals with SUDs; additionally, it can be beneficial for addiction treatment by ameliorating cognitive deficits, alleviating addiction symptoms, and reducing the risk of relapse (30). Many types of cognitive training are implemented using computer programs and videogame-based training, targeting cognitive domains, such as memory, executive functions, abstract reasoning, problem solving, and processing speed (30). Previous studies have shown the efficacy of numerous computerized cognitive training programs and software on the enhancement of cognitive domains in frontal brain regions, including sustained attention, verbal memory, problem-solving, decision-making, cognitive flexibility (31), impulsivity, impulsive control, self-regulation (32, 33), and processing speed (34) among individuals with SUDs.

Meanwhile, cognitive rehabilitation or remediation emphasizes meta-cognitive training and strategy learning by focusing on goal-directed behaviors and decision making in real-world tasks. Top-down goal-driven behaviors require greater complexity and entropy to adapt cognitive strategies to both daily context and future goals. Therefore, cognitive rehabilitation approaches are considered to be more suitable (16). Cognitive rehabilitation or remediation techniques can be utilized to normalize the aberrant activities of brain reward systems and strengthen inhibitory control network of the brain. These techniques include standard cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), cognitive inhibition of craving, motivational interventions, emotion regulation, mindfulness, and neurofeedback training on addiction (35).

In addition, the third wave of CBT, particularly Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) have been implemented in treating SUDs (36). ACT has been shown to effectively treat a wide variety of SUDs, including polydrug use (37), smoking (38), opioids (39), and amphetamines (40). ACT increases psychological flexibility, subsequently reducing aberrant psychological symptoms, such as shame, depression, and anxiety, among individuals with SUDs (37, 4143). Other studies have shown that ACT can effectively reduce impulsivity and its components, including cognitive impulsivity, non-motor impulsivity, and non-planning impulsivity (44). Furthermore, it also shows a trend toward positive changes in executive functions, including inhibitory control, task monitoring, and emotional control (45). Thus, top-down executive control training with cognitive rehabilitation might be beneficial in reducing drug seeking behaviors and relapse.

1.3 The present study

From the aforementioned literature reviews, there are, however, no established cognitive and behavioral interventions that directly and effectively improve inhibitory control among substance abusers in Thailand. This research study therefore aimed to examine the effectiveness of an integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control (ICNIC intervention program) among substance abusers in Thailand. Fundamental principles of neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) were applied to the development of an ICNIC intervention program. The effectiveness of this program was evaluated by both quantitative and qualitative approaches. The conceptual framework of this research study is demonstrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1
Flowchart depicting an Integrative Cognitive Neuropsychological Program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control. It consists of Fundamental Principles, including neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). The program unfolds in three phases: neuropsycho-education, cognitive and behavioral skills development with brain stimulation, and application follow-through. Assessment of effectiveness involves experts' validation, substance abusers' reflection, Behavioral Inhibitory Control Inventory-Substance Use (BICI-SU), and Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST).

Figure 1. Conceptual framework of this research.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and procedure

This research study was designed as a pilot randomized controlled trial using both quantitative and qualitative research methods to investigate the efficacy of a cognitive rehabilitation program, known as the ‘integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control’ (ICNIC intervention program), upon substance abusers who were outpatients at alternative drug treatment centers in several Eastern regions of Thailand (see Figure 1 for research conceptual framework).

Thirty substance abusers (N=30) were selected for this experimental design with the sample size based on methods of F. N. Kerlinger and W. Wiersma (46, 47). These subjects were being treated at alternative drug treatment centers, which were selected using cluster random sampling. Participant inclusion criteria included a) age between 18-60 years old with at least 6 months of admission to the rehabilitation centers, b) being able to communicate in Thai fluently, c) absence of brain trauma and psychiatric illnesses based on medical history, d) right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (48), e) obtaining a low to moderate score in both our self-report measure and cognitive performance task, f) no history of opioid use that may affect inhibitory control, and g) voluntary participation.

Incomplete participation and the use of medications that might affect inhibitory control were considered as exclusion criteria in this study. Recruited participants were treated in accordance with strict ethical guidelines being submitted for ethical review and approval from the Human Research Ethics Committee of Burapha University (approval number IRB1-116/2023).

Simple random sampling was used to assign these participants into either the experimental group (N=15) and control group (N=15). The experimental group consisted of substance abusers who participated the ICNIC training intervention program and regular rehabilitative programs at the alternative treatment centers, while the control group included substance abusers who continued to participate in only regular rehabilitative programs at the alternative treatment centers. Quantitative data were collected using a demographic questionnaire, a self-report measure, the Behavioral Inhibitory Control Inventory-Substance Use (BICI-SU), and a cognitive performance task, the Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST). Participant feedback and evaluations of the ICNIC intervention program were collected as both quantitative and qualitative data.

2.2 Self-report measure

2.2.1 Behavioral inhibitory control inventory - substance use

Behavioral Inhibitory Control Inventory - Substance Use (BICI-SU) is a self-report measure developed by the present researcher based on Adele Diamond's conceptual framework of executive functions (18, 49). BICI-SU is constructed to measure behaviors associated with inhibitory control in the context of drug use during the previous 6 months. It contains 45 items with two main components and four subcomponents: interference control (cognitive inhibition and selective attention) and response inhibition (self-control and discipline) with a 10-minute time completion limit. The items are scored on a six-point Likert scale (1=never, 2=rarely, 3=sometimes, 4=fairly often, 5=often, and 6=always). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.97, and item discrimination values ranged between 0.26 - 0.77. The coefficients of variation ranged from 20.37 to 25.05.

2.3 Cognitive performance task

2.3.1 Stop signal substance task

Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST) is a computer-based task developed to measure the cognitive performance of inhibitory control with the integration of different experimental paradigms, emphasizing the inhibition of substance use (5056). It consists of Go trials (drug images paired with white circles around white arrows, 50% left arrows and 50% right arrows) and Stop trials (drug images paired with red circles around white arrows), with a total of 250 trials (200 Go trials and 50 Stop trials, in an 80:20 ratio). Two types of stop signals were implemented: visual and auditory. For the auditory signal, drug images were paired with red circles around white arrows that appear simultaneously with a beep sound. The Stop Signal Delay (SSD) is 250 ± 50 milliseconds. The scoring criteria for SSST were based on the Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT), and were calculated by subtracting the Stop Signal Delay (SSD) from the correct Go Reaction Time (Go RT).

Impairment of inhibitory control could be demonstrated using the longer SSRT and lower accuracy scores of Go trials. The test-retest reliability coefficients of the Visual SSST were as follows: %Go ACC =0.73, %St ACC = 0.78, SSD = 0.81, GoRT = 0.48, and SSRT = 0.65. For the auditory SSST, the test-retest reliability coefficients were: %Go ACC = 0.85, GoRT = 0.75, %St ACC = 0.81, SSD = 0.52, and SSRT = 0.53 (Figure 2).

Figure 2
Visual and auditory stop signal substance tasks, each with 250 trials: 200 go trials and 50 stop trials. Visual tasks require pressing keys based on circle cues and images, with stop trials indicated by a red circle. Auditory tasks add a beep sound during stop trials. Stimuli last 500 ms, with SSD starting at 250 ms.

Figure 2. Schematic representation of Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST), both visual and auditory tasks.

2.4 Cognitive rehabilitation program

2.4.1 Integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control (ICNIC intervention program)

The ICNIC intervention program was developed using a multidisciplinary approach that integrates principles from neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) to establish values and commitment in strengthening inhibitory control on substance use.

The concept of inhibitory control is based on Diamond’s conceptual framework (18, 49). Additionally, the ICNIC intervention program implemented integrated knowledge and learnable skills promoting the inhibition of drug use in rehabilitation centers. It includes skill training and learning activities related to inhibitory control, consisting of 12 sessions (duration: 50 minutes per session x 2 weeks). Nine sessions were performed in a marathon format within first two days (Day 1 and Day 2) and a brief 50-minute session on Day 5, Day 10, and Day 14 (3 sessions). The program was evaluated by 5 qualified experts in neuroscience, psychiatry, psychiatric nursing, and psychometrics to confirm its suitability and consistency with the program’s objectives, applied theoretical concepts, and targeted population.

The ICNIC intervention program is divided into 3 phases. Phase 1: the neuropsycho-educational phase, which consisted of 3 sessions (1. building rapport, 2. learning for brain plasticity, and 3. brain inhibitory control). Phase 2: cognitive and behavioral skill acquisitions, brain stimulation phase of which consisted of 6 sessions (4. values and goals, 5. parts of life, 6. observation without judgment, 7. inhibiting craving, 8. selective attention, and 9. the world of reality). Phase 3: The application and follow-through phase consisted of 3 sessions (10. checking in, 11. quickness and flexibility, and 12. integration and termination).

2.5 Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were performed using means, standard deviations, frequencies, and percentages. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with one between-group variable and one within-group variable was employed to compare scores of the BICI-SU and SSST during 1) the pre-ICNIC training period, 2) the post-ICNIC training period, and 3) a 3-week follow-up ICNIC training period, in which comparisons were made between an ICNIC training group and a non-training control group. The pairwise comparisons were performed using the Bonferroni method. The qualitative data were analyzed using thematic analysis. Data analyses were conducted using a specialized statistical program.

3 Results

3.1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of substance abusers

A total of 30 substance abusers were recruited from alternative drug rehabilitation centers and randomly assigned into: 1) an ICNIC training group (N=15), who participated in the ICNIC training program along with a regular rehabilitative program, and 2) non-training control group (N=15), which participated in only a regular rehabilitative program.

Demographic data revealed that the majority of substance abusers in both the ICNIC training group (53.33%) and control group (46.67%), engaged in polydrug use. The second most frequently type of substance use was ice (30.00%), followed by amphetamines (13.33%), and combined amphetamines-ice (3.33%), respectively. A majority of subjects in both groups exhibited more than ten years of drug use (66.67%), followed by two to five years (16.67%), six to ten years (10.00%), and less than two years (6.67%). Most participants were admitted for rehabilitation durations between less than one year and two years, with no reported physical illnesses. Although one substance abuser in the control group reported experiencing depression symptoms no such clinical diagnosis had been made (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of substance abusers (N=30).

3.1.1 Efficacy of integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control (ICNIC Intervention Program).

The inhibitory control of substance use among the recruited participants was assessed using a self-report measure and a cognitive performance task to evaluate the efficacy of the ICNIC intervention program. The BICI-SU was implemented to assess self-efficacy on behaviors associated with inhibitory control, while, the SSST was used to examine the cognitive performance of inhibitory control among these abusers during three periods: before ICNIC training (pre-training), after ICNIC training (post-training), and in a three-week follow-up after ICNIC training period.

3.1.2 Analyzed results of behavioral inhibitory control inventory - substance use (BICI-SU)

An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine the BICI-SU scores across groups and time intervals. Before proceeding with further analyses, Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity was performed to assess the assumption of sphericity (Table 2). The results indicated a Mauchly’s W of 0.83, a Chi-Square value of 4.76, and a p-value of 0.092. Since the p-value is above the threshold of 0.05, the assumption of sphericity was not violated, suggesting that the variances of the differences between conditions are approximately equal and thus satisfying the condition of compound symmetry.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of compound symmetry of behavioral inhibitory control inventory - substance use (BICI-SU).

The researchers conducted a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the mean inhibitory control scores at three time points—pre-ICNIC training, post-ICNIC training, and three-week follow-up training—to assess the main effects. Table 3 demonstrates the results of two-way mixed-design ANOVA.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Statistical summary of ANOVA: effects of groups and measurement intervals on inhibitory control.

The interaction between intervals and groups yielded a sum of squares (SS) of 8046.38, with a mean square (MS) of 4023.19. The F-distribution value was 11.14, with a p-value of <0.001 and a partial eta-squared (η2) effect size of 0.29, indicating a moderate to large effect. These results suggest that the interaction between intervals and groups had a statistically significant effect on the mean scores of inhibitory control at the 0.05 level. This significant interaction implies that the effect of intervals on inhibitory control varied depending on groups.

Additionally, the analysis revealed that the measurement interval (Interval) had a sum of squares (SS) of 45,554.79, a mean of squares (MS) of 22,777.40, an F-value of 63.07, a p-value of <0.001, and an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.70, indicating a significant effect on the mean inhibitory control scores at the 0.05 level. Furthermore, the experimental method (Group) showed a sum of squares (SS) of 18,190.09, a mean of squares (MS) of 18,190.09, an F-value of 9.68, a p-value of 0.004, and an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.26. The results suggest that the experimental method (Group) showed a significant effect on the mean inhibitory control scores.

Comparing the mean behavioral inhibitory control scores of BICI-SU within the ICNIC groups reveals significant improvements of inhibitory control, as demonstrated in Table 4. Post-training scores (215.40 ± 18.79) were significantly higher than pre-training scores (188.73 ± 34.14) at the 0.05 level. Furthermore, three-week follow-up scores (265.53 ± 12.85) were significantly elevated compared to both pre-training and post-training scores at p-value < 0.05.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistics and statistical comparison between groups and within groups of BICI-SU scores in three periods: pre-training, post-training, and 3-week follow-up.

Regarding within-group comparison for control groups, the results showed no significant differences between pre-training scores (186.40 ± 37.40) and post-training scores (186.80 ± 45.11). However, the results showed significantly higher mean behavioral inhibitory control scores at the three-week follow-up (215.21 ± 19.43) compared to both their pre-training and post-training scores at p-value < 0.05.

When comparing the mean behavioral inhibitory control scores between the groups, the ICNIC group showed markedly higher scores than the control group in relation to both the post-training scores (215.40 ± 18.79 > 186.80 ± 45.11, p < 0.05) and at the three-week follow-up scores (265.53 ± 12.85 > 215.21 ± 19.43, p < 0.05). However, there were no significant differences noted between the pre-training scores of the ICNIC group (188.73 ± 34.14) and the control group (186.40 ± 37.40) with a p-value of 0.788, which was consistent with the equal baseline level of behavioral inhibitory control between groups. Thus, these results indicate greater self-reported efficacy among substance abusers in terms of their ability to exhibit behavioral inhibitory control after ICNIC training.

3.1.3 Analysis of the results of cognitive performance task: stop signal substance task, both visual and auditory tasks

Performance-based tasks were conducted to assess inhibitory control among substance abusers. Table 5 demonstrates the descriptive statistics of SSST scores across three time points: pre-training, post-training, and a three-week follow-up.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Descriptive statistics of Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST): visual and auditory tasks of substance abusers (ICNIC group and non-training control group) in three periods: pre-training, post-training, and 3-week follow-up.

From the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) descriptive data, shorter SSRT reflects stronger inhibitory control. Our results showed a trend of improved inhibitory control only at the post-training periods in both ICNIC group (SSRT; visual: 111.54 ± 23.09 ms, auditory: 102.23 ± 50.96 ms) and non-training control group (SSRT; visual: 101.62 ± 29.72 ms, auditory: 93.74 ± 36.90 ms) when compared within groups. No trends were evident at pre-training or follow-up. However, none of these differences were considered to be statistically significant.

A slower ‘Go’ reaction time (Go RT) may indicate a more cautious or deliberate response strategy. Our results showed a trend toward slower Go RT among ICNIC group (Go RT; visual: 342.08 ± 18.08 ms, auditory: 331.96 ± 17.74 ms) compared to the non-training control group (Go RT; visual: 319.91 ± 19.91 ms, auditory: 312.56 ± 14.17 ms) during the post-training period. This pattern persisted during the three-week follow-up period, where the ICNIC group exhibited slower Go RT (Go RT; visual: 362.59 ± 30.03 ms, auditory: 358.70 ± 31.96 ms) as compared to the pre-training period (Go RT; visual: 346.64 ± 34.79 ms, auditory: 333.59 ± 21.20 ms).

The higher Stop-trial accuracy is typically interpreted as evidence of enhanced inhibitory control. The ICNIC group showed a trend toward improved Stop-trial accuracy during both the post-training (%St ACC; visual: 99.00 ± 1.88%, auditory: 98.43 ± 3.25%) and the three-week follow-up period assessments (%St ACC; visual: 99.00 ± 1.71%, auditory: 95.43 ± 4.11%) as compared to pre-training (%St ACC; visual: 91.43 ± 26.43%, auditory: 89.28 ± 25.98%). Nonetheless, these trends did not constitute statistical significance.

Although Go-trial accuracy varied across the three time points, no consistent improvement was observed. Overall, there were no statistically significant variations within-subject changes from pre- to post-training or in the follow-up period in terms of SSRT, Stop-trial accuracy, or Go-trial accuracy.

An ANOVA was conducted to examine SSST scores across groups and time intervals. Before proceeding with further analyses, Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity was performed to assess the assumption of sphericity (Table 6). The results indicated that the variables visual_SSD, visual_SSRT, audio_RT, audio_SSD, and audio_SSRT met the assumption of compound symmetry, indicating no violation of sphericity. However, for visual_%ACC, visual_RT, visual_%ACC-Stop, audio_%ACC, and audio_%ACC-Stop, sphericity was violated; thus, the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of compound symmetry of Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST): visual and auditory tasks.

Table 7 shows an inferential statistical analysis of SSST visual scores for the ICNIC training group and the non-training control group across three time points: pre-training, post-training, and the three-week follow-up. Our results demonstrate that the ICNIC training group (Go RT: 342.08 ± 18.08 ms) exhibited a significantly slower Go RT as compared to the non-training control group (Go RT: 319.91 ± 19.91 ms) during post-training period with statistical significance (p-value = 0.007). Furthermore, the ICNIC training group (Go RT: 362.59 ± 30.03 ms) continued to show a statistically significant slower Go RT score than the non-training control group (Go RT: 339.97 ± 22.90 ms) during the three-week follow-up period (p-value = 0.047). Meanwhile, the non-training control group demonstrated a significantly faster Go RT at post-training period (Go RT: 319.91 ± 19.91 ms) than the three-week follow-up period (Go RT: 339.97 ± 22.90 ms) with a p-value = 0.016. This suggests that, the ICNIC group exhibited a more cautious and better response strategy to optimize inhibitory control. This is often interpreted as a form of proactive control or a compensatory adjustment aimed at enhancing inhibitory task performance.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Statistical comparison between groups and within groups of Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST): visual tasks.

However, there were no significant differences in SSRTs between the ICNIC training and non-training control groups across the three time points. Likewise, other SST outcome measures did not show any significant differences between groups.

Table 8 shows the inferential statistical analysis of SSST auditory scores among ICNIC training group and the non-training control group at three time points: pre-training, post-training, and in the three-week follow-up. Our results demonstrate that the ICNIC training group (Go RT: 331.96 ± 17.74 ms) exhibited significantly slower Go RT compared to the non-training control group (Go RT: 312.56 ± 14.17 ms) during post-training period, with statistically significant p-value of 0.009). Furthermore, the ICNIC training group exhibited a markedly slower Go RT during the three-week follow-up period (Go RT: 358.70 ± 31.96 ms) when compared to pre-training period (Go RT: 333.59 ± 21.20 ms) and post-training period (Go RT: 331.96 ± 17.74 ms) with a statistically significant p-value of 0.001), suggesting an increase in response caution over time. Meanwhile, in the non-training control group, Go RTs during the three-week follow-up period (Go RT: 336.98 ± 25.50 ms) and the pre-training period (Go RT: 323.93 ± 20.16 ms) were significantly slower than the post-training period (Go RT: 312.56 ± 14.17 ms), with a p-value of 0.001.

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Statistical comparison between groups and within groups of Stop Signal Substance Task (SSST): audio tasks.

Statistical analysis of the SSST auditory scores demonstrated that the ICNIC group demonstrated a trend toward improved inhibitory control with a shorter SSRT during the post-training period (SSRT: 102.23 ± 50.96 ms) compared to the pre-training period (SSRT: 131.73 ± 73.67 ms), even though this difference was not statistically significant. However, SSRTs significantly increased at the three-week follow-up period (SSRT: 158.22 ± 75.10 ms) as compared to the post-training period (p-value = 0.048), indicating a decline in inhibitory control performance after training cessation. This could imply a potential immediate effect of the ICNIC program on inhibitory control among substance abusers during post-training period. The non-training control group exhibited a similar pattern, with SSRTs showing a non-significant decrease from the pre-training period (SSRT: 123.52 ± 70.60 ms) to the post-training period (SSRT: 93.74 ± 36.90 ms), followed by a significant increase at the three-week follow-up (SSRT: 161.47 ± 75.36 ms) as compared to the post-training period with a p-value of 0.035).

In regard to between-group comparison, there were no significant differences noted in SSRTs between the ICNIC training and non-training control groups across the three time points. Likewise, no other SST outcome measures indicated any significant differences between groups.

3.1.4 Analysis of the results of participants’ satisfaction and feedback questionnaire and interviews: quantitative and qualitative data

The researchers collected the feedback data from substance abusers on the ICNIC program using a 23-item questionnaire with a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.95 and discrimination values between 0.29 - 0.88. Based on responses using a 5-point Likert scale, our results revealed that ICNIC-trained substance abusers exhibited a high level of satisfaction with the ICNIC intervention program (3.98 ± 0.51), as shown in Table 9.

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Descriptive statistics of ICNIC intervention program satisfaction and feedback.

The ICNIC-trained substance abusers provided additional feedbacks on the program, which could be summarized as following: 1) they would like the ICNIC program to be implemented every year for monitoring brain functions, 2) the duration of program was too short because some participants had limited time, 3) they wanted more action-oriented activities rather than thought-provoking methods, 4) the program activities were well-developed, but the time was insufficient, 5) the program activities helped to increase consciousness when experiencing cravings for drugs or other stimuli, 6) it would be beneficial to have more activities that can be practiced, 7) they suggested adding informative video clips about the brain, 8) they expressed that the program activities were beneficial for them as well as for other drug abusers, 9) they wanted more academic lectures or workshops with experts for a large group of members, and 9) they wanted more media, video clips, slides, etc. to be included.

Furthermore, the activities of ICNIC program were diverse and provided opportunities to establish values and goals, as well as improve personal practices. The ICNIC-trained substance abusers offered their thoughts toward inhibitory control during program activities as following:

Mr. A: “I have applied inhibitory control skills in breathing exercises. The obstacles are distracting noises and interactions with people around me.” Regarding the Tower of Hanoi game, “I couldn’t manage my time.” As for the practice of accepting words that one dislikes, “I have adjusted my choices by practicing letting go of thoughts, being aware of changes, practicing breathing for five minutes, and body scanning for five minutes.”

Mr. B: “I have refocused on my set goals and practiced flexible thinking. The activities helped increase my mindfulness when experiencing cravings for drugs and other stimuli.”

Mr. C: “I have improved my choices by playing sports, practicing meditation, and breathing. Playing sports makes me feel good and relaxes my brain. Meditation helps me stay in the present moment more. Breathing exercises help me to be more mindful before doing anything.”

Mr. D: “I have become aware of my own and others’ actions and obstacles, such as my own lack of concentration, laziness, boredom, low patience, and sleep time.”

Mr. E: “I have learned about the outcomes of my choices, my goals, and how to analyze and persevere with the things I choose. I have realized my own determination towards achieving the things I choose.”

4 Discussion

This original research study was conducted to examine the efficacy of a cognitive rehabilitation program, the ICNIC intervention program, in order to enhance inhibitory control in the context of substance use. Quantitative and qualitative data were collected using BICI-SU, SSST, a feedback questionnaire, and reflection interviews. The substance abusers were recruited as outpatients from alternative drug rehabilitation centers, and they were predominantly psychostimulant users with over ten years of drug use history and less than one to two years of rehabilitation. Using a self-report measure (BICI-SU) our results demonstrated these substance abusers reported greater self-efficacy in their ability to engage in behaviors associated with inhibitory control following the ICNIC training.

When we compared training and non-training substance abusers, the BICI-SU data revealed that the ICNIC intervention program could increase behavioral inhibitory control of substance use among these abusers, indicating its potential efficacy in behavioral modification regarding the inhibitory control of substance use, although further studies are needed to elucidate these findings. When assessed using the cognitive performance task SSST, no improvements in inhibitory control were observed, as indicated by the ‘Go’ or ‘Stop’ trial accuracy and SSRT. Additionally, the lack of improved accuracy coincided with slower responses to ‘Go’ stimuli in the ICNIC group when compared to the non-training control group. The slower response of ‘Go’ stimuli could indicate a more cautious and better response strategy for optimizing inhibitory control.

Alternatively, the observed group differences could be driven by faster responses in the non-training control group rather than slower responses in the ICNIC group. Therefore, further studies are needed to validate and interpret these findings comprehensively. However, these substance users nonetheless demonstrated positive responses and reported satisfaction with the ICNIC intervention program. They reflected that the program activities and contents were very helpful in controlling thoughts and behaviors associated with the inhibitory control of substance use.

Inhibitory control deficits are behavioral and cognitive problems among individuals with diverse forms of addiction, such as substance abuse, gambling, internet usage, video game, sexual activities, and shopping (25, 57). The impairment of inhibitory control among substance abusers is the detrimental consequence of prolonged drug use. Subsequently, dysfunctional inhibition is identified as a primary risk factor for relapse and drug-seeking behaviors (26, 58).

Extending from previous research studies, a variety of self-report measures and cognitive performance tasks have been used to assess inhibitory control, impulsivity, and related cognitive constructs among individuals with SUDs. For example, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) has been utilized to assess impulsivity among cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent patients to predict the treatment completion (59). UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale has also been used to measure impulsivity among SUD in-treatment patients to examine the effectiveness of Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention (MBRP). UPPS-P Scale was implemented along with a cognitive performance task, the Stroop Word Color Test, to assess impulsivity and inhibitory control, respectively. However, this previous study showed no significant effect of MBRP on impulsivity dimensions and inhibitory control in patients SUDs in the therapeutic community (60).

On the contrary, our study demonstrated that the incorporation of the ICNIC intervention program with regular rehabilitative programs at the treatment centers can provide the additional therapeutic effect on behavioral inhibitory control, as assessed by the self-report measure BICI-SU. The substance abusers reported greater self-efficacy in engaging in behaviors related to inhibitory control over substance use. Further, our data arising from a cognitive performance task, SSST, did not show any improvement in inhibitory control, even though some parts of the SSST data demonstrated a potential link to an improved or more cautious response strategy that can be used to optimize inhibitory control. Thus, in this study, the self-report measure and the standardized laboratory tasks of inhibitory control did not correlate in evaluating the efficacy of the ICNIC intervention program. Consistent with previous studies, meta-analytic evidence has shown that self-report measures and task-based assessments of cognitive control do not overlap and might capture different psychological processes or constructs (61).

A further report has also indicated a substantial lack of correlation between self-report measures and performance-based tasks, focusing on different aspects of the theoretical construct of self-control (62). The distinction among these two measurements can be explained as follows: 1) self-report measures assess typical or habitual behaviors while tasks assess maximum or optimal performance under certain condition, 2) self-report measures reflect general behavioral patterns while tasks capture momentary and state-level responses, and 3) self-report measures assess broad and cross-domain inhibition while tasks assess narrow and domain-specific control (62). Thus, in the context of cognitive assessments, task-based measures and self-report measures may best be viewed as complementary, yet largely distinct, perspectives on cognitive control (63). In our study, incorporating both types of measurements has provided a more comprehensive assessment of inhibitory control.

In addition, previous research findings have indicated that standardized residential substance abuse treatment could exert therapeutic effects on inhibitory control over the course of treatment, as measured by the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale and a computer-administered stop-signal task, STOP-IT (64). Consistent with our results in this present study, non-training substance abusers who received only regular rehabilitative program at drug treatment centers also exhibited high levels of inhibitory control during the three-week follow up period, as assessed by both BICI-SU and SSST. Accordingly, initial signs of therapeutic outcomes in inhibitory control were observed in the regular rehabilitative program. Furthermore, the incorporation of the ICNIC program as an additional cognitive rehabilitation approach, with a direct emphasis on inhibitory control, appears to provide therapeutic benefits in drug treatment, preventing relapse and drug-seeking behaviors.

The ICNIC program was a researcher-developed, integrative cognitive neuropsychological program which aimed to foster inhibitory control as a therapeutic outcome in the context of drug addiction. This program was designed based on the fundamental principles of neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). It has been positively evaluated by experts in the fields.

Our results demonstrated the beneficial rehabilitative effects of ICNIC program in terms of fostering behavioral inhibitory control and more cautious response strategies as long-term therapeutic outcomes. Several lines of research evidence have indicated that some common characteristics of patients with SUDs undergoing drug treatment and rehabilitation include stress, cue-induced cravings, and loss of cognitive control. The currently applied therapeutic approaches have focused on cognitive-behavioral strategies, aiming to modify behavior that ultimately results in reduced consumption or abstinence in patients with SUDs (65).

Previous research findings have also shown the positive effects of cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation in the context of drug addiction (30). Cognitive deficits are common clinical manifestations in patients with SUDs (15, 16). Moreover, cognitive training exerts a beneficial effect upon the improvement of cognitive functions, including attention, memory, executive functions, abstract reasoning, problem solving, and processing speed among SUD patients, subsequently reducing craving symptoms and drug relapse (30).

Cognitive rehabilitation or remediation techniques primarily use cognitive-behavioral approaches in the treatment of addictive behaviors by incorporating meta-cognitive training and strategy learning that emphasizes goal-directed behaviors, decision-making (16), and cognitive control (35). In addition, these cognitive rehabilitative approaches aim to enhance strategies for coping with stressful conditions and cue-induced craving, as well as targeting erroneous learning mechanisms (65). Such techniques include standard CBTs, the cognitive inhibition of craving, neurofeedback training on addiction, neuromodulation, emotional regulation, mindfulness-based interventions, motivational interventions, cognitive bias modification, reconsolidation-based interventions, and virtual-reality-based cue exposure therapy, as well as pharmacological augmentation strategies (35, 65). Thus, both cognitive function and cognitive rehabilitation focus on brain regions associated with higher cognitive functions, including the PFC and amygdala (65, 66).

Correspondingly, ACT interventions have been shown to primarily promote psychological flexibility and directly improve cognitive function in certain domains, including executive functions, attention, memory, and subjective cognitive functions (67). Furthermore, ACT interventions have been applied in drug treatment settings (36), where they have been shown to improve psychological flexibility and alleviate psychological symptoms such as depression, anxiety, and shame among patients with SUDs (37, 4143). Other research evidence has demonstrated that ACT can effectively reduce impulsivity (44) and s indicates a trend in positive improvement on executive functions, including inhibitory control, task monitoring, and emotional control (45).

In the present study, the ICNIC program applied the concepts of ACT along with neurocognitive stimulation techniques as an additional cognitive rehabilitation program to enhance patients’ therapeutic outcomes at alternative drug rehabilitation centers. The program activities targeted higher cognitive processes, such as memory, reasoning, decision-making, cognitive flexibility, goal-directed persistence, and inhibitory control. Our results in this study could confirm the potential effectiveness of the ICNIC program in enhancing behavioral inhibitory controls in the context of substance use, fostering inhibitory control as a long-term therapeutic outcome.

Supporting the findings in this study, previous clinical and preclinical research studies have also shown that cognitive training and cognitive stimulation through new learning experiences can improve cognitive impairment and drug-induced symptoms by promoting adaptive neuroplastic changes (68). Finally, our ICNIC-training substance abusers indicated high levels of satisfaction towards program activities and gave positive feedback in controlling thoughts and behaviors associated with inhibitory control of substance use.

Similarly, adults and adolescents who received ACT-based interventions demonstrated moderate to high levels of satisfaction (69, 70). Universal ACT web-based intervention has been used to promote well-being and prevent mental health problems among adolescents. Research results demonstrated that adolescents who spend more time performing intervention activities exhibited higher levels of satisfaction, while, adolescents with less time usage on intervention exhibited lower levels of satisfaction (69). In addition, adults with eating disorders stated that they were somewhat satisfied to very satisfied with the open ACT group and that the treatment content was helpful (70).

5 Limitations and future research

This research study examined the beneficial effects of a supplementary ICNIC intervention program appended to regular drug treatment for promoting inhibitory control over substance use. However, this study contains some limitations. First, the small sample size might lead to non-generalizable results in terms of expressing the efficacy of ICNIC intervention programs. Second, the cross-sectional study design may have served to reduce the statistical impact of efficacy in ICNIC intervention programs. Longitudinal studies might provide a better understanding of the efficacy of an ICNIC program on inhibitory control in terms of long-term therapeutic outcomes and adaptive neuroplastic changes. Third, our use of a self-reporting measure, BICI-SU, could undermine the reliability of study results. Finally, the cognitive performance task, SSST, did not indicate any correlation with the self-report measure.

Future research studies might therefore consider incorporating additional cognitive performance tasks to measure the efficacy of ICNIC intervention programs on cognitive functions, particularly executive functions. An increase of the sample size to elevate the reliability of the research results should also be considered.

6 Conclusion

The ICNIC intervention program revealed an improvement in self-efficacy in inhibitory control over substance use and may therefore be associated with more cautious behavioral response strategies, as reflected through inhibitory control tasks having slower response times. However, further research is needed to confirm and expand upon these findings.

The ICNIC intervention program, when used as a supplementary cognitive rehabilitation program, appears to enhance behaviors linked to inhibitory control. The program’s activities appear to contribute to cognitive stimulation which increases inhibitory control and promotes behavioral changes. Hence, we suggest that the program might be implemented as an additional intervention over and above regular rehabilitative programs in order to increase the effectiveness of drug treatment strategies and thereby contribute to more positive therapeutic outcomes.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Human Research Ethics Committee of Burapha University, Burapha University, Thailand. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

JH: Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. WS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JN: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The research study was supported by the Health Systems Research Institute (HSRI) of Thailand (HSRI.66-135). The publication of this article was partially supported by Burapha University.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank staff and participants from rehabilitative centers for their collaboration in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. World drug report 2023 [Internet]. United Nations Publication (2023). Available at: https://www.unodc.org/unodc/en/data-and-analysis/world-drug-report-2023.html (Accessed July 12, 2024).

Google Scholar

2. United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. Synthetic drugs in East and Southeast Asia: latest developments and challenges [Internet]. United Nations Publication (2024). Available at: https://www.unodc.org/roseap/uploads/documents/Publications/2024/Synthetic_Drugs_in_East_and_Southeast_Asia_2024.pdf (Accessed July 12, 2024).

Google Scholar

3. Butler G. What’s in ‘Happy Water,’ a synthetic drug cocktail being taken by partygoers?Vice Media Group (2022). Available online at: https://www.vice.com/en/article/happy-water-drug-cocktail/ (Accessed July 12, 2024).

Google Scholar

4. Kanato M, Sarasiri R, and Leyatikul P eds. ASEAN drug monitoring report 2022. Bangkok: ASEAN Narcotics Cooperation Center (2023).

Google Scholar

5. Courtney KE and Ray LA.Clinical neuroscience of amphetamine-type stimulants: from basic science to treatment development. Prog Brain Res. (2016) 223:295–310. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Jayanthi S, Daiwile AP, and Cadet JL. Neurotoxicity of methamphetamine: main effects and mechanisms. Exp Neurol. (2021) 344:113795. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2021.113795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kim B, Yun J, and Park B. Methamphetamine-induced neuronal damage: neurotoxicity and neuroinflammation. Biomol Ther. (2020) 28:381–8. doi: 10.4062/biomolther.2020.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rusyniak DE. Neurologic manifestations of chronic methamphetamine abuse. Neurol Clin. (2011) 29:641–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2011.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Srisurapanont M, Lamyai W, Pono K, Indrakamhaeng D, Saengsin A, Songhong N, et al. Cognitive impairment in methamphetamine users with recent psychosis: a cross-sectional study in Thailand. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 210:107961. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. D’Souza MS. Brain and cognition for addiction medicine: from prevention to recovery neural substrates for treatment of psychostimulant-induced cognitive deficits. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:509. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00509

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Jan RK, Kydd RR, and Russell BR. Functional and structural brain changes associated with methamphetamine abuse. Brain Sci. (2012) 2:434–82. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2040434

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Wang TY, Fan TT, Bao YP, Li XD, Liang CM, Wang RJ, et al. Pattern and related factors of cognitive impairment among chronic methamphetamine users. Am J Addict. (2017) 26:145–51. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Potvin S, Pelletier J, Grot S, Hébert C, Barr AM, and Lecomte T. Cognitive deficits in individuals with methamphetamine use disorder: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav. (2018) 80:154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Melugin PR, Nolan SO, and Siciliano CA. Bidirectional causality between addiction and cognitive deficits. Int Rev Neurobiol. (2021) 157:371–407. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2020.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bruijnen CJWH, Dijkstra BAG, Walvoort SJW, Markus W, VanDerNagel JEL, Kessels RPC, et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment in patients with substance use disorder. Drug Alcohol Rev. (2019) 38:435–42. doi: 10.1111/dar.12922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Verdejo-Garcia A, Garcia-Fernandez G, and Dom G. Cognition and addiction. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2019) 21:281–90. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/gdom

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hart CL, Marvin CB, Silver R, and Smith EE. Is cognitive functioning impaired in methamphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2012) 37:586–608. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.276

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, and Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Zelazo PD, Blair CB, and Willoughby MT. Executive function: implications for education (NCER 2017-2000). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education (2016).

Google Scholar

21. Quach A, Tervo-Clemmens B, Foran W, Calabro FJ, Chung T, Clark DB, et al. Adolescent development of inhibitory control and substance use vulnerability: a longitudinal neuroimaging study. Dev Cognit Neurosci. (2020) 42:100771. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100771

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Larsen B and Luna B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher- order cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 94:179–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Koyama MS, Parvaz MA, and Goldstein RZ. The adolescent brain at risk for substance use disorders: a review of functional MRI research on motor response inhibition. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2017) 13:186–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zilverstand A, Huang AS, Alia-Klein N, and Goldstein RZ. Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron. (2018) 98:886–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hildebrandt MK, Dieterich R, and Endrass T. Neural correlates of inhibitory control in relation to the degree of substance use and substance-related problems: a systematic review and perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Moeller SJ, Bederson L, Alia-Klein N, and Goldstein RZ. Neuroscience of inhibition for addiction medicine: from prediction of initiation to prediction of relapse. Prog Brain Res. (2016) 223:165–88. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Morein-Zamir S and Robbins TW. Fronto-striatal circuits in response inhibition: relevance to addiction. Brain Res. (2015) 1628:117–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Telang F, Logan J, Jayne M, et al. Cognitive control of drug craving inhibits brain reward regions in cocaine abusers. Neuroimage. (2010) 49:2536–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Goldstein RZ and Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Caetano T, Pinho MS, Ramadas E, Clara C, Areosa T, and Dixe MDA. Cognitive training effectiveness on memory, executive functioning, and processing speed in individuals with substance use disorders: a systematic review. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:730165. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.730165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gamito P, Oliveira J, Lopes P, Brito R, Morais D, Caçoete C, et al. Cognitive training through mHealth for individuals with substance use disorder. Methods Inf Med. (2017) 56:156–61. doi: 10.3414/ME16-02-0012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Brooks SJ, Wiemerslage L, Burch KH, Maiorana SA, Cocolas E, Schiöth HB, et al. The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology. (2017) 234:1911–21. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4597-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Zhu Y, Jiang H, Su H, Zhong N, Li R, Li X, et al. A newly designed mobile-based computerized cognitive addiction therapy app for the improvement of cognition impairments and risk decision making in methamphetamine use disorder: randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. (2018) 6:e10292. doi: 10.2196/10292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rezapour T, Hatami J, Farhoudian A, Sofuoglu M, Noroozi A, Daneshmand R, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation for individuals with opioid use disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2019) 29:1273–89. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1391103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zilverstand A, Parvaz MA, Moeller SJ, and Goldstein RZ. Cognitive interventions for addiction medicine: understanding the underlying neurobiological mechanisms. Prog Brain Res. (2016) 224:285–304. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lee EB, An W, Levin ME, and Twohig MP. An initial meta-analysis of acceptance and commitment therapy for treating substance use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2015) 155:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. González-Menéndez A, Fernández P, Rodríguez F, and Villagrá P. Long-term outcomes of acceptance and commitment therapy in drug-dependent female inmates: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Clin Health Psychol. (2014) 14:18–27. doi: 10.1016/S1697-2600(14)70033-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bricker J, Wyszynski C, Comstock B, and Heffner JL. Pilot randomized controlled trial of web-based acceptance and commitment therapy for smoking cessation. Nicotine Tob Res. (2013) 15:1756–64. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Stotts AL, Green C, Masuda A, Grabowski J, Wilson K, Northrup TF, et al. A stage I pilot study of acceptance and commitment therapy for methadone detoxification. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2012) 125:215–22. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.02.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Smout MF, Longo M, Harrison S, Minniti R, Wickes W, and White JM. Psychosocial treatment for methamphetamine use disorders: a preliminary randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavior therapy and acceptance and commitment therapy. Subst Abus. (2010) 31:98–107. doi: 10.1080/08897071003641578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bahrami S and Asghari F. A controlled trial of acceptance and commitment therapy for addiction severity in methamphetamine users: preliminary study. Arch Psychiatry Psychother. (2017) 19:49–55. doi: 10.12740/APP/68159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hashemi Z, Eyni S, and Ebadi M. Effectiveness of acceptance and commitment therapy in depression and anxiety in people with substance use disorder. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. (2022) 16:1–11. doi: 10.5812/ijpbs.110135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Luoma JB, Kohlenberg BS, Hayes SC, and Fletcher L. Slow and steady wins the race: a randomized clinical trial of acceptance and commitment therapy targeting shame in substance use disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2012) 80:43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0026070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Baghani ZN and Akbari H. Investigating the effect of ACT training on self-efficacy and impulse control of addicts. J Adv Pharm Educ Res. (2020) 10:77–88. Available at: https://www.japer.in/article/investigating-the-effect-of-act-training-on-self-efficacy-and-impulse-control-of-addicts.

Google Scholar

45. Svanberg G, Munck I, and Levander M. Acceptance and commitment therapy for clients institutionalized for severe substance-use disorder: a pilot study. Subst Abuse Rehabil. (2017) 8:45–51. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S132255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kerlinger NF. Foundations of behavioral research. 3rd ed. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston, Inc (1986).

Google Scholar

47. Wiersma W. Research methods in education: an introduction. 7th ed. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon (2000).

Google Scholar

48. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Diamond A and Ling DS. Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In: Smith JK and Johnson LB, editors. Cognitive and working memory training: perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development. Routledge, New York (2020). p. 143–431.

Google Scholar

50. Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Band GP, Beste C, Bissett PG, Brockett AT, et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. Elife. (2019) 8:e46323. doi: 10.7554/eLife.46323

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Verbruggen F and Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cognit Sci. (2008) 12:418–24. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bjork JM, Keyser-Marcus L, Vassileva J, Ramey T, Houghton DC, and Moeller FG. Attentional function and inhibitory control in different substance use disorders. Psychiatry Res. (2022) 313:114591. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114591

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hu S and Li CS. Neural processes of preparatory control for stop signal inhibition. Hum Brain Mapp. (2012) 33:2785–96. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21399

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Karoly HC, Weiland BJ, Sabbineni A, and Hutchison KE. Preliminary functional MRI results from a combined stop-signal alcohol-cue task. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. (2014) 75:664–73. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Li CS, Huang C, Yan P, Bhagwagar Z, Milivojevic V, and Sinha R. Neural correlates of impulse control during stop signal inhibition in cocaine-dependent men. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2008) 33:1798–806. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301568

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Smith JL, Mattick RP, Jamadar SD, and Iredale JM. Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 145:1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Antons S, Brand M, and Potenza MN. Neurobiology of cue-reactivity, craving, and inhibitory control in non-substance addictive behaviors. J Neurol Sci. (2020) 415:116952. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2020.116952

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Jentsch JD and Pennington ZT. Reward, interrupted: inhibitory control and its relevance to addictions. Neuropharmacology. (2014) 76:479–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Winhusen T, Lewis D, Adinoff B, Brigham G, Kropp F, Donovan DM, et al. Impulsivity is associated with treatment non-completion in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent patients but differs in nature as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2013) 44:541–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Donate APG, de Macedo EC, Bedendo A, Félix Júnior I, Gallo GG, Opaleye ES, et al. The effect of mindfulness on decision-making, inhibitory control, and impulsivity of substance use disorder in-treatment patients: A randomized clinical trial. PloS One. (2023) 18:e0293502. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0293502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Göbel K, Hensel L, Schultheiss OC, and Niessen C. Meta-analytic evidence shows no relationship between task-based and self-report measures of thought control. Appl Cognit Psychol. (2022) 36:937–53. doi: 10.1002/acp.3952

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Wennerhold L and Friese M. Why self-report measures of self-control and inhibition tasks do not substantially correlate. Collabra Psychol. (2020) 6:9. doi: 10.1525/collabra.276

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Snyder HR, Friedman NP, and Hankin BL. Associations between task performance and self-report measures of cognitive control: Shared versus distinct abilities. Assessment. (2021) 28:1080–96. doi: 10.1177/1073191120965694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Littlefield AK, Stevens AK, Cunningham S, Jones RE, King KM, Schumacher JA, et al. Stability and change in multi-method measures of impulsivity across residential addictions treatment. Addict Behav. (2015) 42:126–9. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rosenthal A, Ebrahimi C, Wedemeyer F, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, and Beck A. The treatment of substance use disorders: recent developments and new perspectives. Neuropsychobiology. (2022) 81:451–72. doi: 10.1159/000525268

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Verdejo-Garcia A. Cognitive training for substance use disorders: neuroscientific mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 68:270–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Liu H, Liu N, Chong ST, Boon Yau EK, and Ahmad Badayai AR. Effects of acceptance and commitment therapy on cognitive function: a systematic review. Heliyon. (2023) 9:e14057. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e14057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Sampedro-Piquero P, Ladrón de Guevara-Miranda D, Pavón FJ, Serrano A, Suárez J, Rodríguez de Fonseca F, et al. Neuroplastic and cognitive impairment in substance use disorders: a therapeutic potential of cognitive stimulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 106:23–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hämäläinen T, Kaipainen K, Lappalainen P, Puolakanaho A, Keinonen K, Lappalainen R, et al. Usage activity, perceived usefulness, and satisfaction in a web-based acceptance and commitment therapy program among Finnish ninth-grade adolescents. Internet Interv. (2021) 25:100421. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2021.100421

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Mac Neil BA and Hudson CC. Patient experience and satisfaction with acceptance and commitment therapy delivered in a complimentary open group format for adults with eating disorders. J Patient Exp. (2018) 5:189–94. doi: 10.1177/2374373517748640

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: inhibitory control, integrative cognitive neuropsychological program, brain response, substance abusers, cognitive rehabilitation, alternative rehabilitation communities

Citation: Haenjohn J, Supwirapakorn W and Namyen J (2025) Integrative cognitive neuropsychological program emphasizing brain response to enhance inhibitory control among substance abusers at the alternative rehabilitation communities in Eastern Thailand. Front. Psychiatry 16:1531443. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1531443

Received: 20 November 2024; Accepted: 25 July 2025;
Published: 20 August 2025.

Edited by:

MoonSoo Lee, Korea University, Republic of Korea

Reviewed by:

Eric Andrew Woodcock, Wayne State University, United States
Damien Doolub, Henri Laborit Hospital Centre, France

Copyright © 2025 Haenjohn, Supwirapakorn and Namyen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juthamas Haenjohn, anV0aGFtYXNAZ28uYnV1LmFjLnRo; ZHJoYWVuam9obkBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.